Ten Years of Unraveling Juvenile Delinquency

27
Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology Volume 51 Issue 3 September-October Article 1 Fall 1960 Ten Years of Unraveling Juvenile Delinquency Sheldon Glueck Follow this and additional works at: hps://scholarlycommons.law.northwestern.edu/jclc Part of the Criminal Law Commons , Criminology Commons , and the Criminology and Criminal Justice Commons is Article is brought to you for free and open access by Northwestern University School of Law Scholarly Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology by an authorized editor of Northwestern University School of Law Scholarly Commons. Recommended Citation Sheldon Glueck, Ten Years of Unraveling Juvenile Delinquency, 51 J. Crim. L. Criminology & Police Sci. 283 (1960-1961)

Transcript of Ten Years of Unraveling Juvenile Delinquency

Journal of Criminal Law and CriminologyVolume 51Issue 3 September-October Article 1

Fall 1960

Ten Years of Unraveling Juvenile DelinquencySheldon Glueck

Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarlycommons.law.northwestern.edu/jclc

Part of the Criminal Law Commons, Criminology Commons, and the Criminology and CriminalJustice Commons

This Article is brought to you for free and open access by Northwestern University School of Law Scholarly Commons. It has been accepted forinclusion in Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology by an authorized editor of Northwestern University School of Law Scholarly Commons.

Recommended CitationSheldon Glueck, Ten Years of Unraveling Juvenile Delinquency, 51 J. Crim. L. Criminology & Police Sci. 283 (1960-1961)

The Journal of

CRIMINAL LAW, CRIMINOLOGY AND POLICE SCIENCE

VOL 51 SEPTEMBER-OCTOBER 1960 NO. 3

TEN YEARS OF UNRAVELING JUVENILE DELINQUENCY

An Examination of Criticisms

SHELDON GLUECK

The author is Roscoe Pound Professor of Law in Harvard University. Both Professor Glueck andhis wife, Dr. Eleanor T. Glueck, who is a Research Associate in Criminology at Harvard Law School,have contributed frequently to this JoutrNAL over a period of many years.

UNRAVELING JuvENlE DELNQUENCY, a major work by Professor and Mrs. Glueck, was publishedten years ago. The product of eight years' work with the assistance of a sizeable staff, the book pre-sented findings and analyses based upon a research project involving about a thousand boys livingin underprivileged areas of Greater Boston. UNRAVELING attracted a great deal of attention both hereand abroad and provoked a large number of reviews and articles, some of which were highly critical.In the present article, Professor Glueck not only answers the critics of UNRAVELING but also pre-sents significant post-publication data concerning various validation experiments relating to theGluecks' delinquency prediction tables.

The author prepared this article at the special request of the Board of Editors in commemora-tion of the Journal's fifty years of publication.-EDrroR.

Ten years have elapsed since the publication ofUNRAVELING JUVENILE DELINQUENCY' by Dr.Eleanor T. Glueck and the writer. The work onthat book spread over eight years, and to accom-plish the project we had to call upon the aid of astaff of social investigators, a psychiatrist-physi-cian, two physical anthropologists, six psycholo-gists, two Rorschach test analysts, a statisticalconsultant, two statisticians, and several secre-taries.

The book attracted a great deal of attention,both here and abroad. Shortly after publication ofUNRAVE LNG, the Supreme Court of Japan orderedits translation into Japanese; thereafter, a dis-tinguished board of representatives of the severaldisciplines involved completed the translation,3,000 copies of which were distributed to judges,prosecutors, probation officers, and others inJapan. A new edition of the Japanese translationis now in process. Parts of the book have also beentranslated into Hebrew, and a Spanish translationis almost completed. The more popular workfounded on UNRAVELING, DELINQUENTS IN THE

MAKING, 2 has been rendered into French and

I GLuECIC, S. & E. T., UNRAVELING JUVENI.EDELINQuENcY (Foreword by E. N. Griswold) (NewYork, The Commonwealth Fund, Cambridge, Har-vard University Press, 1950, Third Printing, 1957).2 GLUECK, S. & E. T., DELINQUENTS IN THE MAKING(New York, Harper and Bros., 1952).

Italian. Spanish and Japanese versions are inprocess; a German one is contemplated.

The book has greatly influenced research hereand abroad and is well known to students of crimin-ology, psychiatrists, anthropologists, juvenilecourt judges, and others.

We are grateful to all who wrote reviews andarticles about UNMAVELING. It should be realizedthat it was a pioneering effort. We, as well as ourcritics, can profit from its shortcomings-a happyconsequence in the tradition of the history ofscience.

UNRAVEmL G has been honored by three sym-posia of reviews: one in the Harvard Law Review, 3

a second in the Journal of Criminal Law, Crimin-ology, and Police Science, a third in Federal Pro-bation. 4 It has been very widely reviewed in otherjournals both here and abroad. It was the subjectof two extremely critical articles in the AmericanJournal of Sociology.5 On the other hand, most ofthe reviews have been favorable.

3 A Symposium on Unravding Juvenile Delinquency,64 HA.v. L R.V. 1022-41 (1951).

4 Unraveling Jvenile Ddinquency-A Symposiumof Re-deews, 41 J. Cm u L., C. & P. S. 732-59 (1951);15 FED. PROB. 52-58 (No. 1 1951).

5 Rubin, Unravding Juvenile Delinquency. I. Illu-sions in a Research Project Using Matched Pairs, 57Am. J. Soc. 107-14 (1951); Reiss, Unravding JuvenileDelinquency. II. An Appraisal of the Research Methods,id. at 115-20.

SHELDON GLUECK

The most confident and severest critics havebeen a group whose writings have the tone of fire-breathing chevaliers eager to do battle for thatpurest queen of the exact sciences, Sociology, towhich the authors of URAVELING allegedly did notpay adequate tribute. 6

We have waited for most of the reviews to ap-pear before attempting a reply to the principalpoints raised. These may be summarized under theheads of (I) The Sample, (II) The Techniques ofInvestigation, (III) Causative Analysis, (IV) TheRole of Culture, and (V) The Prediction Method.

I. TnE SAliPLE

A. Areas from which the sample of delinquents andthat of nondelinquents were drawn. The simplestway to attack any piece of research involving acomplex biosocial problem is of course to deny atonce the "typicality of the sample." Now it iselementary that any sample, especially one in-volving a social problem, is in some respects atypi-cal of various factors and forces that might berelevant to the issues sought to be illuminated bythe inquiry. Unless one included all the juvenile

delinquents and nondelinquents in the world and

their detailed makeup and background (not to

stress the history of the universe), he would, by

such facile criticism, be wasting his time. It is

particularly difficult to obtain absolute similarity

in respect to the myriads of factors and forces in

any area from which delinquents and nondelin-quents may be drawn.

Some critics fail to fulfill the burden of proving in

exactly what respects the samples compared in

UNRAVELING are atypical. It would seem that they

6 ".... there are occasions on which sociologists canunite-and this exemplifies the sociological principlethat attacks from an external enemy tend to unite anin-group that is at conflict within itself." Hartung,A Critique of the Sociological Approach to Crime andCorrection, 23 LAw & CoNTE iP. PROB. 704 (1958).(They will no doubt unite to attack the present article.)"Edwin H. Sutherland may be said to have enteredthe lists against Unraveling Juvenile Delinquency inadvance." "One of the great values of (Terence)Morris's book is his lengthy historical chapter on re-search during the nineteenth century, which helps toestablish the legitimate claim of sociology to the fieldof criminology." Hartung, id. at 707, 726. In addition,Hartung several times quotes approvingly expressionsof great regret by Sutherland at the way nonsociologiccriminologists have been able to get research fundsfrom foundations, even quoting the word "seduced."For the record, let it be said that the Gluecks havealways had to work hard to obtain subsidization oftheir researches and they have never used seduction,deception or highway robbery to obtain this hard-wonfinancial aid.

should wait until at least one study similar toours-employing the same definitions and verify-ing the raw data with equal care-was done; thenthere would be a much more substantial basis forjudgment of typicality of the samples than themere ipse dixit of the critics.

In the meantime, let us examine the more spe-cific criticisms advanced with reference to thesamples compared in UNRAVELING:

It has been urged that the areas from which thedelinquents were selected were not absolutely simi-lar to the areas from which the nondelinquentswere chosen. In taking both the delinquentsand the nondelinquents from economically andculturally deprived regions, our aim was to "con-trol" major underprivileged area influences andthus set the stage for more detailed comparison ofthe two groups in respect to a great many morespecific traits and factors-anthropologic, medical(health), psychiatric (temperament), personality-character structure (Rorschach test), and varioussocial-psychologic factors of what we call theunder-the-roof culture of the home and school,as well as certain influences and conditions in theboys' neighborhoods.

Professor E. N. Burgess supports his claim thatthe regions from which the delinquent and nonde-linquent boys came were not similar by empha-sizing Table VIII-1- Type of Neighborhood in WhichBoy Lives (UNRAVELING, p. 79).7 In that table wepresent a breakdown of the neighborhoods interms, largely, of Blighted slum tenement area (de-teriorating socially and economically) and Intersti-tial area (merging of business and residential). Thetable shows that 55 per cent of the delinquents'families lived in the first-mentioned type of neigh-borhood, while 34 per cent of the nondelinquents'did; and 31 per cent of the former lived in thesecond type of neighborhood, while 49 per cent ofthe latter did.

However, this is but one type of subclassifica-tion of the neighborhoods. Professor Burgess un-fortunately fails to call attention to the other, moresignificant, comparisons in the book, those of a na-ture much more relevant to the central aim of thecontrol of the general culture for our expressedpurpose of comparing delinquents and nondelin-quents in respect to more specific characters. Forexample, he fails to mention Table IV-2 (p. 36) inwhich the delinquency rates of the two sets of areas

7 Burgess, Symposium on the Gluecks' Latest Re-search, 15 FED. PROB. 52-53 (No. 1 1951).

[Vol. 51

TEN YEARS OF UNRAVELING DELINQUENCY

compared are demonstrated to be remarkablysimilar. Nor does he allude to Table IV-3 (p. 36),presenting the results of a very careful field ap-praisal by trained researchers, who know the re-gions thoroughly, in terms of defined "good,""fair," and "poor" neighborhoods (from the pointsof view of the presence or absence of vice, crime,gangs, etc., on the one hand, and the presence orabsence of reasonably convenient facilities forwholesome recreation, on the other). This expertassessment, like the similarity of the delinquencyrates, demonstrates that the crucial neighborhoodinfluences existing around the homes of our de-linquents were very similar to those of the non-delinquents.

Moreover, while for certain theoretical sociologicpurposes it may be helpful to classify regions into"blighted slum areas" or "interstitial areas," etc.,the sociological area-criminologist himself does notmake this the significant distinction when he getsdown to the study of delinquency in the field. Hefirst defines his areas in terms of the rates of delin-quency (as we did), then he compares the incidenceof certain eco~wmic and socialfactors existing in areasor zones of different delinquency rates (as we did).He analyzes the situation in low income areas asopposed to high income areas8 and relates the otherfactors to that distinction rather than to whetherhe is dealing with a blighted or interstitial area.The well-known works of sociologist CliffordShaw and his associates, for example, do not differ-entiate between areas on the latter basis for thepurpose of ultimate analysis of delinquency causa-tion. Their familiar spot-maps and circular zonalmaps cut across regions part of which may betermed either "blighted slum areas" or "interstitialareas," or both. Shaw and McKay emphasizeeconomic status as one of the most generative andpervasive of the conditions under which "the con-ventional forces in the community become soweakened as to tolerate the development of a con-flicting system of criminal values."9 They say:

"It may be observed, in the first instance,that the variations in rates of officially recordeddelinquents in communities of the city corre-spond very closely with variations in economicstatus. The communities with the highest ratesof delinquents are occupied by those segmentsof the population whose position is most dis-8 See, for example, SHAw & MCKAY, JuvEN LE Dx-

LiNQuENCY AND URBAN AREAs 435 et seq. (Chicago,University of Chicago Press).

9Id. at 437.

advantageous in relation to the distribution ofeconomic, social, and cultural values. Of all thecommunities in the city, these have the fewestfacilities for acquiring the economic goods in-dicative of status and success in our conven-tional culture."" °

Applying such a relevant yardstick, then, as eco-nomico-cultural status, our nondelinquents haveunquestionably been chosen from areas relativelysimilar to those of our delinquents, so like in factthat not a few delinquents lived within the samecity block or two as the nondelinquents and some-times even in the very same tenement house.

It happens that Shaw and McKay had plotteddelinquency rates in Boston shortly before we beganour research for UNRAVELING, and their geo-graphic distribution of various delinquency ratesis very similar to our own. One of their conclusionswas: "Considering the area [of Boston] as a whole,heavy concentrations are noted in old Boston,especially in the areas north and northeast of theCommon, known locally as the West End and theNorth End; and in Charlestown, East Boston, andSouth Boston. Only slightly less concentrated arethe clusters in Roxbury."" Both our nondelinquentgroup and our delinquent group came very largelyfrom these same regions and, in very similar pro-portions, from the same census tracts. (See UN-RAVELING, Table IV-2 at 36).

As we pointed out in UNRAVELING,"Since it was our purpose to draw the boys

only from neighborhoods in which the environ-mental conditions were deleterious to the whole-some development of youth, the fact that fewerdelinquents than nondelinquents... were foundto be living in so-called interstitial areas (merg-ing of business or industrial and residentialelements) and a larger proportion in blightedtenement areas has no significance. What is ofimportance for our purposes is the extent towhich the general neighborhood environmentswere alike in both groups."' 2

It is amusing to find certain sociologic criminolo-gists insisting upon the ultra-precision of the physi-cal and chemical laboratory in this type of com-parison.13 We should be grateful to have our atten-

10 Id. at 437-438.n Id. at 222-223.1UNRAVELING at 35.1Taft says: "Second, the delinquents and non-

delinquents were paired in the neighborhood by physi-cal proximity only. Because of this neglect of socialinfluences in the neighborhood, it is not possible toaccept the Gluecks' claim that the two groups in the

SHELDON GLUECK

tion directed to even one study by them whichachieves such absolute and detailed identity in amatching of two culture-areas. Certainly the"proof" of the favorite sociologic theory of "differ-ential association" is notoriously imprecise. 14 Onecannot help being reminded in this connection ofpoor little Brother Juniper's burning belief (inThornton Wilder's delightful novel, THE BRIDGE

OF SAN Luis REY): "It seemed to Brother juniperthat it was high time for theology to take its placeamong the exact sciences, and he had long intendedputting it there."

