Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

21
(Space) Size matters… Varying space sizes increases memorability and likelihood to take action for newsletter copy Kath Straub, PhD, Chief Scientist Last month, HFI’s User Interface Update newsletter was an experiment. Did you figure that out? At the end of the newsletter there was an invitation to participate in a survey. The survey had a mix of questions. We asked how you felt about the PDF format. You suggested topics you would like to read about. And then we asked about the content of the newsletter—to see how much you remembered from what you read. If you think back, the article was broadly about the usability of Kindle2. But the research review focused on how Kindle would be even better if it automatically improved readability by applying a text formatting algorithm, like ReadSmart. And we reviewed the evidence that the ReadSmart algorithm enhances readability, retention and reader satisfaction. But old habits die hard. And, like any good skeptic, I also wanted to test Bever’s claims. So, last month’s newsletter was actually an A|B test: Half of you received a “regular” copy of the newsletter. The other half received a ReadSmart copy. The survey explored 2 primary questions: 1. Do readers who were exposed to the ReadSmart copy remember more and remember more accurately than those who received the “regular,” unformatted text? 2. Are individuals who read the ReadSmarted copy be more likely to take action than those who read the “regular” version? The answers are yes and yes. The details Two versions of the newsletter were distributed through the normal email, twitter and download channels. 1. Regular/Control: The visually treated newsletter was printed directly to a PDF format from Quark with standard spacing between words and letters. 2. ReadSmart: The visually treated Quark document was submitted to ReadSmart to apply the text formatting algorithm. The algorithm applied psychologically tested, linguistic rules to vary the size of spaces between letters and between words determine optimal line endings. ReadSmart returned the PDF document that was distributed. Unless you know what to look for, its difficult to tell which document is which:

Transcript of Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

Page 1: Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

(Space)Sizematters…Varyingspacesizesincreasesmemorabilityandlikelihoodtotakeactionfornewslettercopy

KathStraub,PhD,ChiefScientist

Lastmonth,HFI’sUserInterfaceUpdatenewsletterwasanexperiment.Didyoufigurethatout?Attheendofthenewslettertherewasaninvitationtoparticipateinasurvey.Thesurveyhadamixofquestions.WeaskedhowyoufeltaboutthePDFformat.Yousuggestedtopicsyouwouldliketoreadabout.Andthenweaskedaboutthecontentofthenewsletter—toseehowmuchyourememberedfromwhatyouread.

Ifyouthinkback,thearticlewasbroadlyabouttheusabilityofKindle2.ButtheresearchreviewfocusedonhowKindlewouldbeevenbetterifitautomaticallyimprovedreadabilitybyapplyingatextformattingalgorithm,likeReadSmart.AndwereviewedtheevidencethattheReadSmartalgorithmenhancesreadability,retentionandreadersatisfaction.Butoldhabitsdiehard.And,likeanygoodskeptic,IalsowantedtotestBever’sclaims.

So,lastmonth’snewsletterwasactuallyanA|Btest:Halfofyoureceiveda“regular”copyofthenewsletter.TheotherhalfreceivedaReadSmartcopy.Thesurveyexplored2primaryquestions:

1. DoreaderswhowereexposedtotheReadSmartcopyremembermoreandremembermoreaccuratelythanthosewhoreceivedthe“regular,”unformattedtext?

2. AreindividualswhoreadtheReadSmartedcopybemorelikelytotakeactionthanthosewhoreadthe“regular”version?

Theanswersareyesandyes.

Thedetails

Twoversionsofthenewsletterweredistributedthroughthenormalemail,twitteranddownloadchannels.

1.Regular/Control:ThevisuallytreatednewsletterwasprinteddirectlytoaPDFformatfromQuarkwithstandardspacingbetweenwordsandletters.2.ReadSmart:ThevisuallytreatedQuarkdocumentwassubmittedtoReadSmarttoapplythetextformattingalgorithm.Thealgorithmappliedpsychologicallytested,linguisticrulesto

• varythesizeofspacesbetweenlettersandbetweenwords• determineoptimallineendings.

