Erentaitė, R., Vosylis, R., Gabrialavičiūtė, I., Raižienė ... and school.pdf · 6 IDENTITY...

39
1 IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT Erentaitė, R., Vosylis, R., Gabrialavičiūtė, I., & Raižienė, S. (2018). How does school experience relate to adolescent identity formation over time? Cross-Lagged associations between school engagement, school burnout and identity processing styles. Journal of Youth and Adolescence 47(4), 760-774, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-017-0806-1 This is a post-peer-review, pre-copyedit version of an article published in the Journal of Youth and Adolescence. The final authenticated version is available at: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-017-0806-1

Transcript of Erentaitė, R., Vosylis, R., Gabrialavičiūtė, I., Raižienė ... and school.pdf · 6 IDENTITY...

1

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

Erentaitė, R., Vosylis, R., Gabrialavičiūtė, I., & Raižienė, S. (2018). How does school experience relate

to adolescent identity formation over time? Cross-Lagged associations between school engagement,

school burnout and identity processing styles. Journal of Youth and Adolescence 47(4), 760-774,

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-017-0806-1

This is a post-peer-review, pre-copyedit version of an article published in the Journal of Youth and

Adolescence. The final authenticated version is available at: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-017-0806-1

2

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

Running Head: IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

How does School Experience Relate to Adolescent Identity Formation Over Time? Cross-Lagged

Associations between School Engagement, School Burnout and Identity Processing Styles

Rasa Erentaitė1,2, Rimantas Vosylis2, Ingrida Gabrialavičiūtė2, Saulė Raižienė2

1 Kaunas University of Technology, Kaunas, Lithuania

2 Mykolas Romeris University, Vilnius, Lithuania

Authors' Contributions

Authors’ contributions: RE conceived of the study, participated in its design and implementation, and

drafted the manuscript; RV participated in the design and implementation of the study, performed the

statistical analysis, and contributed to drafting of the manuscript; IG participated in the design and

implementation of the study, and contributed to drafting of the manuscript; SR participated in the

design and coordination of the study and contributed to drafting of the manuscript. All authors read and

approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Erasmus+ Programme of the European Union, strategic partnership

project “Innovative Curriculum for Strong Identities in Diverse Europe (INSIDE)”, No. 2016-1-LT01-

KA203-023220 and the European Social Fund under the Global Grant measure, No. VP1-3.1-ŠMM-

07-K-02-008.

3

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

Abstract

The existing research findings still do not provide a clear understanding of the links between adolescent

school experience and their identity formation. To address this gap, we analyzed the dynamic links

between adolescent school experiences and identity formation by exploring the cross-lagged

associations between school engagement, school burnout and identity processing styles (information-

oriented, normative and diffuse-avoidant) over a two-year period during middle-to-late adolescence.

The sample of this school-based study included 916 adolescents (51.4% females) in the 9th to 12th

grades from diverse socio-economic and family backgrounds. The results from the cross-lagged

analyses with three time points revealed that (a) school engagement positively predicted information-

oriented identity processing over a two-year period; (b) school burnout positively predicted the reliance

on normative and diffuse-avoidant identity styles across the three measurements; (c) the effects were

stable over the three time points and across different gender, grade, and socio-economic status groups.

The unidirectional effects identified in our study support the general prediction that active engagement

in learning at school can serve as a resource for adolescent identity formation, while school burnout, in

contrast, can hinder the formation of adolescent identity. This points to the importance of taking

developmental identity-related needs of adolescents into account when planning the school curriculum.

Keywords: Identity Styles; School Engagement; School Burnout; Adolescence

4

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

How does School Experience Relate to Adolescent Identity Formation Over Time? Cross-Lagged

Associations between School Engagement, School Burnout and Identity Processing Styles

Introduction

Adolescents strive to form a personal identity through continual relations with their social and

cultural environment (Erikson, 1968). The formation of identity means building a sense of personal

uniqueness and continuity by integrating personal goals, ideals, values, and roles into a personally

meaningful and coherent picture (Erikson, 1968). Though identity formation is often operationalized

through individual-level processes of exploration (i.e., considering personal relevance of various

alternatives) and commitment (personal investment in some of the available options) (Kroger & Marcia,

2011), it is increasingly emphasized that these individual-level processes should be studied as part of

continuous person-context interactions (Bosma & Kunnen, 2008; Eichas, Meca, Montgomery, &

Kurtines, 2015). A number of adolescent identity scholars stress the importance of the school context

and its strong potential for developmental implications related to identity (Flum & Kaplan, 2006, 2012;

Harrell-Levy & Kerpelman, 2010; Lannegrand-Willems & Bosma, 2006).

Indeed, certain aspects of school experience are related to adolescent identity formation. In

particular, higher academic success at school was found to predict stronger educational commitments,

while low achievement predicted difficulties in finding satisfactory commitments (Pop, Negru-

Subtirica, Crocetti, Opre, & Meeus, 2016). Beyond the academic achievement, perceiving the teachers

as role models and studies as meaningful was related to a higher identity exploration and more

confidence in identity commitments in middle and late adolescence (Rich & Schachter, 2012). A more

positive school experience in terms of a more favorable school self-image, better integration in the peer

group and the use of active coping strategies was related to a stronger school identity over the course of

the 8th grade (Lannegrand-Willems & Bosma, 2006). Perceptions of higher basic needs support and

facilitation of identity exploration in school were also related to more advances in adolescent identity

5

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

formation (Madjar & Cohen-Malayev, 2013). Taken together, these findings point to the importance of

the school context for adolescent identity formation.

On the other hand, some aspects of adolescent personal identity may contribute to shaping their

school experiences. Teaching methods based on students’ personal experiences and self-reflection

stimulated adolescent identity exploration, while at the same time, strengthened the students’ academic

motivation and engagement in learning at school (Sinai, Kaplan, & Flum, 2012). Similarly, consistent

efforts to promote identity exploration among the students helped to better engage them in learning

activities even in a challenging educational context (Faircloth, 2012). Thus, despite the predominance

of the findings on the effects of adolescent school experience on their identity formation, the opposite

direction of the link has also received some empirical support.

Taken together, the existing research findings point to a possibility of a reciprocal relationship

between the school experience and identity formation in adolescence. However, the majority of the

previous studies in the field did not take this possibility into account, hence, a clear answer regarding

the extent and directionality of the links in focus is still missing. The main limitations of the studies in

this field include the reliance on cross-sectional data, the use of unidirectional data analytical strategies,

as well as a lack of tools (e.g., in qualitative and cross-sectional studies) to disentangle the potentially

complex dynamic effects between adolescent identity and their school experience. In order to address

these limitations, we employ a strategy, which enables to account for a possible dynamic reciprocal

nature of the effects in focus. Particularly, we explore the longitudinal cross-lagged associations

between the aspects of school experience (school engagement and school burnout) and identity

formation in middle to late adolescence.

The Importance of Supporting Adolescent Identity Formation

In adolescence, identity formation is enabled through such capacities as increasingly complex

socio-emotional regulation and advanced cognitive functioning (Berzonsky, 2004, 2008, 2011; Crone

6

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

& Dahl, 2012; Erikson, 1968). These developing capacities enable adolescents to engage in self-

discovery (i.e., identify activities and choices, which resonate with one’s abilities, interests, and

potential) and self-construction (i.e., testing hypotheses and making decisions related to identity)

(Eichas et al., 2015). School context is the environment where the developing cognitive and emotional

capacities, used in identity formation processes, can be advanced further, e.g., through curriculum-

based academic activities or different forms of social engagement.

