Social Influence and Information Sharing

download Social Influence and Information Sharing

of 22

Transcript of Social Influence and Information Sharing

  • 8/13/2019 Social Influence and Information Sharing

    1/22

    10.1177/1046496403251933

    ARTICLESMALL GROUP RESEARCH / August 2003Henningsen, Henningsen / NORMATIVE AND INFORMATIONAL INFLUENCE

    EXAMINING SOCIAL INFLUENCEIN INFORMATION-SHARING

    CONTEXTS

    DAVID DRYDEN HENNINGSEN

    MARY LYNN MILLER HENNINGSEN

    Northern Illinois University

    Research on hidden profiles has found that groups tend to make decisions reflecting the

    informationsharedamonggroupmembers prior to discussion. However, shared information

    is confounded with individual prediscussion preferences in hidden profile situations. In this

    article,the authors manipulate information distributionto examinehidden,ambiguous (i.e.,

    prediscussion preferences favor no alternative), and clear (i.e., prediscussion preferences

    support the decision alternative favored by the total information) profiles. They examinethe

    influence of profile type on group decisions, use of shared and unshared information, and

    perceptions of normative and informational influence. Results indicate that groups make

    superior decisions and rely more on unshared information in clear and ambiguous profiles

    than in hidden profiles. In addition, perceived social influence is most prevalent in ambigu-

    ous profiles.

    Keywords:normative influence; informational influence; information sharing; hidden pro-

    file; group decision making

    There is a growing body of research focusing on theimportance of

    information exchange in decision-making groups (Brodbeck,

    Kerschreiter, Mojzisch, Frey, & Schultz-Hardt, 2002; Cruz, Boster,

    & Rodriguez, 1997; Cruz, Henningsen, & Smith, 1999; Dennis,

    1996; Gigone & Hastie, 1993, 1997; Gruenfeld, Mannix, Williams,

    & Neale, 1996; Hollingshead, 1996a, 1996b; Kameda, Ohtsubo, &

    Takezawa, 1997; Larson, Christensen, Abbott, & Franz, 1996;

    391

    AUTHORS NOTE: Address correspondence concerning this article to David Dryden

    Henningsen, Department of Communication, Northern Illinois University, Dekalb, IL

    60115.

    SMALL GROUP RESEARCH, Vol. 34 No. 4, August 2003 391-412

    DOI: 10.1177/1046496403251933

    2003 Sage Publications

  • 8/13/2019 Social Influence and Information Sharing

    2/22

    Larson, Christensen, Franz, & Abbott, 1998; Larson, Foster-

    Fishman, & Granz, 1998; Larson, Foster-Fishman, & Keys, 1994;

    Lavery, Franz, Winquist, & Larson, 1999; Mennecke, 1997;

    Postmes, Spears, & Cihangir, 2001; Propp, 1997; Savadori, Van

    Swol, & Sniezek, 2001; Schittekatte, 1996; Schittekatte & Van

    Hiel, 1996; Shaw & Penrod, 1962; Stasser & Stewart, 1992;

    Stasser, Stewart, & Wittenbaum, 1995; Stasser, Taylor, & Hanna,

    1989; Stasser & Titus, 1985, 1987; Stasser, Vaughn, & Stewart,

    2000; Stewart, 1998; Stewart, Billings, & Stasser, 1998; Stewart &

    Stasser, 1995, 1998; Van Hiel & Schittekatte, 1998; Winquist &

    Larson, 1998; Wittenbaum, 1998, 2000; Wittenbaum, Hubbell, &

    Zuckerman, 1999; Wittenbaum & Stasser, 1998; Wittenbaum,Stasser, & Merry, 1996). This research consistently has demon-

    strated thatshared information(i.e., information held by all group

    members) tends to be pooled in discussion more than unshared

    information(i.e., information held by only one group member).

    Overall, past research has indicated that shared information tends

    to have a greater influence on group decision making than does

    unshared information.

    An overreliance on shared information is problematic for groups

    when shared information favors a suboptimal decision alternative

    but the information as a whole favors the best decision alternative.

    Under these circumstances, relying on shared information at the

    expense of unshared information could lead to faultydecisionmak-ing. The negative influence over reliance on shared information can

    have on decision making is clearly demonstrated in research using

    hidden profiles (Stasser, 1988). In a hidden profile, the information

    held by each individual group member favors a suboptimal deci-

    sion alternative. However, the total information held by all group

    members favors a better decision. If group members pool all of the

    unshared information, the bestalternative should become apparent.

    Unfortunately, groups withhidden profiles select the suboptimal

    decision alternative more frequently than they select the best deci-

    sion alternative. In contrast, groups in which all information is ini-

    tially shared select the best alternative more than hidden profile

    groups (e.g., Cruz et al., 1997; Hollingshead, 1996b; Kelly & Karau,

    1999; Stasser & Titus, 1985). Stasser and Titus (1985) showed that

    392 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / August 2003

  • 8/13/2019 Social Influence and Information Sharing

    3/22

    group discussion decreased the percentage of group members mak-

    ing optimal decisions under hidden profile conditions. This indi-

    cates that social influence occurs in decision-making groups deal-

    ing with hidden profiles. In the present study, we will examine how

    social influence works under different information profiles.

    SOURCES OF INFLUENCE

    Researchers have identified two types of social influence in

    decision-making contexts (Deutsch & Gerard, 1955; Kaplan &

    Miller, 1987).Normative influenceoccurs when individuals make

    decisions to gain approval from other group members. Informa-tional influence, on the other hand, occurs when individuals make

    decisions to reach the best possible decision.