ReadSmartreturnedthePDFdocumentthatwasdistributed.Unlessyouknowwhattolookfor,itsdifficulttotellwhichdocumentiswhich:

Page 2: Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

Downloadandcomparethecompletedocuments:StandardFormatversusFormattedwithReadsmart

Thetwoformatsweredistributedrandomly.HalfofsubscribersreceivedReadSmartedformatting.Thesecondhalfreceivedtheregularformatting.Toensurethatthetextformattingrenderedasintended,thedocumentwassentasaPDFfile.EachnewsletterincludedalinkinvitingreaderstoparticipateinabriefsurveyinexchangeforthechancetowinaniPODshuffle.Althoughitwasnotobvious,thelinkswerecodedsothatparticipants’surveyresponsescouldbeindexedtotheformattheyreceived(Readsmartvs.Standard).

Analysiswascompletedforthe380individualswhorespondedtoallthequestionsonthesurvey.(Individualswhosubmittedpartialresponseswerealsoenteredintheipoddrawing).

OveralltheanalysisdemonstratedthatindividualswhoreadtheReadSmartversionandcompletedthesurveyunderstoodthecontentbetter,andweremorelikelytotakeactionwithit.

Specifically,individualswhoreadaReadSmartednewsletterrememberthedetailsofthearticlemoreaccuratelythanthosewhoreadthe“regular,”unformattedtext.

ReadSmartreaderschosecorrectanswers,orfailedtochooseincorrectchoicesmoreoftenin100%ofthequestions.P<.0005(signtest,twotailed).TheimprovedcomprehensionofReadSmartreadersreflectedacombinationofmorecorrectchoicesofcorrectanswers(p<.03)andfewerincorrectchoicesofincorrectanswers(p<.02).

Perhapsmoreimportantly,individualswhoreadtheReadSmartedtextreportedthattheyweremorelikelytotakeactiononit.33%ofreadersoftheReadSmartversionsaidtheywoulddefinitelyorprobablyforwardthestorytosomeoneelse;29%ofreadersofthenormalformatsaidthis.Intermsofclick‐throughs,differencereflectsa+10%likelihoodtoforwardthepieceontoafriendorcolleague.CouplethiswiththePETunderstandingthatinformationthatoriginatesfromtrustedsourceshasgreatercredibilityandinfluence.Ithinkwehavesomethingtowritehomeabout.TextformattingwithReadSmartimprovesreadabilityandactionability.Youdemonstratedit.

Authorcontactpoints

email:[email protected]:kaslinkedIN:KathStraub

FormoreinformationonwhatReadSmartdoesorhowitworks,contact:[email protected]

Page 3: Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

Kindle2: Crack for readers... until you start readingHow text formatting can ruin (or enhance) the readability and persuasiveness of text

Kath Straub, PhD, CUA / Chief ScientistHuman Factors International, Inc.

February / March, 2009

Newsletter

Human FactorsI n te rna t i ona l

Page 4: Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

Kindle2: Crack for readers... until you start reading

Newsletter February / March 2009 2

Maybe it was the early adopter thing. Curiosity about new gadgets. Wanting to touch. To play. To decide if that new little thing will be the next big thing.

Maybe it was the road-warrior reader thing: I travel a lot. And I read a lot. Which book should I take?

Maybe it was the attention deficit thing. Reading one book at a time gets... boring. And all those books get heavy.

However you explain it, I own a Kindle2.

It’s been an interesting ride. I’m ready to get off the bike.

To be sure, part of the fun of Kindle is that Amazon had to balance a lot of design options. For instance, they tried to create an unpacking-the-product-should-be-emotional experience. For an Apple native, that was a bit weird. Somehow, the pull-off paper zipper (think FedEX envelopes) sets the wrong tone. But, they tried.

Out of the box, my first impression was positive. It’s smaller than I thought. The text resolution is better than I had hoped.

But then, there is no backlight. That means the battery lasts a really long time. But it also means that the screen is surprisingly grey and the text contrast is low. And you still need a night light to read.

The keyboard lets you annotate while you read. But it’s awkward. Big. That choice seemed odd since the bigger keyboard meant a smaller reading screen. We are all trained to type on phone-sized keyboards by now, aren’t we?

Navigating isn’t bad. The fact that the Menu button takes you to Shop At the Kindle Store is irritating. Even if I understand why it’s so. The joystick offers a few surprises, like you can’t turn pages with it.

May you live in interesting times

You say red herring, I say red herrings

Page 5: Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

Kindle2: Crack for readers... until you start reading

Newsletter February / March 2009 3

But these problems are red herring(s). Kindle isn’t really about unpacking and navigating. It’s about reading. Unfortunately, the reading part is where Amazon goes wrong.

