Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE...

21
PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 UDC 159.954.072:75 © 2017 by the Serbian Psychological Association DOI: https://doi.org/10.2298/PSI1703319S Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience 7 Ivan Stojilović Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, Faculty of Philosophy, University of Belgrade, Belgrade, Serbia It is often neglected that the experience of artwork is a creative act, and one which requires the audience to be creative. This exploratory study aimed to examine whether creative activity and measures of person`s creativity are correlated with the aesthetic experience of paintings. Eighty-two participants rated 21 paintings, including 7 figural traditional paintings, 7 semi-abstract works, and 7 abstract works. Participants were randomly assigned to one of two experimental groups. One group first created collages and then rated the paintings on five aesthetic preference scales, while the other group first rated the paintings and then created collages. Multilevel regression analysis with two crossed random effects (participants and paintings) was used. Results showed that performing a creative activity prior to rating artwork positively influenced ratings of artwork creativity. In addition, collage creativity was positively correlated with ratings of (semi)abstract paintings as beautiful. It is hypothesized that people become more open to new, unusual experiences, are more flexible and act more freely in their decisions when performing a creative activity, which reflects positively on stronger preferences of paintings. Keywords: aesthetic experience; creativity; beauty; preferences; empirical aesthetics Highlights: That the experience of art is a creative act is often neglected Doing a creative activity positively influences the experience of art Art-domain creativity measures are positively correlated with aesthetic preferences Creative people have stronger preferences for (semi)abstract paintings Creativity is a domain-specific rather than generic process In everyday language, only those who actively create or perform art – painters, sculptors, musicians, singers, actors, in a word ‘artists’ – are considered creative people. However, it is often neglected that observing, enjoying and experiencing artwork are creative acts that require the observers, listeners and Corresponding author: [email protected]

Transcript of Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE...

Page 1: Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 viewers to be both active and creative

PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 UDC 159.954.072:75© 2017 by the Serbian Psychological Association DOI: https://doi.org/10.2298/PSI1703319S

Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience7

Ivan StojilovićLaboratory of Experimental Psychology, Faculty of Philosophy,

University of Belgrade, Belgrade, Serbia

It is often neglected that the experience of artwork is a creative act, and one which requires the audience to be creative. This exploratory study aimed to examine whether creative activity and measures of pe rson`s creativity are correlated with the aesthetic experience of paintings. Eighty-two participants rated 21 paintings, including 7 figural traditional paintings, 7 semi-abstract works, and 7 abstract works. Participants were randomly assigned to one of two experimental groups. One group first created collages and then rated the paintings on five aesthetic preference scales, while the other group first rated the paintings and then created collages. Multilevel regression analysis with two crossed random effects (participants and paintings) was used. Results showed that performing a creative activity prior to rating artwork positively influenced ratings of artwork creativity. In addition, collage creativity was positively correlated with ratings of (semi)abstract paintings as beautiful. It is hypothesized that people become more open to new, unusual experiences, are more flexible and act more freely in their decisions when performing a creative activity, which reflects positively on stronger preferences of paintings.

Keywords: aesthetic experience; creativity; beauty; preferences; empirical aesthetics

Highlights:

• That the experience of art is a creative act is often neglected• Doing a creative activity positively influences the experience of art• Art-domain creativity measures are positively correlated with aesthetic

preferences• Creative people have stronger preferences for (semi)abstract paintings• Creativity is a domain-specific rather than generic process

In everyday language, only those who actively create or perform art – painters, sculptors, musicians, singers, actors, in a word ‘artists’ – are considered creative people. However, it is often neglected that observing, enjoying and experiencing artwork are creative acts that require the observers, listeners and

Corresponding author: [email protected]

Page 2: Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 viewers to be both active and creative

EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE320

PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339

viewers to be both active and creative participants. Collingwood (1938) has previously highlighted the connection between the productive work of an artist and the activities of art consumers. He argues that both the creator and consumer of art participate in the development of the artwork; the role of the public “is not simply receptive, but coll aborative” (Collingwood, 1938, p. 324). Every time we observe artwork, we become its creators together with the artists, because our reaction to the artwork leads to its completion (Zausner, 2007). Audience participation in the joint creation of artwork has recently been emphasized in the Mirror Model of Art (Tinio, 2013). Together, these authors’ ideas suggest that creativity is just as important in the perception and enjoyment as it is in the creation of a work of art. However, the importance of creativity on the aesthetic experience of artwork has been neglected (Vartanian, 2014). The connection between creating and observing a work of art has not been not sufficiently studied in the psychology of aesthetics, creativity and art (Tinio, 2013).

CreativityOne of the first problems a researcher encounters when studying the

phenomenon of creativity is the lack of a unique definition (Plucker & Makel, 2010). Most authors have defined creativity as the act of creating something original (new, innovative, unique) and appropriate (meaningful, useful, applicable) (Beghetto & Kaufman, 2014; Plucker, Beghetto, & Dow, 2004; Simonton, 2012; Sternberg & Lubart, 1999). In the literature on creativity, the 4Ps model (Rhodes, 1961) has gained wide acceptance. This model views creativity as a dynamic phenomenon comprising four components: person, process, product, and press. In this study, effects of creativity are observed in terms of three components: persons’ characteristics (based on divergent thinking (DT) test results and self-assessment using a creativity questionnaire), process (collage creation), and product (collage creativity).

Creative process. Kaufman and Beghetto (2009) argue that we can express creativity in many different ways and suggest the Four C Model of Creativity. In this model, there is a gradation of four dimensions of creativity, from the creativity involved in the learning process (“mini-c”), through everyday creativity (“little-c”) and professional-level expertise in any creative area (“Pro-c”) to eminent creativity (“Big-C”). In this study, the focus is on the everyday creativity (“little-c”), which is associated with concepts such as innovative education, aesthetic assessment, self-actualization (Richards, 2007), practicing hobbies, personal development, productivity and problem solving in everyday life (Ivcevic, 2007), and is represented by activities such as making collages, taking photos or publishing in journals (Kaufman & Paul, 2014).

According to Richards (2007), involvement in a creative activity can make our experience of artwork more vivid, enjoyable, and meaningful. In addition, it can make a person more open, complete, and stable, and can aid our development. This engagement is an opportunity for potentially creative results, because such activity directs the person to abandon the habit of recognizing objects, which

Page 3: Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 viewers to be both active and creative

Ivan Stojilović 321

PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339

is basic in daily life (Cupchik, 1999). A person engaged in a creative activity pays more attention to the formal characteristics of artwork, such as tonality, form, balance, color, and texture, which leads to a more holistic view of the artwork or the natural environment (Cupchik, 1999). Creative activity can have multiple functions, often interlinked, such as problem solving, self-expression, self-actualization, research, and experimentation (Rogers, 1961; Runco, 2007). It allows spontaneous and uninhibited playing with these representations, values, and objectives, bringing them into question, and caricaturing or exaggerating them. Runco (2007) purports that ego strength is being expressed and, more importantly, strengthened during creative acts. Similarly, Csikszentmihalyi (1996) identifies artistic act with the state of flow, increased concentration, total immersion in an activity, and enjoyment, which provides a certain degree of challenge adjusted to a person’s capabilities. Based on the above findings we proposed our first hypothesis: performing a creative activity (in our study collage creation) will have a significantly greater impact on positive aesthetic preferences of paintings that deviate from a naive understanding of artwork (such as semi-abstract and abstract works). After a creative act a person is more open for challenging tasks, more focused on formal characteristics of paintings and more inclined toward mental experimentation.

