EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS AND MAGNETIC FIELDS IN THE … · 2018. 2. 11. · sensory, motor, or...

14
32 EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS AND MAGNETIC FIELDS IN THE HUMAN BRAIN STEVEN A. HILLYARD AND MARTA KUTAS To uncover the neural bases of a cognitive process it is important both to identify the participating brain regions and determine the precise time course of information trans- mission within and among those regions. Although neu- roimaging techniques based on cerebral blood flow or me- tabolism (e.g., positron emission tomography [PET] and functional magnetic resonance imaging [fMRI]) are provid- ing increasingly detailed pictures of the anatomic regions activated during cognitive activity, these methods lack the temporal resolution to reveal the rapid-fire patterning of neuronal communication. Noninvasive recordings of the electrical and magnetic fields generated by active neuronal populations, however, can reveal the timing of brain activity related to cognition with a very high, msec-level resolution. This chapter gives an overview of how these temporally precise recording techniques have been used to analyze per- ceptual and cognitive mechanisms in the human brain. The changes in field potentials that are time-locked to sensory, motor, or cognitive events are known as event- related potentials (ERPs) and the corresponding magnetic field changes are termed event-related fields (ERFs). Both ERPs and ERFs consist of precisely timed sequences of waves of varying field strength and polarity (Fig. 32.1). These observed peaks and troughs in the waveform are often referred to as ‘‘components.’’ Some authors, however, prefer to use the term component to refer to portions of the wave- form that originate from particular neural structures, whereas others consider ERP/ERF components to be those waveform features that are associated with a particular cog- nitive process or manipulation (2). Both ERPs and ERFs are generated primarily by the flow of ionic currents in elongated nerve cells during synaptic activity. Whereas syn- aptic currents flowing across nerve cell membranes into the Steven A. Hillyard: Department of Neurosciences, University of Califor- nia, San Diego, La Jolla, California. Marta Kutas: Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California. extracellular fluid produce ERPs, the flow of synaptic cur- rent through neuronal processes produce ERFs, thereby giv- ing rise to concentric magnetic fields surrounding the cell. When a sufficient number of neurons having a similar ana- tomic configuration are synchronously active, their sum- mated fields may be strong enough to be detectable at the scalp. When ERPs or ERFs are recorded from the surface of the head, the locations of the active neural generators can only be estimated rather than visualized directly. The calcu- lation of generator locations from surface field distributions is known as the inverse problem, which typically has no unique solution. However, the validity of inverse source estimations can be considerably improved by using algo- rithms and models that take into account the geometry of the cortical surface, biophysical properties of the intervening tissues, constraints from neuroimaging data, and statistical likelihood of alternative source locations (3,4). In general, the localization of active neural populations is more straight- forward with surface recordings of ERFs than with ERPs, because magnetic fields, unlike electrical fields, are mini- mally distorted by the physical properties of the intervening tissues. ERP and ERF recordings have been used extensively to investigate the spatiotemporal patterns of brain activity asso- ciated with a variety of perceptual, cognitive, and linguistic processes. The general research strategy has been to discover the mapping between the components of the waveform and specific cognitive processes that are engaged by a particular task. When an ERP/ERF component can be shown to be a valid index of the neural activity underlying a cognitive operation, that component can yield valuable information about the presence or absence of that operation and its tim- ing with respect to other cognitive events. In many cases, such data have been related to psychological models of the underlying processing operations and used to test alternative theoretical positions. In addition, by localizing the neural generators of such components, inferences can be made

Transcript of EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS AND MAGNETIC FIELDS IN THE … · 2018. 2. 11. · sensory, motor, or...

Page 1: EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS AND MAGNETIC FIELDS IN THE … · 2018. 2. 11. · sensory, motor, or cognitive events are known as event-related potentials (ERPs) ... (13–15).Amoreseriousques-

32

EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS ANDMAGNETIC FIELDS IN THE HUMAN

BRAIN

STEVEN A. HILLYARDAND MARTA KUTAS

To uncover the neural bases of a cognitive process it isimportant both to identify the participating brain regionsand determine the precise time course of information trans-mission within and among those regions. Although neu-roimaging techniques based on cerebral blood flow or me-tabolism (e.g., positron emission tomography [PET] andfunctional magnetic resonance imaging [fMRI]) are provid-ing increasingly detailed pictures of the anatomic regionsactivated during cognitive activity, these methods lack thetemporal resolution to reveal the rapid-fire patterning ofneuronal communication. Noninvasive recordings of theelectrical and magnetic fields generated by active neuronalpopulations, however, can reveal the timing of brain activityrelated to cognition with a very high, msec-level resolution.This chapter gives an overview of how these temporallyprecise recording techniques have been used to analyze per-ceptual and cognitive mechanisms in the human brain.

The changes in field potentials that are time-locked tosensory, motor, or cognitive events are known as event-related potentials (ERPs) and the corresponding magneticfield changes are termed event-related fields (ERFs). BothERPs and ERFs consist of precisely timed sequences ofwaves of varying field strength and polarity (Fig. 32.1).These observed peaks and troughs in the waveform are oftenreferred to as ‘‘components.’’ Some authors, however, preferto use the term component to refer to portions of the wave-form that originate from particular neural structures,whereas others consider ERP/ERF components to be thosewaveform features that are associated with a particular cog-nitive process or manipulation (2). Both ERPs and ERFsare generated primarily by the flow of ionic currents inelongated nerve cells during synaptic activity. Whereas syn-aptic currents flowing across nerve cell membranes into the

Steven A. Hillyard: Department of Neurosciences, University of Califor-nia, San Diego, La Jolla, California.

Marta Kutas: Department of Cognitive Science, University of California,San Diego, La Jolla, California.

extracellular fluid produce ERPs, the flow of synaptic cur-rent through neuronal processes produce ERFs, thereby giv-ing rise to concentric magnetic fields surrounding the cell.When a sufficient number of neurons having a similar ana-tomic configuration are synchronously active, their sum-mated fields may be strong enough to be detectable at thescalp.

When ERPs or ERFs are recorded from the surface ofthe head, the locations of the active neural generators canonly be estimated rather than visualized directly. The calcu-lation of generator locations from surface field distributionsis known as the inverse problem, which typically has nounique solution. However, the validity of inverse sourceestimations can be considerably improved by using algo-rithms and models that take into account the geometry ofthe cortical surface, biophysical properties of the interveningtissues, constraints from neuroimaging data, and statisticallikelihood of alternative source locations (3,4). In general,the localization of active neural populations is more straight-forward with surface recordings of ERFs than with ERPs,because magnetic fields, unlike electrical fields, are mini-mally distorted by the physical properties of the interveningtissues.

ERP and ERF recordings have been used extensively toinvestigate the spatiotemporal patterns of brain activity asso-ciated with a variety of perceptual, cognitive, and linguisticprocesses. The general research strategy has been to discoverthe mapping between the components of the waveform andspecific cognitive processes that are engaged by a particulartask. When an ERP/ERF component can be shown to bea valid index of the neural activity underlying a cognitiveoperation, that component can yield valuable informationabout the presence or absence of that operation and its tim-ing with respect to other cognitive events. In many cases,such data have been related to psychological models of theunderlying processing operations and used to test alternativetheoretical positions. In addition, by localizing the neuralgenerators of such components, inferences can be made

Page 2: EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS AND MAGNETIC FIELDS IN THE … · 2018. 2. 11. · sensory, motor, or cognitive events are known as event-related potentials (ERPs) ... (13–15).Amoreseriousques-

Neuropsychopharmacology: The Fifth Generation of Progress428

FIGURE 32.1. The characteristicwaveform of the auditory event-re-latedpotential followingabrief stim-ulus such as a click or tone. The indi-vidual components (peaks andtroughs) are evoked with specifictime delays (latencies) after stimulusonset. Note the logarithmic timebase, whichmakes it possible to visu-alize the earliest waves (I–VI) gener-ated in the auditory brainstem path-ways. Longer latency negative (N)and positive (P) components are gen-erated in different cortical areas.Dashed line shows increasednegativ-ityelicitedbyattendedsounds (nega-tive difference) or by deviant sounds(mismatch negativity), and dottedline shows N2 and P3 components totask-relevant target stimuli. Adaptedfrom Munte TF, Schiltz K, Kutas M.When temporal terms belie concep-tual order. Nature 1998;395:71–73.

about the participating anatomic circuits that can be inter-faced with neuroimaging and neuropsychological data. Thischapter describes recent advances made using this approachfor analyzing the neural and cognitive mechanisms of preat-tentive sensory processing, selective attention, mental chro-nometry, memory storage and retrieval, and language com-prehension and production. The use of ERP/ERFrecordings to evaluate cognitive disorders associated withneurobehavioral and psychopathologic syndromes also is re-viewed.