In this connection I should like also to advertto a criticism by a reviewer who confuses our aimto control the general neighborhood-cultural com-plex of influences for statistical purposes with thefinding of differences in various detailed sociologicfactors which could only have been, and whichwere in fact, brought out by our factoral com-parisons of the delinquents with the nonde-linquents. This critic seems to imply that we wereengaged in some deep-dyed plot to banish socio-logic influences. 5 This is sheer nonsense. just as we

study were actually paired by identical social influencesof the neighborhood." Taft, Implication of the GlueckMethology for Criminal Research, 42 J. CRis. L., C.& P. S. 300 (1951). It is perfectly obvious that absoluteidentity of the myriads of influences involved in neigh-borhoods is impossible, nor did the Gluecks claim that.Reiss's view that "the study largely ignored the in-fluence of primary groups in guiding behavior and inenforcing conformity to sets of norms," the familiarsociological formula in familiar sociological jargon, isa convenient attempt to explain away some of thedisturbing findings of UNRAVELING; but nobody hasdefined these supposed narrow-area and extremelysubtle differences in the locales from which our twogroups of boys were taken; nor has any critic shownexactly how these minute differences could have beendetermined and how they would affect the outcome.Besides, the argument is quite irrelevant because wefound that the great majority of our boys .(90%) werealready delinquent at under 11 years of age, long beforethe alleged influence of "primary groups" in sup-posedly "enforcing conformity to sets of norms" of adelinquent type could make itself felt. A similar com-ment can be made with respect to the more penetratingsuggestion put forth by Gault to the effect that areasmay differ in terms of the leaders of a community.Gault, in Unraveling Juvenile Delinquency-A Symn-posium of Reviews, 41 J. CRnr. L., C. & P. S. at 734-35(1951). The method used in selecting the neighbor-hoods is discussed in UNRAVELING at 30-31.

14 See Glueck, S., Theory and Fact in Criminology, 7BaT. J. DELINQ. 92-109 (1956).

', "In place of a study which sought steadfastly toeliminate environmental factors as well as to eliminatethem from a causal law, the force of social (or environ-mental) causation. of delinquency proves irrepressible."Rubin, op. cit. supra note 5 at 107, 113. Hartung re-peats this misconception: "First, although the Glueckssought steadfastly to eliminate environmental influ-ences, the great influence of social causation proved to

did our utmost to match each delinquent with anondelinquent for age, I.Q., and ethnic derivation,so did we control the general cultural area situa-tion in respect to delinquents and nondelinquentsas a basis for the subsequent detailed comparisonof the incidence of the psychologic and socio-cul-tural traits and factors in the home, school, andneighborhood. 16 As to many of these characters,statistical comparison proved the status of thedelinquents to be significantly different from thatof the nondelinquents. The differences were veryclearly brought out, taking full account of thepossible influence of chance and they establishedfactually the point we made in the introductorychapter of UNRAVELING (p. 5) with respect to thedifference between the typical sociologic "area"or "subculture" study of delinquency and our owneclectic approach-a point which forms a majoraspect of our conception of etiology:

"The area-studies establish that a region ofeconomic and cultural disorganization tends tohave a criminogenic effect on people residingtherein; but the studies fail to emphasize thatthis influence affects only a selected group com-prising a relatively small proportion of all theresidents. They do not reveal why the de-letereous influences of even the most extremedelinquency area fail to turn the great majority

be irrepressible in their data." Hartung, op. cit. supranote 6 at 706. The Gluecks neither "sought stead-fastly to eliminate environmental factors" from theresearch nor to "eliminate them from a causal law";nor does "the force of social (or environmental) causa-tion of delinquency prove irrepressible" from analysisof our materials, which establish a mudticausal influ-ence neither exclusively social nor exclusively bio-logic.

1" "Socio-cultural level. A primary approach to theanalysis of crime causation is the exploration of theforces and circumstances of the family background andthe physical and social environment of the home; forcharacter and conduct may be said to consist of theresult of the interplay between innate constitution andenvironmental forces. The selection of the delinquentsand the nondelinquents from similar neighborhoods ofunderprivilege carried with it, of course, no necessaryassumptign that poverty and its correlates or thecultural forces of urban delinquency areas play no rolein the causation of delinquency. However, just as thematching of delinquents with nondelinquents on thebasis of general intelligence permits us to examine theconstituents and patternings of intelligence, so match-ing by residence in underprivileged neighborhoodsmakes it possible to control the general factor of areaculture and to determine whether subtler differencesexist in respect to the more intimate socio-culturalfactors of home and school and the attitudes andpractices of parents, teachers, and companions asbearers of the home and school culture." UNRAVELINGat 16.

[Vol. 51

TEN YEARS OF UNRAVELING DELINQUENCY

of its boys into persistent delinquents. They donot disclose whether the children who do notsuccumb to the evil and disruptive neighborhoodinfluence differ from those who become delin-quents and, if so, in what respects. Until theytake this factor into account, they cannot pene-tratingly describe even the culture of the delin-quency area. For to say that certain bacteriahave a fatal effect on some individuals but nosuch effect on the majority, without describingor explaining the differential influences, is todescribe that infective agent inadequately."

How do some sociologic critics answer this? By thefollowing reductio ad absurdum: "First, it is gen-erally recognized that there is a gap of unknownsize between 'actual' and 'known' delinquency.Some students think thatfew if any adolescent boysin high-rate areas escape delinquency."'7 It will beseen below that, where convenient, these criticsassume that only 10 per cent or less of slum areaboys are delinquent (that is, those supposedlysubjected to "differential association" with de-linquent persons or patterns); but when necessaryto mount what they conceive to be a really devas-tating attack on the kind of basic argument pre-sented in the quotation above, they conclude thatall boys in high delinquency areas are delinquent!

B. The sample of the delinquents (and of the inmie-linquents) is atypical, because: It did not include allethnic groups. Our samples dealt largely withmatched representatives of such ethnico-racialderivation as Italian, Irish, English, and Slavic.(See Table IV-6 p. 38, and Appendix B of UN-RAVELING). The results have been criticized in somequarters because the samples did not include rep-resentatives of other ethnic groups and because thenumber of Jews was small. The reason for oursample not including more Jewish boys is the verysimple one that it was exceedingly difficult to findenough Jewish delinquent boys in the areas inquestion. As for the other complaint, our samplesdo not include Chinese, West Indians, Hottentots,Eskimos, etc., although there may well be a fewboys descended from these ethnic groups in Boston.If the objection be made that then we had no right

17 Hartung, op. cit. supra note 6 at 728. The specificreference is to Hartung's reply to criticisms of A.Cohen's theory of a delinquency subculture and hypoth-esis that "middle-class" male delinquency is "aconsequence of the middle-class adolescent boy'sanxiety concerning his masculine role." But the quo-tation in the text would apply equally (and I haveseveral times heard the argument seriously advanced)to the specific point made in the text.

to "generalize" about a "tentative causal law" ofdelinquency, we invite the reader's attention toChapter II (especially p. 15) and Chapter XXI(especially p. 272) of UNRAVELING, where we fullyrecognize the limitations of our conclusions arisingout of the necessity of controlling, at the outset,certain generalized sets of variables (includingethnic derivation). The extent to which ourfindings will ultimately prove to hold good forother samples of cases (ethnic, economic, etc.) canonly await testing through further research. But,as will be shown below, our social prediction tablebased on certain sociocultural factors of family lifehas proved to have very high prognostic forcewhen applied to samples of Jewish, Negro, Frenchand Japanese delinquents.

C. The sample of delinquents is atypical because:The boys selected were largely those who had beencommitted to an industrial school and who were,thereby, presumably "institutionalized." On pages13-14 of UNRAVELING we give our definition of

delinquency for the purposes of the research inquestion. We have spent a great many years in theinvestigation and study of delinquents and crimin-als and on the basis of that experience we point out(UNRAVELING p. 13) that children who, once ortwice during the period of growing up in an ex-citing milieu, steal a toy in a ten-cent store, sneakinto a subway or motion picture theatre, playhooky, and the like, but soon outgrow such pecca-dilloes, are not true delinquents even though tech-nically they may have violated the law. 8 We statethat "in order to arrive at the clearest differentia-tion of disease and health, comparison must bemade between the unquestionably pathologic andthe normal."

Certainly, if a laboratory technician is studyingcancer, he must first make sure he is dealing with amalignant and not a benign growth. It is for thatgood reason that we decided to include in oursample only boys whose misbehavior could be saidto be truly delinquent in that it was persistent,rather than either accidental or very occasionaland followed by early abandonment of misconduct.

Is "Indeed, it is now recognized that a certain amountof petty pilfering occurs among many children aroundthe age of six or seven and is to be expected as part ofthe process of trying their wings. Children appear tobe no worse for very occasional and slight experimentaldeviations from socially acceptable norms of conduct.Since they voluntarily abandon such behavior, theirmisconduct or maladaptation cannot be deemed eitherhabitual or symptomatic of deep-rooted causes." UN-RAVELING at 14.

1960]

SHELDON GLUECK

And it is precisely for this reason that we havegrounds to believe that our prediction tables, de-signed to "spot" probable future delinquents atschool entrance, will be able to differentiate trueprospective delinquents showing difficulties of ad-justment in the early years from pseudodelin-quents who display similar external behavior butwhose future is in fact more promising.

One severe critic makes a great to-do about the"institutionalization" of our delinquents as in-validating the findings with reference to delin-quents in general, findings he nonetheless is quickto accept in another part of his critique as proof ofhis claim that, despite our supposed ruling out ofsociocultural influences, "the force of social (orenvironmental) causation proves irrepressible."He produces no proof for his contention about"institutionalization.' 9

Here are the relevant facts:Our boys spent, on the average, 7.12 months in

correctional institutions, 61.8 per cent having beenthere less than six months. (UNRAVELING, TableA-10, p. 296). By contrast, they had had an aver-age of 10.84 months on probation, over half of themsix or more months. (UNRAVELING, Table A-9, p.296). The institutions to which they were com-mitted are open industrial schools with a regimeof education, athletics, recreation, religious guid-ance, etc. No lockstep and bars are involved. Surelynobody with even an elementary first-hand knowl-edge of psychology or the conditioning of delin-quent attitudes and behavior could seriously claimthat the brief stay in such an institution wouldcrucially overbalance all the other experiences ofa young lifetime gained in the home, school, andcommunity! We solemnly assure this critic that ourdelinquent lads did not have the "prison pallor,"did not talk out of the corner of the mouth, didnot glance apprehensively over the shoulder.'0

19 Rubin, op. cit. supra note 5 at 108-109. "Twooutstanding facts emerge from Unraveling JuvenileDelinquency:... (2) Institutionalized children differfrom children who have not been institutionalized."Id. at 113. A similar criticism, made in more temperateterms, is that of P. P. Lejins in the Symposium in 15FED. PROB. at 58 (No. 1 1951).

20 Rubin pontificates: "The most elementary cautionin criminological research is the recognition that anexamination of institutionalized offenders (or de-linquents) will provide information about institution-alized offenders and not about offenders in general. Aninstitutionalized offender is characteristically, in greatpart, an institution product." Rubin, op. cit. supranote 5 at 108-109; Symposium in 15 FED. PRoB. at 55(No. 1 1951). Rubin confuses an institutionalized

Besides, the vast part of our data concerns thepersonal makeup and early conditioning factorsoperative years before these boys were "institu-tionalized."

To the extent that relevant factors exist for com-parison, we have found little difference between thegeneral run of delinquents who appear in a juvenilecourt and our sample. Strong inferential proof ofthis fact, by neutral investigators, is to be seen in astudy significantly entitled, The Close of AnotherChapter in Criminology, by Doctors William Healy,Augusta E. Bronner, and Myra E. Shimberg.nAnother sample of juvenile delinquents had beenused by us in an investigation prior to UNRAVEL-ING, and that research involved a follow-up ofdelinquents who had passed through both theBoston Juvenile Court and the judge Baker Guid-ance Center (Clinic). The findings of recidivismreported in that investigation, ONE THOUSmADJuvENILE DELINQUENTS,n had been questioned onthe ground that, since the cases were those thathad been sent by the judge to the clinic, they musthave been the most serious ones and this must haveaccounted for the high failure rate we discoveredin tracing these boys during a five-year follow-upperiod after court appearance. On the basis of acheck group of a thousand other cases-the generalrun of the mill of the same court (i.e., those who"passed through the.., court, but were not re-ferred... to the" clinic)-the authors of theabove-named check study (two of them at thattime directors of the clinic in question) concluded,after their own careful follow-up of these cases,that "the results of the two studies [the Gluecksand theirs] are amply corroborative." '

It is further significant on the issue of sampling,a point worthy of repetition, that many predic-tion-validation studies (see below) on samples ofboys of a variety of ethnic derivation, religion, cul-ture-, etc., are also "amply corroborative" of ma-jor findings in UNRAVELING.

delinquent with a delinquent who happens to havebeen sent for a brief time to a correctional school. Be-sides, 80% of the boys were 12 years or older by thetime of their first commitment to an institution, andtheir basic character structure was by then quite firmlyfixed. UNRAVELING, Table A-5 at 294.

21Healy, Bronner & Shimberg, The Close ofAnother Chapter in Criminology, 19 MENTAL HYGIENE208 (1935).

12 GLUECK, S. & E. T., ONE THOUSAND JuVENiLnDELINQUENTS (Introduction by Felix Frankfurter)(Cambridge, Harvard University Press, 1934).