Creative person and product. Highly creative people are often described as: spontaneous and free (Maslow, 1971), having increased awareness (Richards, 2007), unconventional, independent (Feist, 1998, 2010). Creativity can also represent sensitivity to problems, and an ability to redefine, such as when transforming thoughts or freedoms beyond functional fixation (Guilford, 1962). Creative people have a high tolerance for uncertainty, prefer complex structures and appreciate intellectual challenges (Barron, 1993; Csikszentmihalyi, 1996; Eysenck, 1993), and tend to prefer disturbing, conflicting figural representations (Giannini & Bonaiuto, 1999). Results of most studies show that creativity is positively correlated with Openness to experience (Chamorro-Premuzic & Reichenbacher, 2008; Dollinger, Urban, & James, 2004; Furnham, Crump, Batey, & Chamorro-Premuzic, 2009; McCrae, 1987). Following these findings we defined our second hypothesis: more creative persons will prefer non-traditional, (semi)abstract artworks. More creative persons are unconventional, free, flexible, prefer complex structures and therefore we assumed that they would prefer non-traditional and challenging paintings.

In this paper, three methods were used to assess people’s creativity: (a) DT tests, (b) consensual assessment of collage creativity, and (c) self-assessment of creative achievements. DT tasks are often used to assess creative thinking potential, creative cognitive style, or ability to generate creative ideas (Runco, Dow, & Smith, 2006; Silvia et al., 2008). In DT tasks, participants are asked to generate as many different and original answers to a question as possible. Consensual assessment is used to determine the creativity of products (collages in our study), and relies on independent assessments of individuals (Hennessey & Amabile, 1999). The creativity of a product reflects the creativity of its

Page 4: Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 viewers to be both active and creative

EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE322

PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339

author. Therefore, the assessment of a person’s product, as a way of observing his/her creativity, is a good starting point (Besemer & O’Quin, 1986). The third method used was self-assessment of creativity, in which the participants assessed their own creativity, focusing on the personality aspect of creativity. The use of different measures of a person’s creativity increases the predictive validity and allows the inclusion of different forms of creativity. Previous results have shown low predictive and convergent validity between different creativity tests (Silvia, 2011; Simonton, 2003). Therefore, we defined our third hypothesis: the correlation between different measures of creativity used in the study will be low.

Aesthetic experienceThe experience of an art object is very idiosyncratic, and many studies

have confirmed that it is “in the eye of the observer”. The quality of an aesthetic experience for the same object can vary from interesting to disgusting (Silvia, 2009), and elicit mild pleasantness (Reber, Schwarz, & Winkielman, 2004) ecstasy, self-transcendence, or extraordinary states of consciousness (Funch, 1997; Marković, 2012). However, one of the dominant views in traditional philosophy of art (Carroll, 2002; Scruton, 2009) and psychology of art (Shimamura, 2013), and among the naïve observers (Augustin, Wagemans, & Carbon, 2012; Stojilović, 2014) is the link between the aesthetic experience and the experience of the beauty of an art object. Another quality that is often associated with the appraisal of artwork, especially in contemporary art, is evaluation of its creativity. As this study explored the impact of creativity on the experience of paintings, the second measure of aesthetic experience used (besides beauty) was an evaluation of the creativity of paintings.

Creativity and aesthetic experienceIn the psychology of art, a few studies have linked creativity to the

experience of artwork. Koestler (1964) was one of the first to explain the aesthetic experience through the mechanism of creative thinking. The essence of creativity lies in perceiving a situation or idea in two self-consistent but habitually incompatible frames of reference. An aesthetic experience has the same mechanism as a creative act, and happens when two incompatible matrices of thought are juxtaposed in a completely new whole. Intellectual illumination and emotional catharsis are, according to Koestler, the essence of aesthetic experiences.

Results from a study by Aks and Sprott (1996) suggest that creativity and aesthetic preferences are related. However, the results are not univocal; findings suggest that people with high scores on DT tests prefer less detailed patterns (low fractal dimension), while self-reported creative individuals have a marginally greater preference for more detailed patterns (high fractal dimension). In research exploring preferences of originals and facsimiles of abstract art (Mondrian and Hirst paintings), it was found that creativity, as measured by the Barron-Welsh scale, is not connected with a stronger preference for original works (Furnham &

Page 5: Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 viewers to be both active and creative

Ivan Stojilović 323

PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339

Rao, 2002). A recent study by Myszkowski, Storme, Zenasni, and Lubart (2014) found a connection between visual aesthetic sensitivity (which is the perceptual ability to identify differences in terms of harmony, good design, or symmetry) and creativity. According to the results of that study, visual aesthetic sensitivity is moderately positively correlated with creative potential, as measured by a figural DT task. In music, Rawlings, Hodge, Sherr, and Dempsey (1995) found high Eysenck’ Psychoticism scores (which the authors associated with creativity) among subjects showing a relative preference for “hard” music, “harsh” chords, and discordant musical triads. Creative potential was found to be related to a preference for more complex musical pieces (Ziv & Keydar, 2009).

This article presents an exploratory study on the interrelation between creativity and aesthetic experience. Two qualities of aesthetic experience were observed: ratings of paintings’ beauty and creativity. In addition to using DT test measures of creativity, a collage creativity assessment, and a self-assessment of creativity, we also explored the influence of practicing a creative activity (collage creation) on the experience of artwork. We can sum up the proposed hypotheses:

Hypothesis 1: Performing a creative activity (collage creation) will have a significantly greater impact on positive aesthetic preferences of abstract paintings (Cupchik, 1999; Richards, 2007; Runco, 2007)

Hypothesis 2 : More creative persons will prefer non-traditional, abstract artworks (Nelson, 2005; Nelson & Rawlings, 2007, 2009, 2010).

Hypothesis 3: The correlation between different measures of creativity used in the study will be low (Silvia, 2011; Simonton, 2003)

Method

ParticipantsThe study comprised 82 participants , all of whom were first-year students at the

Department of Psychology, Faculty of Philosophy, University of Belgrade. Nine participants (11%) were males and 73 (89%) were females. The participants were aged between 18 and 28 years (M = 19.56, SD = 1.54).

StimuliThe study utilized 21 paintings : 7 figural traditional paintings from the Renaissance

and Baroque periods, 7 semi-abstract works created after 1950, and 7 abstract works created after 1920. The selection of paintings was made in the preliminary study, when the author singled out 50 paintings for each of the 3 styles. Through an online survey, three experts (one art historian and two visual artists) evaluated to what extent each of the paintings belonged to the style “identified” by the research author. The scale of assessments ranged from 1 – Does not belong at all (in Serbian Uopšte ne pripada) to 5 – Fully belongs (U potpunosti pripada). From the paintings for which all 3 experts said that they “fully belong” to the identified style, 21 paintings were randomly chosen – 7 figurative, 7 semi-abstract, and 7 abstract. The paintings used are listed in Appendix 1.

Page 6: Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 viewers to be both active and creative

EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE324

PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339

InstrumentsDivergent thinking tests. We used three DT tests: “Unusual use” (creative use of

an everyday object – brick), “Associations” (identifying creative common categories for two concepts – meat and milk), and “Consequences” (proposing creative solutions to a hypothetical situation – if people did not need to sleep) (Kaufman, Plucker, & Baer, 2008). Participants were instructed to try and produce “as much as possible unusual, creative, and rare” answers. All DT tests were given as paper and pencil tests, and each task had to be completed within 3 minutes.