Despite the stimulating potential of the school context, identity formation is a challenging process

for a substantial share of adolescents. For example, the adolescents with increased risk for externalizing

and internalizing difficulties (Crocetti, Klimstra, Hale, Koot, & Meeus, 2013; Crocetti, Klimstra,

Keijsers, Hale, & Meeus, 2009), or from schools with low socio-economic background (Lannegrand-

Willems & Bosma, 2006), can struggle to develop a coherent system of goals, values and beliefs or

avoid engaging in identity exploration entirely. Based on the meta-analysis by Kroger, Martinussen,

and Marcia (2010), the majority of high school graduates leave school without clear identity

commitments. While normative identity development is expected to continue into emerging adulthood

and beyond (Arnett, 2014), the difficulties of engaging in identity exploration and failure to achieve

certain identity commitments during adolescence may hinder favorable development and self-

regulation (Eichas et al., 2015). The challenging choices that adolescents face during the late high-

school years, such as choosing educational and occupational paths, starting romantic relationships,

require at least some identity commitments in place in order to make the important life decisions

personally meaningful, purposeful and self-directed.

In this context, the search for factors and mechanisms, which could serve as resources for

adolescent identity formation in schools, remains highly relevant. The focus on particular resources

depends on how the desirable outcomes of identity formation are defined, which, in turn, depends on

the overall conceptualization of adolescent identity formation. From a variety of approaches used to

7

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

study identity in the school context, we consider the social-cognitive perspective (Berzonsky, 1994,

2004, 2008, 2011) to be particularly relevant, since it can help to understand which, and how, identity-

relevant capacities could be advanced at school and subsequently used by adolescents to facilitate their

identity formation, in particular, their self-construction processes.

Social-Cognitive Perspective on Identity Formation

The social-cognitive perspective on identity formation proposed by Berzonsky (1989, 2011)

builds on cognitive-experiential self-theory (CEST; Epstein, 2003), which postulates the existence of

two fundamental parallel operating information-processing systems. According to Epstein (2003), the

first, experiential system is a rapid information processing system that does it in a way that is holistic,

automatic, affective, associative, and minimally demanding of cognitive resources. In contrast, the

rational information processing system is a slow one and operates in a manner that is analytic,

intentional, logical, abstract, and requires significantly more effort. The experiential system is more

efficient and economical compared to a rational system, which makes it the one that people use more

often in their daily lives. However, being automatic, the experiential system is more prone to cognitive

bias and subjective distortions (Epstein, 2003).

Both of these processing systems play a role in identity formation (Berzonsky, 2011; Epstein,

2003), and the social-cognitive perspective on identity formation focuses on how individuals use these

systems when dealing with identity conflicts and issues, when revising a sense of identity, processing

self-relevant information, making identity-related decisions, etc. (Berzonsky, 2004, 2011). Identity

styles refer to the different approaches to dealing with identity issues. Three identity content processing

orientations or identity styles are distinguished: (1) information-oriented, (2) normative, and (3)

diffuse-avoidant (Berzonsky, 1994, 2004, 2011).

An information-oriented style mostly reflects the use of rational processing system for dealing

with identity issues, however, individuals who adopt this style may also use the experiential processing

8

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

system (Berzonsky, 2008). Specifically, information-oriented style is characterized by an engagement

in a process of identity explorations, i.e., individuals who use this strategy seek out, process and

evaluate identity-relevant information prior to making committed decisions. They also show higher

levels of cognitive complexity, engage in problem-focused coping, are open to new information, and

are willing to revise aspects of their identity when faced with discrepant information (Berzonsky, 1994,

2004, 2011). As such, the information-oriented identity processing requires a lot of effort and

resources.

A normative style mostly reflects the predominant use of the experiential processing system when

dealing with identity issues (Berzonsky, 2008). Specifically, the normative style is characterized by

internalization and reliance on goals, values, and prescriptions derived from significant others. Those

who adopt this style deal with identity issues in a relatively automatic manner. They also tend to

conform to traditional opinions, and their main goal is to conserve and maintain self-views and to guard

against information that may threaten their values and beliefs (Berzonsky, 1994, 2004, 2011). The

normative identity processing style reflects low proactive effort in building one’s identity

commitments, it is thus less demanding in terms of cognitive resources than the information-oriented

identity processing style.

A diffuse-avoidant style also reflects the use of experiential processing system when dealing with

identity issues, however, the diffuse-avoidant style also reflects the avoidance of using the rational

processing system (Berzonsky, 2008). Diffuse-avoidant identity style is characterized by a reluctance to

confront, as well as attempts to avoid and procrastinate when dealing with identity issues. When one is

eventually faced with a need to make a decision, situational demands and consequences eventually will

determine behavioral reactions (Berzonsky, 1994, 2004, 2011). Similarly to the normative identity

style, the diffuse-avoidant processing is also minimally demanding in terms of cognitive resources.

9

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

Numerous studies have established the links between identity styles and identity formation. A

meta-analysis, which summarized 27 studies (n=6563) that addressed the links between styles and the

strength of identity commitments, has indicated: a) a moderate positive association between identity

commitments and information oriented style, b) a moderate-to-large positive association between

commitments and normative style, and c) a moderate-to-large negative association between

commitment and diffuse-avoidant style (Bosch & Card, 2012). These results indicate that information-

oriented and normative styles play a key role in shaping personal identity commitments. Nevertheless,

identity commitments built through active exploration and evaluation of alternatives (i.e., information-

oriented processing) increase, while those based on normative identity processing decrease from

adolescence to adulthood (Bosch & Card, 2012).

Since most academic tasks and activities at school require increasingly advanced rational

processing, school experience may contribute to advances in information-oriented identity processing

over time. However, as emphasized by Flum and Kaplan (2006), the students themselves have to be

actively involved in exploring the academic tasks and their personal relevance in order to activate their

identity processes. Based on these considerations, we suggest that the students’ school functioning

may, to a certain extent, contribute to shaping their identity processing.

The Role of School Engagement and School Burnout in Identity Formation

School engagement can be seen as one aspect of effective functioning in the school context.

Engagement can be considered as commitment and investment in schooling or the qualities of a

student’s active participation in learning activities in the classroom (Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris,

2004). Most researchers define it as a multidimensional construct that consists of behavioral, emotional

and cognitive components (Fredricks et al., 2004). In other words, school engagement refers to

behaviors, emotions and mental efforts that reflect a motivation to master the academic material. All

dimensions of school engagement share a common feature of an observable outward expression. For

10

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

example, emotional reactions to schoolwork can be observed as verbal and nonverbal expressive signs,

strategic learning can be implied from a purposeful approach to academic materials, agentic

engagement is seen in students’ voicing their preferences. With regard to this commonality of all

aspects of school engagement, Hospel, Galand, and Janosz (2016) consider the behavioral component

of engagement as a key construct in school engagement studies. The behavioral component of

engagement can be described as observed behaviors of classroom involvement including effort,

persistence, concentration, attention, and contributing to the class discussion.

In contrast to engagement being an indicator of an effective functioning, ineffective functioning

at school can be described by school burnout, which is experienced as a result of a discrepancy between

individual resources and study demands (Upadyaya & Salmela-Aro, 2013). As such, burnout represents

difficulties to cope with achievement pressures. Its symptoms include feeling exhausted, having a

cynical or distal attitude towards studies and a sense of incompetence at school (Parker & Salmela-Aro,

2011; Upadyaya & Salmela-Aro, 2013). School burnout can be analyzed as an indicator of emotional

disengagement or disaffection at school (Tuominen-Soini & Salmela-Aro, 2014).

Although negatively related, school engagement and school burnout are not the opposite sides of

the same construct. Overall, the more burnout symptoms students experience, the less engaged in

school they are, but the absence of burnout symptoms does not automatically lead to an increase in

engagement. Tuominen-Soini & Salmela-Aro (2014) found that some high school students were

engaged in school and showed symptoms of burnout at the same time. In a study by Wang, Chow,

Hofkens, & Salmela-Aro (2015), engagement and school burnout had different longitudinal

associations with academic and psychological well-being. Thus, negative emotional processes involved

in school burnout have distinct psychological mechanisms in development of school related outcomes.