23 Healy, et al., op. cit. supra note 21 at 211.

[Vol. 51

TEN YEARS OF UNRA VELING DELINQUENCY

II. Tim TECHNIQUES OF INVESTIGATIONAND TESTING

A. Interviewing and investigating. Some criticsclaim that the facts we were able to obtain throughinterviewing the parents of nondelinquents arevery probably not similar to those we could ob-tain through interviewing those of delinquents,on the ground that the latter are under compul-sion to give information, the former not.u Whilethis view is understandable, we believe these criticsto be mistaken. Neither set of families was undercompulsion to give us information. We are notofficials, and our field investigators made theirspecial research interest and nonofficial status veryclear to the parents. There is a technique, care-fully developed over many years of experiencewith this type of research, that enables us to ob-tain data both intensive and verified; the method isdescribed in the first chapters of UNRAVEING andin an unpublished (mimeographed) account of thebuilding up of a typical case history by our twochief investigators. 5 Long experience in investi-gating families, the scrupulous care with whichparents and relatives were interviewed, and, espe-cially, the intensive search for verifying datathrough pre-existing records (in private socialagencies and public offices, and made by others forpurposes entirely different from those of our re-search) render it most unlikely that the intensityand range of the investigations were significantlydifferent in the case of nondelinquents from thatof delinquents. It must further be borne in mindin this connection that the same syllabus of datato be gathered and verified, and the same statisti-cal schedules, were used for nondelinquents as fordelinquents.

B. Reliability of tests and measurements.

(1) The method of anthropomelry and the Sheldoniantechnique of somatotyping are supposedly not re-liable. One critic2 6 first admits he is no specialistin anthropology, then presumes to question the

24 Taft, Implications of the Ghteck Methodology forCrimnological Research, 42 J. CtMi. L., C. & P. S.305-306 (1951). See also, Gault and Anderson, Un-ravding Juvenile Ddinzqueicy, A Symposium of Re-views, 41 J. Cpme. L, C. & P. S. at 734, 746 (1951).2

1 McGRATa & CuNNiNGHAm, THE CASE OF HENRYW., ILLUSTRATING METHOD OF SOCIAL INVESTIGATION.Copies of this mimeographed statement, too bulky tohave been included as an appendix to UNRAVELING, asoriginally planned, have been distributed to principallibraries.2

8 Taft, op. cit. supra note 24 at 303 n. 1.

validity of the anthropometric and somatotypingwork performed by the two highly experiencedphysical anthropologists who did the measuringand morphologic classification of types for UN-RAVELING. Another critic, also not an anthropolo-gist, attacks the anthropological work in reviewingPmiSiQUE AND DELNQUEN CY.21 Why? Because thelate Professor Edwin H. Sutherland, whom bothcritics admire greatly, "with his wonted thorough-ness, gave the Sheldon contribution to criminologya terrific going over." However, Sutherland, also,was no anthropologist. Moreover, the "goingover" referred to a book by William Sheldon in thefield of criminology, which had nothing whatso-ever to do with our own work. We ourselves hadcriticized Sheldon's book, VARIETIES OF DELIN-QuENT BEiAVIOR,2s because, although the somato-typing is excellent, the sociologic data and inter-pretations are, in our opinion, questionable. Butthis is irrelevant to the employment of (and thebasic improvement upon) Sheldon's categorizationof somatotypes (in turn an improvement overKretchmer's) in our study. We are convinced thatthe anthropometry and analysis of the somatotypedata in UNRAVELING are exceptionally meritorious.A distinguished authority in the field, who is atrained anthropologist, Professor C. WesleyDupertuis, is lavish in his praise of this work. 9

Another critic attacks the statistical basis ofPiYsIQUE AND DELINQuENCY, which employed thesomatotype findings of UNRAVELING in intercor-

relating morphologic types with numerous traitsand factors. He is concerned with the fact, for ex-ample, that careful anthropometry, by experts,showed that 60.1 per cent of the delinquent groupcompared to 30.7 per cent of the nondelinquentsare nesortorphic (with "relative predominance ofmuscle, bone and connective tissue") and, at theother extreme, 14.4 per cent of the delinquentscompared to 39.6 per cent of the controls werefound to be ectonwrphic (with relative predomin-ance of linearity and fragility and, in proportionto their mass, with "the greatest surface area andhence relatively the greatest sensory exposure tothe outside world"). (UNRAVELING, pp. 193,

2 Hartung, Book Review, 48 J. CaRn. L., C. & P.S. 638 (1958), reviewing GLuECK, S. & E. T., PHYsIQUEAm DELINQUENCY (1956).

28 SHELDON, VARIETIES OF DELINQUENT BEHAVIOR(Harper & Bros., 1950). Our review appears in 86SURVEY 215 (1950).

21 Dupertuis, Physical Anthropology, 64 HAgv. L.REv. 1031 (1951).

19601

SHELDON GLUECK

344, 54.) This critic, after correctly stating that wepresented 109 tables "in an effort to ascertainwhich traits and factors supposedly exert a sig-nificant differential influence on the delinquencyof four body types," incorrectly concludes that thestatistical treatment is questionable. The argu-ment is that "There are at least forty-six tables inwhich the number of delinquents, or controls, orboth is less than 100," and that "None of the 109tables deals with the entire sample of delinquentsand nondelinquents."' 30

This is a fallacious argument. Of course none ofthe 500 delinquents and 500 nondelinquents hadall the traits and factors embraced in UN-RAVELING.31 To have made that possible it wouldhave been necessary to match myriads of thou-sands of cases in respect to over 400 items includedin the study. But apart from this, the purpose ofthe comparisons in both UNRAVELING and PHY-SIQUE AND DELINQUENCY was to determine thetraits and factors in respect to which the incidencesignificantly differed between the delinquents andthe controls, in the cases where those traits and factorsdid exist. If, as was true of many characters, theincidence was found to vary significantly amongthe two groups compared, this certainly did notindicate that, because a particular trait or factorwas not present in all 500 cases of the delinquentsand in all 500 of the control group, it should beeliminated.

When, for example, it is found that, in relationto all 12 socio-cultural factors of the home con-tributing selectively to delinquency, mesomorphshad the highest incidence in comparison with thethree remaining body types but expecially in con-trast to ectomorphs, and this response is exactlywhat one would expect of the solid bone-musclemesomorphic type as opposed to the fragile, sensi-tive ectomorphic type of person (PHYSIQUE AN)DELINQUENCY, 240), are we to be told that be-cause the factors in question did not exist in all 500delinquents and all 500 nondelinquents the resultshould be ignored or is due to pure chance?

In all this it should be borne in mind thatwhere necessary we have consulted statisticalauthorities.32

30 Hartung, op. cit. supra note 6 at 705.31 ".... it is rare indeed in the social, as in the metereo-

logical field, that we can find an attribute or smallgroup of attributes invariably associated in the pastwith the occurrence or non-occurrence of the phenome-non whose future behavior we wish to predict." Wilson,Prediction, 64 HAxv. L. REv. 1040 (1951).

2It should be mentioned that, unlike the critics, our

And as to the anthropologic soundness of PHY-SIQUE AND DELINQUENCY, which is based on UN-RAVELING, not only the anthropologist Dupertuisbut also one of the world's leading biologists,Julian Huxley, made this judgment: "This is aninteresting and indeed important book... asdemonstrating beyond any doubt the importanceof specific and readily detectable genetic factorsdetermining psycho-physical type, in predisposingboys to delinquency."-"

Under the circumstances, can we be blamed forpreferring the judgment of those with expertise in

the relevant fields to that of those sociologists who

are neither recognized anthropologists nor statisti-cians?N

In fairness it should be pointed out that not all

sociologists have gone off the deep end in an attack

on somatotyping. For example, Professor AlbertMorris, a sociologist and cultural anthropologist

who is both well read and experienced in crimino-logic research, has said, after setting forth a per-

ceptive appraisal of the difficulties involved in the

types of research we are doing:

"These difficulties, usual in research in the

behavioral sciences, have been understood andintelligently met. The techniques for doing this

have elsewhere been clearly discussed. The re-sult, of course, falls short of perfection; but,

perhaps, only those who love certainty more

than truth will be misled by unawareness that

statistical consultants, Professors Edwin BidwellWilson, Jane Worcester, Carl R. Doering, and Mrs.Rose Kneznek, with whom it has been our practice todiscuss statistical problems from time to time, arehighly experienced in statistical theory and technique.See "Explanation of the Statistical Method," by CarlR. Doering, Professor of Biostatistics, Harvard Uni-versity, in UNRAVELING at 75-76 and "Explanation ofStatistical Method," by Jane Worcester in PYsiQUrEAND DEIiNQUENCY.

33Huxley, Review of PHysIQUE AND DELINQUENCY,

3 INT'L J. SocrAL PsycmATRY 71, 72 (1957). A recentresearch in England confirms the presence of excessivemesomorphy among delinquents in a Borstal institu-tion. See GIBBONS, NEw Foi s OF JuvENILE DE:LIN-QuENCY 13, 50 (World Health Organization, mimeo-graphed, June 6, 1960).

zTAx refers to the fact that when he sent theauthors of UNRAVELING a draft of his proposed review,"Professor Glueck properly chides me -ith failure toexplain the basis of my questioning the validity ofthese methods and failure to name the critics"; yetTaft adds: "I leave the brief comment unchanged,because as indicated I am no specialist in thesematters." Taft, op. cit. supra note 24 at 303 n.1. Itwould seem that this fact is a pretty good reason whyTaft (and Hartung and the other nonexperts in thefields involved) should not have dogmatized on theanthropologic and statistical aspects of UNRAVELING.

[Vol, 51

TEN YEARS OF UNRAVELING DzLINQUENCY

the statistician has been given only the oppor-tunity to develop precise summaries of good ap-proximation.... They [the authors] wiselyrecognize that the soundness of their effort isbest judged by the clinical 'good sense' and theinternal consistency evidenced in the inte-grated result."a'

(2) The Rorsczach test is supposedly invalid.Another criticism of a field in which the sociologiccritic is no authority has to do with the Rorschachtest. One such caviler announces that "an eminentpsychologist tells me that the Sheldon and Ror-schach techniques have both been demonstratedto be invalid."3 The unreliability of the criticismsof the Sheldonian somatotypes has been discussed.As to the other, I should like to be informed bywhom. and just how the Rorschach test has been"demonstrated to be invalid"; and invalid forwhat? Nobody claims that the Rorschach test (or,for that matter, any "projective" test or any in-telligence test) is perfect. Yet nobody with evenan elementary acquaintance with psychology andpsychiatry would seriously assert that the Ror-schach test has been "demonstrated to be invalid."

As a matter of fact, until UNRAVELING nobodyhad made a systematic study of a large sample oftrue delinquents as compared with a carefullymatched control group of nondelinquents; nor hadthe Rorschach results been previously systema-tized into a rational clinical set of categories as theyhave been in UNRAVELING (Appendix E). It is tobe emphasized that the boys in that research weresubjected to the tests by different psychologists(in Boston) from those who interpreted the tests(in New York). The critic whose judgment is underreview glides over, en passant, the fact that theNew York experts on the Rorschach test (Dr.Ernst Schachtel and the late Dr. Anna HartochSchachtel) were not informed by us which of the"protocols" sent them for interpretation were thetest-responses of delinquents and which were thoseof nondelinquents. The critic then unwarrantedlyproceeds:

"Delinquents were assumed and then dis-covered to have less fear of authority and de-pendence upon it than 'nondelinquents' ... De-

31 Moanis, Review of PusxQuE Am DELxQUENCy,62 HARv. L. REv. 753-758 (1957).36 Taft, op. cit. supra note 24 at 303 n.l. The use ofthe Rorschach Test was also questioned by Rubin andMonachesi, among others. See Symposium in 15 Ft..PRou. at 55, 57 (No. 1 1951).

linquents were assumed, and then discovered tobe, more unstable and impulsive in their be-havior.... Delinquents were assumed and thendiscovered to be more aggressive and destruc-tive. The logical relationship of these char-acteristics to delinquency is so obvious that acynic might add that delinquents might be as-sumed to be law-breakers and then proven to besuch by elaborate Rorschach devices!"E'

This is patently prejudiced as well as absurd.In the first place, Rorschach responses cannot besuccessfully manipulated by a subject taking thetest. In the second place, there are certain stand-ards of interpretation of the Rorschach results.In the third place, the Rorschach experts who in-terpreted the protocols in New York were not thesame psychologists who had given the tests inBoston; as pointed out, the interpreters did notknow which protocols came from delinquents,which from nondelinquents. In the fourth place,this critic is silent about the Rorschach traits inrespect to which it was found that delinquents donot differ from nondelinquents. (UNRAVELING, pp.223, 226, 228, 233, 238, 272). Finally, any expertin Rorschach psychology can testify that thetraits and factors brought out in the Rorschachmaterials of UNRAVELING are there in the protocolscompleted by the testees; they certainly were not"assumed and then discovered" by either theRorschach experts or the authors of UNRAVELING.

One could turn the tables on this critic by sayingthat he assumes the predominance of general cul-tural influences, and then finds them to predom-inate; except that, contrary to his insistence thatcultural forces are all important in delinquentetiology,3 he now discovers that the variouspsychologic traits he cites bear an "obvious" and"logical relationship to delinquency"-somethinghe evidently did not know until he read the findingsof UNRAVELING.

But another critic seizes upon the Rorschachaspect of UNRAVELING to prove that, far from itsbeing bad, it is the best part of the study! Thisgenerous judgment fits in neatly with his miscon-ception of a statement by Dr. Schachtel, one of theexperts who in New York interpreted the

v Taft, op. cit. supra note 24 at 304.asTaft's textbook on criminology attempts "to

integrate a strong cultural emphasis with a syntheticapproach." (The italics are his.) TArT, C .oLoGy,AN ATTEMPT AT A SYNMETIC INTERPRETATION w= ACULTURAL EimnASis vii (New York, The MacmillanCo., 1492).

19601

SHELDON GLUECK

Rorschach test protocols of the boys who had beentested in Boston. He says that Schachtel

"particularly emphasizes the importance ofclass culture and the milieu in which the de-linquent lives as important factors in delin-quency. Specifically, in making his judgments hestates: 'The judgments would have been differ-ent if the socio-economic background had beendifferent.' For the judgments were made by'asking myself whether his character structure,as I saw it on the basis of the Rorschach test, wasof a type likely to resist the inducements to-ward becoming delinquent offered by poor socio-economic circumstances and by the neighbor-hood.'" (Emphasis added.)39

This critic's interpretation of the quotation isamusingly typical of the way a few sociologists,eager to place their discipline at the head of theprocession in the interpretation of the etiology ofdelinquency (and indeed denying there is any pro-cession), misinterpret the findings of others to suittheir preconceptions. 40 This critic omits alto-gether the crucial statement regarding characterstructure and speaks exclusively about the generalculture. The significant thing in the above quota-tion is not culture but the variations in characterwhich bring about a differential response to asimilar milien. (The role of culture is discussed be-low.)