    Based on thesedefinitions, normative influence occurs in groups

    when discussion content presents the positions favored by other

    group members (Kaplan & Miller, 1987). If normative influence is

    effective, change their positions to the position favored by the

    majority in order to fit in with the group. In contrast, informational

    influence should occur when facts, evidence, or other forms of

    information pertinent to the decision are discussed by group mem-

    bers and cause them to reevaluate their positions (Kaplan & Miller,

    1987). Researchers have operationalized normative influence as

    the statements in discussion that indicate the positions supportedby other group members. Informational influence has been

    operationalized as statements revealing relevant facts or evidence

    (e.g., Kaplan & Miller, 1987; Kelly, Jackson, & Hutson-Comeaux,

    1997). In any decision-making group, both normative and informa-

    tional influence can occur. Next we examine how normative and

    informational influence may occur in hidden profiles.

    SOURCES OF NORMATIVE INFLUENCE

    In the context of hidden profiles, the initial starting positions

    adopted by group members represents a possible source of norma-

    tive influence. Statements that indicate the position favored by a

    majority of group members should provide normative pressure on

    Henningsen, Henningsen / NORMATIVE AND INFORMATIONAL INFLUENCE 393

  • 8/13/2019 Social Influence and Information Sharing

    4/22

    other group members to shift toward that position. Numerous stud-

    ies have indicated that prediscussion positions favored by the

    majority or a plurality have a strong influence on group decisions

    (e.g., Gigone & Hastie, 1993; Gruenfeld et al., 1996; Kameda et al.,

    1997; Larson et al., 1994; Stasser & Titus, 1985, 1987; Stewart

    et al., 1998; Winquist& Larson, 1998).Basedon previous research,

    we argue that normative influence will be strongest when a clear

    consensus exists among group members.

    We argue that normative influence will be stronger in consensus

    groups than in groups without initial consensus. In essence, when

    there is an initial consensus, group members should face strong

    normative pressure to maintain the group harmony by not disagree-ing with the clear group preference. Very little group discussion

    may be necessary for group members to feel this normative pres-

    sure. Evidence of normative pressure in groups with initial consen-

    sus is evidenced by the fact that such groups are less likely to

    change to the position most favored by the information (e.g.,

    Hollingshead, 1996b). In addition to the prevalence of normative

    influence in hidden profiles, informational influence should also

    occur when group members rely on decision-relevant information.

    In the next section, we examine informational influence in hidden

    profiles.

    SOURCES OF INFORMATIONAL INFLUENCE

    Unshared information raised during discussion is a clear source

    of informational influence. Unshared information represents new

    facts or evidence for most of the group members that may be rele-

    vant to the group decision. If the information is deemed accurate

    and relevant, it has the potential to cause group members to reevalu-

    ate their positions to account for the new information. A variety of

    studies have found that the presence of unshared information in the

    group discussion can increase group decision-making accuracy

    (Gigone & Hastie, 1997; Kelly et al., 1997; Larson et al., 1998;

    Winquist & Larson, 1998).

    In addition to unshared information, shared information in dis-

    cussion should also exert informational influence. Mayer, Sonoda,

    394 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / August 2003

  • 8/13/2019 Social Influence and Information Sharing

    5/22

    and Gudykunst (1997) found that groups with more decision-rele-

    vant information made better decisions than groups with less infor-

    mation in a context where all information was shared. This indi-

    cates that, like unshared information, shared information can serve

    as a source of informational influence. Consistent with past

    operationalizations of informational influence (e.g., Kaplan &

    Miller, 1987), raising an item of shared information in discussion

    creates the potential for informational influence. When informa-

    tion is shared prior to discussion, it can be incorporated into each

    group members prediscussion decision preferences (Winquist &

    Larson, 1998). However, when a shared item is mentioned or repeated

    by another group member, that item has the potential to take ongreater weight in the decision-making process. When a group

    member mentions an item of shared information, it can verify for

    the other group members that they accurately recalled that item.

    Furthermore, it can indicate that other members also consider that

    item important and relevant to the decision-making process (see

    Wittenbaum et al., 1999). This verification process may increase

    the weight that group members assignto shared items of information

    and cause the members to readjust their preferences accordingly.

    Evidence of shared information serving as a source of informa-

    tional influence is available in several studies. Recall that Stasser

    and Titus (1985) found that discussion decreased the percentage of

    correct decisions among group members in hidden profile condi-tions. They also found that group members recalled more shared

    than unshared information after discussion when dealing with hid-

    den profiles, indicating a preference for shared information in dis-

    cussion. Larson, Christensen, et al. (1998) found that repetition of

    shared information during discussion increased bias in decisions.

    These studies indicate that the discussion of shared information

    may have had an influence above and beyond the informations

    impact on group members initial preferences.

    We have discussed how shared starting positions can create nor-

    mative pressure in decision-making groups and how discussion of

    shared and unshared information can create informational pres-

    sure. In the next section, we will consider how sources of normative

    and informational influence are confounded in hidden profiles.

    Henningsen, Henningsen / NORMATIVE AND INFORMATIONAL INFLUENCE 395

  • 8/13/2019 Social Influence and Information Sharing

    6/22

    A CONFOUND IN HIDDEN PROFILES

    In hidden profiles, the initial starting positions adopted by group

    members and the information shared by group members are con-

    founded. Each individual starts out with more shared than unshared

    information. Shared and unshared information support different

    decision alternatives, with shared information always favoring a

    suboptimal decision alternative. Thus, both shared information and

    the balance of prediscussion information support the inferior deci-

    sion alternative. This sets up a situationin which it is difficult to dis-

    tinguish the impact of shared information from the impact of initial

    starting positions.