The outcomes of school engagement and burnout have received a lot of research interest. The

positive effect of engagement on school achievement has been well documented in cross-sectional (e.g.

11

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

Appleton, Christenson, Kim, and Reschly, 2006) and longitudinal studies (e.g. Wang & Eccles, 2012).

School burnout was found to be negatively associated with academic achievement (Kiuru, Aunola,

Nurmi, Leskinen, & Salmela-Aro, 2008; Tuominen-Soini & Salmela-Aro, 2014) and indicators of

academic well-being (Wang et al., 2015).

In addition to academic effects, school engagement has cognitive benefits for learning, as it

enhances student capacity for self-regulation and meta-cognition (Larson & Rusk, 2011; Lawson &

Lawson, 2013). Engaged students have opportunities to learn to manage emotions, keep sustained and

goal-directed attention, process information at a deeper level, plan, monitor, and evaluate their personal

progress during the learning process. By eliciting more positive interactions and support from teachers,

engagement also serves as a source of interpersonal attachments (Skinner & Pitzer, 2012). As a student

engages in school, he or she also develops a sense of belonging or connectedness to the school

community, as well as the skills that will carry through all of the life tasks and challenges.

Strong cognitive skills and supportive interpersonal relations play an important role in processing

identity-relevant information and enable to deal with identity conflicts and issues (Berzonsky, 2011).

Thus, we propose that positive benefits of school engagement extend to processing information related

to self and making identity choices. Particularly, we expect that school engagement can contribute to

facilitating more active identity exploration and, thus, to shaping information-oriented identity

processing style. In contrast, school burnout can be seen as an energy depleting experience that

diminishes possibility to use developmental resources. Thus, it should reduce active identity

exploration and prompt the reliance on less demanding identity processing (such as normative and

diffuse-avoidant identity styles), since in the face of high stress and pressures it may be more adaptive

to use the resources more sparingly.

Considering that school engagement and school burnout are both malleable through interactions

in academic and interpersonal contexts (Kiuru, et. al., 2008; Skinner & Pitzer, 2012), it is also

12

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

important to understand whether these aspects of school functioning respond to the changes in identity

processing. As suggested by Flum and Kaplan (2006), engagement in school can be facilitated by

triggering and supporting safe adolescent identity exploration in the school context. Thus, more active

identity exploration (i.e., the use of information-oriented identity processing) should strengthen the

students’ school engagement over time.

The Present Study

The existing literature does not yet provide a satisfactory understanding of the extent and

directionality of the links between adolescent school experience and their identity formation. Most of

the findings emphasize the effects of the school context on adolescent identity formation, but the

opposite links have also been supported. In our study, we aim to analyze the dynamic reciprocal links

between adolescent school experiences and identity formation by exploring the cross-lagged

associations between school engagement, school burnout and identity processing styles (information-

oriented, normative and diffuse-avoidant) over a two-year period during middle-to-late adolescence.

We investigated these links in a sample of Lithuanian adolescents in the grades 9th to 12th. The

structure of the Lithuanian educational system determines the students’ need for a choice of future

educational and vocational goals as early as the 10th grade. At that time the students have to decide

which educational track they would continue - upper secondary education (if so, then what kind of

curriculum they would like to choose) or vocational education and training. Making a personally

meaningful and self-directed choice requires an adolescent to be involved in identity exploration and

commitment making. Consequently, it is important to analyze students’ identity formation in the high

school context.

Schools can facilitate their students’ identity formation by academic tasks, which trigger and

support the students’ safe exploration of the personal meanings and experiences (Flum & Kaplan,

2006). However, a positive effect is possible only if students’ are actively engaged in learning at school

13

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

and deliberately reflect on the academic tasks and their personal meaning (Flum & Kaplan, 2006).

Moreover, higher students’ engagement in school stimulates their meta-cognitive, self-regulatory, and

interpersonal skills (Lawson & Lawson, 2013), necessary for active exploration of identity issues. We

thus hypothesized that higher students’ school engagement would predict, over time, a more intensive

and deliberate exploration of identity-related issues (i.e., a higher use of information-oriented identity

processing style).

On the other hand, experiences of exhaustion, disengagement from studies and a sense of

incompetence in school (i.e., school burnout) can limit the students’ potential to use their personal and

school-based resources for dealing with academic and personal challenges (Tuominen-Soini &

Salmela-Aro, 2014). Accordingly, we hypothesized that higher school burnout would predict a less

intensive use of information-oriented identity style and stronger reliance on ready-made solutions for

identity dilemmas (i.e., the use of normative identity processing style) or avoidance to engage with

identity issues (i.e., the use of diffuse-avoidant identity processing style).

Finally, an active exploration of the personal meanings in the school context can facilitate an in-

depth involvement in the learning process itself and promote a more complex and deeper learning

(Faircloth, 2012; Sinai et al., 2012). Thus, we hypothesized that higher use of information-oriented

identity style would predict stronger school engagement among adolescents.

Method

Participants and Procedure

The data come from the research project on positive youth development (POSIDEV) conducted

in Lithuania (Žukauskienė et. al., 2015). The study was based on a community sampling approach: all

students in the grades 9th to 12th from all five high schools in one municipality were invited to

participate. This municipality is medium sized and shares the same school system as the rest of the

country. The study is school-based, i.e., the participants were recruited and the data were collected in

14

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

five high schools, which participated in the three assessment waves, collected with a one-year interval.

The first assessment took place in 2013.

During the first meeting with study participants, they were informed about the purposes of the

study and that participation was voluntary. The parents were informed about the study through a

written letter and asked to contact the school or the investigators if they did not want their children to

participate. The questionnaires were administered by the researchers and research assistants at the

schools and were completed in classes during regular class hours. Participants were not paid for

participation, but all students who completed the questionnaires were eligible for a lottery reward.

More information about the procedures is provided in Crocetti, Erentaitė, & Žukauskienė (2014) and an

in-depth description of the procedures is provided in Žukauskienė et. al., (2015).

The two older cohorts (11th and 12th graders at T1) have had the school finished at the time of the

later assessments and did not receive the instruments that were used for this study. Therefore, for this

study, only the two younger cohorts were selected (9th and 10th graders at T1), i.e., 916 participants

(51.4% girls; Mage=15.65, SDage=0.73, range 14-17 years at T1). No further exclusions were made. The

response rate for these participants was 95.3% at T2, and 85.6% at T3. The sample was diverse in terms

of socioeconomic background, but ethnically more homogeneous than the overall Lithuanian

population (see Table 1). The sample was also diverse in terms of family structure. Specifically, the

majority of the participants lived with both biological parents (69%), while the rest lived with a mother

(18%), with a mother and a stepfather (7%) or in other family settings. Those not living with both

parents indicated that it was due to either a divorce (67%) or one of the parents was working and living

in a different country (15%), or had passed away (17%). Most of the participants (83%) indicated that

they had at least one sibling. In-depth description of the sample is provided in Žukauskienė et al.,

(2015).

Measures

15

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

Identity processing styles. The Identity Style Inventory (ISI-4; Luyckx, Lens, Smits, &

Goossens, 2010; Smits, et. al., 2009) was used to measure identity styles. The items were scored on a

scale ranging from 1 (not at all like me) to 5 (very much like me). Cronbach’s alphas were .80, .84, and

.84 for the information-oriented style (7 items; e.g., “When facing a life decision, I take into account

different points of view before making a choice”); .60, .67, and .65 for the normative style (8 items;

e.g., “I prefer to deal with situations where I can rely on social norms and standards”); .71, .78 and .76,

for the diffuse-avoidant style (9 items; e.g., “I’m not really thinking about my future now; it’s still a

long way off”), at T1, T2, and T3, respectively. More details about the translation procedures and

psychometric features of the Lithuanian version of ISI-4 are provided in Crocetti, Erentaitė, and

Žukauskienė (2014).