It was a small detail in the Kindle ad that tipped my decision to try it. Unless you’ve spent years thinking about the psychology of reading, you probably won’t notice it.

The text on the right side of the “page” is not right-justified. It has a ragged-right edge. And that, for me, was tantalizing.

In justified text, the size of spaces between words is varied to make the lines come out even (like in this paragraph). The size of the spaces is irregular but not meaningful. The goal is just to make the lines come out even. But your brain registers that the spaces are different sizes. And it tries to sort out what that information conveys. Trying to interpret signals that are just noise slows you down and makes reading feel more effortful.

It may seem counter-intuitive that a small detail like where lines end would make text easier to read. If it’s true, why do publishers of books, journals, magazines and newspapers right-justify everything? It’s a question that a lot of us who think about reading think about, a lot.

Step away from the edge

Just because you can, doesn’t mean you should

Page 6: Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

Kindle2: Crack for readers... until you start reading

Newsletter February / March 2009 4

Our most charitable guess is that publishers think that justified text looks better. It does. If you like rectangles. But the research shows that people read ragged-right copy faster than right- justified copy (Hartley & Burnhill, 1971; Jandreau & Bever, 1992). So, to justify or not to justify depends on whether the goal is a prettier page or an easier read.

I vote for easier to read. And that is why I was enticed by the raggedy edged Kindle ad.

And the first publication I opened (the New Yorker) lived up to the promise: Hertzberg, in ragged right. With the cartoons thoughtfully aggregated into one section. Joy.

But the second one I opened (Nudge; Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth and Happiness), and the third, fourth, fifth, and sixth (White Tiger, a novel; I was told there’d be cake, essays; Technology Review; and the Wall Street Journal, respectively) all have right-justified text. Gain the buyer’s trust. Violate the buy-er’s trust.

To be fair, it may not be Amazon that is making the choice. But they could. And if any single group can make reading better, it is Amazon. Well, maybe Amazon and ReadSmart.

Actually, there’s more to enhancing readability than where lines end. Remember how the random size of spaces between words in right-justified text undermines reading? The reverse is also true. Bever and colleagues, linguists and psychologists at the University of Arizona, have shown that when line endings and space sizes offer clues to how words should be grouped, reading is faster and feels easier (Bever, Jandreau, Burwell, Kaplan & Zaenan, 1990; Jandreau & Bever, 1992; among many others.)

To show this, Bever and team engineered (and patented) a text processing / formatting algorithm (which they call ReadSmart) that “reads” text input and adjusts inter- and intra-word spacing based on psychologically tested, linguistic rules. The new, meaningful spaces guide readers’ eyes and help them to group the words correctly even as they read. “ReadSmarted” text is easier to read

One stop past the end of the line...

Page 7: Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

Kindle2: Crack for readers... until you start reading

Newsletter February / March 2009 5

because part of work of reading is already done for you. But unlike other text formatting algorithms, ReadSmart improves readability without changing the length of the text or the way that it looks on the surface.

Bever’s early studies of linguistic formatting (including more than 500 students in the U.S. and abroad) showed that when the spaces between / within words predict the structure, comprehension and reading speed increase up to 20%. Similar comprehension improvements have been documented for readers under duress and second- language readers.

More recent industry studies suggest that improving readability and comprehen-sion may also increase persuasiveness. The scenario supposedly goes something like this:

1. A potential donor receives solicitation letters. If they like the cause, they may open the letter.

2. If they open them, they tend to skim, for about as long as it takes to get from the front door / mail box to the kitchen / trash can.

3. Since Readsmarted text reads faster and more easily, potential donors get further into the message to the emotional hook that removes the block or amplifies their drive toward commitment.

4. Along the way, since the cognitive burden of “reading” is reduced by ReadSmarting text, readers can commit more mental energy to processing and remembering the message. Remembered messages are more persuasive.

Even if the scenario is not exactly right, the effect of formatting text to improve readability is profound. Direct mail donor acquisition letters that are formatted by ReadSmart work better. In a meta-analysis of 5 direct marketing campaigns reaching 393,000 households (over multiple charities), the formatted letters triggered 22% more responses than donor letters with standard text. And, people who responded to a formatted solicitation ultimately donated more (48% more on average)1.