    In addition, both informational and normative sources of influ-

    ence favor the inferior decision alternative in hidden profiles,

    whereas only the potential informational influence in the form of

    unshared information favors the best alternative. In this study, we

    attempt to extend hidden profile research by examining situations

    in which the confound between shared information and initial start-

    ing positions are reduced and reversed. We propose to do this while

    maintaining a key aspect of hidden profiles. Specifically, in this

    study, overall shared information and unshared information always

    favor different alternatives.

    In contrast to a hidden profile, an ambiguous profileis created

    when information distribution does not create an initial preference

    for either the best or the suboptimal decision alternative. In an

    ambiguous profile, each group member starts with the same num-

    ber of items favoring high- and low-quality decision alternatives.

    The balance of shared information favors the less desirable deci-

    sion alternative, and the balance of unshared information favors the

    more desirable decision alternative. Therefore, in an ambiguous

    profile, initial starting positions should be no more likely to favor

    the position supported by shared information than the position sup-

    ported by unshared information.

    In a clear profile, the initial information distribution creates a

    preference for the superior decision alternative. Opposite to the

    classic hidden profile, in a clear profile, each group member startswith more information favoring the superior than the inferior deci-

    sion alternative. Consistent with hidden and ambiguous profiles,

    396 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / August 2003

  • 8/13/2019 Social Influence and Information Sharing

    7/22

    the balance of shared information favors the inferior decision alter-

    native and the balance of unshared information favors the superior

    decision alternative in clear profiles. In the next sections, we pres-

    entourhypothesesconcerning hidden, ambiguous, andclear profiles.

    GROUP DECISION HYPOTHESIS

    In conjunction with past research, we predict that under hidden

    profile conditions, groups will choose the inferior decision alterna-

    tive more frequently than the superior decision alternative (e.g.,

    Cruz et al., 1997; Hollingshead, 1996b; Stasser & Titus, 1985).

    This reflects the fact that both initial starting positions and sharedinformation favor the incorrect decision, whereas only unshared

    information favors the correct decision.

    With ambiguous profiles, we anticipate that groups will be more

    likely to select the best decision alternative than under hidden pro-

    file conditions. Here, shared information favors the poor decision

    alternative and unshared information favors the best decision alter-

    native. Initial starting position is neutral, thus reducing its potential

    to influence decisions one way or the other.

    In clear profile conditions, unshared information and initial

    starting positions favor the best decision alternative. This reverses

    the bias created in hidden profile conditions. Under these circum-

    stances, we would expect groups to make the correct decision morefrequently than in theambiguous or thehidden profile conditions.

    Hypothesis 1: Groups with clear profiles will be more likely toselect the best alternative than groups with ambiguous profiles;groupswith ambiguousprofileswill be more likelyto selectthe bestalternative than groups with hidden profiles.

    NORMATIVE AND INFORMATIONAL INFLUENCE HYPOTHESES

    The stronger the initial preferences are for a given position, the

    more that initial starting positions will influence group decisions.

    There are many examples of research indicating that the initialstarting positions of group members significantly predict group

    decisions (e.g., Gigone & Hastie, 1993; Gruenfeld et al., 1996;

    Henningsen, Henningsen / NORMATIVE AND INFORMATIONAL INFLUENCE 397

  • 8/13/2019 Social Influence and Information Sharing

    8/22

    Kameda et al., 1997; Stasser & Titus, 1985, 1987; Stewart et al.,

    1998). Social influence generated by the shared initial starting

    positions of group members should take the form of normative

    influence. The nature of hidden and clear profiles should lead

    groups to more prediscussion consensus than will occur in ambigu-

    ous profiles. Thus, we feel that normative influence will be more

    prevalent in hidden and clear profiles than in ambiguous profiles.

    In contrast, when there is less initial support for a specific deci-

    sion alternative, the use of normative influence should be reduced,

    creating an opportunity for group members to use informational

    influence. Some support for this contention is provided by research

    that shows increased use of information by decision-makinggroups with conflicting starting positions (Brodbeck et al., 2002;

    Propp, 1997; but see also Gigone & Hastie, 1993; Stasser & Titus,

    1985). Overall, we predict that groups will rely more on informa-

    tional influence under ambiguous profiles than under clear or hid-

    den profiles. Thus, we propose the following hypotheses:

    Hypothesis 2:Group members will perceive more normative influ-ence in hidden and clear profile conditions than in ambiguous pro-file conditions.

    Hypothesis 3: Group members will perceive more informationalinfluence in ambiguous profile conditions than in hidden and clear

    profile conditions.

    Various studies have shown that shared information is discussed

    more than unshared information during group discussions of hid-

    den profiles (e.g., Cruz et al., 1997; Hollingshead, 1996b; Savadori

    et al., 2001). As noted previously, in hidden profiles, shared infor-

    mation and initial starting positions favor the suboptimal decision

    alternative. When a clear profile exists, the advantage of shared ini-

    tial starting position should augment the position favored by

    unshared rather than shared information. Dennis (1996) found that

    groups were more likely to discuss information supporting their

    initial positionsthan information that was negative or neutral to that

    position (see also Schittekatte, 1996). Kelly and Karau (1999)found that group members were more likely to discuss information

    that favored the best decision alternative if they initially preferred

    398 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / August 2003

  • 8/13/2019 Social Influence and Information Sharing

    9/22

    that alternative. These studies indicate that initial preferences tend

    to promote discussion of items supporting those preferences. In

    clear profiles, that should lead to increased discussion of the

    unshared information.