School engagement. A behavioral school engagement subscale of the Engagement vs.

Disaffection in School questionnaire (Ryzin, Gravely, & Roseth, 2007) was used to measure school

engagement. The subscale consists of 10 items (e.g., “In school, I work as hard as I can”), which were

scored on a scale ranging from 1 (not at all true) to 4 (very much true). Cronbach’s alphas were .81 at

T1, .80 at T2, and .80 at T3. The Lithuanian version of this scale was prepared by one of the co-authors

of this article and had already been used in several earlier longitudinal studies. During preparation, a

back-translation was also made to reveal any inconsistencies, which were then corrected. More details

about the psychometric features of the Lithuanian version of this instrument are provided in

Žukauskienė et. al., (2015).

School burnout. The School Burnout Inventory (SBI; Salmela-Aro, Kiuru, Leskinen, & Nurmi,

2009) was used to measure burnout. The items were scored on a scale ranging from 1 (completely

disagree) to 6 (completely agree). The scale consists of 10 items measuring exhaustion at school (e.g.,

“I feel overwhelmed by my schoolwork”), cynicism toward the meaning of school (e.g., “I feel that I

am losing interest in my schoolwork”), and a sense of inadequacy at school (e.g., “I often have feelings

16

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

of inadequacy in my schoolwork”), which reflect an overall school burnout. In this study, we were

interested only in the overall school burnout score and for this Cronbach’s alphas were .84 at T1, .86 at

T2, and .87 at T3. The Lithuanian versions of the School Burnout Inventory was also prepared by one

of the co-authors, using the same procedures as for school engagement scale. More details about the

psychometric features of the Lithuanian version of this instrument are provided in Raižienė,

Pilkauskaitė-Valickienė, & Žukauskienė (2014) and Žukauskienė et al., (2015).

The participants were also asked to provide demographic information including their age (“how

old are You?”), sex (“please indicate Your sex”; coded 0 = males, 1 = females), grade (“please indicate

Your grade”; values 9 and 10), free nutrition at school (“do you receive free meal at school?; coded 0 =

no, 1 = yes) and ethnicity (“What is your ethnicity?”). In this study, “receives free nutrition at school”

variable was used as an SES status indicator.

Analytic Strategy

Structural equation modeling (SEM) analyses using Mplus 7.4 with Maximum Likelihood Robust

(MLR) estimator were conducted to address our research questions1. The Comparative Fit Index (CFI),

Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), and Standardized Root Mean Square Residual

(SRMR) were used to assess the model fit. CFI higher than .90, RMSEA and SRMR lower than .08

indicated acceptable fit and CFI higher than .95, RMSEA and SRMR lower than .05 indicated good fit

(Little, 2013). The statistically significant difference between the nested models was tested using the

Scaled χ2 Difference test (Satorra & Bentler, 2001) and the practically significant – using model fit

statistics. We used ΔCFI ≥ -.01 as an indicator of a substantial decrease in model fit (Little, 2013).

1 In general, we focused on running analyses with latent variables. However, to avoid our models being

overly complex in terms of free parameters, we used item parcels instead of items as indicators of latent

variables. Three parcels were formed to represent each latent variable using item-to-construct balance approach,

based on corrected item-total score correlations (Little, 2013). Latent variables were scaled using effects coding

method (Little, Slegers, & Card, 2006).

17

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

Full Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML) estimation was used to deal with missing data

and to avoid a loss of power and other drawbacks associated with a list-wise deletion (Enders, 2010).

Little’s MCAR test indicated that a pattern of missing data was at least somewhat related to existing

data (p < .001), although a normed version of the index indicated that the relationship was small

(χ2/df = 1.08). Nevertheless, to avoid any parameter bias that could occur due to the missing values we

screened the complete dataset for variables that were related to missingness (Asendorpf, Schoot,

Denissen, & Hutteman, 2014). Once we selected them, we reduced the number of variables by

extracting 13 principal components (PCs) (Howard, Rhemtulla, & Little, 2015) using Quark (Lang,

Chesnut, & Little, 2016) in R software, and included extracted PC’s in all of our models using a

“saturated correlates” approach (Graham, 2003).

In addition, we calculated intra-class correlation coefficients (ICC) to evaluate the amount of

variance in school burnout, school engagement and identity styles accounted by the nested structure of

our data, i.e., between-class variability compared to total variability. The results indicated that the

effects of clustering were quite low2. Nevertheless, in all of our analyses, we used a "type = complex"

option in Mplus, indicating class as a cluster variable (Muthén & Muthén, 2012), to control for the

effects that clustering may have on the standard errors.

Results

Preliminary Analysis and Analysis of Longitudinal Measurement Invariance

Means, standard deviations and correlations between identity styles and school engagement and

burnout are presented in Table 2. As a preliminary step to cross-lagged panel model analyses, we

examined if measurement models for constructs in our study were invariant across time. We conducted

2 Specifically, ICCs for school engagement were .03 for T1, T2, and T3. ICCs for low for school burnout were .03 for T1,

.04 for T2, and .06 for T3. ICCs for information-oriented identity style were .04 for T1, .06 for T2 and .05 for T3. ICCs for

normative identity style were .07 for T1 and T2, and .08 for T3. ICCs for diffuse-avoidant identity style were .08 for T1, .04

for T2 and .05 for T3. Small clustering effects also indicated that multilevel analyses were not appropriate in this study.

18

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

these analyses for the three constructs of our study taken together in one model3. Taken together, the

results clearly established measurement invariance at different levels (see Table 3). An unconstrained

(configural) model had a good fit and adding equality constraints for loadings across time did not

produce any statistically or practically significant change of model-data fit, i.e., neither was ΔMLR χ2

significant (p = .741), nor did ΔCFI indicate a substantial decrease in model fit (ΔCFI = .001). Adding

intercept constraints did produce statistically significant change (p < .001) of model-data fit, however,

ΔCFI indicated that this change was only trivial (ΔCFI = -.003). Similarly, adding residual error

constraints did produce a significant change of fit (p = .012), however, this change was small (ΔCFI =

.001). Finally, adding variance equality constraints did not produce any significant changes to model-

data fit (p = .305), which indicated that variances of latent variables were equal across time. Summing

up, the results indicated that the measurement models for our constructs were equivalent across the

measurement waves.

Cross-Lagged Panel Model Analyses

Next, we focused on cross-lagged panel model analyses. To begin with, we tested if a strict

invariance measurement model, in which three additional covariates (grade, gender, and SES) were

included and were allowed to freely correlate with all latent variables, had a good fit. Alternative model

fit indices indicated an acceptable fit with data (see M0 in Table 3), therefore, we used this model as a

baseline for the cross-lagged model.

Next, we specified a cross-lagged panel model to investigate the bidirectional effects between

school engagement, school burnout and identity styles. Our model included: a) autoregressive

3 Following Little (2013), we tested for configural, weak, strong and strict longitudinal invariance. Although strong

(loadings and intercepts constrained to be equal across waves) and strict (loadings, intercepts, and residuals constrained to

be equal across waves) invariance was not necessary for our research questions (we did not address mean level or reliability

change across waves), we tested it anyway, to see if more parsimonious models could fit to the data. In addition, we tested if

variances of our latent variables were equal across time, as a preliminary step to testing the equivalence of cross-lagged

paths between different waves (Newsom, 2015).

19

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

(stability) paths for all constructs between adjacent time points (T1 and T2, and T2 and T3); b) cross-

lagged paths between all constructs between adjacent time points (no cross-lagged paths were included

between T1 and T3 variables); c) within-time associations (correlations between latent variables at T1

and correlations between disturbances at T2 and T3); d) covariates and their links to latent variables,

i.e., latent variables at T2 and T3 were regressed on covariates and latent variables at T1 were allowed

to correlate with covariates. This model was significantly different (p = .004) from the baseline model,

however, change in alternative model fit indices indicated only a small difference (see M1 in Table 3).