Participants’ answers were independently assessed by three trained evaluators on two aspects: fluency (the number of created ideas) and originality (the quality of the ideas). Training of evaluators involved an introduction to the scales, the scoring method, and typical responses made in previous studies. Fluency scores were obtained by counting the responses that were evaluated as adequate for the relevant DT test. Originality scores, the uniqueness of responses to a given stimuli, were obtained by summing the originality marks of participants’ responses for each DT test, where the marks ranged from 0 = Not original (in Serbian Nije originalan) to 3 = Highly original (Visoko originalan).

Two-way mixed average intraclass correlation coefficients showed sufficient consistency among the raters for all three DT tests: “Unusual use” ICC (2, 3) = .960, p <.001; “Associations” ICC (2, 3) = .904, p <.001; and “Consequences” ICC (2, 3) = .877, p <.001. Ratings were averaged, creating the results of the three DT tests (LeBreton & Senter, 2007). As the correlation coefficients between three DT tests were non-significant or low (ranging from –.12 to .42**), the DT tests were considered as three separate measures.

Collage creativity. Consensual assessment technique was used to assess the creativity of the participants’ collages (Amabile, 1983; Hennessey & Amabile, 1999; Kaufman & Beghetto, 2009). Five art teachers with more than 10 years of work experience in high schools rated each collage. High school teachers were chosen as raters because they interact with people who are not professional artists, similar to the participants in this study. Each rater assessed the collages independently. The raters listed the collages starting from the least creative (collage creativity = 1) to the most creative (collage creativity = 100), with the possibility to give equal number of points to the collages they assessed as equally creative. One of the raters did not show an adequate level of intersubjective consistency and because of this only the assessments of other four raters were used in further analysis. Their consistency was satisfactory ICC (2, 4) = .750, p <.001, and therefore their rates for collage creativity were averaged, creating the variable Collage creativity (Hennessey & Amabile, 1999).

Self-assessment of creativity. The Creativity Scale for Different Domains questionnaire, created by Kaufman and Baer (2004), was used. This questionnaire assesses general creativity and creativity in nine specific areas: science, interpersonal relationships, writing, art, communication, solving own personal problems, mathematics, handicrafts, and physical (body) movements. Each domain was rated on a Likert scale ranging from 1= Not at all (in Serbian Uopšte nisam) to 5=Extremely (Izuzetno). Internal consistency of these 10 items measured by Cronbach alpha was .78, and therefore they were averaged into a single measure.

Interest in art. The participants’ degree of interest in art was determined using four questions related to the number of visits to museums and galleries in the past year, and the degree of interest in art and paintings (Stojilović, 2012). Questions related to the number of visits museums and galleries in the last year were rated on a Likert scale ranging from 0=No visit (in Serbian Nijedanput) to 4=10 or more times (10 ili više puta). Using a similar Likert scale ranging from 0=Not at all (in Serbian Uopšte nisam) to 4=Very much (Veoma mnogo) the degree of interest in art and paintings was rated. These four questions were averaged to obtain an overall value for Interest in art (Cronbach alpha = .82).

Page 7: Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 viewers to be both active and creative

Ivan Stojilović 325

PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339

Aesthetic experience. The participants rated each of the paintings viewed on the following five (seven-point) Likert-type scales: beauty, creativity, interestingness, pleasantness, and comprehensibility. The beauty and creativity of the paintings were used as the dependent variables. These two variables were selected because the experience of beauty is most often associated with the evaluation of a visual artwork, while creativity is one of the most important qualities of the (contemporary) visual art (Perniola, 2005; Vasiljković, 2005). The other three measures of painting preferences (pleasantness, interestingness, and comprehensibility) were used as predictors of ratings on two scales of aesthetic experience. These three measures were introduced as covariates to statistically control for the effects they have on two dependent variables. These covariates include three distinctive characteristics of aesthetic experience: affective, motivational and cognitive aspect (Marković, 2011, 2012). In addition, these three measures were chosen based on previous research which showed that these underlie the higher level factors of aesthetic experience (Stojilović, 2014).

ProcedureStudents were invited in groups of six to eight, and were randomly assigned to one of

two experimental groups. The first activity of the “Collage – Evaluation” group was to make a collage, which sh ould be as creative as possible. Participants were informed that they could use all the materials positioned in front of them (newspapers, coloured paper, crayons, glue) and that they had 45 minutes for the activity. Each participant was given a white A3 piece of paper on which he/she created a collage. This group then evaluated the set of 21 paintings on the 5 aesthetic preference sca les. The paintings, which were presented randomly, were exhibited using an LCD projector, without time limits. A subsequent painting was presented after all participants had completed their ratings of the previous one. The “Evaluation – Collag e” group first rated the paintings and were then given 45 minutes to create a collage. Afterwards, both groups completed the interest in art and self-assessment of creativity questionnaires, and the three DT tests. Finally, the participants received a short debriefing on the purpose of the research.

DesignEighty-two participants observed 21 different paintings and evaluated in terms of

beauty and creativity scales (a total of 3442 ratings, 2 ratings were missing). The independent variables were: time-varying ratings of pleasantness, interestingness, and comprehensibility of paintings; style of the observed painting (three levels: figural, semi-abstract, and abstract); experimental groups (two levels), results on interest in art, three DT tests, self-assessment of creativity, and assessment of collage creativity.

The multilevel modeling o f cr ossed random effects analysis was used (Heck, Thomas, & Tabata, 2013; Hoffman, 2015; Hox, 2010). Multilevel models are statistical models of parameters that vary at more than one level (Hox, 2010). This analysis provides se veral advantages over the traditional univariate or multivariate analysis on one level (Heck et al., 2013) and allows avoiding the problem of whether to do the analysis on the level of rating done by individuals, individuals themselves or paintings. Although multilevel modeling is usually applied to models with a hierarchical structure, multilevel models are also used for models where random factors are crossed at the same level (Locker, Hoffman, & Bovaird, 2007). Crossed random effects analysis allows different combinations of random intercept effects or random slope effects to be specified directly for participant and painting covariates (Yu, 2015). The crossed random effects model is able to model multiple variations – between participants and paintings and their interaction – at the same time. Individual ratings are nested within participants and within the paintings. It has an advantage over hierarchical multilevel models in that it avoids the problem of whether to analyze data on the level of ratings made by individuals within paintings or on the level of paintings within individuals (Fischer, Carbon, Rutar, Stojilović, & Ortlieb, 2016; Stojilović, 2014).

Page 8: Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 viewers to be both active and creative

EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE326

PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339

Results

The results of descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1. For the three DT tests and for collage creativity, column α presents the measures of mutual agreement of raters (ICCs). For all other variables in this column, Cronbach’s alpha values are shown as measures of internal consistency. The scope of reliability is from .75 to .96, and all results can be considered sufficiently consistent (Solomon & Sawilowsky, 2009).Table 1Descriptive statistics (left table) and Pearson correlation coefficients between variables (right table) (N = 82) Variable Range Average SD α 1 2 3 4 5 6 71 Gender F = 73 (89%) M = 9 (11%) 2 Group E-C 37 (45.1%) C-E 45 (54.9%) .163 Collage creativity 6–92 46.06 21.20 .75 -.08 -.164 Unusual use test 1.0–16.3 5.97 2.71 .96 -.05 -.05 .135 Associations test 0.3–12.3 5.65 2.35 .90 -.04 .05 .02 -.116 Consequences test 0.3–13.7 4.46 2.41 .88 -.01 .01 .01 .15 .42**

7 Self-assessed creativity 1.9–4.8 3.34 0.34 .78 .13 -.08 -.05 .23* -.07 -.08

8 Interest in art 0.25–3.75 1.60 0.75 .82 -.18 -.07 .25* .25* -.02 .18 .45**

Note. Gender – females were the referent group. E-C = “Evaluation-Collage” group, C-E = “Collage-Evaluation” group; “Evaluation-Collage” group were the referent group. * p <.05, ** p <.01

The same table also presents the Pearson correlation coefficients of these measures. There is a noticeable absence of correlations between most creativity measures (with only two significant results, which both fall into the category of low to moderate correlation), indicating that the applied measures of creativity assess different concepts, result similar to some previous findings (Silvia, 2011; Simonton, 2003; Zeng, Proctor, & Salvendy, 2011). Collage creativity, the “Unusual use” DT test, and self-assessment of creativity were positively correlated with interest in art. Self-assessment of creativity and “Unusual use” showed a small positive correlation, and the “Associations” and “Consequences” DT tests had a moderate correlation.