It’s much more persuasive if I read it

1 To be sure, the scale of the behavior change resulting from ReadSmarting text makes it feel a bit like a but-wait!-there’s more! paid-for-TV commercial. I’d be far more skep-tical, if I didn’t have direct knowledge of the psycholinguistic research behind text-format-ting generally and ReadSmart specifically. (Fair balance: Bever was my thesis advisor.)

Page 8: Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

Kindle2: Crack for readers... until you start reading

Newsletter February / March 2009 6

Ultimately, linguistically-informed text formatting algorithms like ReadSmart make text more persuasive by reducing the mental burden of reading. Readers understand better with less effort. By extension, organizations and agencies that apply these algorithms can benefit as well. And that promise makes ReadSmart tantalizing.

Sort of like the Kindle2 was. And could be again. If Amazon made “ReadSmart my book” the default menu item. Until then, I’m going back to paperbacks.

[The ReadSmart text formatting algorithm improves readability on paper, in fixed-width websites, and on mobile phones. To learn more go to www.readsmart.com.]

Help us make the newsletter more relevant to you... and win an iPod shuffle... that talks to you!

Please take 4 quick minutes to answer our 10 question survey about the news-letter. Tell us what you like/don’t like about the newsletter. Which topics would you like to read about?

And, if you give us contact information, you will automatically be entered into our shuffle drawing. www.surveygizmo.com/s/114074/newsletter-march09The iPod shuffle drawing will be held on Friday, April 3, 2009.

We know that if users can’t find it, they can’t be persuaded by it. But this goes a bit beyond. It shows that if the user must work hard to get to content (even in a small detail like text quality) it can make a material impact on persuasion. Usability is no longer enough, but it is absolutely needed.

More than meets the eye

It’s not just small talk!

The Pragmatic Ergonomist, Dr. Eric Schaffer

Page 9: Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

Kindle2: Crack for readers... until you start reading

Newsletter February / March 2009 7

Anglin, J. M., & Miller, G. A. (1968). The role of phrase structure in the recall of meaningful verbal material. Psychonomic Science, 10, 343–344.

Bever, T. G., & Robbart, J. (2008). System and method of determining phrasing in text. U.S. Patent No. 7,346,489. Washington, DC: U. S. Patent and Trademark Office.

Bever, T. G., & Robbart, J. (2008). System and method for formatting text according to phrasing. Patent pending. Washington, DC: U. S. Patent and Trademark Office.

Bever, T. G., & Robbart, J. (2006). System and method for formatting text according to linguistic, visual and psychological variables. U.S. Patent No. 7,069,508. Washington, DC: U. S. Patent and Trademark Office.

Bever, T. G., Nicholas, C. D., Hancock, R., Alcock, K. W., & Jandreau, S. M. (2007). System, plug-in and method for improving text composition by modify-ing character prominence according to assigned character information mea-sures. Patent pending. Washington, DC: U. S. Patent and Trademark Office.

Bever, T. G., Jandreau, S., Burwell, R. , Kaplan, R., & Zaenan, A. (1990). Spacing printed text to isolate major phrases improves readability. Visible Language, 25, 74–87.

Coleman, E. B., & Kim, I. (1961). Comparison of several styles of typography in English. Journal of Applied Psychology, 45, 262–267.

Hartley, J. (1980). Spatial cues in text. Visible Language, 14, 67–79.

Hartley, J., & Burnhill, P. (1971). Experiments with unjustified text. Visible Language, 5, 265–278.

Jandreau, S., & Bever, T. G. (1992). Phrase-spaced formats improve comprehen-sion in average readers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 143–146.

Nicholas, C. D., Maher, J. Ashley, K. L., Berendt, L. H. (2009). System and meth-od for converting the digital typesetting documents used in publishing to a device-specific format for electronic publishing. Patent pending. Washington, DC: U. S. Patent and Trademark Office.

References

Page 10: Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

Kindle2: Crack for readers... until you start reading

Newsletter February / March 2009 8

Klare, G. R., Nichols, W. H., & Shufford, E. H. (1957). The relationship of typo-graphic arrangement to the learning of technical material. Journal of Applied Psychology, 41, 41–45.

Mason, J. M., & Kendall, J. R. (1979). Facilitating reading comprehension through text structure manipulation. The Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 25, 68–76.