Before inspecting the results of this model, we checked if a more parsimonious model had a

similar fit with data. First, we constrained autoregressive paths, to check if stability between different

waves was the same. Fixing these coefficients did not have any statistical (p = .330) or practical

(ΔCFI = .000) effect on the model fit. Next, we constrained cross-lagged paths to be time-invariant, to

evaluate if the causal effects were the same between different waves. Again, the model fit change

statistics did not indicate any statistically or practically significant change (p = .398; ΔCFI = .000).

Finally, we constrained within-time associations, indicative of correlated relative change, as well as

disturbance variances to be time invariant. These constraints also did not worsen model fit either

(p = .568; ΔCFI = .000) and we turned to the inspection of parameter estimates.

In terms of reciprocal longitudinal links (cross-lagged effects), the results of the cross-lagged

model analyses were mostly in-line with our expectations (Figure 1 presents the results). Specifically,

we found that school engagement positively predicted an increase of information-oriented identity style

and school burnout positively predicted an increase of normative and diffuse-avoidant styles. In

addition, school burnout negatively predicted the level of school engagement. Identity styles did not

20

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

predict any change in school engagement or school burnout. Findings were not sensitive to gender,

grade and SES status4.

In terms of cross-sectional associations, results were also similar: school engagement had a

medium sized (Cohen, 1988) link with information-oriented style (positive) and diffuse-avoidant style

(negative); school burnout had a medium-sized link with diffuse-avoidant style (negative) and a strong

negative link with school engagement. Normative identity style was not associated with both school

engagement variables. Finally, results indicated that some significant correlations exist between

disturbance variances of school engagement and identity styles (positive for information-oriented and

negative for diffuse-avoidant) and disturbance variances of school burnout and identity styles (positive

for diffuse-avoidant and negative for information-oriented). These correlations indicate that at least

some change of identity styles and school engagement during these intervals may be affected by other

common factors, which were not assessed in this study.

Discussion

The school context is seen as the environment where the capacities and resources, important for

adolescent identity formation, can be developed (Flum & Kaplan, 2006, 2012; Harrell-Levy &

Kerpelman, 2010; Lannegrand-Willems & Bosma, 2006). However, the existing literature does not yet

4 We checked if exclusion of covariates in this model had any effect on reciprocal relationships. Specifically, we

ran the same model, but without inclusion of covariates. Exclusion of covariates had small effect of the size of

relationships, thus indicating the need to include the covariates, however, the paths that were significant and

those that were not with covariates included, remained to be the same. In addition, we checked if the links we

found were moderated by groups based on gender, grade and SES. Specifically, we tested if these variables

moderate the structural associations between the study variables, using multiple-group (MG) analyses. We

conducted these analyses with factor scores (calculated using a measurement model with strict invariance

constraints). To test for the moderation effects we compared the constrained model (in which the cross-lag

associations were set to be equivalent across groups) with an unconstrained model (in which the cross-lag

associations were allowed to vary across groups). The differences between the unconstrained and constrained

models were not significant in the case of gender (Δχ2(20) = 21.31, p = .379; ΔCFI = .000), grade (Δχ2

(20) = 20.35,

p = .436; ΔCFI = .000) and SES (Δχ2(20) = 29.91, p = .071; ΔCFI = -.004). Thus, the results indicated that gender,

grade, and SES did not moderate the relationships between the study constructs.

21

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

provide a satisfactory understanding of the extent and directionality of the links between adolescent

school experience and their identity formation. Few attempts exist to disentangle the potentially

complex dynamic effects between adolescent identity and their school experience. We address this gap

by analyzing the longitudinal cross-lagged associations between adolescent school experience (school

engagement and school burnout) and identity processing styles (information-oriented, normative and

diffuse-avoidant) in middle to late adolescence. The results of our analyses reveal the unidirectional

pattern of the effects and support the general prediction that active engagement in learning at school

can serve as a resource for adolescent identity formation, while school burnout, in contrast, can hinder

the formation of adolescent identity over time. While the two aspects of the adolescent school

functioning (engagement and burnout) are closely negatively related, their patterns of predicting

identity processing in adolescence are quite distinct. Below we discuss the specific findings from our

study in more detail.

The Effects of School Engagement and Burnout on Identity Processing Styles

Based on our findings, the adolescents who are actively involved in learning activities at school

become more inclined to use the information-oriented identity processing style over time (i.e., to

actively seek out, analyze, discuss, and critically evaluate self-relevant information). This effect is

stable over a two-year period from middle to late adolescence and applies regardless of gender, grade

or socio-economic status of an adolescent. This finding complements the previous studies, which have

shown that a number of characteristics of adolescent-school interactions, for example, positive student

perceptions of teaching (Rich & Schachter, 2012), positive school experiences (Lannegrand-Willems &

Bosma, 2006), and perceived support for the basic needs at school (Madjar & Cohen-Malayev, 2013),

can facilitate favorable aspects of youth identity formation, such as achieved commitments and in-

depth examination of existing commitments. Taken together, the findings from our and the previous

22

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

studies suggest that different (and mostly interrelated) aspects of adolescent-school interactions could

be the components of a stimulating, yet safe school context for adolescent identity formation.

Despite the positive effect on the use of information-oriented identity processing style, increased

levels of school engagement do not directly discourage the reliance on the other two identity processing

styles (normative and diffuse-avoidant). This result is consistent with the social-cognitive perspective

on identity, which suggests that the three distinct styles of approaching the self-relevant information are

based on two fundamentally different information processing systems: a rational and an experiential

(Berzonsky, 2011). While the active involvement in school learning activities can contribute to the

development of the first (rational) system, mostly used in the information-oriented identity processing,

apparently, it does not impair the experiential system of information processing used in normative and

diffuse-avoidant identity styles. In turn, we see no links between school engagement, as assessed in our

study, and the use of normative and diffuse-avoidant identity styles.

Instead, as expected, these two styles of identity formation (normative and diffuse-avoidant) are

positively related to the experiences of school burnout. Higher levels of school burnout predict an

increasing reliance on automatic cognitive processing, that is, either making unreflected identity

commitements (normative identity processing style) or avoiding considerations of identity issues at all

(diffuse-avoidant style). Being based on intuitive, automatic cognitive processes (Berzonsky, 1994,

2004, 2008), these two identity formation styles require relatively low mental resources and efforts on

behalf of an adolescent. It is reasonable that these identity processing styles become increasingly used

in the situation of a school burnout, since burnout indicates that the mental resources of a person are

exceeded by the existing demands (Upadyaya & Salmela-Aro, 2013).

On the one hand, such resource-saving strategies as reliance on normative and diffuse-avoidant

identity processing styles may seem helpful from a short-term perspective, since they can help to deal

with identity issues in a fast way or postpone considering them entirely and thus reduce the total amont

23

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

of stressful demands faced in a particular life situation. On the other hand, from a long-term

perspective, their utility is questionable, since over-reliance on these strategies can lead to suboptimal

outcomes for adolescents. This is particularly relevant in the case of predominantly diffuse-avoidant

processing of identity issues. First of all, this style is substantially negatively related to identity

commitment (Bosch & Card, 2012), which means that a meaningful and coherent system of personal

goals, values, roles and life scenarios is incompatible with diffuse-avoidant identity processing.

Moreover, a number of studies have shown that the reliance on a diffuse-avoidant style is consistently

linked to the “dark side of identity development” (Crocetti, Beyers, & Çok, 2016): a higher

reconsideration of the existing commitments (Negru-Subtirica, Pop, & Crocetti, 2017), a diffused

identity status (Adams, Berzonsky, & Keating, 2006), and low scores on self-regulating identity

functions (Crocetti, Sica, Schwartz, Serafini, & Meeus, 2013). The diffuse-avoidant identity style, as

well as its identity correlates, are consistently linked to maladjustment and low well-being in

adolescence. Particularly, the diffuse-avoidant identity style is negatively related to psychological well-

being (Berzonsky & Cieciuch, 2016), psychological resources (Adams et al., 2006), self-esteem,

optimism/ efficacy and some other aspects of well-being and adjustment (Phillips & Pittman, 2007).