PREATTENTIVE SENSORY PROCESSING

Much of the early ERP waveform, and some later compo-nents as well, represent sensory-evoked neural activity inmodality-specific cortical areas. These evoked componentsvary with the physical parameters of the stimuli and in manycases are associated with the preattentive encoding of stimu-lus features. In the visual modality, for example, the earlyC1 component (onset latency 50 to 60 msec) originatesin retinotopically organized visual cortex (5) and varies inamplitude according to the spatial frequency of the stimulus(6). Similarly, the early auditory cortical components P50and N100 (and their magnetic counterparts, M50 andM100) arise in part from generators in tonotopically organ-ized supratemporal auditory cortex and reflect the encodingof perceived pitch (7).

In general, ERP amplitudes decrease when the time be-tween successive stimulus presentations is made shorter thanthe refractory or recovery period of the component understudy. Although the neural processes underlying ERP refrac-tory effects are not well established, some candidate mecha-nisms include synaptic fatigue, active inhibition, and the

persistence of sensory memory for the preceding stimulus.In line with this latter idea, the refractory period of theauditory M100 has been found to have a similar time courseto that of sensory or ‘‘echoic’’ memory for stimulus loudness(8).

The P50 and Sensory Gating

The refractory properties of the auditory P50 (P1) compo-nent have been studied extensively over the past 20 yearsas a possible marker of abnormal sensory input control inschizophrenia (9–12). In the standard paradigm, pairs ofauditory stimuli are presented with an ISI of 0.5 sec, andthe amplitude ratio of the P50 evoked by the second stimu-lus (S2) relative to the first stimulus (S1) is calculated. Ingeneral, schizophrenic subjects do not show as large a reduc-tion in the P50 amplitude to S2 relative to S1 as do normalcontrols. This refractory reduction of P50 amplitude to S2has been interpreted as a sign of preattentive sensory gating,which occurs because the initial S1 automatically activatesan inhibitory system that suppresses responsiveness to S2 (9,10). This inhibitory system presumably prevents irrelevantinformation from ascending to higher levels of cortical pro-cessing. The abnormally large S2/S1 amplitude ratio forP50 seen in schizophrenics was thus considered evidencefor impaired sensory gating, which was suggested to be theprincipal sensory deficit of the disease process.

This pattern of more rapid P50 recovery in schizophreniahas been widely reported, but there have been some notableexceptions that raise questions about the exact conditionsneeded to produce the effect (13–15). A more serious ques-tion, however, is whether existing studies have, in fact, dem-onstrated a reliably abnormal S2/S1 ratio of the auditoryP50 in schizophrenics. This concern stems from the way the

Page 3: EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS AND MAGNETIC FIELDS IN THE … · 2018. 2. 11. · sensory, motor, or cognitive events are known as event-related potentials (ERPs) ... (13–15).Amoreseriousques-

Chapter 32: Event-Related Potentials and Magnetic Fields 429

P50 has typically been measured—as the maximal positiveamplitude within a time window (e.g., 40 to 80 msec) thatencompasses the P50 peak. Such peak measures may beartificially inflated by increased levels of background noisein the EEG recordings, originating from either intracranialor extracranial sources. Thus, if a patient group has higherEEG noise levels, then the measured P50 amplitudes tendto be greater because the noise peaks are at times mistakenfor the actual peak of the P50. This type of error is morepronounced when measuring the P50 to S2, because itsamplitude is diminished relative to the noise owing to re-fractory effects. Reports of increased variability and lowerreliability of P50 measures in schizophrenics (12,16) suggestthat background noise levels are indeed higher in the patientgroups. Several studies, however, have reported that the P50evoked by S1 is smaller in schizophrenics (11,12,16), whichsuggests that overall response amplitudes may be lower and/or response latencies more variable in these patients. Furtherstudies are needed to determine whether the actual S2/S1amplitude ratio is reliably higher in schizophrenics, orwhether this reported effect is a product of noise-sensitivemeasurement procedures.

Even if the S2/S1 ratio for P50 were determined to begreater in schizophrenia, there would still be some questionabout its functional interpretation. There is little evidencethat refractory reductions of ERP amplitudes to stimuli re-peated at 0.5 sec ISIs are associated with any reduction in theperceptual information reaching higher centers. Nor does itappear that the amplitude of P50 to S2 is reduced onlywhen S2 is irrelevant (17), calling into question the hypoth-esis that the P50 refractory effect reflects the selective gatingof irrelevant versus relevant sensory inputs. In addition, ithas been reported that schizophrenic patients showing themost severe perceptual anomalies did not differ from nor-mals in their P50 refractory effects (15). Thus, there seemsto be scant evidence that reduced P50 refractoriness in schiz-ophrenia, if such exists, is related to the selective gating orfiltering of irrelevant sensory information in the auditorycortical pathways.

Auditory Feature Encoding

The preattentive coding of auditory features is indexed withconsiderable precision by the mismatch negativity (MMN)component and its magnetic counterpart, the MMNm. TheMMN is a scalp-negative component with a latency of 120to 250 msec that is specifically elicited by a deviant soundin a repetitive auditory sequence (18–20). The MMN canbe triggered by any discernible change in the ongoingsounds, such as deviations in frequency, intensity, duration,rise-time, timing, and spatial location. MMNs also havebeen recorded to changes in more complex sound propertiessuch as the phonetic structure of speech sounds and thepatterning of tone sequences (20,21). Naatanen (22) hasproposed that the MMN is generated by an automatic com-

parator process that contrasts current auditory input againsta multidimensional trace of the previous repeating sound’sfeatures held in preperceptual sensory memory. This mis-match detection process may represent an early stage in thealerting and orienting of the organism toward novel andpotentially important changes in the acoustic environment.

The MMN provides a window on auditory sensory or‘‘echoic memory’’ because it is initiated by a mismatch withthe memory traces of the preceding sounds (23). Indeed,the maximal interstimulus interval (ISI) at which the MMNcan be maintained is of the order of 10 sec, correspondingwell with behavioral estimates of the duration of echoicmemory (19,20). The MMN also can be used to study morepermanent auditory memory traces, such as those for thephonemic characteristics of one’s native language (19) asthey emerge during the first year of life (21).

It has been proposed that a supratemporal componentof the MMN originating in auditory cortex reflects the pre-attentive sensory store and automatic change detection pro-cess, whereas a frontal component indexes the involuntaryorienting of attention to the deviant event (24,25). Forspeech sounds, however, the MMN/MMNm appears toarise from sources in auditory cortex of the left hemisphere,in accordance with proposals that the left posterior temporalcortex is the locus of language-specific auditory traces (19).

Given that the MMN may be elicited with minimal co-operation from the subject, it has found wide applicabilityfor the diagnosis and evaluation of a variety of neurobehav-ioral and psychiatric disorders (24,26). Schizophrenic pa-tients have reduced or prolonged MMNs to pitch or dura-tion deviants, with the degree of abnormality depending onthe specific parameters of the stimulus deviance (24,27,28).These findings provide clear evidence for a deficit in preat-tentive auditory processing in schizophrenia, although thereis some debate about whether the impairment is primarilyin temporal processing (28) or auditory encoding and traceformation (27). A different pattern of abnormality has beenobserved in Alzheimer’s disease, with MMN amplitude re-ductions becoming more prominent at longer ISIs (29),suggesting a more rapid decay of auditory sensory memorytraces. MMN abnormalities indicative of auditory process-ing deficits have also been reported in cases of learning disor-ders, language and speech impairments, depression, autism,parkinsonism, and HIV infection. (See refs. 19, 21, 24, and29 for reviews.) Drugs that interfere with NMDA-receptormediated neurotransmission also disrupt the MMN, whichis consistent with models of schizophrenia that posit a dis-turbance in glutaminergic/NMDA functioning (27).