Another criticism of the sample from the pointof view of the Rorschach test has recently emerged:It is insisted that to list individual traits is allwrong; that the Rorschach is a "holistic technique,the entire pattern of responses is what is significant,not one response factor alone," and that theSchachtels evidently did not know their businessin checking on individual traits.4' To show that

contradictory criticisms do not prevent the critics

39 Reiss, op. cit. supra note 5 at 118.40 One reviewer of the most recent Glueck work has

this diagnosis about the attitude of certain sociolo-gists: "Since the foreign experience demonstrates thesecritics to be so wrong, the question arises as to whethermajor portions of their criticisms may not be tracedto damaged professional pride due to the failure of theGluecks to include a sociologist on their research staff."Fox, Review of PREDICTING DELIQUENCY AND CRIME,40 B. U. L. Rv. 157 n. 13 (1960).4 1 DATTA, SOCIOLOGICAL TEEORY IN CONTEMPORARYAMERICAN SOCIOLOGY (unpublished thesis in Universityof West Virginia Library, 1955), quoted by Hartung,op. cit. supra note 6 at 705. In contrast to this student'samateur status, it is not altogether irrelevant to pointout that the Schachtels, whose work she criticizes socavalierly, had had many years' experience with theRorschach Test and were trained by Rorschach him-self.

from being mutually wrong, it should be pointedout that not only did the Schachtels list traits butbefore doing so they interpreted the Rorschachprotocols in terms of whole character structure(the "psychogram"), taking into account the"test situation." This is what enabled them tomake such a good record in deciding, without beingtold in advance, which of the protocols came fromdelinquents, which from nondelinquents. AppendixE of UNRAVELING describes the Schachtel method

of analysis in detail and presents sample recordsand trait analyses.

When it is considered that, despite the fact thatthe psychiatrist and the Rorschach test in-terpreters, although neither was permitted by us tosee the results of individual examinations by theother, achieved a very high incidence of agreementin diagnoses (we found what seemed to be incon-sistencies between the two sets of diagnoses in only74 out of some one thousand cases and in only 6was there continued disagreement after the twoexperts went over the cases and straightened outsemantic misunderstandings),2 it must be con-

ceded that both the Rorschach test and the psy-chiatric interview yielded extraordinarily reliableresults.

So much, then, for the criticisms of the samplesfrom the point of view of the Rorschach test.

(3) The psychiatric interview was inadequate. It hasbeen pointed out that, since the psychiatrist inter-viewed most of the boys only once, there may besome weaknesses in the psychiatric findings. Weanticipated that this criticism would be made. Weourselves would have preferred more contacts;43

but who can say how many more interviews wouldhave been sufficient-two, ten, or a completepsychoanalytic technique requiring years? More-over, it must be borne in mind that the aim of thepsychiatric interview was diagnostic rather thantherapeutic.

The psychiatrist of the research, Dr. BryantMoulton, is an expert with long experience in in-terviewing maladjusted children (having been for

twelve years with the Judge Baker GuidanceCenter) and with a special gift of winning rapportwith boys. But apart from this fact there is an in-ternal test of the reliability of the psychiatrist'sfindings; that is, the evidence thrown up by a very

42 See UNRAVELING at 242-43 n. 8.43 UNRAVELING at 60. Criticism of the single psychi-

atric interview was made, among others, by J. Satten,a psychiatrist, in the Symposium in 15 FED. PROB. at55 (No.1 1951).

[Vol. 51

TEN YEARS OF UNRAVELING DELINQUENCY

significant aspect of the design of the research,namely, the fact that care was taken to avoid infec-tion of the findings of one scientist by those ofanother investigator. Thus, as already pointedout, the psychiatrist was not given access to theRorschach test findings, nor were the Rorschachinterpreters aware of the psychiatric diagnoses.Yet when we compared the diagnoses on the boysas determined by these single psychiatric inter-views with the findings evolved through inde-pendent interpretation of the Rorschach test pro-tocols, a remarkable similarity emerged." So, also,when we compared the highly predictive factors(that is, those most markedly differentiating de-linquents from nondelinquents) which resultedfrom the psychiatrist's interviews with those de-rived from the Rorschach tests, they were, takenas a pattern, quite consistent.45

I. CAUSATIVE ANAxYsIs

A. It has been claimed that, since our investiga-tion "was eclectic and not guided by any theory,"the interpretations of the findings "were necessar-ily ex post facto." 4 We have several things to sayabout such a judgment:

In the first place, no unilateral theory advancedby sociologists-whether culture conflict, the"ecologic characteristics of the interstitial area,'"differential association," or "delinquency sub-culture"-is sufficiently comprehensive, suffi-ciently specific or sufficiently close to the realities ofboth individual and social psychology and theactualities of behavior to have been used as a guideto our research. Differential association, forexample, is a very thin and distant abstractionwithout capacity to guide practical research.47

And the delinquency subculture did not fall fromthe heavens; it was made, and is being sustained,by delinquent people, and to attribute etiologicexclusiveness or priority to it is to reason in acircle.

In the second place, our eclectic approach, broadand comprehensive as it is, was much better de-signed than any existing sociologic theory to getat the specific facts that differentiate delinquentsfrom nondelinquents. In this connection, no re-

44 UNRAVELING, notes at 242-43, 252-53.45.Psychiatric: Adventuresomeness, suggestibility,

stubbornness, extroversion in action, emotional insta-bility; Rorschach: social assertiveness, suspiciousness,defiance, destructiveness, emotional lability.4

1 Hartung, op. cit. supra note 6 at 706, citing Reiss.'7 Glueck, S., Theory and Fact in Criminology, 7

BraT. J. DELINQ. 92 (1956).

search, either before or after UNRAVELING, has socomprehensively and pointedly dug up traits andfactors that are very probably etiologic.

In the third place, there was nothing ex postfactoabout the interpretations; on the contrary, everyprecaution was taken to avoid "reading in," andany careful student of UNRAVELING will find thisto be so. The process was the familiar scientificone of comparison of a representative and sub-stantial sample of true delinquents with an equalsample of true nondelinquents. (The charge of expost facto reasoning is, by the way, an interestingillustration of the psychoanalytic concept of "pro-jection"; it is certain sociologists, not we, who, de-sirous to "prove" their theories, manage to omitfactual data running counter to their preconcep-tions, such, for example, as the biologically-rootedchanges in interests, attitudes, and behavior thattend to occur with changes in age-spans no matterwhat the general culture or subculture may be.)

Discussion of the etiologic implications arrivedat, not through preexisting preconceptions aboutthis or that individual theory, cause, or discipline,but inductively, on the basis of the evidenceemerging from comparison of over 400 factors atdifferent levels of inquiry, is given in pages 281-282 of UNRAVELING and developed further inChapter XV, "Riddle of Delinquency," of ourmore popular book based on UNRAVELING, DE-

LINQUENTS IN THE MAKING.48

B. Ourpositionas to etiology maybe stated in theseterms:

At the outset we had to reexamine the implica-tions of the entire history of the study of the prob-lem of crime causation, especially the causes ofjuvenile misconduct; for as Dostoevsky long agoshrewdly observed in his famous novel, TnE HousEor THE DEAD, "With ready-made opinions onecannot judge of crime. Its philosophy is a littlemore complicated than people think." The samemay be said of ready-made theories.

It is well known that throughout the centuriesthere have been favorite, unilateral explanations-call them facts or theories-of criminalism. Someinsight into the causation of delinquency or crimecan be obtained from almost any approach thatmay seem to bear a relationship to the problem.Even meteorology can contribute; investigationshave been made which show seasonal and climaticvariations in crime and delinquency. Numerousstudies have been conducted, especially in Europe,

48Harper and Bros., 1952.

19601

SHELDON GLUECK

of fluctuations in various indexes of economicconditions (prices of basic commodities, businessactivity, and the like) as related to the ups anddowns of crime or delinquency. Many researcheshave been made, especially in Chicago, on the rela-tionship of neighborhood conditions to crime.Numerous investigations have studied specificfactors of environment and culture, such as theconflict of cultures between foreign-born parentsand native-born children or between residents ofdifferent sections of the same region, the effect ofbad companions, the dearth of adequate recrea-tional facilities, and the like. More recently, oldwine has been poured into new bottles under suchgeneral titles as the "differential associationtheory," the "delinquency subculture," etc.

There have also been many biologically-orientedinvestigations of the causes of delinquency andcrime, from the famous and now questionableworks of the Italian anthropologic criminologist,Lombroso, involving variations on the theme of the"born criminal," atavism or hereditary "throw-back," or degeneration, and certain forms of epi-lepsy, tc the traditional psychiatric studies whichemphasize psychoses, "psychopathic personality,"and psychoneuroses. Finally, in more recent years,there have been a few psychoanalytic investiga-tions, and numerous studies (based on inadequatesamples and lack of comparable control groups ofnondelinquents) of various individual psychologic,emotional, or characterial elements or patternsalleged to be causal of a tendency to criminalism.

Examination of existing researches in juveniledelinquency disclosed a tendency grossly to over-emphasize a particular science or explanation. Pro-ponents of various theories of causation still toooften insist that the truth is to be found only intheir own special fields of study. Like the fabledblind men examining the elephant, each builds theentire subject in the image of that piece of realitywhich he happens to have touched.

Obviously then, a many-sided approach to thishighly complex problem of human nature-a prob-lem which has puzzled philosophers, scientists,parents, and clergy for hundreds of years, andjudges and probation officers more recently-is,we were and are convinced, the only type of firststep that gives promise of yielding useful insightsleading to inductively arrived at theories and hy-potheses which can then be subjected to morefruitful tests.

It is such a many-sided study, and for the rea-

sons noted, which Mrs. Glueck and I attempted inUNRAVELING.

To avoid as many pitfalls as possible, we de-signed an investigation in which we comparednumerous persistently delinquent boys ranging inage from 11 to 17, with as many truly nondelin-quent boys, matching them, boy for boy, by age,by ethnic (racial) derivation, by general intelli-gence (I.Q.), and by'residence in underprivileged"slum" areas of Boston. This matching was neces-sary because the factors that had to be analyzedin order to dig closer to the roots of delinquency areso numerous that a way had to be found to avoidconfusion. In order to study a large enough sampleof variables, certain other sets of factors had firstto be "held constant." In deciding which suchfactors to equalize among the two groups, throughthe pairing off of delinquents with nondelinquentsas a preliminary to their later detailed study, wewere guided by several methodologic aims: First,it was felt that since the ultimate comparison oughtto deal with subtle processes of personality and en-vironment, the more general or cruder factorsshould be kept as nearly as possible alike in thematching; secondly, those traits which typicallyaffect a whole interrelated range of factors couldalso usefully be held constant; thirdly, those gen-eral characteristics which had already been ex-plored sufficiently by other investigators and aboutwhich there was considerable agreement (such asthe partial, indirect role of poverty, or of residencein a socially deteriorated urban area) could alsobe usefully held constant in the matching, to theextent possible in such an inexact field.

It was only after this careful matching of per-sistent delinquents with true nondelinquents thatthe two sets of boys were systematically and mi-nutely compared in respect to the percentage-incidence of numerous traits and factors, measuredor assessed at the following levels:(1) Family and personal background. This is a com-parative picture of the kinds of homes and familiesthese two sets of boys came from, based on homevisits and on the reconstruction, from numerousrecorded sources, of the history of delinquency andcriminalism, alcoholism, intellectual and emotionaldeficiency, physical ailments, economic and educa-tional conditions and achievements, not only of themembers of the boys' immediate families but alsoof grandparents, uncles, and aunts. The aim was todetermine the conditions under which the parentsof the two groups of boys had themselves been

[Vol. 51

TEN YEARS OF UNRA VELING DELINQUENCY

raised, a situation that must have influenced theideals, attitudes and practices which they, in turn,brought to the task of child-rearing so far as ourboys were concerned.

The cultural, intellectual, and emotional condi-tions in the homes of our boys were of course like-wise subjected to intensive study.(2) Boys' habits and use of leisure. Attention herewas especially directed to the age at whichdeviating behavior began and the nature of theearliest signs of antisocial conduct. There was alsosecured a detailed history of each boy's progress inschool, as well as an assessment of the variousforms of school misbehavior.(3) Physique types. The bodily structure and form ofthe boys was determined by the use of photographsand their anthropologic measurement and classifi-cation in respect to fundamental physique pat-terns or somatotypes. After various measurements,the boys were classified anthropologically ac-cording to the predominance of one of the threeroot-components that entered into their develop-ment: endowrphy, in which softness and roundnessof the various regions of the body predominate;inesomorphy, in which solid muscle, bone and con-nective tissue, and bodily compactness predomi-nate; and ectonwrphy, characterized by the re-lative emphasis of linearity and fragility of bodyform. A balanced type (relative similarity of allthree components) was also identified. The im-portance of comparing delinquents and nondelin-quents in terms of physique-type lies in the factthat the dominance of one or another of the threeroot components of body form may well implybasic variations in energy output, temperament,affect, and differential response to "differentialassociation."(4) Health. A significant comparison of delinquentswith nondelinquents obviously had to include amedical examination, since poor health is oftenalleged to be a significant causal factor in delin-quency. Through this we got a comparative pictureof the general health and of the gross evidence ofvarious diseases in the two sets of boys.(5) Intelligence. It is commonly believed that thehows and whys of human behavior are largelygoverned by the degree and quality of intellectualpower. One of the bases of original matching wasgeneral intelligence as summarized in the I.Q.This equating of global intellectual capacity gave usa chance to compare the delinquents with the con-trols in respect to various constituent elements of

intelligence; two boys of like I.Q. can have quitevarying intellectual qualities in terms of specifictraits, talents and deficiencies.(6) Temperament and character structure. In recentyears, those who have studied the human mindand behavior have concluded that the tempera-mental and affective life of the individual, andespecially the feeling-laden experiences and traumaof the first few years in the home, have a greatdeal to do with the development of personality,the structuring of character, and the channellingand habituation of behavior-tendencies. To reachthe main features of character-structure, various"projective" tests have been evolved. TheRorschach (or ink-blot) Test, which the psycholo-gists applied to both sets of boys, is one of the mostrevealing of such probers of the foundations of per-sonality and character.