North, A. J., & Jenkins, L. B. (1951). Reading speed and comprehension as a function of typography. Journal of Applied Psychology, 35, 225–228.

Page 11: Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

Kindle2: Crack for readers... until you start reading

Newsletter February / March 2009 9

Kath Straub guides organizations and agencies to design communication and interactions systems that inform, educate, and shape key behaviors. To that end, she integrates multi-disciplinary research methods to uncover the psychological and emotional drives and barriers that inform human attitudes, decisions, and behaviors. Most recently, Kath’s interests have shifted to under-standing how emerging communication methods (e.g., microblogging) and tech-nologies (e.g., mobile) augment the existing information ecosystem and evolve consumer/citizen expectations and behavior. She applies this knowledge to help organizations develop outreach strategies, and to proactively assimilate and draw upon the new channels and changing behaviors. Electronic channels are replacing the “picket fences” we used to gossip over. Is your organization ready?

Kath Straub is the Chief Scientist and Executive Director of Human Factors International. She holds a Ph.D. in Brain and Cognitive Science from the University of Rochester (NY). She has been recognized by Federal Computer Week Magazine as a Rising Star in Government.

About the author Kath Straub, PhD, CUA Chief ScientistHuman Factors International

Page 12: Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

410 West Lowe, P.O. Box 2020Fairfield, IA 52556Phone: 800.242.4480Fax: [email protected]

© 2009 Human Factors International, Inc.

Human FactorsI n te rna t i ona l

Page 13: Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

Kindle2: Crack for readers... until you start readingHow text formatting can ruin (or enhance) the readability and persuasiveness of text

Kath Straub, PhD, CUA / Chief ScientistHuman Factors International, Inc.

February / March, 2009

Human FactorsI n te r n a t i o n a l

Newsletter

Page 14: Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

Kindle2: Crack for readers... until you start reading

Newsletter February / March 2009 2

Maybe it was the early adopter thing. Curiosity about new gadgets. Wanting totouch. To play. To decide if that new little thing will be the next big thing.

Maybe it was the road-warrior reader thing: I travel a lot. And I read a lot. Which bookshould I take?

Maybe it was the attention deficit thing.Reading one book at a time gets... boring.And all those books get heavy.

However you explain it, I own a Kindle2.

It’s been an interesting ride. I’m ready to getoff the bike.

To be sure, part of the fun of Kindle is that Amazon had to balance a lot ofdesign options. For instance, they decided to tried to create an unpacking-the-product-should-be-emotional experience. For an Apple native, that was a bitweird. Somehow, the pull-off paper zipper (think FedEX envelopes) sets thewrong tone. But, they tried.

Out of the box, my first impression was positive. It’s smaller than I thought. Thetext resolution is better than I had hoped.

But then, there is no backlight. That means the battery lasts a really long time.But it also means that the screen is surprisingly grey and the text contrast islow. And you still need a night light to read.

The keyboard lets you annotate while you read. But it’s awkward. Big. Thatchoice seemed odd since the bigger keyboard meant a smaller reading screen.We are all trained to type on phone-sized keyboards by now, aren’t we?

Navigating isn’t bad. The fact that the Menu button takes you to Shop At theKindle Store is irritating. Even if I understand why it’s so. The joystick offers afew surprises, like you can’t turn pages with it.

Interesting. In the Chinese sense.

You say red herring. I say red herrings.

Page 15: Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

Kindle2: Crack for readers... until you start reading

Newsletter February / March 2009 3

But these problems are red herring(s). Kindle isn’t really about unpacking andnavigating. It’s about reading. Unfortunately, the reading part is where Amazongoes wrong.

It was a small detail in the Kindle ad that tipped my decision to try it. Unlessyou’ve spent years thinking about the psychology of reading, you probablywon’t notice it.

The text on the right side of the "page" is not right-justified. It has a ragged-right edge. And that, for me, was tantalizing.

In justified text, the size of spaces between words is varied to make the linescome out even (like in this paragraph). The size of the spaces is irregular but notmeaningful. The goal is just to make the lines come out even. But your brain reg-isters that the spaces are different sizes. And it tries to sort out what that infor-mation conveys. Trying to interpret signals that are just noise slows you downand makes reading feel more effortful.

It may seem counter-intuitive that a small detail like where lines end wouldmake text easier to read. If it’s true, why do publishers of books, journals, magazines and newspapers right-justify everything? It’s a question that a lot ofus who think about reading think about, a lot.