SELECTIVE ATTENTION

The brain’s attentional systems include a central controlnetwork with projections to the sensory pathways of thedifferent modalities that enable the selective modulation of

Page 4: EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS AND MAGNETIC FIELDS IN THE … · 2018. 2. 11. · sensory, motor, or cognitive events are known as event-related potentials (ERPs) ... (13–15).Amoreseriousques-

Neuropsychopharmacology: The Fifth Generation of Progress430

incoming information. A good deal of research on attentionover the past few decades has been aimed at determiningwhether incoming sensory information is selected at ‘‘early’’or ‘‘late’’ levels of processing—that is, before or after stimu-lus properties are fully analyzed (30). Current evidence fromboth behavioral and physiologic studies indicates that atten-tion can select stimuli at different levels of the sensory path-ways, depending on the features being attended and the taskrequirements.

Auditory Attention

In the auditory modality, ERPs have demonstrated that at-tentional selection occurs at early levels of cortical process-ing, but not in the brainstem pathways (31). In dichoticlistening tasks with rapidly presented tones to the left andright ears, the earliest ERP component that is reliably influ-enced by paying attention selectively to one ear is a smallpositive wave with a latency of 20 to 50 msec (termed theP20–50), which has been localized using magnetoencepha-lography (MEG) to sources in or near primary auditorycortex. This short-latency modulation provides evidence foran attentional mechanism of sensory gain control at theearliest levels of cortical processing. A much stronger atten-tional modulation of auditory input takes place at 50 to 70msec after stimulus onset in the form of a negative difference(Nd) potential that augments the amplitude of the evokedN1 wave to attended-channel sounds (Fig. 32.1). This N1/Nd attention effect also has been localized to auditory cortexby MEG recordings and is considered to be an index offurther processing of attended sound information, or alter-natively, of the closeness of match between incoming stimuliand the acoustic features that define the attended channel(30). These negative ERP modulations indicate the precisetiming with which different auditory features are attendedor rejected (32) and provide strong evidence for early selec-tion theories of attention. Schizophrenic subjects reportedlyshow abnormally reduced Nd amplitudes when attending tomultiple sound features, suggesting a deficit in their controlfunctions for allocating attentional resources during selec-tive listening (33).

In recent studies, auditory ERPs have been used to studyhow attention is allocated in a noisy environment with mul-tiple, competing sound sources (34). When subjects listenedselectively to sounds coming from one loudspeaker in a free-field array, the spatial focusing of auditory attention tookplace in two distinct stages: an early, broadly tuned inputselection occurring over the first 80 to 180 msec (indexedby N1/Nd) was followed by a more sharply focused selectionof target sounds that began at around 250 msec (indexedby the late positive P3 component). These findings indicatethat auditory spatial attention is deployed as a sharply tunedgradient around an attended sound source in a noisy envi-ronment. Under these conditions congenitally blind personswere found to have sound localization capabilities superior

to those of sighted control subjects (35). ERP data indicatedthat this enhanced capability of blind persons is mediatedat least in part by an attentional tuning mechanism thatoperates within the first 100 msec after sound onset.

Visual Attention

Covertly directing attention to a specific location in thevisual fields typically results in faster and more accuratedetections or discriminations of stimuli at that location.Recordings of brain activity in both humans and animalshave identified a number of sites along the visual pathwayswhere afferent information is modulated under the influ-ence of visual-spatial attention. Neurophysiologic studies inmonkeys demonstrated strong influences of spatial attentionon neural activity in extrastriate cortical regions, includingretinotopic areas V2, V3A, and V4 and higher areas of boththe ventral (inferior temporal lobe) and dorsal (area MT,posterior parietal lobe) processing streams (36). These find-ings are congruent with human ERP studies showing thatstimuli at attended locations elicit enlarged P1 (70 to 130msec) and N1 (150 to 190 msec) components (Fig. 32.2),which have been localized to generators in extrastriate visualcortex (37). This amplitude enhancement of the P1 andN1 waves occurs with little or no change of the componentlatencies, suggesting that spatial attention exerts a gain con-trol or selective amplification of attended inputs within thevisual-cortical pathways in the interval between 70 and 200msec after stimulus onset (38).

In experiments that combined PET neuroimaging andERP recordings, the calculated dipolar sources of the P1attention effect were found to correspond closely with re-gions of increased blood flow in retinotopically organizedextrastriate areas, including areas V3/V4 and the posteriorfusiform gyrus (37). Significantly, however, the earlier C1component (onset at 50 msec), which appears to originatefrom generators in primary visual cortex (area V1), wasfound to remain invariant with changes in the direction ofspatial attention. These findings suggest that spatial atten-tion modulates the flow of visual information at a higherlevel than the primary cortex.

Recent studies in monkeys, however, reported that stim-ulus-evoked activity in area V1 may be affected by spatialattention when competing stimuli are present in the visualfield (39). The participation of V1 in spatial attention hasalso been inferred from recent fMRI studies in humans (40).In a study combining fMRI with ERP recordings, Martınezand colleagues (41) observed focal increases in blood flow(BOLD signal) in area V1 as well as areas V2, V3/VP, andV4 at sites corresponding to the retinotopically mappedposition of the attended stimulus; however, the amplitudeof the C1 component again remained invariant (Fig. 32.2).The earliest influence of attention was on the P1 component(75 to 130 msec), which was localized through dipole mod-eling to dorsal and ventral extrastriate sources. It was sug-

Page 5: EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS AND MAGNETIC FIELDS IN THE … · 2018. 2. 11. · sensory, motor, or cognitive events are known as event-related potentials (ERPs) ... (13–15).Amoreseriousques-

Chapter 32: Event-Related Potentials and Magnetic Fields 431

CA

B

FIGURE 32.2. A: Visual event-related potential waveforms from several scalp sites in responseto stimuli in left visual field in study by Martınez and colleagues (41). Subjects were required toattend to one field at a time while randomized stimulus sequences were presented concurrentlyto left and right fields. Note increased amplitude of P1 and N1 components when stimuli wereattended and lack of change in C1 component. B: Voltage topographies of C1 component andof the P1 attention effect (increased positivity with attention) in two different time ranges. C:Locations of dipolar sources calculated for the C1 component (in primary cortex of calcarine fissure,left) and the early and late P1 attention effects (dorsal and ventral extrastriate cortex, respectively,right).

gested that the attentional modulation of V1 activity re-vealed by fMRI may take place at a latency longer than theinitial geniculo-striate response represented by the C1 andis consequent on delayed feedback of enhanced visual signalsback to V1 from higher extrastriate areas. Such long-latencymodulations in V1 have been observed in animals and mayenhance figure/ground contrast in attended regions of thevisual field (39).

The spatial allocation of attention has also been studiedwith the steady-state visual evoked potential (SSVEP),which is the oscillatory response of the visual cortex evokedby regularly repeating stimuli such as a light that flickers ata rate of 8 Hz or more (42). The amplitude of the SSVEPelicited by such a flickering stimulus is substantially in-creased in amplitude when attention is directed to its loca-tion, thereby indexing amplification of visual inputs withinthe spotlight of attention. The continuous nature of theSSVEP as a measure of cortical facilitation makes it suitablefor measuring the time course of attention shifts amongstimuli in the visual fields.

In contrast with spatial attention, when stimuli are se-lected on the basis of nonspatial features such as color,shape, or spatial frequency, a different pattern of ERP com-

ponents is observed. Stimuli having the attended featureelicit a prominent ‘‘selection negativity’’ (SN) over the pos-terior scalp that begins at 120 to 220 msec and may extendfor several hundreds of msec (37). The onset of the SNprovides a precise measure of the time point at which aparticular feature is discriminated and selectively processed,and localization of its neural generators points to the brainregions involved in the selection. When stimuli are selectedon the basis of two or more features concurrently, recordingsof the SN can indicate whether the features are selectedindependently or in an interactive, contingent manner (43).

MENTAL CHRONOMETRY

Motor preparation, execution, and evaluation are indexedby a series of electric (and magnetic) components both pre-ceding and following movement onset. Prime among theERPs indexing preparation is the readiness potential (RP),which is a slowly ramping negativity that starts about 1 sbefore the onset of a voluntary or self-paced movement andpeaks around movement onset (44). The initial, bilaterallysymmetric portion of the RP is generated in the supplemen-

Page 6: EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS AND MAGNETIC FIELDS IN THE … · 2018. 2. 11. · sensory, motor, or cognitive events are known as event-related potentials (ERPs) ... (13–15).Amoreseriousques-

Neuropsychopharmacology: The Fifth Generation of Progress432

tary motor area (SMA). Approximately 200 to 500 msecbefore movement onset the RP becomes asymmetric, beingmaximal over central scalp contralateral to the active muscu-lature. This lateralized portion of the RP has a somatotopicdistribution over the motor cortex and has been localizedto the primary motor cortex (45). This asymmetric portionof the RP can be seen in stimulus-locked averages (46) andserves as the basis for an index of differential motor prepara-tion termed the lateralized readiness potential (LRP). Sub-tracting for each hand separately the activity over the ipsi-lateral from that over the contralateral central site, and thenaveraging the activity for the two hands together derive theLRP.