    We have discussed how initial preferences lead to increased dis-

    cussion of preference-consistent information. In addition, Boster

    and Mayer (1984) found that arguments presented supporting the

    majority position were considered to be stronger than arguments

    offered supporting the minority position. Initial preferences seem

    to add weightto shared information in hiddenprofile conditions but

    should augment unshared information in clear profile conditions.

    Thus, we propose our 4th hypothesis:

    Hypothesis 4:Groups will favor shared information over unsharedinformation in hidden profile conditions, will equally favor sharedand unshared information under ambiguous profile conditions, andwill favor unshared information over shared information in clear

    profile conditions.

    Finally, because we are interested in social influence, we want to

    determine whether perceptions of normative and informational

    influence are accurate predictors of actual social influence in

    groups. To that end, we propose to examine how group members

    perceptions of normative and informational influence predict the

    actual social influence that occurs during group decision making.

    We propose the following hypothesis to examine this issue:

    Hypothesis 5: Perceptions of normative and informational influ-ence will be positive predictors of shiftsin group membersdecision

    preferences from pre- to postdiscussion.

    METHOD

    PARTICIPANTS

    One hundred eleven undergraduates at a large midwestern uni-versity (63 females, 43 males, 5 unreported) were randomly assigned

    Henningsen, Henningsen / NORMATIVE AND INFORMATIONAL INFLUENCE 399

  • 8/13/2019 Social Influence and Information Sharing

    10/22

    to three-person groups (N= 37) to take part in the study. Partici-

    pants were volunteers who received extra course credit for their

    participation.

    DESIGN

    The design of the experiment was a three-group, one-way inde-

    pendent groups design with random assignment to experimental

    condition. The independent variable was the type of profile to

    which groups were assigned: hidden profile, ambiguous profile, or

    clear profile.

    DECISION MATERIALS AND MEASURES

    Decision materials from Cruz et al. (1997) were modified for use

    in this study. The materials included the descriptions of two job

    applicants for the position of assistant professor. Each description

    focused on 14 characteristicsof each professor (the same character-

    istics for each). A criterion expressing the desired qualifications on

    each of 14 items for the job applicant also was provided. Each item

    of information either satisfied a criterion for a preferred candidate

    (i.e., supporting information) or failed to satisfy a criterion for the

    preferred candidate (i.e., opposing information). One additional

    item was added to the Cruz et al. materials to accommodate three-person groups.

    Each group member received both shared and unshared infor-

    mation representing a proportion of the total information about

    each candidate. For each condition, the balance of shared informa-

    tion favored the inferior decision alternative and the balance of

    unshared information favored the superior decision alternative.

    The information was distributed so that initial starting positions

    favored (a) the suboptimal alternative in the hidden profile condi-

    tion, (b) neither the optimal nor the suboptimal alternative in the

    ambiguous profile condition, or (c) the optimal alternative in the

    clear profile condition.

    The amount of shared information in the information sets varied

    slightly across conditions. We varied shared information so that we

    400 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / August 2003

  • 8/13/2019 Social Influence and Information Sharing

    11/22

    would not have to add items or subtract items to/from any of the

    conditions. In essence, to modify the number of unshared items of

    information distributed to each group member, a net change of

    three items of information was needed, whereas changing shared

    information allowed for a change of only one item. Shared items

    that were changed were randomly selected. The distribution of

    shared and unshared items supporting or opposing either candidate

    is presented in Table 1.

    To measure social influence, sixteen 7-point, Likert-type items,

    derived from Henningsen, Henningsen, Cruz, and Morrill (2002),

    were used to measure perceived normative influence (e.g., People

    used their opinions to change other peoples minds, My behaviorwasguided by a desire for group harmony) with= .71. Sixteen 7-

    point, Likert-type items derived from Henningsen et al. were also

    used to assess perceived informational influence (e.g., I thought

    the group members used information to sway my decisions, Peo-

    ple used their information to change other peoples minds) with

    = .72.

    PROCEDURES

    After arriving at the laboratory, participants were randomly

    assigned to three-person groups and to one of the three experimen-

    tal conditions. Individuals were seated separately and were eachprovided with a packet of information. The packet described a

    hypothetical job search in which the participants were instructed to

    role-play the part of a member of the search committee. A list of 14

    criteria was explained in the packet. In addition, each person

    received a description of two job candidates, including information

    about how each candidate fit some of the 14 established criteria for

    the position. Each group member received five items about each

    candidate that was shared with all other group members and three

    items that were held uniquely in the members group. After reading

    the information contained in the packet, each group member indi-

    vidually recorded his or her initial decision preference. Individuals

    were then seated with their group members and instructed to dis-

    cuss the decision task until they arrived at a consensus decision.

    Henningsen, Henningsen / NORMATIVE AND INFORMATIONAL INFLUENCE 401

  • 8/13/2019 Social Influence and Information Sharing

    12/22

    Following recording of the group decision, group members were

    asked to list the items of information they felt were most important

    to the decision that they made. Then, group members were again

    separated, recorded their individual preferences, and filled out the

    normative and informational influence questionnaires. On com-

    pleting the questionnaires, participants were debriefed and

    released.