As pointed out, there was also an interview by askilled psychiatrist with each boy. This revealedthe more obvious personality traits of the boys,uncovered their many emotional stresses and con-flicts and how they resolved them and providedsome dues to the reasons for the persistency of mis-behavior of the delinquent lads and the conven-tional conduct of the control group.

Such an extensive and intensive exploration atso many levels involved years of careful investiga-tion, tireless verification of data from manysources, entering of the numerous factors on sta-tistical schedules according to carefully prepareddefinitions, and meeting many delicate problems ofpublic relations. Several chapters in UNRAVELINGrender full account of these various techniques, sothat the interested reader can judge for himself thereliability of the raw materials that went into thenumerous tables and statistical computations fromwhich the basic conclusions of this comprehensivestudy were derived.

Such, oversimplified in the above description,are the philosophy and technique of UNRAVELING

JUVENILE DELINQUENCY.

C. How do these fit into our practical view of"causation"?

(1) There has been altogether too much seeminglyprofound but essentially superficial writing on thetheme (borrowed from the pure sciences) that onemust never use the term "cause," but only suchevasive expressions as "associated factor," or"decision theory," etc. But theissue is a pragmatic,not a semantic, one. Where a considerable number

19601

SHELDON GLUECK

of factors that "make sense" from the point of viewof common and clinical experience are found to char-acterize delinquents far more than nondelinquents(the difference not being attributable to chance)it is highly probable that what is involved is anetiologic connection between them; in other words,the delinquency not only follows the traits and fac-tors that have been found to precede it, but fol-lows from them. The soundness of such a concep-tion of "causation" from a practical point of viewis provable by (a) the fact that the concatenationof different traits and factors yields high predictivepower in the sense of identification of delinquentsand nondelinquents when applied to a considerablevariety of populations, and (b) the fact that whensuch patterns of assumedly criminogenic traits andfactors are absent from or are eliminated from asituation, delinquency usually does not exist or isgreatly diminished.

The possibility that some day variations in thebehavior of people may be explainable largely inthe more ultimate terms of differences in, say,endocrine gland function, or of microscopicphysico-chemical reactions does not, in the mean-time, prevent effective action on the basis of exist-ing cruder assessments of reality, any more thanthe recent development of nuclear science pre-vented effective coping with many problems ofnature through employment of prenuclear chemis-try and physics. In the meantime, it can serve nouseful purpose for criminologists to keep wringingtheir hands about the inadequacies of the etiologicresearches thus far produced. Medicine madetherapeutic strides in several fields long before thespecific etiologic agents in certain diseases were dis-covered (in the treatment of malaria, for example).

Nor should too much time and energy be ex-pended in the armchair search for some grand, all-unifying theory. Fruitful theories and hypothesesusually spring only from soundly gathered factswhich suggest dues or influences leading to furtherinvestigations. The multidisciplinary evidence ofour comparisons of delinquents with nondelin-quents convinces us that, taken in the mass, ifboys in the underprivileged urban areas have intheir makeup and early conditioning certainidentifiable traits and factors found markedly todifferentiate delinquents from nondelinquents, theboys are very likely to turn out to be delinquent.In this general sense of high probability of per-sistency of antisocial conduct related to the pres-ence of a sufficiently weighty combination of

differentiative characteristics of person and milieu,a rough etiologic relatiopship was established.

(2) It should be emphasized that throughout thework in analyzing the data for UNRAVELING, weinsisted on the fundamental importance of sequencein time. That is why we ruled out gang membership(frequently emphasized as a cause of delinquency)and other influences which were found to have oc-curred long after definite proof of antisocial be-havior. The onset of persistent misbehavior ten-dencies was at the early age of seven years oryounger among 48 per cent of our delinquents, andfrom eight to ten in an additional 39 per cent; thusa total of almost nine-tenths of the entire groupshowed clear delinquent tendencies before the timewhen boys generally become members of organizedboys' gangs. The leading authorities on the sub-ject recognize the gang as "largely an adolescentphenomenon"; for example, of some 1,200 cases ofgang membership studied in Chicago, only 1.5 percent of the boys were six to twelve years old, while63 per cent were classified as adolescents.

(3) We further decided that "cause" (we wouldhave preferred to use a less controversial term)involves a totality of conditions necessary to bringabout the delinquency result; individual traits orfactors are only parts of the total cause. That iswhy our nondelinquents sometimes had in theirmakeup and background some of the traits andfactors found among the delinquents. Not all thecharacteristics arrived at inductively and includedin our general etiologic formulation will be foundin all delinquents and be absent in all nondelin-quents.

Consider, for example, the fact that twice asmany delinquents as non-delinquents were foundto be of the closely knit, muscular, athletically in-dined (mesomorphic) type. The very fact that 30per cent of the nondelinquents were also of thisphysique immediately contradicts any conclusionthat mnesomorphy inevitably "causes" persistentdelinquency. Or, consider such a trait as defiance,which one would naturally regard as closely re-lated to delinquent behavior tendencies. True, 50per cent of the delinquents had this characteristic,but 12 per cent of the nondelinquents also had it;and the very fact that half the delinquent groupdid not display this trait further reveals the in-adequacy of conclusions about causation derivedfrom a single factor.

Thus, a single factor (or even a small group of

[Vol. 51

TEN YEARS OF UNRA VELING DELINQUENCY

factors) may be involved, and even frequently in-volved, in delinquent behavior and yet will not, ofitself, have sufficient weight or potency to tip thescales among boys who remain nondelinquent. Inany realistic sense, the cause of an effect is thattotality of conditions sufficient to produce it. Thefact noted that as many as half of the delinquentsdid not have the trait of defiance is but one illustra-tion of the absence among a considerable group ofdelinquents of characteristics which are present insome other large group of delinquents. It revealsthe usually unrecognized truism that persistentdelinquency is not the potential result of only onespecific combination of factors which markedlydifferentiates delinquents from nondelinquents,but of each of several different combinations. justas death, although always the same terminal event,may be the result of various preceding sequenr'es ofconditions, so the terminal event of persistent de-linquency may be the result of a variety of differentsequences. For we are dealing with a complex ag-gregation of internal and external conditions whichare associated with socially maladjusted, unlawfulbehavior, and not all of them are indispensable tothe result in any single case.

In criminal conduct, as in most other forms ofhuman expression, every person has his individualbreaking point. It is difficult for all members of anysociety at any one time to conform to the require-ments and prohibitions of socially desirable be-havior, because this involves a subordination of thenatural impulses of sex expression, aggression, andthe like, to those conduct norms which the law hasdeclared necessary to the general welfare. Butmost persons are able (through various combina-tions of numerous factors of native endowmentand elaborate conditioning in home, school, church,neighborhood and suppoitive elements in the gen-eral culture) to meet the requirements of the majorlegal standards of the age and place wherein theylive. If a boy persists in delinquency, it means thathis power of resistance to natural impulse, or hisdesire to obey socio-legal mandates, has been over-balanced by the strength of the other cir-cumstances that incline to antisocial behavior.

(4) Closely related to the preceding concepts is theconcept of probability. Nowadays, even thephysicalsciences state their generalizations in terms not ofabsolute inevitability but only of high probability.The statistical method of comparing delinquents,as a group, with nondelinquents, as a group, is notdesigned to bring out any point-to-point causal

sequence that will always hold good for each andevery case. It is rather intended to disclose whetheror not a group of boys having a certain duster offactors in its makeup and background will muchmore probably turn out to be delinquent than agroup not so loaded down; or, to put it differently,whether the "typical" delinquent is likely to be theresult of such a concatenation of factors.

(5) The intellectual fruitfulness of our etiologicconceptions compared to the unilateral theory ofthe sociologist is shown by an example to be foundin PREDICTmG DELxQoUENCY AND CnR= 8 Our

Social Prediction Table (discussed below) is effec-tive in about 90 per cent of the new cases to whichit is applied. Why does it not also select potentialdelinquents and nondelinquents in the remaining10 per cent? The answer is that there are cases inwhich the etiologic-predictive nexus based on thefactors of family life shown to be highly discrimina-tive in most cases is not adequate to identify de-linquents and nondelinquents among the 10 percent because in those cases certain other character-istics are sufficiently potent to counteract thefamily influences. In other words, there is a coretype of delinquent from an etiologic-predictivepoint of view, and there are one or more fringetypes. If we examine the characteristics of the 10per cent who were not successfully identified bythe Social Prediction Table, it turns out that theseatypical delinquents, although they come fromwholesome families, differ from the typical (core)group in respect to miny characteristics, particu-larly those derived from the Rorschach test andpsychiatric examination.49 Some of these traitssuggest a quite consistent pattern of neurotic at-tributes (e.g., marked submissiveness, enhancedfeelings of insecurity, fear of failure and defeat),together with a low incidence of such characteris-tics as destructiveness and adventurousness whichwere found to be highly differentiative in the core-type of delinquent. Other traits suggest the possi-bility of there existing still another fringe type. Wefelt justified in calling this pilot analysis "FromPrediction to Etiology," because it is by thismethod of inductive analysis, imperfect as it stillconcededly is, that one approaches the determina-tion of true etiologic involvements.

So much for our conceptions of causation.

48 GLucEK, S & E. T., PREDICTING DELINQUENCYAND CR (19595.49Id. at 263-73.

SHELDON GLUECK

D. We are convinced that the criticisms of ourtentative etiologic formulation are unsound:

(1) One critic points out that the etiologic "law"'"is untested.5'

50 "The delinquents as a group are distinguishablefrom the non-delinquents: (1) physically, in beingessentially mesomorphic in constitution (solid, closelyknit, muscular); (2) temperamentally, in being rest-lessly energetic, impulsive, extroverted, aggressive,destructive (6ften sadistic)-traits which may berelated more or less to the erratic growth pattern andits physiologic correlates or consequences; (3) in atti-tude, by being hostile, defiant, resentful, suspicious,stubborn, socially assertive, adventurous, uncon-ventional, non-submissive to authority; (4) psycho-logically, in tending to direct and concrete, rather thansymbolic, intellectual expression, and in being lessmethodical in their approach to problems; (5) soco-culturally, in having been reared to a far greater extentthan the control group in homes of little understanding,affection, stability, or moral fibre by parents usuallyunfit to be effective guides and protectors or, accordingto psychoanalytic theory, desirable sources for emu-lation and the construction of a consistent, well-balanced, and socially normal superego during theearly stages of character development. [For footnoteto this, see UNRAVELING at 283 n. 9.] While in indi-vidual cases the stresses contributed by any one of theabove pressure-areas of dissocial-behavior tendencymay adequately account for persistence in delinquency,in general the high probability of delinquency is de-pendent upon the interplay of the conditions and forcesfrom all these areas.

"In the exciting, stimulating, but little-controlledand culturally inconsistent environment of the under-privileged area, such boys readily give expression totheir untamed impulses and their self-centered desiresby means of various forms of delinquent behavior.Their tendencies toward uninhibited energy-expressionare deeply anchored in soma and psyche and in themalformations of character during the first few yearsof life.

"This 'law' may have to be modified after moreintensive, microscopic study of the atypical cases. Forexample, there are instances in which the delinquentsare more ectomorphic than mesomorphic in consti-tution, and cases in which the delinquents are of theintroverted, psychoneurotic temperament. There arealso some non-delinquents who have been reared inimmoral and criminalistic homes. While all these groupsare relatively small in number, they deserve furtherstudy, and their more intensive consideration mayresult in modification of the basic analysis. 'A scientificlaw must always be considered as a temporary state-ment of relationships. As knowledge increases this lawmay require modification. Even the natural sciencesstate all their generalizations in terms of probability.' "(The quote within the quotation is from SELLIN,CULTURa CONFLICT AND CRIME.) UNRAVELING at 281-82. The summary is preceded by a careful analysisof the findings of the research. See UNRAVELING, ch.XXI, "Dynamic Pattern of Delinquency."

61 Rubin, op. cit. supra note 5 at 112. He quotes theGluecks as saying "physical anthropologists have notyet answered a major question, namely, whether or notthe somatotype remains constant and, if it does,whether, in the formative years of growth around theage of six or seven, when children normally enterschool, the physique type is as yet reliably distinguish-

One thing at a time. It was clearly presented astentative; and no claim whatsoever was made thatit is final or perfect. On the contrary, the entirediscussion made clear the need of testing the gen-eral conclusion by further researches. We shouldbe pleased if a team of specialists similar to thosewho worked on UNRAVELING, but aided by thepresence of a relatively unbiased sociologist, wereto make a study similar to ours, benefiting from ourmistakes.

(2) This critic further points out that the "law isnot limited as to place, age, or administrativepolicy, to accord with the limitations of the sam-ple.,'

52

Onpage272 of UNRAVELING the limitations of thegeneral sample are set forth. However, we are nowable to say that the results of a number of gratify-ing applications of the Glueck Social PredictionTable would seem to indicate the likelihood of thefindings being essentially repeated on anothersample of delinquents compared with nondelin-quents.

(3) This critic also calls attention to the fact thatsome of the differentiative factors affect only smallgroups of delinquents and nondelinquents.n

This matter has already been discussed in con-

able," and asks "what justification is there to includephysique in the law?" He overlooks the fact that theboys were much older than six or seven when somato-typed for UNRAVELING, their average age then havingbeen about 142 years. Rubin then points out that werejected physique for predictive purposes. The reasonfor this (as plainly indicated in the quotation focusedupon by Rubin) was that we were not as yet confidentthat the physique type was reliably distinguished atage six or seven, when prediction is attempted. Thereis thus no inconsistency between our use of somato-types on the boys in our samples (at about 141J) andour failure to use them for prediction (at about 6 or 7).