The LRP has proven especially useful in studies of mentalchronometry aimed at answering questions about the dy-namics of information processing. For example, LRP datahave shown that whether a person responds quickly andaccurately is in large part a function of whether he or sheis prepared to do so before the stimulus appears. Appropriatepreparation leads to fast and correct responses, whereas in-appropriate preparation leads to fast but wrong responses,and no preparation at all leads to slow but accurate re-sponses. More important, LRP data revealed that peopledevelop biases that influence how they prepare to respond.(See refs. 47 and 48 for review.)

In a number of sensory-motor discrimination tasks, LRPrecordings have provided strong support for ‘‘continuousflow’’ models that specify that transmission of perceptualinformation to the response system occurs continuouslyrather than in discrete, all-or-none stages. Such studies haverevealed partial transmission of perceptual information (e.g.,about letter identity) to the response system and have pro-vided a means of tracking the time course of the extractionof various types of information. The LRP also has been usedto determine the timing of ‘‘the point of no return;’’ thatis, the time in the course of response preparation beyondwhich response execution cannot be stopped (47).

Readiness potentials have been examined in a numberof patient populations. They are abnormal in a large major-ity of individuals with Parkinson’s disease (PD), presumablybecause of abnormal activation of the SMA by the basalganglia (49,50). Because the early part of the RP is sensitiveto attention, it has been suggested that motor performancein PD patients might be improved by having them attendto movements that they might otherwise try to execute auto-matically (51). Individuals with tardive dyskinesia (TD) alsoshow abnormal RPs that are larger in amplitude than thoseof normal controls and schizophrenic patients without TD(52). Unlike voluntary leg movements, involuntary myoclo-nic leg movements in patients with restless leg syndromedo not elicit an RP, suggesting that these involuntary move-ments have a subcortical or spinal origin (53).

Purposeful movements are generally monitored and eval-uated so that remedial action may be taken if an error incommitted. Performance monitoring of this sort is indexed

by an ERP known as the error-related negativity (ERN),which peaks about 100 msec after the onset of the electro-myographic activity associated with an erroneous response.ERN amplitude covaries with the perceived inaccuracy ofa response (54) and is influenced by how similar the givenresponse is to the correct one. An ERN is also elicited bya feedback stimulus that lets the subject know an error wasmade. ERN generators have been localized to the anteriorcingulate gyrus and are modulated by lateral prefrontal cor-tex (55). Patients with lateral prefrontal cortical damage areimpaired in correcting their behaviors and produce equal-sized ERNs for correct and incorrect responses. ERN ampli-tude is sensitive to mood and personality variables (56),especially when correct responses are rewarded and/or incor-rect responses are punished (57).

MEMORY

Working Memory

Unexpected events typically require us to revise or updateour current working mental model of the environment.Donchin and colleagues (58) proposed that this updating ofthe working memory (temporary, limited capacity) system isindexed by the P3 (also known as the P300 or P3b) compo-nent. (See refs. 59 and 60 for alternative views.) The P3bis a positive, broadly distributed component with a centro-parietal maximum and peak latency between 300 and 800ms. It is elicited by infrequent target events in a sequence ofhigher probability nontarget events that are being attended,although irrelevant stimuli that draw attention may alsotrigger a P3. In general P3 amplitude grows with the rele-vance, salience, and utility of the target or ‘‘oddball’’ stimu-lus. The P3 can be elicited by many different simple andcomplex events, including the occasional absence of a pre-dicted stimulus. (See ref. 61 for review.) The differences inthe distribution and timing of P3s in various modalities areconsistent with the proposal that there are multiple workingmemory stores.

P3 amplitude is inversely related to the overall probabil-ity of the target events, and varies with the fine structureof event sequences. The more difficult the categorization ofthe target events, the longer the P3 latency. P3 latency isnot correlated with the variance in reaction time that iscaused by response execution, thereby making it a ratherpure measure of stimulus evaluation/categorization time.The combined sensitivity of the P3 to attention and stimu-lus evaluation makes it a good index of the availability andallocation of capacity-limited perceptual resources. Mea-surements of P3 latency and RT together can be used topinpoint the processing locus of individual differences inperformance, as was done, for example, to analyze cognitiveslowing with normal aging (62). Similarly, P3 data havedemonstrated that the prolongation of response time forthe second of two decisions made in rapid succession (‘‘psy-

Page 7: EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS AND MAGNETIC FIELDS IN THE … · 2018. 2. 11. · sensory, motor, or cognitive events are known as event-related potentials (ERPs) ... (13–15).Amoreseriousques-

Chapter 32: Event-Related Potentials and Magnetic Fields 433

chological refractory period’’) is owing to interference at astage that follows perceptual categorization, presumably thatof response selection (63).

Updating working memory has consequences for an indi-vidual’s subsequent performance. For example, the relativeamplitude of the P3 on a trial when a subject commits anerror is predictive of the performance (accuracy and re-sponse speed) on the next trial; moreover, the larger theP3 to an item, the greater the likelihood that it will beremembered subsequently. (See ref. 64 for review.) Intracra-nial recordings in individuals with epilepsy have revealedP3-like potentials in the hippocampal region of the medialtemporal lobe (65). The scalp-recorded P3, however, pri-marily reflects activity in a number of neocortical (frontal,central, parietal, temporal-parietal junction) and perhapssubcortical generators and is mediated by several neuro-transmitter systems (66). Not surprisingly, therefore, manypatient populations show abnormally small or delayed P3bsincluding schizophrenic patients, individuals at risk for alco-holism, patients with probable Alzheimer’s dementia, andindividuals with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder,among others (61,66).

The storage of information in working memory may bemodulated by attention and appears to be strongly sup-pressed during the ‘‘attentional blink’’ that follows detectionof the first of two target stimuli presented in a rapid se-quence. Luck and colleagues showed that the P3 was absentin response to targets that went undetected during the atten-tional blink, suggesting that no updating of working mem-ory occurred. (See ref. 67 for review.) However, undetectedtarget words did elicit late negative ERPs, indicating thatthey had been identified at the level of lexical/semantic pro-cessing. Thus, the ERP data provided strong evidence thatthe attentional blink acts at the postperceptual stage ofworking memory storage.

A frontally distributed late positivity (P3a) is elicited byrare and unexpected stimuli for which there is no memorytemplate readily available. It appears to reflect an orientingresponse to stimulus novelty and is reduced in patients withprefrontal cortical injury (68). Maintenance of informationin working memory is also reflected in sustained ERPs last-ing on the order of seconds. These potentials vary in theirscalp distribution as a function of the information beingheld in working memory, consistent with proposals of inde-pendent short-term buffers for verbal and nonverbal infor-mation, among others (69).

Long(er)-Term Memory

Encoding

Encoding processes (transforming sensory input into a last-ing representation) are associated with an increased positiv-ity between 200 and 800 msec that spans several compo-nents. Items that call for preferential processing because they

FIGURE 32.3. Averaged event-related potentials (ERPs) frommidline parietal site (filled in circle in map on the right) sortedas a function of subsequent memory in a cued recall test. Theresponses to words subsequently recalled (solid line) are over-lappedwith those subsequently not recalled (dashed line). Partici-pants were presented the first three letters of a word and askedto use this stem as a clue for verbally recalling the words theyhad just studied. The voltage map of this difference related tomemory (Dm) effect at 550 ms was computed by subtracting theERPs towords subsequently not recalled to ERPs from those subse-quently recalled. A: Semantically anomalous word. B: Unexpectedword. Adapted from Paller KA. Recall and stem-completion prim-ing have different electrophysiological correlates and are modi-fied differentially by directed forgetting. J Exp Psychol Learn MemCogn 1990;16:1021–1032.

stand out, for example, are better recalled and elicit largerP3 components (70). Likewise, the more deeply (semanti-cally) an item is analyzed, the more likely it is to be remem-bered, and this is reflected in greater late positivity (71).Even among items that are all deeply processed, those thatwill in fact be remembered later elicit a larger positivityduring encoding than those that will be forgotten (Fig.32.3). These late components produced during encodingthat are predictive of subsequent memory performance arecollectively termed Dm effects (71).