    RESULTS

    MANIPULATION CHECKS

    To ensure that the profile manipulation was successful, a series

    of2 tests were performed using individual prediscussion prefer-

    402 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / August 2003

    TABLE 1: Shared and Unshared Information Favoringthe Optimal and Suboptimal

    Decision Alternatives

    Applicant Shared Information Unshared Information

    Optimal alternative

    Hidden profile

    Items supporting 0 9

    Items opposing 5 0

    Ambiguous profile

    Items supporting 1 9

    Items opposing 4 0

    Clear profile

    Items supporting 2 9

    Items opposing 3 0

    Suboptimal alternativeHidden profile

    Items supporting 5 0

    Items opposing 0 9

    Ambiguous profile

    Items supporting 4 0

    Items opposing 1 9

    Clear profile

    Items supporting 3 0

    Items opposing 2 9

    NOTE: All shared items are held by all group members. Unshared items are divided equallyamong group members.

  • 8/13/2019 Social Influence and Information Sharing

    13/22

    ences. Under hidden profiles, we expected group members to pre-

    fer the inferior decision alternative. For the hidden profile condi-

    tion,2 (1) = 11.11, p < .01. Of 36 individuals provided with thehidden profile, 28 chose the inferior decision alternative, indicating

    that the hidden profile successfully biased initial preferences in

    favor of the suboptimal decision alternative.

    We expected individuals under ambiguous profiles to show no

    clear preference for the superior or the inferior decision alternative.

    For the ambiguous profile conditions,2 (1) = 2.08,p= .11. Of 39individuals provided with the ambiguous profile, 15 selected the

    best decision alternative and 24 selected the inferior decision alter-

    native. The finding that this difference is not significant supportsour contention that the ambiguous profile condition didnot provide

    a clear bias for either candidate.

    Finally, we anticipated that groups under clear profile conditions

    would initially favor the best decision alternative over the lesser

    decision alternative. For the clear profile conditions,2 (1) = 7.11,p< .01. Of 36 individuals with a clear profile, 26 selected the best

    decision alternative and only 10 selected the lesser decision alter-

    native. Again,this supports our contention that the clear profile cre-

    ated a bias favoring the best decision alternative.

    TESTS OF HYPOTHESES

    In Hypothesis 1, we predicted that groups would be most likely

    to make thecorrect decision in theclear profile groups, less likelyto

    make the correct decision in the ambiguous profile groups, and

    least likelyto make thecorrect decision in hiddenprofile groups. To

    test these predictions, we used a one-way ANOVA design using

    type of profile (hidden, ambiguous, or clear) as the independent

    variable and group decision (correct decision = 1; incorrect deci-

    sion = 2) as the dependent variable. To test the hypothesis, we

    examined a planned contrast model that tested the linear contrast

    (see Table 2 for means and standard deviations). We conducted a

    contrast analysis because it allowed us to test the specific pattern of

    means predictedin Hypothesis 1. A significant contrast model indi-

    cates that our hypothesis did accurately predict our findings. A

    Henningsen, Henningsen / NORMATIVE AND INFORMATIONAL INFLUENCE 403

  • 8/13/2019 Social Influence and Information Sharing

    14/22

    nonsignificant residual variance indicates that we did not overlook

    any nonhypothesized significant effects.

    The linear contrast model was significant,F(1, 37) = 15.02,p .10, 2 < .01, was significant.Therefore, we conclude that Hypothesis 2 is not supported.

    In Hypothesis 3, we predicted that group members would per-ceive more informational influence in the ambiguousprofile condi-

    tion than in the hidden or clear profile conditions. A planned con-

    trast analysis of the hypothesized quadratic effect wasperformedto

    test this prediction (see Table 4 for means and standard deviations).

    The contrast model was significant,F(1, 37) = 3.94, p = .05,2 =.10, whereas the residual variance was not significant,F(1, 37) =

    .03,p > .10,2 < .01. Thus, Hypothesis 3 was supported. Groupmembers perceived more informational influence under ambigu-

    ous profiles than under either of the other conditions.

    We predicted in Hypothesis 4 that reliance on shared informa-

    tion would be highest in hidden profiles and would be reduced as

    groups moved to ambiguous and clear profiles. Conversely, we pre-

    dicted that reliance on unshared information would steadily

    404 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / August 2003

    TABLE 2: Means and Standard Deviations for Group Decision Contrast Model

    M SD n

    Hidden profile 1.92 .29 12

    Ambiguous profile 1.62 .51 13

    Clear profile 1.25 .50 12

  • 8/13/2019 Social Influence and Information Sharing

    15/22

    increase from hidden profiles to ambiguous profiles to clear pro-

    files. A planned contrast model was designed to test the linear

    effects proposed in Hypothesis 4 (see Table 5 for means and stan-

    dard deviations). To determine how much reliance groups placed

    on shared or unshared information, each group recorded the items

    of information they felt were most influential to their groups in

    makingthe decision.These items were coded as shared or unshared

    based on the distribution of information in each group. The number

    of items of shared and unshared information each group reported

    they relied on were used as a measure of that groups reliance on

    each type of information. Type of information (shared or unshared)

    is a within-group variable, and the decision condition (hidden,

    ambiguous, or clear profile) is a between-group variable. Overall,

    the contrast model was significant, F(1, 37) = 13.57,p< .01,2 =.26, and the residual was not significant,F(1, 37) = .09,p > .10,2 .10, thepartial regression coefficient for informational influence was posi-

    tive and significant, = .65,p< .01. This indicates that the socialinfluence that occurred in the group decisions tended to be per-

    ceived as more informational than normative and provides partial

    support for Hypothesis 5.