Thus, predominant reliance on diffuse-avoidant identity processing can have certain costs for

adolescents in terms of their adjustment and well-being.

This can partly also be the case with the normative identity processing style. While the reliance

on normative identity style is linked to higher scores on a number of well-being indicators (Berzonsky

& Cieciuch, 2016; Phillips & Pittman, 2007), at the same time, it is related to low scores on such

aspects of adolescent functioning as autonomy and personal growth (Berzonsky & Cieciuch, 2016), as

well as broader developmental outcomes, such as civic engagement (Crocetti et al., 2014). Thus, the

reliance on normative identity processing style can have both benefits and costs for adolescent

development.

24

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

From the applied perspective, our findings suggest that the school demands and the

developmental tasks faced by adolescents may contribute to building a cumulative pressure, which can

lead to a burnout situation due to discrepancies between the individual resources and demands. If a

greater emphasis is placed on dealing with the academic demands, the students may start “saving

resources” in the sphere of developmental tasks, for example, by increasingly avoiding or automatically

processing the newly arising identity-related issues. From a developmental perspective, such tendency

can have undesirable effects on adolescent self-regulation and motivational functioning (Eichas et al.,

2015), well-being (Berzonsky & Cieciuch, 2016), and other aspects of psycho-social functioning (see

Crocetti et al., 2016). Thus, the developmental needs of adolescents, in particular, the need to form a

coherent sense of personal identity, have to be taken into account when planning the requirements, the

content and the format of the academic and other school activities and tasks. A number of authors

emphasize that schools should consistently foster their students’ identity exploration skills and orient

the whole educational process towards their students’ identity development (Flum & Kaplan, 2006;

Harrell-Levy & Kerpelman, 2010; Schachter & Rich, 2011). This includes providing safe spaces and

opportunities for adolescent identity exploration, offering academic tasks and activities based on the

students’ personal experiences and meanings, supporting student autonomy, focusing on the process of

learning, encouraging interaction with peers and experts and building a continuous dialogue with

students on the personal purpose and meaning of learning (Flum & Kaplan, 2006).

Taken together, our findings on the longitudinal effects of school engagement and school burnout

on adolescent identity processing suggest that these two closely negatively related aspects of

adolescent-school interactions may have quite distinct roles in adolescent identity formation. While

school engagement only contributes to the information-oriented style, which is primarily based on the

rational processing, school burnout only predicts the two identity styles primarily based on experiential

processing. This points to a relative independence of the two cognitive systems underlying the identity

25

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

processing styles, as well as to a different nature of school engagement and school burnout, as

measured in our study. Even if, in some cases, school burnout can be considered as an indicator of

school disengagement (Tuominen-Soini & Salmela-Aro, 2014), in the case of adolescent identity

formation both factors have to be considered to understand their distinct role over time.

Limitations and Suggestions for the Future

The findings of our study have to be considered in the light of its strengths and limitations. To

operationalize the construct of school engagement we have used its behavioral component, which is

considered central for analyzing students’ involvement with school (Hospel et al., 2016). The

behavioral engagement covers such school conduct as being attentive, listening to teachers,

participating in class discussions, and working hard at school. Even though these behaviors reflect the

extent of involvement in learning, they do not directly cover emotional, cognitive and agentic aspects

of school engagement. Since these components are related, adding all aspects of school engagement in

predictions of identity processing styles could provide a more accurate and comprehensive picture.

Future studies on adolescent identity formation in the school context should explore the links of all four

aspects of school engagement with identity.

Another limitation is related to the partial perspective on identity formation used in our study.

While identity formation includes two aspects – self-discovery and self-construction (Eichas et al.,

2015) – our study only focused on the latter by applying the social-cognitive perspective on identity.

Thus, the aspect of self-discovery was left out of the analysis. This could explain the absense of

reciprocity in the links between school engagement and burnout with identity processing styles in our

study. While we found no effects of identity processing on the level of school engagement and burnout,

some previous studies have documented such effects. For example, Faircloth (2012) has shown, in a

sequence of qualitative studies, that by strengthening the identity components of the learning content

(i.e., encouraging students to share their personal perspectives and discover the personal meaning of the

26

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

academic tasks) it is possible to strengthen students’ active participation and emotional responsiveness

to learning. A similar pattern was discovered in Sinai and colleagues' (2012) study, where school

activities intended to promote identity exploration facilitated engagement in learning. The seeming

contradiction between these findings and ours can be resolved when considering the two aspects of

identity formation – self-discovery and self-construction. While our study focused on the latter, the two

previous studies included a very salient self-discovery component. Taken together, these findings and

ours suggest that the links between identity formation and school engagement and burnout may vary

depending on the self-discovery vs. self-construction perspective on identity. In future studies, this

assumption could be tested by analyzing both aspects of identity formation in the context of adolescent-

school interactions.

Finally, our findings are bound within the measurement intervals designed for our longitudinal

study. Year-long intervals could be considered rather long in a study on identity formation in middle to

late adolescence, since this developmental period is very intensive with regard to identity development.

Some studies have documented intensive adolescent identity dynamics even on a daily basis (Klimstra

et al., 2010). Some of the effects between school engagement, school burnout and identity processing

styles may be overlooked when using long periods between assessments. Future longitudinal studies on

the subject should carefully consider the optimal period between assessments.

Conclusion

Our findings extend the literature on the links between adolescent-school interactions on identity

development. School engagement supports the adolescent identity formation by facilitating

information-oriented processing of identity issues. School burnout, in contrast, leads to the reliance on

normative and diffuse-avoidant identity styles, based on experiential, automatic, non-reflective ways of

cognitive processing. Thus, while school engagement can serve as a resource for adolescent identity

formation, school burnout can hinder the development of adolescent identity. These tendencies are

27

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

robust in middle to late adolescence and apply regardless of gender, grade, or socio-economic status of

an adolescent. These findings suggest that the schools should respect the developmental needs of

adolescents, particularly the need to form a coherent sense of personal identity, by supporting

adolescent engagement in school activities, preventing school burnout and offering safe opportunities

for identity exploration in the school context.

28

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

References

Adams, G. R., Berzonsky, M. D., & Keating, L. (2006). Psychosocial resources in first-year university

students: The role of identity processes and social relationships. Journal of Youth and

Adolescence, 35(1), 78–88. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-005-9019-0

Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., Kim, D., & Reschly, A. L. (2006). Measuring cognitive and

psychological engagement: Validation of the Student Engagement Instrument. Journal of

School Psychology, 44(5), 427–445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2006.04.002

Arnett, J. J. (2014). Emerging adulthood: The winding road from the late teens through the twenties

(2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

Asendorpf, J. B., Schoot, R. van de, Denissen, J. J. A., & Hutteman, R. (2014). Reducing bias due to

systematic attrition in longitudinal studies: The benefits of multiple imputation. International

Journal of Behavioral Development, 38(5), 453–460.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025414542713

Berzonsky, M. D. (1994). Individual differences in self-construction: The role of constructivist

epistemological assumptions. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 7(4), 263–281.

https://doi.org/10.1080/10720539408405234

Berzonsky, M. D. (2004). Identity processing style, self-construction, and personal epistemic

assumptions: A social-cognitive perspective. European Journal of Developmental Psychology,

1(4), 303–315. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405620444000120

Berzonsky, M. D. (2008). Identity formation: The role of identity processing style and cognitive

processes. Personality and Individual Differences, 44(3), 645–655.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.09.024

29

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

Berzonsky, M. D. (2011). A Social-cognitive perspective on identity construction. In S. J. Schwartz, K.