Step away from the edge.

Just because you can, doesn’t mean you should.

Page 16: Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

Kindle2: Crack for readers... until you start reading

Newsletter February / March 2009 4

Our most charitable guess is that publishers think that justifiedtext looks better. It does. If you like rectangles. But the researchshows that people read ragged-right copy faster than right-justi-fied copy (Hartley & Burnhill, 1971; Jandreau & Bever, 1992). So,to justify or not to justify depends on whether the goal is a pret-tier page or an easier read.

I vote for easier to read. And that is why I was enticed by theraggedy edged Kindle ad.

And the first publication I opened (the New Yorker) lived up to the promise:Hertzberg, in ragged right. With the cartoons thoughtfully aggregated into one section. Joy.

But the second one I opened (Nudge; Improving Decisions about Health, Wealthand Happiness), and the third, fourth, fifth, and sixth (White Tiger, a novel; Iwas told there’d be cake, essays; Technology Review; and the Wall StreetJournal, respectively) all have right-justified text. Gain the buyer’s trust. Violatethe buyer’s trust.

To be fair, it may not be Amazon that is making the choice. But they could. Andif any single group can make reading better, it is Amazon. Well, maybe Amazonand ReadSmart.

Actually, there’s more to enhancing readability than where lines end.Remember how the random size of spaces between words in right-justified textundermines reading? The reverse is also true. Bever and colleagues, linguistsand psychologists at the University of Arizona, have shown that when line end-ings and space sizes offer clues to how words should be grouped, reading isfaster and feels easier (Bever, Jandreau, Burwell, Kaplan & Zaenan, 1990;Jandreau & Bever, 1992; among many others.)

To show this, Bever and team engineered (and patented) a text processing / formatting algorithm (which they call ReadSmart) that "reads" text input andadjusts inter- and intra-word spacing based on psychologically tested, linguisticrules. The new, meaningful spaces guide readers’ eyes and help them to groupthe words correctly even as they read. "ReadSmarted" text is easier to read

One stop past the end of the line...

Page 17: Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

Kindle2: Crack for readers... until you start reading

Newsletter February / March 2009 5

because part of work of reading is already done for you. But unlike other textformatting algorithms, ReadSmart improves readability without changing thelength of the text or the way that it looks on the surface.

Bever’s early studies of linguistic formatting (including more than 500 studentsin the U.S. and abroad) showed that when the spaces between / within wordspredict the structure, comprehension and reading speed increase up to 20%.Similar comprehension improvements have been documented for readersunder duress and second- language readers.

More recent industry studies suggest that improving readability and compre-hension may also increase persuasiveness. The scenario supposedly goessomething like this:

1. A potential donor receives solicitation letters. If they like the cause, theymay open the letter.

2. If they open them, they tend to skim, for about as long as it takes to getfrom the front door / mail box to the kitchen / trash can.

3. Since Readsmarted text reads faster and more easily, potential donors get further into the message to the emotional hook that removes the block or amplifies their drive toward commitment.

4. Along the way, since the cognitive burden of "reading" is reduced byReadSmarting text, readers can commit more mental energy to processing and remembering the message. Remembered messages are more persuasive.

Even if the scenario is not exactly right, the effect of formatting text to improvereadability is profound. Direct mail donor acquisition letters that are formattedby ReadSmart work better. In a meta-analysis of 5 direct marketing campaignsreaching 393,000 households (over multiple charities), the formatted letterstriggered 22% more responses than donor letters with standard text. And, peo-ple who responded to a formatted solicitation ultimately donated more (48%more on average)1.

It’s much more persuasive if I read it.

1 To be sure, the scale of the behavior change resulting from ReadSmarting text makesit feel a bit like a but-wait!-there’s more! paid-for-TV commercial. I’d be far more skepti-cal, if I didn’t have direct knowledge of the psycholinguistic research behind text-format-ting generally and ReadSmart specifically. (Fair balance: Bever was my thesis advisor.)

Page 18: Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

Kindle2: Crack for readers... until you start reading

Newsletter February / March 2009 6

Ultimately, linguistically-informed text formatting algorithms like ReadSmartmake text more persuasive by reducing the mental burden of reading. Readersunderstand better with less effort. By extension, organizations and agenciesthat apply these algorithms can benefit as well. And that promise makesReadSmart tantalizing.