    406 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / August 2003

    TABLE 5: Means and Standard Deviations for Shared and Unshared Information

    Contrast Model

    M SD

    Shared information

    Hidden profile .50 .90

    Ambiguous profile 1.46 1.26

    Clear profile 1.83 1.83

    Unshared information

    Hidden profile 2.25 1.13

    Ambiguous profile 1.38 1.19

    Clear profile .66 .65

  • 8/13/2019 Social Influence and Information Sharing

    17/22

    DISCUSSION

    In this study, we began with the proposition that the effect of

    shared information and initial starting positions are confounded in

    hidden profiles because both favor a suboptimal decision alterna-

    tive. We proposed that groups would make significantly better deci-

    sions under ambiguous and clear profiles than under hidden pro-

    files. Overall, we found support for this position. Groups moved

    from inferior to superior decisions as they moved from hidden pro-

    files to clear profiles.

    We also predicted that normative influence would be more prev-

    alent in groups with hidden and clear profiles than in groups withambiguous profiles. This hypothesis was not supported. No signifi-

    cant differences existed across conditions for perceptions of nor-

    mative influence. In contrast, as hypothesized, significantly more

    informational influence was perceived in the ambiguous profile

    condition than in the hidden and clear profile conditions. This find-

    ing was consistent with our predictions.

    We also found support for our hypothesis that groups reliance

    on shared information was greatest in hidden profile conditions and

    lowest in clear profile conditions, while the reverse held true for

    reliance on unshared information. It appears that coupling shared

    starting positions with either shared or unshared information tends

    to increase the weight that information has in group decisions. Thisis consistent with Boster and Mayers (1984) finding that argu-

    ments from majorities are perceived as stronger than arguments

    coming from minority positions within groups. When the

    prediscussion majority favorsthe best decision, the group members

    are more likely to rely on information that supports that decision

    (i.e., unshared information). However, when the prediscussion

    majority favors the inferior decision, the group members are more

    likely to rely on the subset of information favoring the inferior deci-

    sion (i.e., shared information). Thus, the weight accorded to items

    of information is influenced by prediscussion support for the posi-

    tion that information supports.

    Perhaps the most intriguing result from this study is the finding

    that perceptions of informational influence are a better predictor of

    Henningsen, Henningsen / NORMATIVE AND INFORMATIONAL INFLUENCE 407

  • 8/13/2019 Social Influence and Information Sharing

    18/22

    overall influence than perceptions of normative influence. This

    seems counter to the findings of Cruz, Henningsen, and Williams

    (2000), in which normative statements had a greater influence than

    informational statements on individual decisions. One reason for

    this may be due to how Cruz et al. operationalized normative influ-

    ence. Cruz et al. used recastingstatements to manipulate normative

    influence. Recasting occurs when a group member either discounts

    (i.e., diminishes the importance) or bolsters (i.e., emphasizes the

    importance) an item of information. Thus, a person engages in

    recasting in a group when he or she discusses the importance (or

    lack thereof) of an item of information rather than simply mention-

    ing the item. In essence, recasting reflects how group members tryto influence how much weight an item of information should hold

    for the group decision.

    As Cruz et al. (2000) noted, recasting could serve as a source of

    normative or informational influence. By addressing the weight

    that should be given a piece of information, a group member indi-

    cates the position he or she supports and thus may apply normative

    influence. However, inferences drawn about information are indi-

    cated when information is recast. For instance, Cruz et al. (1997)

    offered the following example of discounting. A group member

    argues that the high teaching ratings of a job candidate are more an

    indicator that the candidate was an easy grader than an indicator

    that the candidate was a good teacher. In this example, anothergroup member may recognize the speaker opposed a specific can-

    didate (the candidate with better evaluations). However, the other

    group members also may consider the claim that good teaching rat-

    ings are the result of easy grading as information relevant to the

    decision. Thus, recastingmay be perceivedas informational or nor-

    mative influence.

    It may also be that normative influence was reduced in the pres-

    ent study because we used three-member groups. Asch (1955)

    found a substantial increase in social influence for individuals fac-

    ing a 3-to-1 majority compared to a 2-to-1 majority. It is possible

    that in using smaller groups, we reduced the potential for group

    members to successfully exert normative influence. In Cruz et al.

    408 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / August 2003

  • 8/13/2019 Social Influence and Information Sharing

    19/22

    (2000), individuals potentially could face consistent pressure from

    all four members of the hypothetical group.

    LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS FOR RESEARCH

    How recasting is interpreted by individuals in decision-making

    groups presents a limitation of this study. Namely, we did not

    record group discussions and so we were unable to look at the rela-

    tionship between statements of information, statements of prefer-

    ence, and recasting statements and our perceptual measures of nor-

    mative and informational influence. Clearly, such data would be

    beneficial to a fuller understanding of normative and informationalinfluence and is a clear direction for future research.

    As it is, we feel that we have made a significant contribution to

    the hidden profile literature. For the first time, to our knowledge, an

    information distribution in which initial preferences coincided

    with unshared rather than shared information was examined. The

    creation of a clear profile offers conditions under which reliance on

    unshared information exceeds reliance on shared information in

    decision-making groups. This provides a clearer understanding of

    why shared information is favored in hidden profile conditions. We

    havefound that the nature of a profile has a strong effect on (a) deci-

    sions made by group members, (b) perceptions of informational

    influence, and (c) reliance on shared and unshared information dur-ing group decision making.