Luyckx, & V. L. Vignoles (Eds.), Handbook of identity theory and research (pp. 55–76).

Springer: New York.

Berzonsky, M. D., & Cieciuch, J. (2016). Mediational role of identity commitment in relationships

between identity processing style and psychological well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies,

17(1), 145–162. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-014-9588-2

Bosch, L. A., & Card, N. A. (2012). A meta-analytic review of Berzonsky’s Identity Style Inventory

(ISI). Journal of Adolescence, 35(2), 333–343.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.08.007

Bosma, H. A., & Kunnen, E. S. (2008). Identity-in-context is not yet identity development-in-context.

Journal of Adolescence, 31(2), 281–289. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.03.001

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence

Erlbaum Associates.

Crocetti, E., Beyers, W., & Çok, F. (2016). Shedding light on the dark side of identity: Introduction to

the special issue. Journal of Adolescence, 47, 104–108.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.01.002

Crocetti, E., Erentaitė, R., & Žukauskienė, R. (2014). Identity styles, positive youth development, and

civic engagement in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 43(11), 1818–1828.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-014-0100-4

Crocetti, E., Klimstra, T. A., Hale, W. W., Koot, H. M., & Meeus, W. (2013). Impact of early

adolescent externalizing problem behaviors on identity development in middle to late

adolescence: A prospective 7-year longitudinal study. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,

42(11), 1745–1758. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-013-9924-6

30

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

Crocetti, E., Klimstra, T., Keijsers, L., Hale, W. W., & Meeus, W. (2009). Anxiety trajectories and

identity development in adolescence: A five-wave longitudinal study. Journal of Youth and

Adolescence, 38(6), 839–849. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-008-9302-y

Crocetti, E., Sica, L. S., Schwartz, S. J., Serafini, T., & Meeus, W. (2013). Identity styles, dimensions,

statuses, and functions: Making connections among identity conceptualizations. Revue

Europeene de Psychologie Appliquee, 63(1), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2012.09.001

Crone, E. A., & Dahl, R. E. (2012). Understanding adolescence as a period of social–affective

engagement and goal flexibility. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13(9), 636–650.

https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3313

Eichas, K., Meca, A., Montgomery, M. J., & Kurtines, W. (2015). Identity and positive youth

development: Advances in developmental intervention science. In K. C. McLean & M. Syed

(Eds.), The Oxford handbook of identity development. New York: Oxford University Press.

https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199936564.013.020

Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied missing data analysis. New York: Guilford Press.

Epstein, S. (2003). Cognitive-experiential self-theory: an integrative theory of personality. In T. Millon,

M. J. Lerner, & I. B. Weiner (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Personality and social

psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 5, pp. 159–184). Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity, youth, and crisis. New York: W. W. Norton.

Faircloth, B. S. (2009). Making the most of adolescence: Harnessing the search for identity to

understand classroom belonging. Journal of Adolescent Research, 24(3), 321–348.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0743558409334248

Faircloth, B. S. (2012). “Wearing a mask” vs. connecting identity with learning. Contemporary

Educational Psychology, 37(3), 186–194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2011.12.003

31

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

Flum, H., & Kaplan, A. (2006). Exploratory orientation as an educational goal. Educational

Psychologist, 41(2), 99–110. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4102_3

Flum, H., & Kaplan, A. (2012). Identity formation in educational settings: A contextualized view of

theory and research in practice. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 37(3), 240–245.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2012.01.003

Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). School engagement: Potential of the

concept, state of the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74(1), 59–109.

https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543074001059

Graham, J. W. (2003). Adding missing-data-relevant variables to FIML-based structural equation

models. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 10(1), 80–100.

https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM1001_4

Harrell-Levy, M. K., & Kerpelman, J. L. (2010). Identity process and transformative pedagogy:

teachers as agents of identity formation. Identity, 10(2), 76–91.

https://doi.org/10.1080/15283481003711684

Hospel, V., Galand, B., & Janosz, M. (2016). Multidimensionality of behavioural engagement:

Empirical support and implications. International Journal of Educational Research, 77, 37-49.

Howard, W. J., Rhemtulla, M., & Little, T. D. (2015). Using principal components as auxiliary

variables in missing data estimation. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 50(3), 285-299.

https://doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2014.999267

Kiuru, N., Aunola, K., Nurmi, J.-E., Leskinen, E., & Salmela-Aro, K. (2008). Peer group influence and

selection in adolescents’ school burnout: A longitudinal study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 54(1),

23–55. https://doi.org/10.1353/mpq.2008.0008

32

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

Klimstra, T. A., Luyckx, K., Hale, W. A., Frijns, T., van Lier, P. A. C., & Meeus, W. (2010). Short-

term fluctuations in identity: Introducing a micro-level approach to identity formation. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(1), 191–202. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019584

Kroger, J., & Marcia, J. E. (2011). The identity statuses: origins, meanings, and interpretations. In

Handbook of Identity Theory and Research (pp. 31–53). New York: Springer.

https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-7988-9_2

Kroger, J., Martinussen, M., & Marcia, J. E. (2010). Identity status change during adolescence and

young adulthood: a meta-analysis. Journal of Adolescence, 33(5), 683–698.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.11.002

Lang, K. M., Chesnut, S., & Little, T. D. (2016). Package “quark” (Version 0.6.1.) [R].

Lannegrand-Willems, L., & Bosma, H. A. (2006). Identity development-in-context: The school as an

important context for identity development. Identity, 6(1), 85–113.

https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532706xid0601_6

Larson, R. W., & Rusk, N. (2011). Intrinsic motivation and positive development. Advances in Child

Development and Behavior, 41, 89–130. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-386492-5.00005-1

Lawson, M. A., & Lawson, H. A. (2013). New conceptual frameworks for student engagement

research, policy, and practice. Review of Educational Research, 83(3), 432–479.

https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654313480891

Little, T. D. (2013). Longitudinal structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford Press.

Luyckx, K., Lens, W., Smits, I., & Goossens, L. (2010). Time perspective and identity formation:

Short-term longitudinal dynamics in college students. International Journal of Behavioral

Development, 34(3), 238–247. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025409350957

Madjar, N., & Cohen-Malayev, M. (2013). Youth movements as educational settings promoting

personal development: Comparing motivation and identity formation in formal and non-formal

33

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

education contexts. International Journal of Educational Research, 62(Supplement C), 162–

174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2013.09.002

Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2012). Mplus user’s guide. (Seventh edition). Los Angeles, CA:

Muthén & Muthén.

Negru-Subtirica, O., Pop, E., & Crocetti, E. (2017). A longitudinal integration of identity styles and

educational identity processes in adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 53(11), 2127-2138.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000325

Parker, P. D., & Salmela-Aro, K. (2011). Developmental processes in school burnout: A comparison of

major developmental models. Learning and Individual Differences: Journal of Psychology and

Education, 21(2), 244–248. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.005

Raižienė, S., Pilkauskaitė-Valickienė, R., & Žukauskienė, R. (2014). School burnout and subjective

well-being: evidence from cross-lagged relations in a 1-year longitudinal sample. Procedia -

Social and Behavioral Sciences, 116(Supplement C), 3254–3258.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.01.743

Phillips, T. M., Pittman, J. F. (2007). Adolescent psychological well-being by identity style. Journal of

Adolescence, 30(6), 1021-1034. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2007.03.002

Pop, E. I., Negru-Subtirica, O., Crocetti, E., Opre, A., & Meeus, W. (2016). On the interplay between

academic achievement and educational identity: a longitudinal study. Journal of Adolescence,

47, 135–144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2015.11.004

Rich, Y., & Schachter, E. P. (2012). High school identity climate and student identity development.