Dm effects are larger in semantic than in nonsemantictasks and are not seen for items that have no preexistingrepresentation in long-term memory. Van Petten and col-leagues (73) suggested that this positivity indexes the rich-ness of associative elaboration engendered by the to-be-remembered event. Consistent with this proposal, the Dmeffect varies with the encoding task and information re-trieved from long-term memory and shows substantial vari-ability in onset latency, duration, and scalp topography(74).

Retrieval

Retrieval processes are indexed by several ERP effects thatvary with whether or not the rememberer is in a retrievalmode, whether memory is queried directly or indirectly,what aspect of the memory is being queried, and whetheror not the retrieval attempt is successful (75,76). Retrievalitself is indexed by slow potentials sustained over severalseconds with an amplitude determined by the difficulty ofthe retrieval and a scalp topography determined by the na-

Page 8: EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS AND MAGNETIC FIELDS IN THE … · 2018. 2. 11. · sensory, motor, or cognitive events are known as event-related potentials (ERPs) ... (13–15).Amoreseriousques-

Neuropsychopharmacology: The Fifth Generation of Progress434

ture of the information retrieved (77). These results fit withthe notion that the brain areas involved in explicit memoryare the same as those carrying out the initial encoding andperception and argue against the concept of a single, amodalmemory store.

In a typical retrieval paradigm items are presented twice,and ERPs to the first and second (i.e., repetition) presenta-tions are compared. When subjects are asked to recognizeand detect the repeated items, the task is considered to probememory directly or explicitly. By contrast, when the old ornew distinction is irrelevant, as in tasks involving lexicaldecision, semantic judgment, or identification of visuallydegraded words, the stimuli may only tap memory indirectlyor implicitly and may not produce actual recollection. Inboth implicit and explicit memory tasks, stimulus repetitionproduces large and reliable ERP effects. The first is a reduc-tion in the amplitude of negativity between 250 and 500msec (N400) that is associated with semantic processing(76). The N400 is reduced by repetition, whether or notthe task explicitly calls for detection of repeated items, evenin amnesic individuals with damage to the medial temporallobes (78). Some authors have linked a frontal subcompo-nent of the N400 to repetition independent of recognition(79).

Another ERP consequence of word repetition is a changein the amplitude of a late positive component (LPC), whichtypically begins around 400 to 500 msec, and and is some-what larger over the left than right scalp. There is mountingevidence that this LPC reflects conscious recollection. Fac-tors that influence perceptual priming do not modulate LPCamplitude (80), whereas factors that influence recognitionmemory do. There is an LPC repetition effect whether

A B

FIGURE 32.4. A: A prototypical N400 recorded at the vertex in response to a semantically anoma-lous word (dashed line) at the end of a sentence, compared with the ERP to the expected ending(solid line), and an anomalous ending that is semantically related to the expected ending (dottedline). Note reduced N400 for nonsensical ending (‘‘hive’’) that is semantically related to expectedending. B: The N400 recorded at amidline parietal site elicited by aword that fits with the ongoingdiscourse context (solid line) versus that to a word that is less expected and does not fit as wellwith the ongoing discourse context (dashed line). Data taken from van Berkum JJA, Hagoort P,Brown CM. Semantic integration in sentences and discourse: evidence from the N400. J Cog Neu-rosci 1999;11:657–671.

memory is tested implicitly or explicitly. When participantsare asked to indicate whether an item is old or new, correctlyrecognized old items elicit larger LPCs than do unrecog-nized old items or correctly recognized new items, althoughits distribution across the scalp varies somewhat with thematerials (81). The LPC to correctly recognized old itemsis larger for confident than less confident decisions, and foritems that participants actually ‘‘remember’’ (82).

When a subject attempts to remember some aspect ofthe context in which an item was studied or some attributeof the item that it shared with others in the study task, alarge, late, frontally distributed (sometimes right lateralized)positivity is elicited (83). This large positivity over prefron-tal sites occurs in addition to the standard LPC effect. Theprefrontal locus of this ERP source retrieval effect fits withthe known impairments that patients with frontal lobe dam-age have in retrieving source information about items thatthey recognize (84).

LANGUAGE

Semantic Analysis

The semantic analysis of verbal and nonverbal stimuli isindexed by the N400 component (85). The N400 is abroadly distributed component, with a negative-going peakover centroparietal sites often with a slightly right predomi-nance; in young adults, it has an onset around 200 msecand a peak around 400 msec. The largest N400s are elicitedby unexpected, semantically anomalous words in a sentence(Fig. 32.4). However, all potentially meaningful items (e.g.,words and pseudowords, environmental sounds, pictures,

Page 9: EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS AND MAGNETIC FIELDS IN THE … · 2018. 2. 11. · sensory, motor, or cognitive events are known as event-related potentials (ERPs) ... (13–15).Amoreseriousques-

Chapter 32: Event-Related Potentials and Magnetic Fields 435

faces) can elicit some N400 activity with an amplitude thatis determined by a variety of factors. With little or no con-textual constraint, N400 amplitudes are inversely related tothe frequency of the eliciting word in the language (86).

The N400 is typically considered an ERP index of se-mantic processing or contextual integration because its am-plitude is modulated by its relation and fit to the ongoingcontext, be it a single word, a sentence, or a multisentencediscourse. (See refs. 87 and 88 for review.) N400 amplitudesare enlarged to a word in unrelated word pair or in anincongruous or weak context relative to the response to thesame word in a related pair or strong congruous sentence.The N400 in these cases is almost identical in timing anddistribution over the head, indicating that by 400 msec atthe latest, lexical, sentential, and discourse processes all con-verge to influence language comprehension in a similarmanner. Visual half-field studies of the N400 show thatthe left hemisphere, in particular, uses the organization ofsemantic memory tapped by context words to aid in itsonline predictions, whereas the right hemisphere waits andintegrates (89).

As would be expected of an index of semantic processingand contextual integration, N400 amplitude is greatly atten-uated and its latency delayed in aphasic patients with mod-erate to severe comprehension deficits (90) N400 latencyis also prolonged with normal aging and various dementias.Although ERP evidence for a differential organization ofsemantic memory in schizophrenia is equivocal, a delay inN400 latency has been reported. (See ref. 87 for review.)Intracranial recordings from patients with epilepsy showpotentials functionally similar to the scalp N400 in the ante-rior fusiform gyrus (91).

Syntactic Analysis

The processing of language at a syntactic level is indexedby a several ERP components, both negative and positive.Many, although not all, syntactic violations elicit a late posi-tivity variously called the P600 or the syntactic positive shift(SPS) (92–94). The P600 is typically elicited when someaspect of sentence structure violates the rules of the lan-guage—for example, if the subject of the sentence does notagree with its verb in number or if a word in a phrase isout of order. The P600 also may be elicited when processingdifficulties arise at a structural level (87). Some researchershave proposed that the P600 belongs to the family of P3waves (95). In addition to the P600, many syntactic viola-tions also elicit a left anterior negativity (LAN), which someresearchers have interpreted as an index of working memoryusage (96,97).

The fact that an N400 or P600 is elicited shortly aftera semantically anomalous or grammatically incorrect word,respectively, regardless of its ordinal position in a sentence,is most consistent with those models of sentence processingthat emphasize the immediate and online nature of compre-

hension (98). That is, the language processing system seemsto use all information as it becomes available, often to pre-dict what words or ideas are likely to come next (89,99).That processing at a semantic and syntactic level yields dif-ferent patterns of electrophysiologic activity suggests thatthe processes differ, if not the representations. Further, thepresence of different ERP patterns to various syntactic viola-tions indicates that syntax is not a unitary phenomenonmediated by a single neural generator. Many aspects of sen-tence processing at semantic, syntactic, referential, thematic,prosodic, and discourse levels are indexed by transient ERPeffects and/or slow potentials that encompass the entire sen-tence (100,101). In short, the reported patterns of ERPeffects are inconsistent with a view of language comprehen-sion that gives syntactic analysis precedence over semanticanalysis or a system wherein syntactic processes are isolatedfrom all other processes. Instead, ERP data provide consid-erable evidence for parallel processing, interaction, and top-down effects during language processing. The brains ofreaders and listeners work very much online using all infor-mation as it becomes available to anticipate upcomingitems, concepts, and schemas to achieve the aim of an effi-cient and error-free understanding of the incoming language(even if at times these predictions may lead to misunder-standing).