    REFERENCES

    Asch, S. E. (1955). Opinions and social pressures. Scientific American,193, 31-35.

    Boster, F. J., & Mayer, M. (1984). Choice shifts: Argument qualities or social comparisons.

    Communication Yearbook,8, 393-409.

    Brodbeck, F. C., Kerschreiter, R., Mojzisch, A. Frey, D., & Schultz-Hardt, S. (2002). The

    dissemination of critical, unshared information in decision-making groups: The effects

    of prediscussion dissent.European Journal of Social Psychology,32, 35-56.

    Cruz, M. G.,Boster,F.J., & Rodriguez, J. I. (1997). Theimpactof groupsize andproportion

    of shared informationon the exchangeand integrationof informationin groups. Commu-nication Research,24(3), 291-313.

    Henningsen, Henningsen / NORMATIVE AND INFORMATIONAL INFLUENCE 409

  • 8/13/2019 Social Influence and Information Sharing

    20/22

    Cruz, M. G., Henningsen, D. D., & Smith, B. A. (1999). The impact of directive leadership

    on group information sampling, decisions, and perceptions of the leader. Communica-

    tion Research,26(3), 349-369.

    Cruz, M. G.,Henningsen,D. D.,& Williams,M. L. M. (2000). Thepresenceof norms in the

    absence of groups? The i mpact of normative influence under hidden-profile conditions.

    Human Communication Research,26(1), 104-124.

    Dennis, A. R. (1996). Information exchange and use in small group decision making.Small

    Group Research,27(4), 532-550.

    Deutsch, M., & Gerard, H. B. (1955). A study of normative and informational social influ-

    ence upon social judgment. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,51, 629-636.

    Gigone, D., & Hastie, R. (1993). The common knowledge effect: Information sharing and

    group judgment.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,65(5), 959-974.

    Gigone, D., & Hastie, R. (1997). The impact of information on small group choice. Journal

    of Personality and Social Psychology,72(1), 132-140.

    Gruenfeld, D. H., Mannix, E. A., Williams, K. Y., & Neale, M. A. (1996). Group composi-

    tion and decision making: How member familiarity and information distribution affect

    process and performance.Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,

    67(1), 1-15.

    Henningsen, M. L. M., Henningsen, D. D., Cruz, M. G., & Morrill, J. (2002, November).

    Normative influence in decision-making groups: Evaluating the perceptual experience

    of pressure versus influence statements. Paper presentedat theNew Orleans Convention

    of the National Communication Association.

    Hollingshead, A. B. (1996a). Information suppression and status persistence in group deci-

    sion making: The effects of communication media. Human Communication Research,

    23(2), 193-219.

    Hollingshead, A. B. (1996b). The rank-order effect in group decision making. Organiza-

    tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes,68(3), 181-193.

    Kameda, T., Ohtsubo, Y., & Takezawa, M. (1997). Centrality in sociocognitivenetworksand

    social influence: An illustration in a group decision-making context. Journal of Person-

    ality and Social Psychology,73, 296-309.

    Kaplan, M. F., & Miller, C. E. (1987).Group decision makingand normative versus informa-tionalinfluence: Effects oftype ofissue andassigneddecisionrule.Journalof Personal-

    ity and Social Psychology,53, 306-313.

    Kelly, J. R., Jackson, J. W., & Hutson-Comeaux, S. L. (1997). The effects of time pressure

    and task differences on influence modes and accuracy in decision-making groups. Per-

    sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin,23(1), 10-22.

    Kelly, J. R.,& Karau,S. J. (1999). Group decision making: Theeffects of initial preferences

    and time pressure.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,25(11), 1342-1354.

    Larson, J. R., Jr., Christensen, C., Abbott, A. S., & Franz, T. M. (1996). Diagnosing groups:

    Charting the flow of information in medical decision-making teams. Journal of Person-

    ality and Social Psychology,71, 315-330.

    Larson, J. R., Jr., Christensen, C., Franz, T. M., & Abbott, A. S. (1998). Diagnosing groups:

    Managementand impactof sharedand unsharedcase informationin team-basedmedical

    decision making.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,75(1), 93-108.

    Larson, J. R.,Jr., Foster-Fishman,P.G., & Granz,T.M. (1998). Leadership style andthe dis-

    cussion of sharedand unshared informationin decision-making groups.PersonalityandSocial Psychology Bulletin,24(5), 482-495.

    410 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / August 2003

  • 8/13/2019 Social Influence and Information Sharing

    21/22

    Larson, J. R., Jr., Foster-Fishman, P. G., & Keys, C. B. (1994). Discussion of shared and

    unsharedinformation in decision-makinggroups.Journalof Personalityand Social Psy-

    chology,67(3), 446-461.

    Lavery, T. A., Franz, T. M., Winquist, J. R., & Larson, J. R., Jr. (1999). The role of informa-

    tion exchange in predicting group accuracy on a multiple judgment task. Basic and

    Applied Social Psychology,21(4), 281-289.

    Mayer, M. E., Sonoda, K. T., & Gudykunst, W. B. (1997). The effect of time pressure and

    typeof informationon decision quality. SouthernCommunication Journal, 62, 280-292.

    Mennecke,B. E. (1997). Usinggroupsupport systems to discoverhidden profiles:An exam-

    ination of the influence of group size andmeeting structures on information sharing and

    decision quality.International Journal of Human-Computer Studies,47, 387-405.