Effects of creativity on aesthetic experienceA multivariate analysis of crossed random effects modelling was conducted

to further analyse the data. The restricted maximum likelihood method was used to evaluate the model. The significance of fixed effects was assessed using p values of the Wald test, and the significance of random effects was estimated using – 2ΔLL likelihood ratio tests and informative criteria (AIC and BIC) between two models that include the same fixed effects (Hoffman, 2015). At both levels (participants and paintings), the diagonal correlation matrix was defined, which allows independent variance for the intercepts of the two aesthetic experience scales. The values for denominator degrees of freedom were obtained by a Satterthwaite correction.

Page 9: Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 viewers to be both active and creative

Ivan Stojilović 327

PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339

The beauty and creativity ratings of the paintings were the dependent variables, used to represent a “cluster” of aesthetic experience. Simultaneous modelling of creativity and beauty ratings allows adjustment of parameters for the expected correlations between ratings on two scales. Ratings of pleasantness, interestingness, and comprehensibility of the observed paintings were time-varying covariates whose values varied with each new aesthetic evaluation. Measures of participants’ creativity, interest in art, and experimental group assignment were predictors at the level of the participants, and the style of paintings was a predictor at the level of the paintings; all variables had unchanging values during the aesthetic evaluations. All covariates were centered on the grand mean.

The first model included individual ratings of pleasantness, interestingness, and comprehensibility of the paintings. All three covariates had a significant effect on beauty and creativity ratings. In the next model (model I, Table 2), the style of the painting was included as a factor. In the model II, the effects of creativity measures and interest in art on the subjective experience of the paintings were tested. After the fixed effects have been included in the model II, in the final model III it was examined to what extent the effects of the paintings predictors show systematic individual differences by adding subject random slopes for the painting predictors to the model. All statistically insignificant effects and interactions (based on the F test value) were removed.

Table 2Unstandardized regression coefficients of the predictors in the models (with standard errors in parentheses)

Model I (variables at the level of

paintings added)

Model II (variables at the level of

participants added)

Model III(adding subject random slopes

for the painting predictors)Fixed part – Parameter Creativity Beauty Creativity Beauty Creativity BeautyFigural paintings (from model II ref. C-E group) 4.340 (0.124) 4.497 (0.089) 4.431 (0.131) 4.518 (0.097) 4.447 (0.132) 4.538 (0.097)Semi-abstract paintings (from model II ref. C-E group)

4.747 (0.109) 4.102 (0.069) 4.801 (0.118) 4.115 (0.079) 4.815 (0.119) 4.135 (0.081)

Abstract paintings (from model II ref. C-E group) 4.308 (0.113) 4.225 (0.074) 4.489 (0.122) 4.190 (0.084) 4.493 (0.122) 4.204 (0.085)Figural painting x pleasantness rating 0.066 (0.037) 0.419 (0.036) 0.067 (0.037) 0.420 (0.036) 0.092 (0.038) 0.407 (0.037)Semi-abstract painting x pleasantness rating 0.074 (0.034) 0.530 (0.034) 0.081 (0.035) 0.534 (0.034) 0.091 (0.037) 0.541 (0.036)Abstract painting x pleasantness rating 0.082 (0.035) 0.573 (0.034) 0.089 (0.035) 0.571 (0.034) 0.082 (0.037) 0.577 (0.036)Figural painting x interestingness rating 0.705 (0.033) 0.382 (0.032) 0.701 (0.033) 0.364 (0.032) 0.660 (0.037) 0.348 (0.035)Semi-abstract painting x interestingness rating 0.632 (0.030) 0.312 (0.029) 0.625 (0.030) 0.299 (0.029) 0.612 (0.033) 0.301 (0.032)Abstract painting x interestingness rating 0.694 (0.031) 0.347 (0.030) 0.695 (0.031) 0.339 (0.030) 0.684 (0.035) 0.327 (0.032)Figural painting x comprehensibility rating -0.074 (0.041) 0.189 (0.040) -0.075 (0.041) 0.185 (0.040) -0.085 (0.044) 0.197 (0.041)

Page 10: Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 viewers to be both active and creative

EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE328

PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339

Model I (variables at the level of

paintings added)

Model II (variables at the level of

participants added)

Model III(adding subject random slopes

for the painting predictors)Semi-abstract painting x comprehensibility rating 0.060 (0.031) 0.118 (0.030) 0.053 (0.031) 0.106 (0.030) 0.037 (0.034) 0.092 (0.031)Abstract painting x comprehensibility rating 0.021 (0.030) 0.130 (0.029) 0.019 (0.029) 0.134 (0.029) 0.026 (0.033) 0.141 (0.03)Figural paintings (E-C group) -0.204 (0.106) -0.041 (0.089) -0.247 (0.112) -0.063 (0.093)

Semi-abstract paintings (E-C group) -0.112 (0.106) -0.033 (0.090) -0.161 (0.108) -0.077 (0.091)

Abstract paintings (E-C group) -0.405 (0.106) 0.081 (0.089) -0.432 (0.111) 0.090 (0.092)

Figural painting x collage creativity rating 0.000 (0.003) -0.003 (0.002) 0.000 (0.003) -0.004 (0.002)

Semi-abstract painting x collage creativity rating 0.002 (0.003) 0.006 (0.002) 0.002 (0.003) 0.007 (0.002)Abstract painting x collage creativity rating 0.003 (0.003) 0.005 (0.002) 0.002 (0.003) 0.006 (0.002)Figural painting x “Unusual use” DT test 0.004 (0.020) 0.046 (0.017) 0.001 (0.021) 0.048 (0.017)Semi-abstract painting x “Unusual use” DT test 0.013 (0.020) 0.048 (0.017) 0.012 (0.020) 0.049 (0.017)Abstract painting x “Unusual use” DT test -0.022 (0.019) 0.019 (0.016) -0.019 (0.020) 0.023 (0.017)

Random effects and residualsResidual variance 0.820 (0.020) 0.815 (0.020) 0.733 (0.028) 0.706 (0.027)Subject Random Intercept Variance 0.113 (0.024) 0.052 (0.014) 0.104 (0.023) 0.040 (0.013) 0.106 (0.024) 0.042 (0.013)Subject Random Pleasantness Slope Variance

0.009 (0.004) 0.013 (0.005)

Subject Random Interestingness Slope Variance

0.014 (0.005) 0.013 (0.004)

Subject Random Comprehensibility Slope Variance

0.015 (0.005) 0.007 (0.004)

Paintings Random Intercept Variance 0.059 (0.024) 0.014 (0.009) 0.059 (0.024) 0.015 (0.009) 0.058 (0.024) 0.017 (0.009)

Note. E-C = Evaluation-Collage, C-E = Collage-Evaluation, ref. = referent group, DT = divergent thinking. Bold values are significant at the level p <.05, italic values are significant at the level p <.10.