Language Production

As in language comprehension, many of the controversies inlanguage production revolve around the issue of the relativetiming of the different levels of processing that are engaged.Although there is a general consensus that producing a co-herent utterance involves information at the levels of mean-ing, syntax, and phonetics there is no agreement as towhether meaning comes first and then phonologic form(i.e., a serial model), these processes overlap somewhat intime (i.e., a cascade model), or they unfold in parallel (102).Two ERP measures—the LRP and N200—can be used totrack the time course of information availability as peopleprepare to speak, even if they never actually utter a word.In studies using a two choice go/no-go paradigm, subjectswere shown a picture of an item on each trial about whichthey were asked to make two decisions (Fig. 32.5). Acrossexperiments, decisions were based on semantic, syntactic,and phonologic aspects of the pictured item and its name.The timing of the N200 and LRP on no-go trials indicatedthat semantic information becomes available before syntac-tic information (by about 80 msec), which is in turn avail-able before phonologic information (by about 40 msec)(103,104). Electrophysiologic data from the scalp thus sup-port a serial model of speech production, indicating thatpeople first figure out what they want to say and then chooseexactly how to say it.

Page 10: EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS AND MAGNETIC FIELDS IN THE … · 2018. 2. 11. · sensory, motor, or cognitive events are known as event-related potentials (ERPs) ... (13–15).Amoreseriousques-

Neuropsychopharmacology: The Fifth Generation of Progress436

FIGURE 32.5. Overlapped are the N200 difference waves (no-gominus go event-related potentials) recorded at amidline prefron-tal site (as marked on head icon) when the decision to respondor not (go/no-go) was contingent on a semantic (dashed line)versus a syntactic (solid line) attribute of the pictured object. Notethat the N200 effect contingent on the semantic analysis peaksaround 380 ms, whereas that contingent on syntax peaks around500 ms. Data from Schmitt B, Munte TF, Kutas M. Electrophysio-logical estimates of the time course of semantic and phonologicalencoding during implicit picture naming. Psychophysiology 2000Jul;37(4):473–484.

CONCLUSION

Specific components of ERPs and ERFs recorded from thesurface of the head are sensitive to a wide range of sensory,perceptual, motor, mnemonic, and linguistic processes. Itappears that many cognitive acts engender synchronousneural activity patterns that produce electrical and magneticfields precisely time-locked to informational transactions inthe brain. Recordings of ERPs/ERFs thus provide criticalinformation about the timing and neural substrates of theprocessing stages that underlie cognitive activity. Thesephysiologic data are being used increasingly to test alterna-tive functional models and to constrain psychological theo-ries (2,64,105).

Considerable progress has been made in demonstratingreliable associations between ERP/ERF components and awide range of psychopathologic syndromes. In no case, how-ever, is a single ERP/ERF component absent or abnormalin such a way as to be diagnostic. Rather, a given syndrome(e.g., schizophrenia) usually manifests abnormalities in oneor more parameters of several different ERP components,and a given component (e.g., the P3) appears abnormalacross a range of neurobehavioral syndromes. This is to beexpected, except in the unlikely (and perhaps nonexistent)case in which the psychopathology would only affect a sin-gle, isolated cognitive subprocess that had a unique ERP/ERF marker. Thus, instead of seeking a single ERP marker,it seems more likely that various patient populations willbe distinguished by different profiles of ERP effects acrossa number of different tasks (much like the approach takenin neuropsychological testing). Many of the same interpreta-tional issues that are of concern with neuropsychologicaltesting may become relevant for testing with an ERP bat-tery, together with some that are specific to these physiologicmeasures. For example, the considerable synaptic plasticityof the neocortex suggests that even normal individuals’ com-

ponent amplitudes and latencies are likely to show consider-able variability, depending on their life experiences. Suchvariability within the normal population clearly exacerbatesthe difficulty of uniquely identifying ERP/ERF markers ofspecific clinical syndromes. Further progress in achievingdiagnostic specificity and sensitivity may require comparingERPs/ERFs across multiple tasks in each patient to revealreliable abnormalities that are related to specific cognitivemanipulations. Such ERP/ERF abnormalities will becomeincreasingly informative about the specific processing mech-anisms that are dysfunctional in patient groups as the cogni-tive specificity of the distinctive components is sharpenedthrough studies in normals and as better methods are devel-oped for measuring and isolating those components. Thesedevelopments should make it possible to incorporate ERP/ERF data into multimeasure diagnostic batteries to aid inclassifying and subtyping psychopathologic syndromes.

Recent technical advances have made it possible to obtainmore accurate information about the neural bases of ERPs/ERFs and their relationships with cognitive and behavioralvariables. The neural generators of surface recorded ERP/ERF activity can be localized with increased precision usingalgorithms that exploit more accurate bioelectric models ofthe head and constrain the generators to lie within the corti-cal mantle as reconstructed from MRI scans. (See ref. 105for review.) Source localizations can be further improvedby incorporating functional imaging data (e.g., from fMRI)into the inverse calculations, thereby providing a more ve-ridical picture of the spatiotemporal patterning of cognitive-related brain activity (3,4,106). New approaches also havebeen developed for decomposing these complex patternsof brain activity arising from multiple, concurrently activegenerators into functionally meaningful subcomponents.Among these, the technique of Independent ComponentAnalysis (107) has shown considerable promise for decom-posing ERP data sets from multiple task conditions intotemporally independent and spatially localizable compo-nents that may be related to cognitive operations on theone hand and to fMRI activation patterns on the other.Newer spatiotemporal filtering procedures (e.g., wavelet fil-tering) have improved our ability to extract the ERP/ERFsignal from ongoing brain activity and other backgroundnoise. (See ref. 105 for review.) These methods ultimatelymay allow reliable detection of event-related signals on asingle-trial basis without relying on the usual computer aver-aging procedure. Single-trial analyses are important not onlyfor achieving a closer correspondence between brain activityand behavioral performance but also for ascertaining thedegree of trial-to-trial variability that may characterize dif-ferent clinical syndromes. All of these techniques will sub-stantially increase the utility of ERP/ERF recordings foranalyzing the neural bases of both normal and disorderedcognition.

Page 11: EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS AND MAGNETIC FIELDS IN THE … · 2018. 2. 11. · sensory, motor, or cognitive events are known as event-related potentials (ERPs) ... (13–15).Amoreseriousques-

Chapter 32: Event-Related Potentials and Magnetic Fields 437

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This work was supported by NIH grants MH-25594, HD-22614, and AG-08313. We thank Carole Montejano, MattMarlow, and Tom Urbach for assistance with manuscriptpreparation.

REFERENCES

1. Hillyard SA. Electrical and magnetic brain recordings: contribu-tions to cognitive neuroscience. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1993;3:217–224.

2. Kutas M, Dale A. Electrical and magnetic readings of mentalfunctions. In: Rugg MD, ed. Cognitive neuroscience.Cambridge,MA: MIT Press, 1997:197–242.

3. Dale AM, Liu AK, Fischl BR, et al. Dynamic statistical paramet-ric mapping: combining fMRI and MEG for high-resolutionimaging of cortical activity. Neuron 2000;26:55–67.

4. Schmidt DM, George JS, Wood CC. Bayesian inference appliedto the electromagnetic inverse problem. Hum Brain Map 1999;7:195–212.

5. Clark VP, Fan S, Hillyard SA. Identification of early visuallyevoked potential generators by retinotopic and topographicanalyses. Hum Brain Map 1995;2:170–187.

6. Kenemans JL, Baas JMP, Mangun GR, et al. On the processingof spatial frequencies as revealed by evoked-potential sourcemodeling. Clin Neurophys 2000;111:1113–1123.

7. Pantev C, Elbert T, Ross B, et al. Binaural fusion and therepresentation of virtual pitch in the human auditory cortex.Hear Res 1996;100:164–170.

8. Lu ZL, Williamson SJ, Kaufman L. Behavioral lifetime ofhuman auditory sensory memory predicted by physiologicalmeasures. Science 1993;258:1668–1670.

9. Adler LE, Pachtman E, Franks RD, et al. Neurophysiologicalevidence for a defect in neural mechanisms involved in sensorygating in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 1982;17:639–654.

10. Freedman R, Adler LE, Gerhardt GA, et al. Neurobiologicalstudies of sensory gating in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1987;13:669–678.

11. Clementz BA, Geyer MA, Braff DL. P50 suppression amongschizophrenia and normal comparison subjects: a methodologi-cal analysis. Biol Psychiatry 1997;41:1035–1044.

12. Patterson JV, Gierczak M, Hetrick WP, et al. Effects of temporalvariability on P50 and the gating ratio in schizophrenia. ArchGen Psychiatry 2000;57:57–64.