Contemporary Educational Psychology, 37(3), 218–228.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2011.06.002

34

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

Ryzin, M. J. V., Gravely, A. A., & Roseth, C. J. (2007). Autonomy, belongingness, and engagement in

school as contributors to adolescent psychological well-being. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,

38(1), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-007-9257-4

Salmela-Aro, K., Kiuru, N., Leskinen, E., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2009). School Burnout Inventory (SBI):

Reliability and validity. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 25(1), 48–57.

https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.25.1.48

Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (2001). A scaled difference chi-square test statistic for moment structure

analysis. Psychometrika, 66(4), 507–514. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02296192

Schachter, E. P., & Rich, Y. (2011). Identity education: A conceptual framework for educational

researchers and practitioners. Educational Psychologist, 46(4), 222–238.

https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2011.614509

Sinai, M., Kaplan, A., & Flum, H. (2012). Promoting identity exploration within the school curriculum:

A design-based study in a junior high literature lesson in Israel. Contemporary Educational

Psychology, 37(3), 195–205. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2012.01.006

Skinner, E. A., & Pitzer, J. R. (2012). Developmental dynamics of student engagement, coping, and

everyday resilience. In Handbook of research on student engagement (pp. 21–44). Boston:

Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_2

Smits, I., Soenens, B., Berzonsky, M., Luyckx, K., Goossens, L., Kunnen, S., et al. (2009). The identity

style inventory—version 4: A cross-national study in scale development and validation. I. Smits

(Ed.), Identity styles in adolescence: Measurement and associations with perceived parenting,

personal well-being, and interpersonal functioning (pp. 57–105). Unpublished doctoral

dissertation, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Belgium. Retrieved from: https://goo.gl/tHSP91

35

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

Tuominen-Soini, H., & Salmela-Aro, K. (2014). Schoolwork engagement and burnout among Finnish

high school students and young adults: Profiles, progressions, and educational outcomes.

Developmental Psychology, 50(3), 649–662. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033898

Upadyaya, K., & Salmela-Aro, K. (2013). Development of school engagement in association with

academic success and well-being in varying social contexts. European Psychologist, 18(2),

136–147. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000143

Žukauskienė, R., Raižienė, S., Malinauskienė, O., Gabrialavičiūtė, I., Garckija, R., Vosylis, R.,

Kaniušonytė, G., Truskauskaitė-Kunevičienė, I., Kajokienė, I. (2015). Pozityvi jaunimo raida

Lietuvoje [Positive youth development in Lithuania]. Vilnius: Mykolas Romeris University.

Wang, M.-T., & Eccles, J. S. (2012). Adolescent behavioral, emotional, and cognitive engagement

trajectories in school and their differential relations to educational success. Journal of Research

on Adolescence, 22(1), 31–39. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2011.00753.x

36

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

Table 1

The Main Socio-Demographic Characteristics of the Study Sample (N = 916) and Country-Wide

Data for Comparison

Sex1 SES2 Ethnicity3

Women Men Low Medium /

high Lithuanian Other

Study sample 51% 49% 22% 78% 99% 1%

Country-wide data 48% 52% 33% 67% 84% 16%

Notes.

Study sample compared to 1Students in upper-secondary education in Lithuania (2012–2014 data from Statistics

Lithuania). 2All schoolchildren in Lithuania (based on 2012 data from the Lithuanian Ministry of Social Security and

Labour). 3General Lithuanian population (2011 data from Statistics Lithuania). SES—socioeconomic status, defined in

this study as the percentage of students receiving free nutrition at school.

37

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

Table 2

Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations Among Study Variables (N = 916)

Time 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15

1. Information-oriented style 1 -

2. Information-oriented style 2 .53*** -

3. Information-oriented style 3 .44*** .60*** -

4. Normative style 1 .24*** .06 .01 -

5. Normative style 2 .13*** .26*** .11*** .51*** -

6. Normative style 3 .09** .11*** .19*** .39*** .53*** -

7. Diffuse-avoidant style 1 -.14*** -.20*** -.22*** .39*** .22*** .17*** -

8. Diffuse-avoidant style 2 -.12*** -.07* -.16*** .25*** .39*** .18*** .53*** -

9. Diffuse-avoidant style 3 -.09** -.15*** -.15*** .23*** .23*** .37*** .39*** .50*** -

10. School engagement 1 .33*** .29*** .30*** .06 .05 .06 -.21*** -.21*** -.19*** -

11. School engagement 2 .32*** .32*** .33*** .05 .01 .01 -.22*** -.22*** -.19*** .51*** -

12. School engagement 3 .21*** .30*** .33*** -.01 .04 .04 -.18*** -.15*** -.24*** .45*** .48*** -

13. School burnout 1 -.01 -.02 -.10*** .03 .01 -.01 .26*** .24*** .22*** -.15*** -.14*** -.14*** -

14. School burnout 2 -.05 -.06 -.09*** -.02 -.04 -.02 .16*** .26*** .22*** -.10** -.14*** -.11*** .51*** -

15. School burnout 3 -.04 .00 -.11*** .01 .01 -.02 .08* .21*** .23*** -.13*** -.08* -.06 .42*** .63*** -

Mean 3.75 3.65 3.54 3.07 3.01 2.99 2.84 2.81 2.82 2.30 2.27 2.27 3.26 3.23 3.40

SD 0.61 0.60 0.63 0.51 0.53 0.52 0.59 0.59 0.57 0.39 0.42 0.40 0.99 1.02 1.04

Note.

* - p < .05; ** - p <.01; *** - p < .001.

38

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

Table 3

Results of Longitudinal Measurement Invariance and Cross-Lagged Panel Model Analyses (N = 916)

Model tested Model fit statistics Model fit change statistics

χ2 df npar CFI RMSEA [90% CI] SRMR Δχ2 Δdf ΔCFI

Longitudinal invariance tests

Configural 1648.966*** 795 285 .947 .034 [.032 .037] .054 - - -

Weak 1646.405*** 815 265 .948 .033 [.031 .036] .055 15.61 20 .001

Strong 1727.608*** 835 245 .945 .034 [.032 .036] .056 81.66*** 20 -.003

Strict 1775.799*** 865 215 .944 .034 [.032 .036] .057 50.31* 30 -.001

Variances 1786.141*** 875 205 .944 .034 [.031 .036] .060 11.71 10 .000

Cross-lagged panel model analyses

M0 1942.481*** 955 269 .940 .034 [.031 .036] .055 - - -

M1 1989.978*** 980 244 .938 .034 [.031 .036] .056 47.71** 25 -.002

M2 1996.206*** 985 239 .938 .033 [.031 .036] .056 5.76 5 .000

M3 2017.631*** 1005 219 .938 .033 [.031 .035] .057 20.99 20 .000

M4 2028.648*** 1020 204 .938 .033 [.031 .035] .057 13.44 15 .000

Note.

CFI - Comparative fit index; RMSEA – Root mean square error of approximation. SRMR – Standardized Root Mean Square Residual; CI – Confidence interval. npar –

number of free parameters in the model. M0: Measurement model with covariates; M1: Cross-lagged panel model without any constraints; M2: Cross-lagged panel

model with stability coefficients constrained to be equal across waves; M3: Cross-lagged panel model with stability and cross-lag coefficients constrained to be equal

across waves; M4: Cross-lagged panel model with stability, cross-lag, disturbance variances and residual correlation coefficients constrained to be equal across waves.

* - p < .05; ** - p <.01; *** - p < .001.

39

IDENTITY STYLES AND SCHOOL CONTEXT

Figure 1. Standardized results of the cross-lagged model linking identity styles, engagement in learning and

school burnout. Since the models with time-invariant structural paths were retained as the final ones, we present

only two time-points (T and T+1), and all coefficients displayed (except for correlations between latent variables

at T1) represent the averaged standardized coefficients over three-time intervals. Although not displayed for

reasons of clarity, this model includes non-significant cross-lagged paths between all variables, observed

indicators of latent variables (parcels) and three covariates (gender, SES, grade).