Evaluations of beauty in all three styles of paintings were most strongly correlated with ratings of pleasantness (bs from 0.41 to 0.58, Cohen`s ds from 0.43 to 0.72), followed by interestingness (bs from 0.30 to 0.35, ds from 0.41 to 0.51); ratings of painting comprehensibility had the weakest effect (bs from 0.09 to 0.20, all bs significant at the level of .05, ds from 0.13 to 0.22, Table 2, model III).

Creativity evaluations were most correlated with interestingness ratings (bs from 0.63 to 0.70, ds from 0.74 to 0.90), and had a much lower correlation with pleasantness ratings (bs from 0.08 to 0.09, ds from 0.09 to 0.10, all bs significant at the level of .05). Painting comprehensibility ratings had no effect on creativity evaluations.

Page 11: Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 viewers to be both active and creative

Ivan Stojilović 329

PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339

For both experimental groups, figural paintings were rated as significantly more beautiful than semi-abstract and abstract paintings. On the scale of creativity, both groups evaluated semi-abstract paintings as more creative than figural and abstract paintings.

There was a significant effect of participation in creative activities on the aesthetic experience of paintings (F (6, 185.3) = 3.20, p <.01). The group that created a collage before observing the paintings rated the abstract and figural paintings as significantly more creative than the group that observed the paintings before creating a collage (b = 0.43, t (194.3) = 3.89, p <.01, d = 0.28 and b = 0.25, t (199.6) = 2.19, p <.05, d = 0.16, respectively for abstract and figural paintings).

Two other measures of creativity had a significant effect on the aesthetic experience of the observed paintings: collage creativity (F (6, 188.6) = 3.65, p <.01) and the “Unusual use” DT test (F (6, 192.0) = 2.63, p <.05). Both of these predictors had a weak effect on aesthetic experience. Participants whose collages were evaluated as more creative perceived the semi-abstract and abstract paintings as more beautiful than those whose collages were considered less creative (b = 0.007, t (292.2) = 3.25, p <.01, d = 0.19 and b = 0.006, t (285.5) = 2.60, p <.05, d = 0.15 respectively for semi-abstract and abstract paintings). Perceived beauty for the figural and semi-abstract paintings increased with higher “Unusual use” DT test scores (b = 0.005, t (286.1) = 2.84, p <.01, d = 0.17 and b = 0.005, t (289.2) = 2.91, p <.01, d = 0.17 respectively for figural and semi-abstract paintings).

In the final model, the variance was significantly explained at the level of participants (pseudo R2

Participants = .605) and at the level of paintings (pseudo R2

Paintings = .998). These findings indicate a strong relationship between the predictors entered and the aesthetic experience of paintings in the observed population. The model explained 65.2% of the residual variance.

Discussion

This exploratory study examined the relationship between different measures of creativity and the aesthetic experience of visual art. The visual art domain is considered the prototypical domain of creative activity (Sawyer, 2012). The results show that there is a weak positive correlation between some measures of creativity and aesthetic preferences. Hypotheses 1 and 2 have been partially confirmed, while hypothesis 3 has been fully confirmed. Hypothesis 1 has been confirmed in part, as performing a creative activity (collage making) prior to the evaluation of paintings was shown to have a positive influence on ratings of abstract painting creativity, but the impact on beauty ratings was not confirmed. In addition, there was a significant impact on the evaluation of figural painting creativity, which the original hypothesis did not account for. We hypothesized that people with higher scores on measures of creativity would prefer abstract artwork (hypothesis 2). Two measures of creativity were weakly

Page 12: Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 viewers to be both active and creative

EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE330

PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339

positively correlated with the perceived beauty of paintings, namely collage creativity and the “Unusual use” DT test.

The results revealed a lack of correlation between the measures of creativity employed (hypothesis 3); the low and absence of correlation between the three DT tests was particularly unexpected. Although all three DT tests are of the same type (verbal), and they require rich ideation, fluency and mental attitudes, their results did not converge. These results indicate that the measures of creativity employed actually assess unrelated phenomena. The low correlation among different measures supports a domain-specific understanding of creativity (Baer, 2010, 2011; Ivcevic, 2007; Kaufman & Baer, 2004; Kozbelt, Dexter, Dolese, Meredith, & Ostrofsky, 2014; Rawlings & Locarnini, 2007). Namely, “[d]omain generality would be supported by high intercorrelations among different creative behaviors [...], while domain specificity would be supported by relatively low correlations among different behaviors” (Ivcevic, 2007, p. 272). Domain-specific theories of creativity argue “that the skills and other factors leading to creative performance vary across domains” (Baer, 2011, p. 77).

Our study found that two creativity measures positively correlating with aesthetic preferences were directly related to the visual art domain: creating collages acts and collage creativity.. Therefore, we may conclude that in choosing the measures of creativity and studying their effects on the aesthetic preferences it is important to use measures of creativity corresponding to the visual art domain as much as possible. Another finding is that these two measures of creativity were both positively correlated with abstract painting style, but on different aspects of the aesthetic experience (beauty and creativity). Below we propose different mechanisms of action for these two measures of creativity. However, it is important to note that we refer to two different aspects of creativity: the process and the product. Also, a creative act is a process presented as a discrete measure, defined based on whether the participant took part in the creative act before evaluating the paintings or not. The collage is a product whose creativity is evaluated by a continuous measure, obtained through consensual interpersonal assessment.

Participation in creative activities prior to aesthetic evaluations resulted in the figural and abstract paintings being perceived as more creative than when ratings were made immediately. The created collages were most often in the domain of semi-abstract works that served as a means of personal expression. Creative activity introduces a participant to a freer and more flexible state of mind, especially for new and unusual works such as abstract paintings. In studies by Nelson and Rawlings (Nelson, 2005; Nelson & Rawlings, 2007, 2009, 2010), several specific characteristics of professional artists’ subjective experience of the creative process were obtained, such as deep absorption into the process itself, the state of the inspiration, the feeling of satisfaction with the process of creation, a sense of excitement and freedom, emotional sensitivity, and the feeling of the flow. Although we cannot assume the existence of all these processes and their full experience within naive creators (especially in

Page 13: Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 viewers to be both active and creative

Ivan Stojilović 331

PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339

laboratory settings), we can presume they exist in a rudimentary form coinciding with a general feeling of comfort during the creative activity. It is assumed that self-expression, a free mind, and a heightened positive mood lead to a more vivid experience of artwork. Other possible explanation is that the process of creating collages influenced the participants during the assessment of paintings to better understand and appreciate more the efforts of artists to create an original artwork. Because of this, after a creative act, the preference of originality of the observed paintings has been increased.

Figure 1. Examples of participants’ collages

The measure of collage creativity was positively and weakly correlated with the rating of semi-abstract and abstract paintings as beautiful. It is assumed that the characteristic of Openness to experience underlies the production of creative collages and a greater preference for these artworks. Creative people are often described as spontaneous and free, unconventional, independent, open, striving to expand their own experience, having flexible attitudes, which can be subsumed under the personality dimension of openness to experience (Costa & McCrae, 2008; Ivcevic, 2007). Openness includes many essential processes

Page 14: Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 viewers to be both active and creative

EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE332

PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339

that are involved in creativity in the visual art domain, including: active imagination, aesthetic sensitivity, preference of diversity, intellectual curiosity, and independent thinking. Openness is linked to greater preference of: the visual arts in general (Chamorro-Premuzic, Reimers, Hsu, & Ahmetoglu, 2009; Feist & Brady, 2004); pop art paintings (Furnham & Walker, 2001); sophisticated, intense, mellow, and contemporary style (Cleridou & Furnham, 2014); and particularly abstract style (Chamorro-Premuzic et al., 2009; Feist & Brady, 2004; Furnham & Walker, 2001; Rawlings, 2000; Swami & Furnham, 2014). More open, flexible, imaginative people on the one hand produce more creative and original solutions, and on the other hand respond more positively to unclear, unconventional stimuli. To explain this relationship we may apply a concept wider than openness, such as the recently suggested concept of an “Aesthetic Quotient” (AQ). AQ comprises various sources of individual differences — openness, sensitivity to complexity, art knowledge, aesthetic empathy, attention, exploratory perception tendencies — important both for aesthetic evaluation, but also for creative production (Myszkowski & Zenasni, 2016).