13. Kathmann N, Engel RR. Sensory gating in normals and schizo-phrenics: a failure to find strong P50 suppression in normals.Biol Psychiatry 1990;27:1216–1226.

14. Guterman Y, Josiassen RC. Sensory gating deviance in schizo-phrenia in the context of task related effects. Int J Psychophysiol1994;18:1–12.

15. Jin Y WE, Bunney J, Sandman CA, et al. Is P50 suppressiona measure of sensory gating in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry1998;43:873–878.

16. Jin Y, Potkin SG, Patterson JV, et al. Effects of P50 temporalvariability on sensory gating in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res1997;70:71–81.

17. Jerger K, Biggins C, Fein G. P50 suppression is not affected byattentional manipulations. Biol Psychiatry 1992;31:365–377.

18. Naatanen R. The mismatch negativity: a powerful tool for cog-nitive neuroscience. Ear Hear 1995;16:6–18.

19. Naatanen R, Escera C. Mismatch negativity: clinical and otherapplications. Audiology Neuro-Otology 2000;5:105–110.

20. Picton TW, Alain C, Otten L, et al. Mismatch negativity: differ-ent water in the same river. Audiology Neuro-Otology 2000;5:111–139.

21. Kraus N, Cheour M. Speech sound representation in the brain.Audiology Neuro-Otology 2000;5:140–150.

22. Naatanen R, Alho K. Mismatch negativity: the measure forcentral sound representation accuracy. Audiology Neuro-Otology1997;2:341–352.

23. Ritter W, Deacon D, Gomes H, et al. The mismatch negativityof event-related potentials as a probe of transient auditory mem-ory: a review. Ear Hear 1995;16:52–67.

24. Gene-Cos N, Ring HA, Pottinger RC, et al. Possible roles formismatch negativity in neuropsychiatry.Neuropsychiatry Neuro-psychol Behav Neurol 1999;12:17–27.

25. Escera C, Alho K, Schroger E, et al. Involuntary attention anddistractibility as evaluated with event-related brain potentials.Audiology Neuro-Otology 2000;5:151–166.

26. Csepe V, Molnar M. Towards the possible clinical application ofthe mismatch negativity component of event-related potentials.Audiology Neuro-Otology 1997;2:354–369.

27. Javitt DC. Intracortical mechanisms of mismatch negativity dys-function in schizophrenia. Audiology Neuro-Otology 2000;5:207–215.

28. Michie PT, Budd TW, Todd J, et al. Duration and frequencymismatch negativity in schizophrenia. Clin Neurophys 2000;111:1054–1065.

29. Pekkonen E. Mismatch negativity in aging and in Alzheimer’sand Parkinson’s diseases. Audiology Neuro-Otology 2000;5:216–224.

30. Naatanen R. Attention and brain function.Hillsdale, New Jersey:LEA, 1992.

31. Hillyard SA, Mangun GR, Woldorff MG, et al. Neural systemsmediating selective attention. In: Gazzaniga MS, ed. The cogni-tive neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1995:665–681.

32. Woods DL, Alho K, Algazi A. Stages of auditory feature con-junctions: an event-related brain potential study. J Exp PsycholHum Percept Perform 1994;20:81–94.

33. Michie PT, Fox AM, Ward PB, et al. Event-related potentialindices of selective attention and cortical lateralization in schizo-phrenia. Psychophysiology 1990;27:209–228.

34. Teder-Salejarvi WA, Hillyard SA, Roder B, et al. Spatial atten-tion to central and peripheral auditory stimuli as indexed byevent-related potentials (ERPs). Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 1999;8:213–227.

35. Roder B, Teder-Salejarvi W, Sterr A, et al. Improved auditoryspatial tuning in blind humans. Nature 1999;400:162–166.

36. Desimone R. Visual attention mediated by biased competitionin extrastriate visual cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci1998;353:1245–1255.

37. Hillyard SA, Anllo-Vento L. Event-related brain potentials inthe study of visual selective attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA1998;95:781–787.

38. Hillyard SA, Vogel EK, Luck SJ. Sensory gain control (amplifi-cation) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiologi-cal and neuroimaging evidence. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B BiolSci 1998;353:1257–1270.

39. Posner MI, Gilbert CD. Attention and primary visual cortex.Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1999;96:2585–2587.

40. Tootell RBH, Hadjikhani N, Hall EK, et al. The retinotomyof visual spatial attention. Neuron 1998;21:1409–1422.

41. Martınez A, Anllo-Vento L, Sereno MI, et al. Involvement ofstriate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention.Nature Neurosci 1999:364–369.

42. Muller MM, Teder-Salejarvi WA, Hillyard SA. The time course

Page 12: EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS AND MAGNETIC FIELDS IN THE … · 2018. 2. 11. · sensory, motor, or cognitive events are known as event-related potentials (ERPs) ... (13–15).Amoreseriousques-

Neuropsychopharmacology: The Fifth Generation of Progress438

of cortical facilitation during cued shifts of spatial attention.Nature Neurosci 1998;1:631–634.

43. Hillyard SA, Teder-Salejarvi WA, Munte TF. Temporal dynam-ics of early perceptual processing. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1998;8:202–210.

44. Deecke L, Scheid P, Kornhuber HH. Distribution of readinesspotential, pre-motion positivity, and motor potential of thehuman cerebral cortex preceding voluntary finger movements.Exp Brain Res 1969;7:158–168.

45. Bocker KB, Brunia CH, Cluitmans PJ. A spatio-temporal dipolemodel of the readiness potential in humans. II. Foot movement.Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1994;91:286–294.

46. Kutas M, Donchin E. Preparation to respond as manifested bymovement-related brain potentials. Brain Res 1980;202:95–115.

47. Coles MGH, Smid HGOM, Scheffers MK, et al. Mental chro-nometry and the study of human information processing. In:Rugg MD, Coles MGH, eds. Electrophysiology of mind: event-related brain potentials and cognition.New York: Oxford Univer-sity Press, 1995:86–131.

48. Fabiani M, Gratton G, Coles MGH. Event-related brain poten-tials methods, theory and applications. In: Cacioppo J, Tassi-nary L, Bernston G, eds. Handbook of psychophysiology. Cam-bridge: Cambridge University Press, 2000:53–84.

49. Oishi M, Mochizuki Y, Du C, et al. Contingent negative varia-tion and movement-related cortical potentials in Parkinsonism.Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1995;95:346–349.

50. Dick JP, Rothwell JC, Day BL, et al. The Bereitschaftspotentialis abnormal in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 1989;112:233–244.

51. Cunnington R, Iansek R, Bradshaw JL. Movement-related po-tentials in Parkinson’s disease: external cues and attentionalstrategies. Mov Disord 1999;14:63–68.

52. Adler LE, Pecevich M, Nagamoto H. Bereitschaftspotential intardive dyskinesia. Mov Disord 1989;4:105–112.

53. Trenkwalder C, Bucher SF, Oertel WH, et al. Bereitschafts-potential in idiopathic and symptomatic restless legs syndrome.Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1993;89:95–103.

54. Scheffers MK, Coles MG. Performance monitoring in a confus-ing world: error-related brain activity, judgements of responseaccuracy, and types of errors. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform2000;26:141–151.

55. Gehring WJ, Knight RT. Prefrontal-cingulate interactions inaction monitoring. Nature Neurosci 2000;3:516–520.

56. Luu P, Collins P, Tucker DM. Mood, personality, and self-monitoring: negative affect and emotionality in relation to fron-tal lobe mechanisms of error monitoring. J Exp Psychol Gen2000;129:43–60.

57. Dikman ZV, Allen JJ. Error monitoring during reward andavoidance learning in high- and low-socialized individuals. Psy-chophysiology 2000;37:43–54.

58. Donchin E, Coles MGH. Is the P300 component a manifesta-tion of context updating. Behav Brain Science 1988;11:357–374.

59. Verleger R. Event-related potentials and cognition: a critiqueof the context updating hypothesis and an alternative interpreta-tion of P3. Behav Brain Science 1988;11:343–427.

60. Picton TW. The P300 wave of the human event-related poten-tial. J Clin Neurophysiol 1992;9:456–479.

61. Polich J, Kok A. Cognitive and biological determinants of P300:an integrative review. Biol Psychol 1995;41:103–146.

62. Bashore TR, Ridderinkhof KR, van der Molen MW. The de-cline of cognitive processing speed in old age.Curr Direct PsycholScience 1997;6:163–169.