Sort of like the Kindle2 was. And could be again. If Amazon made "ReadSmartmy book" the default menu item. Until then, I’m going back to paperbacks.

[The ReadSmart text formatting algorithm improves readability on paper, in fixed-width websites, and on mobile phones. To learn more go to www.readsmart.com.]

Anglin, J. M., & Miller, G. A. (1968). The role of phrase structure in the recall ofmeaningful verbal material. Psychonomic Science, 10, 343–344.

Bever, T. G., & Robbart, J. (2008). System and method of determining phrasingin text. U.S. Patent No. 7,346,489. Washington, DC: U. S. Patent and TrademarkOffice.

Bever, T. G., & Robbart, J. (2008). System and method for formatting textaccording to phrasing. Patent pending. Washington, DC: U. S. Patent andTrademark Office.

Bever, T. G., & Robbart, J. (2006). System and method for formatting textaccording to linguistic, visual and psychological variables. U.S. Patent No.7,069,508. Washington, DC: U. S. Patent and Trademark Office.

Bever, T. G., Nicholas, C. D., Hancock, R., Alcock, K. W., & Jandreau, S. M. (2007).System, plug-in and method for improving text composition by modifying char-acter prominence according to assigned character information measures.Patent pending. Washington, DC: U. S. Patent and Trademark Office.

Bever, T. G., Jandreau, S., Burwell, R. , Kaplan, R., & Zaenan, A. (1990). Spacingprinted text to isolate major phrases improves readability. Visible Language, 25,74–87.

More than meets the eye.

References

Page 19: Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

Kindle2: Crack for readers... until you start reading

Newsletter February / March 2009 7

Coleman, E. B., & Kim, I. (1961). Comparison of several styles of typography inEnglish. Journal of Applied Psychology, 45, 262–267.

Hartley, J. (1980). Spatial cues in text. Visible Language, 14, 67–79.

Hartley, J., & Burnhill, P. (1971). Experiments with unjustified text. VisibleLanguage, 5, 265–278.

Jandreau, S., & Bever, T. G. (1992). Phrase-spaced formats improve comprehen-sion in average readers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 143–146.

Nicholas, C. D., Maher, J. Ashley, K. L., Berendt, L. H. (2009). System andmethod for converting the digital typesetting documents used in publishing to adevice-specific format for electronic publishing. Patent pending. Washington,DC: U. S. Patent and Trademark Office.

Klare, G. R., Nichols, W. H., & Shufford, E. H. (1957). The relationship of typo-graphic arrangement to the learning of technical material. Journal of AppliedPsychology, 41, 41–45.

Mason, J. M., & Kendall, J. R. (1979). Facilitating reading comprehension throughtext structure manipulation. The Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 25,68–76.

North, A. J., & Jenkins, L. B. (1951). Reading speed and comprehension as afunction of typography. Journal of Applied Psychology, 35, 225–228.

Page 20: Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

Kindle2: Crack for readers... until you start reading

Newsletter February / March 2009 8

Kath Straub guides organizations and agencies to design communication and interactions systems that inform, educate, and shape key behaviors. To thatend, she integrates multi-disciplinary research methods to uncover the psycho-logical and emotional drives and barriers that inform human attitudes, deci-sions, and behaviors. Most recently, Kath’s interests have shifted to understand-ing how emerging communication methods (e.g., microblogging) and technolo-gies (e.g., mobile) augment the existing information ecosystem and evolve con-sumer/ citizen expectations and behavior. She applies this knowledge to helporganizations develop outreach strategies, and to proactively assimilate anddraw upon the new channels and changing behaviors. Electronic channels arereplacing the “picket fences” we used to gossip over. Is your organizationready?

Kath Straub is the Chief Scientist and Executive Director of Human FactorsInternational. She holds a Ph.D. in Brain and Cognitive Science from theUniversity of Rochester (NY). She has been recognized by Federal ComputerWeek Magazine as a Rising Star in Government.

About the author Kath Straub, PhD, CUAChief ScientistHuman Factors International

Page 21: Straub Read Smart Findings Plus Example Text

410 West Lowe, P.O. Box 2020Fairfield, IA 52556Phone: 800.242.4480Fax: [email protected]

© 2008 Human Factors International, Inc.

Human FactorsI n te r n a t i o n a l