Participants with higher scores on the “Unusual use” DT perceived comprehensible (figural and semi-abstract) paintings as more beautiful than those with lower scores, hence this kind of verbal task is somehow related to the figural visual art domain. People with higher “Unusual use” DT test scores have richer ideation and a more vivid imagination, and there is a correlation between this test and fluid intelligence test results (Batey & Furnham, 2006; Furnham, Batey, Booth, Patel, & Lozinskaya, 2011). As the comprehensibility of paintings increases, it is assumed that people with a vivid imagination add their own content and descriptions to the observed paintings, enriching and embellishing them, while those with low ideation linger on what is presented within a painting. We assume that the evaluation of comprehensible paintings as more beautiful is related to the greater flexibility of thinking and ideation, primarily on the specific material as it concerned the “Unusual use” DT test.

The weakly positive or absent correlation between different measures of creativity represents a significant limitation of this study, but it also indicates the nature of creativity. This clearly confirms that “creativity is extremely difficult to define and measure” (Runco, 2004, p. 20). A probable reason for the lack of correlation between the creativity measures employed is their reference to different domains of creativity (Furnham et al., 2011). While the DT tests are primarily based on verbal skills and ideation fluency, the creation of collages is related to the openness and perception, and the self-assessment of creativity is associated with openness. Creativity may also involve problem solving, detecting problems, or self-expression, which may have completely different, and potentially contrasting, measures. Therefore, although the weak correlation between the creativity measures points to a potential deficiency of this research, it also highlights the complexity of studying creativity and suggests that the term

Page 15: Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 viewers to be both active and creative

Ivan Stojilović 333

PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339

should be defined more clearly so that it can undergo rigorous scientific analysis (Baer, 1994; Clapham, 2004; Kaufman, Baer, Cole, & Sexton, 2008).

Another potential shortcoming of this study is the problematic generalization of the findings outside the group who participated in the study. Psychology students are distinguished by particular characteristics that may restrict more general application of the results. Perhaps the biggest issue with the study is its ecological validity. It is hard to conduct a laboratory-based study with rigorous control that is compatible with originality and creativity. Strict control of the research conditions in such studies must always be balanced against constructing a context that does not limit creative production. In our study, attention was paid to creating conditions that would enhance creativity (an encouraging experimenter, an emphasis on originality, reassurance that there were no unusual or wrong solutions, working in small groups, dim light), but the context itself significantly limited creative capacities, particularly those of introverted and socially inhibited persons. It is recommended that similar research is replicated in a more natural, non-laboratory environment and that the potentially negative effects of a social environment are better controlled.

Conclusion

In sum, despite the abovementioned limitations, this study is one of the first to examine the relationship between creativity and aesthetic experience. The paper used several indicators of creativity, among which the creative act (making of the collage), collage creativity and the “Unusual use” DT test proved to be significant predictors of aesthetic preference. Those with higher scores on these measures enjoyed the observed paintings more. The weak correlation between the creativity measures used makes it more difficult to identify a unique effect of creativity on aesthetic experience. We suggest that the common feature of these creativity measures is their relation to the visual arts domain. Creativity generates greater openness to new and unusual experiences, it enhances positive mood, and leads to greater flexibility and freedom of ideation, which positively reflects on a stronger preference for the paintings. Few studies in the field of psychology of art and creativity have investigated the relationship between creativity and aesthetic preferences; however, our research shows important links between the two phenomena and provides new clues for future studies.

The results obtained indicate that the experience of artwork is a creative act. A stronger preference for certain visual artworks is influenced by participation in a creative process and by a person’s creativity in the visual arts domain. An artwork does not attain its final shape when an artist finishes his/her work, but is continuously being recreated by the audience. Creativity is important throughout the “life” of an art object, from the moment the artist first sets their eyes on it until the last beholder experiences it.

Page 16: Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 viewers to be both active and creative

EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE334

PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339

References

Aks, D. J., & Sprott, J. C. (1996). Quantifying aesthetic preference for chaotic patterns. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 14(1), 1–16. doi:10.2190/6v31–7m9r-t9l5-cdg9

Amabile, T. M. (1983). The social psychology of creativity (Vol. 11). New York: Springer-Verlag.Augustin, M. D., Wagemans, J., & Carbon, C. C. (2012). All is beautiful? Generality vs.

specificity of word usage in visual aesthetics. Acta Psychologica, 139(1), 187–201. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.10.004

Baer, J. (1994). Divergent thinking is not a general trait: A multidomain training experiment. Creativity Research Journal, 7(1), 35–46. doi:10.1080/10400419409534507

Baer, J. (2010). Is Creativity Domain Specific? In J. C. Kaufman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 321–341). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Baer, J. (2011). Why grand theories of creativity distort, distract, and disappoint. IJCPS-International Journal of Creativity and Problem Solving, 21(1), 73–100.

Barron, F. (1993). Controllable oddness as a resource in creativity. Psychological Inquiry, 4(3), 182–184. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli0403_3

Batey, M., & Furnham, A. (2006). Creativity, intelligence, and personality: A critical review of the scattered literature. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 132(4), 355–429. doi:10.3200/mono.132.4.355–430

Beghetto, R. A., & Kaufman, J. C. (2014). Classroom contexts for creativity. High Ability Studies, 25(1), 53–69. doi:10.1080/13598139.2014.905247

Besemer, S., & O’Quin, K. (1986). Analyzing creative products: Refinement and test of a judging instrument. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 20(2), 115–126. doi:10.1002/j.2162–6057.1986.tb00426.x

Carroll, N. (2002). Aesthetic experience revisited. The British Journal of Aesthetics, 42(2), 145–168. doi:10.1093/bjaesthetics/42.2.145

Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Reichenbacher, L. (2008). Effects of personality and threat of evaluation on divergent and convergent thinking. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(4), 1095–1101. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2007.12.007

Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Reimers, S., Hsu, A., & Ahmetoglu, G. (2009). Who art thou? Personality predictors of artistic preferences in a large UK sample: the importance of openness. British Journal of Psychology, 100(3), 501–516. doi:10.1348/000712608X366867

Clapham, M. M. (2004). The Convergent Validity of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking and Creativity Interest Inventories. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 64(5), 828–841. doi:10.1177/0013164404263883

Cleridou, K., & Furnham, A. (2014). Personality Correlates of Aesthetic Preferences for Art, Architecture, and Music. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 32(2), 231–255. doi:10.2190/EM.32.2.f

Collingwood, R. G. (1938). The principles of art (Vol. 11). London: Oxford University Press.Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (2008). The Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R).

The SAGE handbook of personality theory and assessment, Volume 2 — Personality Measurement and Testing, 179–198. doi:10.4135/9781849200479.n9

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (199 6). Creativity: flow and the psychology of discovery and invention (Vol. 1st ed.). New York: Harper Perennial.