63. Luck SJ. Sources of dual-task interference: evidence fromhuman electrophysiology. Psychol Sci 1998;9:223–227.

64. Rugg MD, Coles MGH, eds. Electrophysiology of mind. Oxford:Oxford University Press, 1995.

65. McCarthy G, Wood CC, Williamson PD, et al. Task-depen-dent field potentials in human hippocampal formation. J Neu-rosci 1989;9:4253–4268.

66. Frodl-Bauch T, Bottlender R, Hegerl U. Neurochemical sub-strates and neuroanatomic generators of the event-related P300.Neuropsychobiology 1999;40:86–94.

67. Luck SJ, Hillyard SA. The operation of selective attention atmultiple stages of processing: evidence from human and monkeyelectrophysiology. In: Gazzaniga MS, ed. The new cognitive neu-rosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2000:687–700.

68. Knight RT, Nakada T. Cortico-limbic circuits and novelty: areview of EEG and blood flow data. Rev Neurosci 1998;9:57–70.

69. Ruchkin DS, Berndt RS, Johnson RJ, et al. Modality-specificprocessing streams in verbal working memory: evidence fromspatio-temporal patterns of brain activity. Brain Res Cogn BrainRes 1997;6:95–113.

70. Karis D, Fabiani M, Donchin E. ‘‘P300’’ and memory: individ-ual differences in the von Restorff effect. Cog Psychol 1984;16:177–216.

71. Paller KA, Kutas M, Mayes AR. Neural correlates of encodingin an incidental learning paradigm. Electroencephalogr Clin Neu-rophysiol 1987;67:360–371.

72. Paller KA. Recall and stem-completion priming have differentelectrophysiological correlates and are modified differentially bydirected forgetting. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 1990;16:1021–1032.

73. Van Petten C, Senkfor AJ. Memory for words and novel visualpatterns: repetition, recognition and encoding effects in theevent-related brain potential. Psychophysiology 1996;33:491–506.

74. Wagner AD, Koutstaal W, Schacter DL. When encoding yieldsremembering: insights from event-related neuroimaging. PhilosTrans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1999;354:1307–1324.

75. Duzel E, Cabeza R, Picton TW, et al. Task-related and item-related brain processes of memory retrieval. Proc Natl Acad SciUSA 1999;96:1794–1799.

76. Rugg MD, Allan K. Event-related potential studies of memory.In: Tulving E, Craik FIM, eds. The Oxford handbook of memory.New York: Oxford University Press, 2000:521–537.

77. Rosler F, Heil M, Henninghausen E. Slow potentials duringlong-term memory retrieval. In: Heinze HJ, Munte TF, Man-gun GR, eds. Cognitive electrophysiology. Boston: Birkhauser,1994:149–168.

78. Olichney J, Van Petten C, Paller KA, et al. Word repetition inamnesia: electrophysiological measures of impaired and sparedmemory. Brain 2000;123:1948–1963.

79. Rugg MD, Walla P, Schloerscheidt AM, et al. Neural correlatesof depth of processing effects on recollection: evidence frombrain potentials and positron emission tomography. Exp BrainRes 1998;123:18–23.

80. Paller KA, Gross M. Brain potentials associated with perceptualpriming vs explicit remembering during the repetition of visualword-form. Neuropsychologia 1998;36:559–571.

81. Senkfor AJ, Van Petten C. Who said what? An event-relatedpotential investigation of source and item memory. J Exp PsycholLearn Mem Cogn 1998;24:1005–1025.

82. Rubin SR, Petten CV, Glisky EL, et al. Memory conjunctionerrors in younger and older adults: event-related potential andneuropsychological data. Cog Neuropsychol 1999;16:459–488.

83. Wilding EL, Rugg MD. An event-related potential study ofrecognition memory with and without retrieval of source. Brain1996;119:889–905.

84. Janowsky JS, Shimamura AP, Squire LR. Memory and meta-

Page 13: EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS AND MAGNETIC FIELDS IN THE … · 2018. 2. 11. · sensory, motor, or cognitive events are known as event-related potentials (ERPs) ... (13–15).Amoreseriousques-

Chapter 32: Event-Related Potentials and Magnetic Fields 439

memory: comparisons between patients with frontal lobe lesionsand amnesic patients. Psychobiology 1989;17:3–11.

85. Kutas M, Hillyard SA. Reading senseless sentences: brain poten-tials reflect semantic incongruity. Science 1980;207:203–205.

86. Van Petten C, Kutas M. Influences of semantic and syntacticcontext on open and closed class words. Mem Cogn 1991;19:95-112.

87. Kutas M, Federmeier K, Coulson S, et al. Language. In: Caci-oppo J, Tassinary L, Bernston G, eds.Handbook of psychophysiol-ogy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2000:576–601.

88. van Berkum JJA, Hagoort P, Brown CM. Semantic integrationin sentences and discourse: evidence from the N400. J CogNeurosci 1999;11:657–671.

89. Federmeier KD, Kutas M. Right words and left words: electro-physiological evidence for hemispheric differences in languageprocessing. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 1999;8:373–392.

90. Swaab TY, Brown CM, Hagoort P. Spoken sentence compre-hension in aphasia: event-related potential evidence for a lexicalintegration deficit. J Cog Neurosci 1997;9:39–66.

91. Nobre AC, Allison T, McCarthy G. Word recognition in thehuman inferior temporal lobe. Nature 1994;372:260–263.

92. Osterhout L, Holcomb PJ. Event-related brain potentials elic-ited by syntactic anomaly. J Mem Lang 1992;3:785–806.

93. Hagoort P, Brown CM, Groothusen J. The syntactic positiveshift (SPS) as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. LangCogn Proc 1993;8:439–483.

94. Munte TF, Matzke M, Johannes S. Brain activity associatedwith syntactic incongruencies in words and pseudo-words. J CogNeurosci 1997;9:318–329.

95. Coulson S, King JW, Kutas M. Expect the unexpected: event-related brain response to morphosyntactic violations. Lang CognProc 1998;13:21–58.

96. Hahne A, Friederici AD. Electrophysiological evidence for twosteps in syntactic analysis: early automatic and late controlledprocesses. J Cog Neurosci 1999;11:194–205.

Neuropsychopharmacology: The Fifth Generation of Progress. Edited by Kenneth L. Davis, Dennis Charney, Joseph T. Coyle, andCharles Nemeroff. American College of Neuropsychopharmacology � 2002.

97. Kluender R, Kutas M. Bridging the gap: evidence from ERPson the processing of unbounded dependencies. J Cog Neurosci1993;5:196–214.

98. Just MA, Carpenter PA. A theory of reading: from eye fixationsto comprehension. Psychol Rev 1980;87:329–354.

99. Van Petten C, Coulson S, Rubin S, et al. Time course of wordidentification and semantic integration in spoken language. JExp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 1999;25:394–417.

100. Steinhauer K, Alter K, Friederici AD. Brain potentials indicateimmediate use of prosodic cues in natural speech processing.Nature Neurosci 1999;2:191–196.

101. Munte TF, Schiltz K, Kutas M. When temporal terms belieconceptual order. Nature 1998;395:71–73.

102. Levelt WJM. Speaking: from intention to articulation. Cam-bridge: MIT Press, 1989.

103. Schmitt B, Munte TF, Kutas M. Electrophysiological estimatesof the time course of semantic and phonological encoding dur-ing implicit picture naming. Psychophysiology 2000 Jul;37:473–484.

104. van Turennout M, Hagoort P, Brown CM. Electrophysiologicalevidence on the time course of semantic and phonological pro-cesses in speech production. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn1997;23:787–806.

105. Munte TF, Urbach TP, Duzel E, et al. Event-related brainpotentials in the study of human cognition and neuropsychol-ogy. In: Boller F, Grafman J, Rizzolatti G, eds. Handbook ofneuropsychology. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science, 2000:1–97.

106. Ahlfors SP, Simpson GV, Dale AM, et al. Spatio-temporal activ-ity of a cortical network for processing visual motion revealedby MEG and fMRI. J Neurophys 1999;82:2545–2555.

107. Makeig S, Westerfield M, Jung T-P, et al. Functionally indepen-dent components of the late positive event-related potential dur-ing visual spatial attention. J Neurosci 1999;19:2665–2680.

Page 14: EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS AND MAGNETIC FIELDS IN THE … · 2018. 2. 11. · sensory, motor, or cognitive events are known as event-related potentials (ERPs) ... (13–15).Amoreseriousques-