It should be noted that since publication of UN-RAVELING, Mrs. Glueck has worked out a predictiondevice using somatotype data, because evidence wasbeginning to appear that somatotyping can in fact bemade early in life (at age 7, for example). In an Englishstudy, "It has been shown that the somatotype cangive some indication of the kind of behaviour to expectin different individuals, and that exact measurementssupport what has for centuries been appreciated in lessprecise form, namely, that physical constitution playsan important part in shaping people's lives." Davidson,Mcnnes & Parnell, The Distribution of PersonalityTraits in Seven-Year Old Children: A Combined Psycho-logical, Psychiatric and Somatolype Study, 27 BRIT. J.EDuc. PSYCHOLOGY 48-61 (1957). See, also, Parnell,Physique and Family Structure, 51 TH EuGENIcs REV.75-88 (1959).

m Rubin, op. cit. supra note 5 at 112. Note the state-ment regarding the limitations of our tentative formu-lation in UNRAVELING at 272.

'- Rubin, op. cit. supra note 5 at 112.

[Vol. 51

TEN YEARS OF UNRA VELING DELINQUENCY

nection with a similar criticism of the tables inPHYSIQUE AND DELINQUENCY. It should be re-emphasized that the fact that certain differentia-tive factors affect relatively small groups of delin-quents and nondelinquents is proof of the existenceof fringe types in which the etiologic syndromediffers, in whole or in part, from that which charac-terizes the core type of delinquent.

The formulation suggested by the facts of ourresearch yields a point of view toward causationequally applicable to urban and rural regions, tocrime in primitive systems and in developed cul-tures, to "white collar" and black collar crime, tocrime in 1960 and in George Washington's day,etc., because it recognizes a variety of etiologicpatterns and takes account of the continuous con-flict between the individual's tendencies toward thegratification of his urgent egoistic desires, on theone hand, and sociocultural taboos and prohibitionson the other. Until the sociologists can produce amore fruitful and realistic formulation than theyhave thus far developed, we prefer to adhere to ourmulticausal analysis rather than to have our re-vealing factual findings, in Tennyson's words,"Veneer'd with sanctimonious theory."

E. The same critic protests that "none of the lawgoes beyond mere correlation." He has a naive con-ception of causation, especially in the biosocialfield. High intercorrelation is of the very essence ofetiologic implication, provided the factors foundto be greatly interrelated with delinquency andnot with nondelinquency are those that "makesense" from the point of view of clinical experienceand provided it is possible to test the influence ofthe etiologic factors by removing them (or enoughof them) from the personal-social situation in aseries of cases and noting whether or not the be-havior changes. 4

IV. THE ROLE OF CULTUPm

A. Almost all the sociologic critics claim that wehave ignored what they conceive to be the most sig-

"This type of approach, and not adherence to somea priori theory through thick and thin, is the methodof science. "The main cause of this unparalleled prog-ress in physiology, pathology, medicine and surgeryhas been the fruitful application of the experimentalmethod of research, just the same method which hasbeen the great lever of all scientific advance in modemtimes." Dr. William H. Welch, Argument againstAntivivisection Bill, S. Doc. No. 34, 56th Cong., 1stSess. (Feb. 21, 1900), quoted in 1 CUSHING, LiFE oFSm WALax OsixR 521. Of course, in the field underdiscussion experiment must take a less precise and morehumane path than in the laboratory.

nificant, if not indeed the exclusive, source of de-linquency-the cultural milieu. Our view as to this(already touched upon above in discussing thecontrol of neighborhood as a prerequisite to thedetailed comparison of the delinquents with thenondelinquents) is that individuals differ withreference to the elements of culture that affectthem. They tend to choose, or to succumb to,those aspects of a culture which are naturally morecongenial. Individuals vary in constitution, tem-perament, strength of innate drives, and degree ofintegrative and inhibitory capacity. These differ-ences are the result not only of early conditioning,but also, to an extent as yet unmeasurable, of he-redity. Especially when the educative and suppor-tive social agencies are inadequate or in process ofrapid change does the reaction of different in-dividuals subjected to a similar culture vary.Some find it impossible to inhibit their primitiveimpulses in the absence, or even in the presence,of the deterrent influence of external force; othershave so efficiently "internalized" the psychologicaccompaniments of various forms of authoritythat they have an efficient superego (conscience)which, despite major changes in cultural controls,still enables them to "toe the mark."

But apart from the evidence of general experi-ence as to nature and nurture, there are severalcrucial statistics that cast serious doubt upon theview held by some sociologists that such culturalentities as the gang, or the "delinquency sub-culture," or the "lower-class subculture," or thewider general culture comprise the most potent(if not the exclusive) "cause" of delinquency.

(1) As to gang membership, as previously pointedout, it has been established, in UNRAVELING JUvE-

NLE DELINQUENCY, that almost nine-tenths of thedelinquent youths had already shown clear signsof antisocial behavior when they were under elevenyears of age; and the typical "gang age" is wellbeyond that period, in adolescence. 55

55The charge that "the Gluecks ignored their ownfinding that almost all of the delinquents were membersof gangs and that only three of the nondelinquentsbelonged to gangs" (Hartung, quoting Clinard, op. cit.supra note 6 at 705) is simply not true. In the firstplace, not almost all delinquents were found to belongto gangs, but 56%. (UNRAVELING, Table XIII-16 at163). In the second place, as pointed out in the textthe reason for excluding gang-membership in theetiology of delinquency is the fact that gang-member-ship is an adolescent phenomenon and nine-tenths ofour delinquents were already manifesting clear signsof delinquency at age 10 or less. "Factors that comeinto play after persistent antisocial behavior is es-

SHELDON GLUECK

(2) As to neighborhood culture, even in the mostmarked "delinquency areas" or delinquencysubcultures of our cities, not more than a smallfraction of the boys (say 10 or 15 per cent) becomedelinquent. It is unreasonable, therefore, to empha-size the role of neighborhood influences on thesmall percentage of boys who become delinquentand utterly ignore the fact that the vast majorityof the boys in the same neighborhoods somehowmanage not to follow a persistent antisocial career.

(3) As to the wider general culture, the New YorkCity Youth Board has established that in Ameri-ca's leading urban center no fewer than 75 percent of the delinquents are contributed by only 1per cent of the families.m

If the neighborhood subculture and the generalcultural patterns are as permeatively and inevita-bly criminalistic as they are said to be, and if thevalues they represent are as powerful in theircriminogenic influences as claimed, how accountfor these statistics?

B. It will not be amiss to say a few words aboutthe relationship of culture to the individualdifferences of those subjected to it, a crucialmatter which certain sociologists either ignore orexplain away very unconvincingly:

Suppose ten youths go out in a boat on a lake.The boat springs a leak and fills with water. Twoboys drown; the other eight successfully reachshore. It happens that one of the drowned boysdid not know how to swim, and the second couldswim but had a weak heart which could not standthe excessive exertion; the other eight were goodswimmers in fine physical condition.

Under the circumstances, what is the morerational procedure: to focus primarily and (accord-ing to some even exclusively) upon the composi-tion of the water in the lake or, while not ignoringthe lake as the setting of the deaths to be explained,to concentrate on the relevant varying charac-teristics of the individuals subjected to the verysame hazard but with such widely differing results?

tablished can hardly be regarded as relevant to theoriginal etiology of maladaptation, except as they mayreflect deep-rooted forces which do not make them-selves felt in a tendency to dissocial behavior untilpuberty or adolescence is reached." UNRAVELING at272. See, also, DELINQUENTS IN rH MAKING (basedon UNRAVELrNG) at 166.

61a The basic importance of family life has beenconfirmed in Japan: "(e) family circumstances aremore important prediction factors than neighborhoodcircumstances." JUVENrLE DELINQUENCY AS SEEN INTHE FAMILy CouRT OF JAPAN 15 (Ministry of Justice,Japan, 1957).

What was the chief "cause" of the drowningof the first two boys? Was the water the cause?This cannot be so because, despite the fact thattwo of the youths drowned, eight others, subjectedto the same hazard, managed to survive.

The eight youths were saved, not because thewater, in their case, was less deep or less wet,but because they could swim and Were in goodphysical condition. The first boy was drowned, notbecause the water, in his case, was different fromthat of the others, but because he could not swim.The second lad was drowned, not because thegeneral "cause" inevitably would result in peopledrowning, but because, although he could swim,he did not have the necessary strength to swimthe required distance to shore.

Now the water is equivalent to the "delinquencysubculture"; of ten persons subjected to a likeexternal influence, only two succumbed. Thecondition that affected them is general. It isequivalent to the existence of the institution ofproperty and of laws against theft: all men aresubject to such a uniform situation, but the vastmajority of them do not commit larceny.

Now suppose this same little drama were enactedon the high seas, where the background forces aremuch stronger than in a lake. All youths are bythis force majeure reduced to an almost similarstate. True, the first two boys probably drownearlier than the others; but the general environ-mental condition with which all the boys have tostruggle is now so overpowering as to makevirtually irrelevant any difference in individualcapacity or equipment.

Of course, the analogy is not perfect; the char-acteristics of a culture-medium are much moresubtle and complex than are the properties of alake, as is also the dynamic interplay between theculture and the human organism. But the basicprinciple illustrated by the foregoing example issimilar, as an explanatory device, to the principleinvolved in assessing the role in criminogenesis ofthe special subculture of gang membership, or theinterstitial area, or the working or middle classsubculture, or the process of differential associa-tion-favorite explanations of delinquency ad-vanced by certain criminologists.

It cannot be denied that despite the manyunwholesome and antisocial features of our culturethe great majority of people are, in normal times,relatively law-abiding. In the very research underreview, we were able to find 500 boys, living inhigh delinquency areas of Greater Boston, whom

[Vol. 51

TEN YEARS OF UNRAVELING DELINQUENCY

extremely careful investigation showed to benondelinquent. In times of exceptional crisis, suchas widespread economic depression, some personswho have been treading a precarious zone betweenlaw-abidingness and criminalism go over, or arepushed over, into antisocial territory; but even insuch times the great majority remain law-abiding.Yet they have been living and making decisionsand acting in the same malign culture as thosewho become delinquent or criminal. To insist,therefore, that in such a situation cultural influ-ences are the most satisfactory explanation of theincidence of delinquency and crime is seriously todistort the actual picture.

Antisocial aspects of culture are only potentialor possible causes of delinquency; persons of variedinnate nature and early parent-child relationshipsrespond to those elements of the culture whichthey wish, or are impelled, to introject and therebytransform into antisocial motives. In brief, certainsociologists, in their eagerness to promote theirprofession and its assumedly deserved leadershipin explanation of delinquency and crime, forgetthat environment--"culture"-plays no role inconduct unless and until it is, as it were, emotion-ally swallowed, becoming part of the motivatingforce against the taboos and demands of theprevailing culture. This oversight, among others(including a defective conception of the learningprocess and of motivation), accounts for the super-ficiality and puerility of the differential associationtheory.5" To whose who cling to the theory, thereseemingly are no such realities as the gene andthe germ plasm or individual differences in bodilymorphology, temperament, intelligence, etc.; noris there much significance to them in that mostcrucial of all cultural influences, parent-childrelationships during the first few years of life. Tothese theorists it is all the peer-group, the gang,the regional subculture, the general culture, theMasseninensch. How they can reconcile theirextreme overemphasis of environmental condi-tioning with actual differences among humanbeings from birth is a question they do not deignto answer.

C. The critic whose misinterpretation of Schach-tel's Rorschach statement I have mentioned alsoclaims, it will be recalled, that the Gluecks ignoreculture completely. With certain loyal sociologistsit is all or nothing. Although all experience, in

56 Glueck, S., Theory and Fact in Criminology, 7BraiT. J. DELiNQ. 92 (1956).

botany, zoology, biology, medicine, proves dailythat the resultant product is the outcome of theinterplay of the seed and the soil, they write as ifnothing counts but the soil.u But in point of fact,the Gluecks do not ignore culture. In stating theirviews in the opening chapter of UNRAVELNG,

s

and in the close of their summary of the dynamicetiologic pattern arrived at inductively, they say,in italics:

"In the exciting, stimulating, but little-controlled and culturally inconsistent environ-ment of the underprivileged area, such boysreadily give expression to their untamed impulsesand their self-centered desires by means ofvarious forms of delinquent behavior."w9

But they add, on the basis of carefully assembled,verified, and as nearly as possible measured data,that the

"tendencies toward uninhibited energy-expres-sion are deeply anchored in soma and psycheand in the malformations of character [compareSchchted's misinterpreted statement] duringthe first few years of life."6 0

V. THE PREDICTION METHOD

A. Ever since the publication of UNRAVELING, a

few critics have attacked that work on the groundthat the prediction tables could not possiblyforecast efficiently because they are based onequal proportions of delinquents and nondelin-quents (500:500), whereas the proportion ofnondelinquents to delinquents, even in the mostextreme "delinquency area," is only about 9:1.

We have avoided taking issue with these critics,although from the beginning we believed themwrong and certainly premature in their theorizing,because we felt that the most effective answerwould have to be the pragmatic one of the "proofof the pudding"'" through actual experience.

7 IbMd. There are of course many exceptions; thenames of Professors Paul Tappan, Thorsten Sellin,Albert Morris, Peter Lejins, Marvin Wolfgang and, inEurope, Hermann Mannheim and Wolf Middendorf,come to mind.

58 UNRAvELNG at 5-6.59 Id. at 282.60 Ibid. See also UNSAvELiNG at 15.61 "In their present work, after a long and careful

analysis of a large number of physical, psychologicaland social characters attributable to 500 delinquentsand to an equal number of controls (non-delinquentsmatched pairwise with the delinquents in respect toage, ethnic origin, intelligence and type of area ofresidence), the Gluecks set up a series of proposedprediction tables based on attributes common amongdelinquents and uncommon among the controls, or

SHELDON GLUECK

Sufficient evidence has now been accumulatedin various validation experiments of one of thetables (known in the literature as the GlueckSocial Prediction Table) to enable us to givecareful consideration to these criticisms.

The basic objection is entered, for example, byReiss, who states dogmatically that "unless this[50 %] is the actual rate in a similar population forwhich the predictions are made, the tables willyield very poor prediction."6 2 Reiss recomputedthe Glueck Social Prediction Table, taking accountof an assumed 9:1 ratio of nondelinquents todelinquents, and purports to prove that, by hisfigures, the original table can show very littlepredictive capacity:

"It can be seen in Table 2 that, so far asprediction of delinquency or nondelinquency isconcerned, the table has a low predictive effi-ciency when the rate of delinquency is estimatedat 10 per cent. For example, in the score 250-99the chances of delinquency were 63.5 in theGluecks' table, while the chances are only 16.2per hundred in the table which assumes a rateof 10 per cent habitual delinquency." 63

Reiss and similar critics have not clearly ex-plained just why the adjustment to a supposedactual proportion of 9:1 is necessary; or, why dif-ferences in the incidence of delinquents and non-delinquents in any population should and wouldhave a serious distorting influence on the distri-bution of scores of the predictive factors as pre-sented in our 50-50 table.