    Postmes, T., Spears, R.,& Cihangir, S.(2001).Quality ofdecisionmaking andgroup norms.

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,80(6), 918-930.

    Propp,K. M. (1997). Information utilization in small group decision making: A study of the

    evaluative interaction model.Small Group Research,28(3), 424-453.

    Savadori, L., Van Swol, L. M., & Sniezek, J. A. (2001). Information sampling and confi-

    dence within groups and judge advisor systems. Communication Research,28(6), 737-

    771.

    Schittekatte,M. (1996).Facilitatinginformationexchangein smalldecision-makinggroups.

    European Journal of Social Psychology,26, 537-556.

    Schittekatte, M., & Van Hiel, A. (1996). Effects of partially shared information and aware-

    ness of unshared information on information sampling.Small Group Research,27(3),

    431-449.

    Shaw, M.E., & Penrod, W. T., Jr. (1962). Does more information availableto a groupalways

    improve group performance? Sociometry,25, 377-390.

    Stasser, G. (1988). Computer simulation as a research tool: The DISCUSS model of group

    decision making.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,24, 393-422.

    Stasser, G., & Stewart, D. D. (1992). Discovery of hidden profiles by decision-making

    groups: Solving a problem versusmaking a judgment.Journal of Personalityand Social

    Psychology,63(3), 426-434.

    Stasser, G., Stewart, D. D., & Wittenbaum, G. M. (1995). Expert roles and informationexchange during discussion: The importance of knowing who know what.Journal of

    Experimental Social Psychology,31, 244-265.

    Stasser, G., Taylor, L. A., & Hanna, C. (1989). Information sampling in structured and

    unstructured discussions of three- and six-person groups. Journal of Personality and

    Social Psychology,57(1), 67-78.

    Stasser, G., & Titus, W. (1985). Pooling of unshared information in group decision making:

    Biased information sampling during discussion.Journal of Personality and Social Psy-

    chology,48(6), 1467-1478.

    Stasser, G., & Titus, W. (1987). Effects of information load and percentage of shared infor-

    mation on the dissemination of unshared information during group discussion. Journal

    of Personality and Social Psychology,53(1), 81-93.

    Stasser, G., Vaughn, S. I., & Stewart, D. D. (2000). Pooling unshared information: The bene-

    fitsof knowing howaccess to informationis distributed amonggroup members. Organi-

    zational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,82(1), 102-116.

    Stewart, D. D. (1998). Stereotypes, negativity bias, andthe discussion of unshared informa-tion in decision-making groups.Small Group Research,29(6), 643-669.

    Henningsen, Henningsen / NORMATIVE AND INFORMATIONAL INFLUENCE 411

  • 8/13/2019 Social Influence and Information Sharing

    22/22

    Stewart, D. D., Billings, R. S., & Stasser, G. (1998). Accountability and the discussion of

    unshared, critical information in decision-making groups. Group Dynamics: Theory,

    Research, and Practice,2(1), 18-23.

    Stewart, D. D., & Stasser, G. (1995). Expert role assi gnment and information sampling dur-

    ing collectiverecall and decision making.Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology,

    69(4), 619-628.

    Stewart, D. D.,& Stasser,G. (1998). Thesampling of critical, unshared information in deci-

    sion-making groups: The role of an informed minority.European Journal of Social Psy-

    chology,28, 95-113.

    Van Hiel, A.,& Schittekatte, M. (1998). Information exchange in context: Effects of gender

    compositionof group, accountability, and intergroupperceptionon group decision mak-

    ing.Journal of Applied Social Psychology,28(22), 2049-2067.

    Winquist, J.R., & Larson, J.R., Jr. (1998). Information pooling:Whenit impactsgroup deci-

    sion making.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,74(2), 371-377.

    Wittenbaum, G. M. (1998). Information sampling in decision-makinggroups:The impactof

    members task-relevant status.Small Group Research,29(1), 57-84.

    Wittenbaum, G. M. (2000). The bias toward discussing shared information: Why are high-

    status group members immune?Communication Research,27(3), 379-401.

    Wittenbaum, G. M., Hubbell, A. P., & Zuckerman,C. (1999). Mutual enhancement:Toward

    an understanding of the collective preference for shared information.Journal of Person-

    ality and Social Psychology,77(5), 967-978.

    Wittenbaum, G. M.,& Stasser, G.(1998). Thereevaluation of information duringgroup dis-

    cussion.Group Processes & Intergroup Relations,1(1), 21-34.

    Wittenbaum, G. M., Stasser, G., & Merry, C. J. (1996). Tacit coordination in anticipation of

    smallgroup taskcompletion.Journalof Experimental SocialPsychology, 32, 129-152.

    David Dryden Henningsen (J.D., 1992; Ph.D., 1999, University of Wisconsin) is

    assistant professor in the Department of Communication at Northern Illinois Uni-

    versity. He has active lines of research looking at both group decision making andinterpersonalrelationships. Across bothlines of researchhe is interestedin hownor-

    mative and informational processes influence human behavior. His research has

    touched on decision making, flirting, and deception.

    Mary Lynn Miller Henningsen (Ph.D., 2001, University of Wisconsin) is assistant

    professor in the Department of Communication at Northern Illinois University. Her

    research focuses on message evaluation in social influence and interpersonal con-

    texts. In particular, it has focused on small group decision-making interactions and

    date initiation.

    412 SMALL GROUP RESEARCH / August 2003