Cupchik, G. C. (1999). Pe rception and Creativity. In M. A. Runco & S. R. Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Creativity (1st ed.)(pp. 355–360). San Diego, USA: Academic Press.

Dollinger, S. J., Urban, K . K., & James, T. A. (2004). Creativity and openness: Further validation of two creative product measures. Creativity Research Journal, 16(1), 35–47. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1601_4

Eysenck, H. J. (1993). Crea tivity and personality: Suggestions for a theory. Psychological Inquiry, 4(3), 147–178. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli0403_1

Page 17: Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 viewers to be both active and creative

Ivan Stojilović 335

PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339

Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta- analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and social psychology review, 2(4), 290–309. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0204_5

Feist, G. J. (2010). The func tion of personality in creativity: The nature and nurture of the creative personality. In J. C. Kaufman & R. Sternberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 113–130). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Feist, G. J., & Brady, T. R. ( 2004). Openness to experience, non-conformity, and the preference for abstract art. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 22(1), 77–89. doi:10.2190/Y7CA-TBY6-V7LR-76GK

Fischer, U. C., Carbon, C. C., Rutar, D., Stojilović, I., & Ortlieb, S. (2016). The big picture of aesthetics changes depending on the level of analysis. Paper presented at the European Conference on Visual Perception, Barcelona.

Funch, B. S. (1997). The psycho logy of art appreciation: Museum Tuscul anum Press.Furnham, A., Batey, M., Booth, T. W., Patel, V., & Lozinskaya, D. (2011). Individual difference

predictors of creativity in Art and Science students. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 6(2), 114–121. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2011.01.006

Furnham, A., Crump, J., Batey, M ., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2009). Personality and ability predictors of the “Consequences” Test of divergent thinking in a large non-student sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 46(4), 536–540. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.12.007

Furnham, A., & Rao, S. (2002). Pe rsonality and the aesthetics of composition: A study of Mondrian and Hirst. North American Journal of Psychology, 4(2), 233–242.

Furnham, A., & Walker, J. (2001). Personality and judgements of abstract, pop art, and representational paintings. European Journal of Personality, 15(1), 57–72. doi:10.1002/per.340

Giannini, A. M., & Bonaiuto, P. (19 99). Contrasting aesthetic evaluations of “Graffiti” images as a function of incongruity intolerance levels. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 17(2), 131–154. doi:10.2190/nxu5-xbn6–0n5q-djhg

Guilford, J. P. (1962). Factors that aid and hinder creativity. Teachers College Record.Heck, R. H., Thomas, S. L., & Tabata, L. N. (2013). Multilevel and longitudinal modeling

with IBM SPSS. New York: Routledge.Hennessey, B. A., & Amabile, T. M. (1 999). Consensual Assessment. In M. A. Runco & S. R.

Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Creativity (pp. 34–36). San Diego, USA: Academic Press.Hoffman, L. (2015). Longitudinal analy sis: Modeling within-person fluctuation and change.

New York: Routledge.Hox, J. J. (2010). Multilevel Analysis : Techniques and Applications (2nd ed.). New York:

Routledge.Ivcevic, Z. (2007). Artistic and Every day Creativity: An Act‐Frequency Approach. The

Journal of Creative Behavior, 41(4), 271–290. doi:10.1002/j.2162–6057.2007.tb01074.xKaufman, J. C., & Baer, J. (2004). Sure, I’m Creative—But Not in Mathematics!: Self-

Reported Creativity in Diverse Domains. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 22(2), 143–155. doi:10.2190/26hq-vhe8-gtln-bjjm

Kaufman, J. C., Baer, J., Cole, J. C., & Se xton, J. D. (2008). A Comparison of Expert and Nonexpert Raters Using the Consensual Assessment Technique. Creativity Research Journal, 20(2), 171–178. doi:10.1080/10400410802059929

Kaufman, J. C., & Beghetto, R. A. (2009). Be yond big and little: The four c model of creativity. Review of General Psychology, 13(1), 1–12. doi:10.1037/a0013688

Kaufman, J. C., Plucker, J. A., & Baer, J. (2 008). Essentials of creativity assessment (Vol. 53). New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons.

Kaufman, S. B., & Paul, E. S. (2014). Creativ ity and schizophrenia spectrum disorders across the arts and sciences. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1145. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01145

Koestler, A. (1964). The act of creation. Lon don, UK: Hutchinson & Co.

Page 18: Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 viewers to be both active and creative

EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE336

PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339

Kozbelt, A., Dexter, S., Dolese, M., Meredith , D., & Ostrofsky, J. (2014). Regressive Imagery in Creative Problem‐Solving: Comparing Verbal Protocols of Expert and Novice Visual Artists and Computer Programmers. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 49(4), 263–278. doi:10.1002/jocb.64

LeBreton, J. M., & Senter, J. L. (2007). Answer s to 20 Questions About Interrater Reliability and Interrater Agreement. Organizational Research Methods, 11(4), 815–852. doi:10.1177/1094428106296642

Locker, L., Hoffman, L., & Bovaird, J. A. (2007) . On the use of multilevel modeling as an alternative to items analysis in psycholinguistic research. Behavior Research Methods, 39(4), 723–730. doi:10.3758/bf03192962

Marković, S. (2011). Perceptual, semantic and aff ective dimensions of experience of abstract and representational paintings. Psihologija, 44(3), 191–210. doi:10.2298/psi1103191m

Marković, S. (2012). Components of aesthetic exper ience: aesthetic fascination, aesthetic appraisal, and aesthetic emotion. Iperception, 3(1), 1–17. doi:10.1068/i0450aap

Maslow, A. H. (1971). The farther reaches of human nature. Oxford, England: Viking.McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinkin g, and openness to experience. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 1258–1265. doi:10.1037/0022–3514.52.6.1258Myszkowski, N., Storme, M., Zenasni, F., & Lubart, T. (2014). Is visual aesthetic sensitivity

independent from intelligence, personality and creativity? Personality and Individual Differences, 59, 16–20. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.10.021

Myszkowski, N., & Zenasni, F. (2016). Individual Diffe rences in Aesthetic Ability: The Case for an Aesthetic Quotient. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 750. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00750

Nelson, C. B. (2005). The creative process: A phenomen ological and psychometric investigation of artistic creativity. (PhD thesis), University of Melbourne. Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.net/11343/38999

Nelson, C. B., & Rawlings, D. (2007). Its Own Reward: A Phenomenological Study of Artistic Creativity. Journal of Phenomenological Psychology, 38(2), 217–255. doi:10.1163/156916207X234284

Nelson, C. B., & Rawlings, D. (2009). How Does It Feel? The Development of the Experience of Creativity Questionnaire. Creativity Research Journal, 21(1), 43–53. doi:10.1080/10400410802633442

Nelson, C. B., & Rawlings, D. (2010). Relating schizotyp y and personality to the phenomenology of creativity. Schizophrenia bulletin, 36(2), 388–399. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbn098

Perniola, M. (2005). Estetika dvadesetog veka [20th Centu ry Aesthetics]. Novi Sad, Serbia: Svetovi.