(1) If their point is, simply, that where the pro-portion of nondelinquents to delinquents in apopulation is far higher than one-half, one canjust as readily "predict" by assuming all boys tobe nondelinquent as by going to the trouble ofexamining the families to see which boys possessthe deleterious categories of the predictive factorsin their background and which do not, then thepoint is insignificant. Of course one can assume

vice versa. I do not see how they could have donebetter, but the proof of the pudding can come only withits eating." Wilson, Prediction, 64 HAlv. L. REv. 1040(1951).

6 Reiss, op. cit. supra note 5 at 118. Similar criti-cisms have been made by Shaplin & Tiedman, Commenton the Juvenile Delinquency Prediction Tables in theGluecks' "Unraveling Juvenile Dedinquincy," 16 Am.Soc. REv. 544 (1951); Stott, The Prediction of De-linquency from Non-Delinquent Behaviour, 10 BRIT. J.DELINQ. 195 (1960); Duncan, Review of "PredictingDelinzency and Crime," by S. and E. T. Glueck, 65Am. J. Soc. 537 (1960).

1 R iss, op. cit. supra note 5 at 119.

that in a population in which the proportion ofnondelinquents to delinquents is 9:1 all boys arenondelinquent and thereby triumphantly pointout that one has guessed wrongly in only 10 percent of all cases, which is alleged to be as goodas the Glueck table is able to do. 64

But that is not the issue.The issue is whether one can identify, individ-

ually, the future delinquents and the future non-delinquents; otherwise one is not really predictingat all but asserting what was known, ex hypothesi,beforehand.

(2) It may be, however, that what the criticshave in mind is the possibility that there couldbe factors involved in distinguishing delinquentsfrom nondelinquents other than the ones used inthe Glueck table, which express themselves in theresult that in the community to which a 50-50table is to be applied the proportions are really90:10.

This raises certain important issues. There areseemingly three problems involved in the criticismunder discussion:

(a) Will the Glueck table's distribution of scoresfor samples of delinquents, as found in the originalBoston study, reproduce itself in other communi-ties?

(b) Will the table's distribution of scores fornondelinquents reproduce itself in other popula-tions?

(c) Even assuming that the distributions of newpopulations of delinquents and nondelinquents areidentical with the Glueck table, will the tableprove to be an effective predictive instrument ina population in which the proportion of nonde-linquents is much higher than one-half?

It seems to us that in respect to all three of thesequestions the critics are confusing the countingof heads with the weighing of heads; a blind censuswith a device for pinpointing delinquents and non-delinquents; the percentage-incidence of nonde-linquents and delinquents in a particular regionwith the incidence of certain criminogenic factorsin a representative sample of delinquents com-pared with a representative sample of nondelin-quents. To put it differently, they are confusingthe effect of differences in the size of two statistical

A Indeed, one critic rushed into print in the NewYork City press (claiming, incidentally, that he repre-sents a society of some 2,000 psychologists and therebyimplicating them) with the assertion that by "pullingnames out of a hat" he could do just as well as can bedone by the Glueck table!

[Vol. 51

TEN YEARS OF UNRAVELING DELINQUENCY

"universes" with differences in the size of sub-stantial, representative samples drawn from thosetwo universes. By confusing the concept of uni-verse with that of sample, the critics are beggingthe very question at issue, which is: Will the fac-tors embodied in the Glueck table actually identifyprospective delinquents and potential nonde-linquents with a high degree of accuracy, irrespec-tive of other influences, including the differences inthe size of the universes from which the originalsamples were drawn and to which they are applied?

Our prediction table is not designed to forecastthe probable chances of a boy's delinquency if helives in Boston or a city like Boston; but ratherthe chances of his becoming delinquent if he hap-pens to have in his makeup and background thecrucial factors which have been shown, in com-paring numerous delinquents with numerous non-delinquents, to be highly associated with persist-ency in misbehavior.

(3) The use of equal numbers in the samples origi-nally compared is not only legitimate but impor-tant for the accurate determination of the inci-dence of the factors under comparison. It is, forexample, a frequent technique in medical research.The difference in the proportion of the pools fromwhich the samples of nondelinquents and delin-quents were drawn can affect the outcome onlyif influential factors other than those embraced inthe samples are omitted or included; and this is tobe determined, not a priori, by manipulating theoriginal table to reflect a 90:10 proportion, butonly empirically, by the application of the tableto other populations. Assuming that the sample ofdelinquents and the sample of nondelinquents arefairly representative of the populations from whichthe cases were derived, the fact that the totalgroup of nondelinquents in the general populationis nine times as numerous as the total group ofdelinquents can have little to do with the outcomewhen comparing the two samples; and it should,equally, have little to do with the outcome whenapplying the table to new populations.

If one were making a study comparing the in-cidence of blood pressure, pulse, certain chemicalsin the blood and urine, etc., of persons with amalignant disease, with their incidence amonghealthy persons, would it make any differencewhether the general incidence of such diseasedpersons in the particular community amounted to10 per cent or 50 per cent? And, assuming that inthe city in which the original experiment was done

the population proportions of the well and the illwere 50-50, would this fact interfere with the pre-dictive capacity of a table of indications and symp-toms when applied to a city in which the propor-tions were .90:10?

Our own view has been and is that the varia-tion in the proportion of nondelinquents to de-linquents as between the original population andthe new one to which the table is applied shouldhave very little to do with the capacity of thetable to "spot" the delinquents and the nondelin-quents. The reason is that the characteristicsinductively arrived at as most markedly distin-guishing the two groups were selected from amongmore than four hundred factors as to the incidenceof which the 500 pairs of matched delinquentsand nondelinquents had been compared at levelsof study ranging, widely, from anthropometricand psychiatric to social. It should be borne inmind, tbo, that by chi-square calculation we re-garded as statistically significant only those factorsin which the probability of the difference foundbetween the delinquents and the controls beingdue to chance or random sampling was less thanone in a hundred. 5

We are aware of the fact, however, that, despiteall this, empirical evidence might disprove ourhypothesis as to the high identifying capacity ofthe prediction table. But one should not dogmatizeat the outset that the influence of differences inproportions will seriously affect the outcome; onemust await the proof of the pudding.

B. This brings me to the series of validation studiesthat have thus far been made. They have beencarried out by different investigators, some inCambridge and Boston, Massachusetts, some inNew York City, one in New Jersey, one in Ohio,one in Strasbourg, France, several in Japan. Thetable has so far been applied to many hundreds ofchildren and adolescents (in three instances tofemales). In each study the table has given highlyencouraging results.

Dr. Eleanor T. Glueck has analyzed the resultsin two articles, 6 with the following general con-clusion:

"The accumulated evidence thus far gatheredfrom 'retrospective' and 'prospective' studies

6 See "Explanation of Statistical Method," by CarlR. Doering, in UNRAVEraINo at 75-76.65 Glueck, E. T., Spotting Potential Delinquents:

Can it Be Done?, 20 FED. PROB. 7 (No. 3 1956); Glueck,E. T., Efforts to Identify Delinquents, 24 FED. PROB.49 (No. 2 1960).

1960]

SHELDON GLUECK

both in the United States and in foreign coun-tries all seems to be tending in the same direc-tion. A total of 18 inquiries in which the SocialPrediction Table has been applied are all sug-gestive of its usefulness. The studies includefour samples of nondelinquents (the latter in-corporated into the first of the two investigationsby Thompson in 1954, in the New York CityYouth Board Study, in the Maximum BenefitsProject [Washington, D. C.], and in the firstof the Japanese inquiries); three studies includegirls (the second Thompson study, the studyby Glick and Donnell of 150 unmarried mothers,and the Maximum Benefits Project). '66

Two types of validation studies have been madeby various investigators:

(1) The retrospective checkups include a study byBlack and Glick of the Jewish Board of Guardianson 100 Jewish delinquent boys at the Hawthorne-Cedar Knolls School (the 500 matched pairs inthe Glueck study comprised, largely, English-American, Italian, and Irish boys of Protestantand Catholic religions; only 10 Jewish boys wereincluded67); one by Thompson on 100 boys of thewell-known Cambridge-Somerville Youth Study66

(involving differentiation of true delinquentsfrom pseudodelinquents among children showingbehavioral difficulties); a study in 1955 from theNew Jersey State Department of Institutions andAgencies on 51 delinquents on parole; a study in1956 by clinicians of the Thom Clinic for children(Boston) on 57 "antisocial young children"; astudy by Thompson, 1957, comprising 50 boys whohad been before the Boston Juvenile Court in 1950and 50 girls committed by that tribunal to the careof the Massachusetts Youth Service Board; a studyby Glick and Donnell of 150 young unmarriedmothers; a study by Glick of 150 boys of a largersample from upper-income families ($7,500 andthereabouts), to which it had been originallyplanned to apply the table.

The typical outcome of these checkups is thatin some nine-tenths of the cases (very occasionallya somewhat lower proportion) the Glueck tablewould have correctly identified the boys at a veryearly age as potential persistent delinquents or(where the study involved them) as potential non-delinquents.

66 Glueck, E. T., Efforts to Identify Delinquents,op. cit. supra note 65a at 54.67 UNRAVELiNG, Table IV-6 at 38.

68 These 100 were chosen, essentially, by a methodintended to select a random sample from a muchlarger quantity of cases.

As suggested, there have been other retrospec-tive checkups of the table both here and abroad: 9

In 1956 it was applied by a psychiatrist to 100boys, "chosen at random for a wide variety ofbehavior and emotional difficulties," who had beenreferred to the Thom Clinic, Boston. Of 31 boyspreviously diagnosed by the clinicians as of "anti-social character," all "without exception" hadbeen placed by the Glueck table in the group witha high likelihood of persistent delinquency.

In Japan the first study applied the table to 30delinquent and 30 nondelinquent boys and foundthat 87 per cent of the delinquents and 92 percent of the nondelinquents were correctly identi-fied; other inquiries there made, and some underway, are producing similarly favorable results,though specific figures are as yet unavailable.

In Strasbourg, France, application of the tableto a group of 46 delinquents by trained psycholo-gists (a pilot study to be followed by a more com-prehensive one) showed that 91.4 per cent of theboys would have been correctly identified at theage of six. A second investigation in 1959, by thesame group, under the oversight of ProfessorJacques L~aut6 of the Institut de Science Crirninelleof the Faculty of Law, involved 140 court delin-quents and 63 boys brought in by parents forincorrigibility; 89.9 per cent of the first group andalmost all of the second would have been correctlyidentified by the table as potential delinquents.

So much for the retrospective checkups.

(2) As to prospective studies, the most importantone is that of the New York City Youth Board,originally set up in 1953 in two public schools inregions of very high delinquency rates. The tablewas applied to boys entering the first grade (usuallyat age six).76 The boys have been followed for someseven years into a great many schools in New YorkCity. On the 223 boys involved, the findings todate show that of 186 predicted at school entranceto be nondelinquents, 176, or 94.6 per cent, arestill (1960) in fact nondelinquent-a remarkableconfirmation when it is considered that these boyscame from the same high delinquency areas asdid the boys identified to be delinquent. Of 37

69 References will be found in Glueck, E. T., Effortsto Identify Delinquents, op. cit. supra note 65a. PublicProsecutor Haruo Abe is preparing a comprehensivestudy of Japanese prediction experiments, includingvalidations of the Glueck Social Prediction Table.

70 The claim that tables based on boys around 14Myears old could not possibly be predictive of the be-havior of boys of 6-7 was made, among others, byRubin; op. cit. supra note 5 at 112; Reiss, op. cit. supranote 5 at 117; Burgess, op. cit. supra note 7 at 53-54;Monachesi, op. cit. supra note 36 at 57.

[Vol. 51

TEN YEARS OF UNRAVELING DELINQUENCY

boys predicted as delinquents, 13 are already ad-judicated delinquents and 4 more are "unofficial"offenders, making a total of 46 per cent-again aremarkable confirmation of a forecast when it isconsidered that the peak age of arrest for delin-quency is in the middle and late 'teens and theseboys are of an average age of only 12-13 years.,It is necessary to continue to follow up the careersof these youths at least until they reach 18, thejuvenile court limit; but it would be astonishingindeed if the results of the seven-year follow-upwere to be nullified by the five or six years thestudy still has to run.

A second checkup on future behavior is apreliminary report on the Maximum Benefits Proj-ect, begun in Washington, D. C., in 1954. Childrenselected for study and treatment were those whopresented various classroom problems and whoseteachers thought them to be in need of professionalaid. The study involved 179 youngsters receivedover three academic years for behavioral difficulties.Application of the Glueck table to these childrenshowed that only 21 of the 179 had scores less than250, which we deem to be a satisfactory cut-offpoint for distinguishing between children unlikely,and those likely, to become delinquent; the remain-ing 158 (88.3%) had at least a 63.5 per cent chance(half of them an 89.2 per cent chance) of becomingdelinquent.

In a follow-up study early in 1958, 58 of thesechildren had already been to court or police fordelinquent acts; and the Glueck score had beenover 250 in all but one of these. It must be bornein mind that the checking is far from complete;and, as indicated, the maximum age of arrests isin the middle and late 'teens.

(3) It is true that the follow-up studies thusfar made deal with relatively small samples ofcases; but they are all random samples and all of

71 In UNRAVEriNG, the average age of the delinquentsat first court appearance was 12.4 years, and 25.8%were first summonsed at 14, 15 or 16.