Plucker, J. A., Beghetto, R. A., & Dow, G. T. (2004). Why Isn’t Creativity More Important to Educational Psychologists? Potentials, Pitfalls, and Future Directions in Creativity Research. Educational Psychologist, 39(2), 83–96. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep3902_1

Plucker, J. A., & Makel, M. C. (2010). Assessment of creat ivity. In J. C. Kaufman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 48–73). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Rawlings, D. (2000). The interaction of openness to experie nce and schizotypy in predicting preference for abstract and violent paintings. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 18(1), 69–92. doi:10.2190/71ct-aa49-xrmg-c842

Rawlings, D., Hodge, M., Sherr, D., & Dempsey, A. (1995). To ughmindedness and preference for musical excerpts, categories and triads. Psychology of Music, 23(1), 63–80. doi:10.1177/0305735695231005

Rawlings, D., & Locarnini, A. (2007). Validating the Creativi ty Scale for Diverse Domains Using Groups of Artists and Scientists. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 25(2), 163–172. doi:10.2190/q538–2307–2627–1256

Page 19: Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 viewers to be both active and creative

Ivan Stojilović 337

PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339

Reber, R., Schwarz, N., & Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluen cy and aesthetic pleasure: is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience? Personality and social psychology review, 8(4), 364–382. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0804_3

Rhodes, M. (1961). An analysis of creativity. Phi Delta Kappan, 42 (7), 305–310.Richards, R. E. (2007). Everyday Creativity: Our Hidden Potential. In R. E. Richards (Ed.),

Everyday creativity and new views of human nature: Psychological, social, and spiritual perspectives (pp. 25–53). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Rogers, C. R. (1961). On becoming a person. Boston: Houghton Mifflin .Runco, M. A. (2004). Everyone has creative potential. In R. J. Stern berg, E. L. Grigorenko, &

J. L. Singer (Eds.), Creativity: From potential to realization (pp. 21–30). Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association.

Runco, M. A. (2007). To Understand is to Create: An Epistemological P erspective on Human Nature and Personal Creativity. In R. E. Richards (Ed.), Everday Creativity and New Views of Human Nature: Psychological, Social, and Spiritual Perspectives (pp. 91–108). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Runco, M. A., Dow, G., & Smith, W. R. (2006). Information, experience, and divergent thinking: An empirical test. Creativity Research Journal, 18(3), 269–277. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1803_4

Sawyer, R. K. (2012). Explaining creativity: The science of human innov ation (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

Scruton, R. (2009). Beauty (Vol. 262). London, UK: Oxford University Pr ess.Shimamura, A. P. (2013). Experiencing Art: In the Brain of the Beholder . New York: Oxford

University Press.Silvia, P. J. (2009). Looking past pleasure: Anger, confusion, disgust, pride, surprise, and

other unusual aesthetic emotions. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3(1), 48–51. doi:10.1037/a0014632

Silvia, P. J. (2011). Subjective scoring of divergent thinking: Examinin g the reliability of unusual uses, instances, and consequences tasks. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 6(1), 24–30. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2010.06.001

Silvia, P. J., Winterstein, B. P., Willse, J. T., Barona, C. M., Cram, J. T., Hess, K. I., . . . Richard, C. A. (2008). Assessing creativity with divergent thinking tasks: Exploring the reliability and validity of new subjective scoring methods. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2(2), 68–85. doi:10.1037/1931–3896.2.2.68

Simonton, D. K. (2003). Expertise, competence, and creative ability. In R. J. Sternberg & E. L. Grigorenko (Eds.), The psychology of abilities, competencies, and expertise (pp. 213–240). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Simonton, D. K. (2012). Taking the US Patent Office criteria seriously: A qu antitative three-criterion creativity definition and its implications. Creativity Research Journal, 24(2–3), 97–106. doi:10.1080/10400419.2012.676974

Solomon, S. R., & Sawilowsky, S. S. (2009). Impact of rank-based normalizing t ransformations on the accuracy of test scores. Journal of Modern Applied Statistical Methods, 8(2), 448–462.

Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1999). The concept of creativity: Prospects and paradigms. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (Vol. 1, pp. 3–15). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Stojilović, I. (2012). Eksperimentalna provera teorije procene (Experimental examination of Appraisal Theory). (Magister). Univerzitet u Beogradu, Belgrade.

Stojilović, I. (2014). Evaluation of Paintings: Effects of lectures. Psihologija, 47(4), 415–432. doi: 10.2298/PSI1404415S

Swami, A., & Furnham, A. (2014). Personality and Aesthetic experiences. In P. P. Tinio & J. K. Smith (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of the Psychology of Aesthetics and Arts (pp. 540–561). London, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Page 20: Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 viewers to be both active and creative

EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE338

PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339

Tinio, P. (2013). From artistic creation to aesthetic reception: The mirror model of art. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7(3), 265–275. doi:10.1037/a0030872

Vartanian, O. (2014). Empirical aesthetics: hindsight and foresight. In P. P. L. T inio & J. K. Smith (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of the Psychology of Aesthetics and the Arts (pp. 6–34). London, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Vasiljković, K. (2005). Metafizika i dijalektika fenomena lepo versus genius loci [ Metaphysics and dialectic phenomena versus nice genius loci]. In M. Zurovac & N. Grubor (Eds.), Položaj lepog u estetici [Position of the beautiful in aesthetics]. Beograd: Estetičko društvo Srbije.

Yu, H.-T. (2015). Applying linear mixed effects models with crossed random effects to psycholinguistic data: Multilevel specification and model selection. The Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 11(2), 78–88. doi:10.20982/tqmp.11.2.p078

Zausner, T. (2007). Artist and audience: Everyday creativity and visual art. In R. E . Richards (Ed.), Everyday creativity and new views of human nature: Psychological, social, and spiritual perspectives (pp. 75–89). Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association.

Zeng, L., Proctor, R. W., & Salvendy, G. (2011). Can Traditional Divergent Thinking T ests Be Trusted in Measuring and Predicting Real-World Creativity? Creativity Research Journal, 23(1), 24–37. doi:10.1080/10400419.2011.545713

Ziv, N., & Keydar, E. (2009). The Relationship Between Creative Potential, Aesthetic R esponse to Music, and Musical Preferences. Creativity Research Journal, 21(1), 125–133. doi:10.1080/10400410802633764

RECEIVED 14.11.2016.REVISION RECEIVED 09.04.2017.

ACCEPTED 13.07.2017.

© 2017 by the Serbian Psychological Association

This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution ShareAlike 4.0 International license

Page 21: Effects of creativity on aesthetic experience · 320 EFFECTS OF CREATIVITY ON AESTHETIC EXPERIENCE PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339 viewers to be both active and creative

Ivan Stojilović 339

PSIHOLOGIJA, 2017, Vol. 50(3), 319–339

Appendix A

List of paintings used in the studyFigural Semi-abstract AbstractPieter Bruegel, Landscape with the Fall of Icarus (1558)

Jean-Michel Basquiat, Untitled (Fallen Angel), 1981

Josef Albers, Homage to the Square (1956)

Claude Lorrain, Landscape With The Marriage Of Isaac And Rebekah (1648)

Francis Bacon, Figure Writing Reflected In a Mirror (1976)

Wassily Kandinsky, On White II (1923)

Giotto, Resurrection (Noli Me Tangere) (circa 1306)

Willem de Kooning. Woman V (1953)

Howard Hodgkin, Reading in Bed (1990–91)

Giorgone, The Tempest (1508)

Peter Doig, Hitchhiker (1990)

Morris Louis, Where (1960)

Titian, The Rape of Europa (1562)

Anselm Kiefer, Nuremberg (1982)

Mark Rothko, Orange and Yellow (1956)

Nicolas Poussin, The Funeral of Phocion (1648)

Luc Tuymans, Gas Chamber (1986)

Antoni Tàpies, Figura paisaje en gris (1956)

Salomon van Ruysdael, Landscape with Cornfields (1638)

Neo Rauch, Weiter (2005) Cy Twombly, Naples (1961)