72 Several more follow-up studies have been calledto our attention since the above was written. For amore recent summary of prediction validation studiesin Japan which confirm the earlier findings in regard tothe validity of the Glueck Social Prediction Table, seeJuhei Takeuchi, "Juvenile Delinquency in Japan-Characteristics and Preventive Programs," a speechprepared for the Second United Nations Congress on thePrevention of Crime and the Treatment of Offenders,London, 1960, especially notes 24-32 (mimeographed).Those interested in the growth of Japanese predictionstudies are invited to write to the Criminological Re-search Division of the Research Training Institute ofthe Ministry of Justice, 1-1 Kasumigaseki, Chiyo-daku, Tokyo, Japan.

them, without exception, support the originaltable. It can hardly be claimed that this consistentoutcome, in such a variety of cases and places, isattributable to "the long arm of coincidence."Should later evidence nevertheless run counter tothe record thus far established, we will be thefirst to make it known.

(4) It should be pointed out that the tables re-flecting the experience in these checkups resemblenot the adjusted tables of Reiss and others, butthe original Glueck table. Thus, for example, thereport by the New Jersey Department of Institu-tions and Agencies on delinquent boys on parolesays: "It will be observed that the closeness of thefindings on the basis of the New Jersey data withthe original findings in the study of UnravelingJuvenile Delinquency is rather noteworthy, sincethe New Jersey boys were selected at random,and no attempt was made to match the individualcharacteristics of the New Jersey delinquent boyswith the delinquent boys included in the HarvardLaw School study.' 'T The resemblance of the dis-tribution of scores for samples of nondelinquentsand delinquents in such a study as, for example,the New York City Youth Board's, suggests that,had the adjusted table of Reiss or other critics beenused instead of the original, the utility of our tablewould have been seriously reduced, the predictionswould have greatly miscarried, and there wouldhave been many more "false positives" (a conceptdiscussed below) than turned up through. the useof the original table.

(5) On the basis of the checkup studies thus farmade, we may now consider the first two questionsposed at the outset of this section:

Does the Glueck table's distribution of scoresfor samples of delinquents reproduce itself else-where?

Nobody can read the evidence marshalled inthe papers by Mrs. Glueck and too briefly sum-marized herein without acknowledging that theanswer can already safely be Yes.

Does the Glueck table's distribution of scoresfor new samples of nondelinquents reproduce itselfelsewhere?

Here, while it is true that only two of the valida-tion studies so far carried out are prospective,74 the

7 3 STATE DEPARTMENT Op INsTITUTioNs AND AGEN-

CIES, PREDICTING JUVENILE DELINQUENcY, ResearchBulletin No. 124, Trenton, N. J. (April, 1955).

74 The first Thompson study may also be deemedprospective, because it involved a follow-up of casespreviously prognosticated by a Selection Committeeas either potentially delinquent or potentially non-delinquent on the basis of study of case-history data.

19601

SHELDON GLUECK

evidence is already reasonably persuasive that theanswer will again be Yes. After some seven years,of the 186 boys predicted as future nondelinquentsthough residing in the same high delinquency areasas the delinquents, 176, or 94.6 per cent, are stillnondelinquent, a fact determined by intensivefollow-up.

Before turning to the final question posed above,I should like to point out that not only do thevarious checkup experiments embrace a variety ofpotential and actual delinquency, ethnic deriva-tion, economic status, religion, cultural back-ground, and even sex, but-something especiallypertinent to the claim that, to have predictivepower, the table must be applied to a populationsimilar in the proportion of delinquents to non-delinquents to that of the original table--thecheckups also covered populations presenting avariety of proportions of nondelinquents to de-linquents.

Thus it can reasonably be vouchsafed, on thebasis of the empirical evidence so far adduced,that the Glueck table will tend to reproduce itsdistribution of scores for both delinquents andnondelinquents when it is applied to other popu-lations.

The essential support of the table's pattern offactor-scores as related to behavior-outcomes byits application to a variety of samples elsewhereis very important to criminology; for as empha-sized by the distinguished biostatistician, Pro-fessor Edwin Bidwell Wilson, "Science advancesby broadening the base upon which empiricaluniformities are established; indeed, it is only bythis broadening that we come to know that thedescriptions of our samples are uniformities.""7

(6) This brings me to the third question involvedin the issue raised by the critics: To what extent isthe Glueck table an "efficient" predictive instrument;that is, to what degree does it improve on a "pre-diction" made by simply calling every boy in thecommunity nondelinquent?

Since no prediction device can reasonably beexpected to forecast accurately in all cases, therewill inevitably be some "false positives," that is,some persons wrongly forecast as future delin-quents and some wrongly spotted as future non-delinquents. To be sure, true prediction, especiallyin this field, cannot be 100 per cent accurate; butthe Glueck table is indubitably much more correct

7Wilson, Prediction, 64 HARv. L. Rlv. 1040,1041 (1951).

in identifying potential delinquents and potentialnondelinquents than the method suggested bythe critics which does not identify any child butsupplies only the proportion of nondelinquents todelinquents that was estimated at the outset.

The "false positives" aspect of the problem isnot a scientific one but an issue in social ethicsand social policy. In this connection, it is veryimportant to point out that mistakes in not spot-ting future delinquents can be very costly to society,while mistakes in assuming a few persons to bepotential delinquents (10 out of the 186 in theNew York study, for example) who neverthelessultimately turn out not to be delinquents can dolittle harm and might even do considerable good.Professor Samuel Stouffer, a social scientist forwhom we have high respect, has suggested thatif, for example, the Glueck table accurately "spots"17 out of 20 potential delinquents but does so atthe cost of predicting, say, 18 nondelinquents tobe delinquents,

"the answer might be that the 18 'false positives'may not be hurt by the extra close watching wegive them, for many of them may really beborderline or incipient delinquents. Further-more, in view of the specific social factors usedin prediction, these kids may be really in needof some help in loco parentis. Hence, the answerfrom the point of view of social ethics might bethat the social gain in spotting the 17 correctlymore than justified any damage which mightbe done to the 18 spotted incorrectly. If every-body scoring above 250 were to be sent to aninstitution, society would scream. But if thekind of watchful and helpful treatment whichthe 'false positives' might get, along with thetrue delinquents would not hurt them or wouldactually be good for them, you would have amost convincing case.""It should be added, first, that the New York

City Youth Board's validation study still hasseveral years to run, and if it proceeds along itspresent lines the chances are that the percentage of"false positives" will be very small indeed. Sec-ondly, Professor Lloyd Oblin, who has had boththeoretical and practical experience with prcdic-tion methods, has rated the efficiency of the ClueckSocial Prediction Table very high in comparisonwith others:

"An analysis of published results shows thatthe predictive efficiency of the experience tables

76 Letter to authors by Professor Stouffer.

[Vol. 51

TEN YEARS OF UNRA VELING DELINQUENCY

in follow-up samples varies from an efficiency of25 per cent to a loss of 41 per cent in efficiencyover what could be achieved with a simpleprediction of success for all cases, a notableexception to these modest prediction resultsbeing the predictive efficiency of the GlueckSocial Prediction Scale from Unraveling JuvenileDelinquency which has been computed by Ohlinas 55 per cent."77

(7) There is a related aspect of social policy to beconsidered-the question whether, assuming ahigh predictive potency of the Table, it is desirableto apply it in public schools as the basis of a pro-phylactic program, when this might entail "stig-matizing" a few children. It has been claimed thatsuch a procedure would "label" such children aspredelinquent and by that very process turn theminto delinquents to live up to the role thus assignedthem. 78 Apart from the superficial conception ofchild psychology revealed in such a dogmatic claim,it cannot be supposed that trained social workerswould typically force themselves into a home anddramatically announce, "Your child has beenpredicted as a delinquent!"; nor would the child betold this. In the New York City experiment,neither teachers, nor parents, nor children 'havebeen told which boys are probably potentialdelinquents and which are not. If that experimentshould be followed by a program of delinquencyprophylaxis, it is presumed that trained social caseworkers will be entrusted with the job, and theycan be expected to be more perceptive and tactfulthan to "stigmatize" a child as a predelinquent. Itmust be remembered, also, that, typically, thefamilies involved are already the clients of socialagencies for all sorts of problems other than de-linquency.

If the argument of those who oppose the use ofidentification techniques to disclose which childrenare vulnerable were sound, we should logicallydose all of our child guidance clinics, dismiss our

7 Report by L. E. Ohlin to Third InternationalCongress of Criminology, London, 1955; GLuECK, S.& E. T., PREDIcTrING DELiNQUENcY AND CRnm at 150,n. 5.78 "No magic formula exists for identifying boys and

girls who will later commit delinquent acts except bystigmatizing as 'pre-delinquent' many who will nevercome before a court." Report to the Congress onJuvenile Delinquency, U. S. Department of Health,Education, and Welfare, Children's Bureau. It is sub-mitted that in the light of the discussion in the text,the quotation expresses a superficial interpretation andan unwise social policy. We have never claimed any"magic formula."

school counselors and visiting teachers, and sitback complacently (as, unfortunately, we too oftendo today) until the child has developed into a truedelinquent or gang member and then haul himinto court with the usual far from satisfactoryresult.

The choice presented to a community is whetherits citizens prefer to let potentially delinquentchildren ripen into persistent offenders or to in-tervene, prophylactically, at a stage which givesthe greatest promise of changing their dangerousattitudes and behavior, by aiding parents to mod-ify their damaging disciplinary and nonaffectionalattitudes and practices which have been found, inthousands of cases, to be potent influences ininclining children to delinquency. Our follow-upresearches have consistently shown the tragic roleof deep-rootedness in rendering antisocial be-havior impervious to the usual methods of treat-ment thus far invented by society. 79 Finally, itshould be pointed out that the application of thetable might be limited to cases reported by teachersas presenting difficulties in class, in order to dis-tinguish true delinquents from pseudodelinquents,or the services offered the parents of a child foundvulnerable can be voluntary, not compulsory; butit is hoped that with the passage of time parentswill learn to welcome aid in the behavioral field asthey have in the medical.

CONCLUSION

I have attempted to give due consideration toall the major criticisms leveled at UNRAVEINGJUVENILE DELINQUENCY.n Naturally, the authors

79 In PEDicriNG DE:LiNQUENCY AND CRane op. cit.supra note 48a at 82-83, it is shown that in 18 out of 30prediction tables dealing with adult offenders and 3of 10 concerned with juvenile delinquents, during allforms of sentence-treatment and for 15 years there-after, the factor of age at onset of antisocial behaviorhad to be included as a differentiative indicator be-cause of its strong predictive influence as betweensuccesses and failures. In all but four of the tablesdealing with adult offenders, the earlier the onset of thedelinquency, the higher the failure-score under one oranother form of peno-correctional treatment and there-after; or, to state it differently, the deeper the rootsof childhood maladjustment, the smaller the chanceof adult adjustment.80 There is a criticism by psychiatrists typified bythe following statement, which I have not attemptedto go into thoroughly because it would require a sepa-rate and lengthy paper: "A truly dynamic approach... recognizes that any individual's behavior is theresult of the interaction between internal and externalforces, and can be understood dynamically not in termsof his membership in any class, but only in terms of hisunique life situation-what he is reacting to and ex-

19601

SHELDON GLUECK

of that work are convinced, and I trust I haveconvinced the reader, that the theories advancedby the critics are essentially unsound. With ill-advised Olympian hauteur, two of our severestcritics call attention to the literature on actuarialwork which they claim we should have read. 81 In

ressing in his behavior." Satten, in Symposium on theGecks' Latest Research, 15 FED. PROB. 53 (No. 1 1951).There is some truth in this, of course; but there is alsothe danger of the dynamic psychiatrist "individual-izing" quite inaccurately. To take account of all theinfluences of a single person's "unique life situation,"is something that only God can do. The criticism underdiscussion has been largely directed against the use ofpredictive devices in aid of clinician and judge. Here Ican only say that it springs from a misconception ofboth the aims of UNxRnvxLiNo and the prediction tablesdeveloped therein. It is of course not intended thateither clinicians or judges should make their decisionsmechanically, exclusively on the "odds" presented bythe prediction device. The aim is not to substitutemechanical gadgets for either clinical or judicial ex-pertise based on much experience but rather to furnishpsychiatrist and judge an instrument reflecting organ-ized experience based on the follow-up of numerouscases that have gone before. This is clearly superiorto exclusive reliance on "clinical experience" or judicial"hunch." Nor does it counteract such individuali-zation as is possible. As the Illinois experience with aprediction device used in parole has shown, "the netresult of the use of the tables has been to challenge theapplication of mechanical formulas at every point andto force more detailed examination of the unique meritsof the individual case." Ohlin, quoted in Glueck, S.,Prognosis of Recidivism, Sec. IV GENERAL REPORT,Thn=D INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS OF CRIMINOLOGY19 n.14 (London, Sept. 1955).

51 "But then there is a technical literature on thetechnique of prediction." Reiss, op. cit. supra note 5 at119. "The relevant literature is so voluminous andlucid that ignorance can be no excuse for failure tomeet criteria of acceptable research design .... It issupererogatory to publish an introduction to the book[PREDIcTING DELINQUENCY AND CPIus] by the Chief

the light of the foregoing analysis, I trust they willforgive me if I too call attention to several bits of"literature."

First, I would call attention to the prescientjudgment of Professor Edwin Bidwell Wilson: "Apriori argument will not get far, howsoever it beextended. What one needs is trial and observa-tion." 82 And in this connection, some advice onthe relation of theory to fact, offered by two dis-tinguished scientists of the past, are not withoutrelevancy:

In the wise bequest left by the eminent Russianphysiologist-psychologist, Ivan Pavlov, just beforehis death in 1936, he said:

"Perfect as is the wing of a bird, it neverwould raise the bird up without resting on air.Facts are the air of a scientist. Without themyour 'theories' are vain efforts."

And the distinguished physiologist, Claude Ber-nard, laid down this safe rule to follow:

"When you meet with a fact opposed to aprevailing theory, you should adhere to thefact and abandon the theory, even when thelatter is supported by great authorities andgenerally adopted."

Justice of the United States Supreme Court [sic] callingfor 'unbiased consideration' of the authors' predictiontables on the part of 'people with open minds, mindsthat are open to accept or reject the thesis solely onreason.'" Duncan, op. cit. supra note 62 at 539. (Ah,we pure scientists !)

In the light of the analysis in the text, I believe Ican confidently leave it to the reader to judge for him-self with whom the charge of supererogation mightmore fairly be lodged.

2 Wilson' Book Review, 64 HARv. L. REv. 1039,1041 (1951).

(Vol. 51