Communication Failure

download Communication Failure

of 4

Transcript of Communication Failure

  • 7/29/2019 Communication Failure

    1/4

    Communication Failure

    Bosco, F.M., Tirassa, M. (2010)Communication failure.In: The pragmatics encyclopedia, ed. L. Cummings (pp. 63-65).London and New York: Routledge.

    Francesca M. Bosco and Maurizio Tirassa

    Universit di TorinoDipartimento di Psicologia & Centro di Scienza Cognitivavia Po, 1410123 Torino (Italy)email [email protected], [email protected]

    Successful communicationis to be defined in terms of the partners recognition of aparticular set of mental states of the speaker, in whom there is the intention to achieve such an

    effect on the partner (Grice 1989). Within this perspective, communication failure may be

    defined as an unsuccessful attempt on the part of the speaker to modify the partners mentalstates in the desired way. Failure repair then is a new attempt to produce the intended

    communicative effect. Traditionally, a major research perspective on the topic has

    been conversation analysis (CA). However, communication failures have received little

    attention in recent years.

    Within a CA perspective, Schegloff et al. (1977) showed that a structural-based preference

    for self- versus other-initiation of repair exists in turn-takingafter a breakdown (see also Zahn

    1984). Friedland and Miller (1998) also found that in brain-injured patients over 80 per cent

    of repairs were self-initiated. Fox and Jasperson (1995) classified different types of self-repair

    based on the operation included: repeating or recycling, replacing or substituting, adding or

    inserting, and abandoning or restarting. Self-repair is preferably initiated in the same turn

    where the trouble has occurred or in the third turn to the trouble source turn, i.e. the turnsubsequent to the one which follows the trouble source (Schegloff et al. 1977; but see also

    Schegloff 1992, 1997a). Self-repairmay be carried out in response to other-initiation of repair

    (Schegloff 2004). When the repair is initiated by the partner, it is almost always initiated in

    the turn that follows the trouble source (Schegloff 1997b).Robinson (2006) has argued

    thatthe meaning of other-initiated forms of repair can affect the speakers response. Forexample, if the trouble-source speaker understands himself to be blameworthy for the

    breakdown, he is more likely to correct, rather than merely repeat, the trouble source, and to

    engage in other types of accounting behaviours, such as apologizing.CA aims at a descriptive analysis of communication failures, which it views as

    breakdowns or trouble occurring during conversation. More recently, some authors have

    proposed that a specific case of communication failure,misunderstanding, be viewed as an

    integral part of the comprehension process rather than just a breakdown (Dascal 1985;

    Weigand 1999). In line with such a perspective Bosco et al. (2006) have claimed that all

    communication failures, not only misunderstanding, integrally belong to the

    cooperativeprocess (Grice 1957) in which agents are involved during communication. Within

    a cooperative model of communication, the replies received from a partner provide the

    speaker with the grounds on which to realize that a communicative attempt has failed.

    Recognizing that a failure has occurred provides in its turn a starting point for repair.

    Focusing on the complexity of the cognitive processes involved in failure recognition and

    repair and following the assumptions ofcognitive pragmatics theory (Airenti et al. 1993;Bara 2008), Bosco et al. (2006) have proposed a taxonomy of the types of failures that may

  • 7/29/2019 Communication Failure

    2/4

    occur in communicative interaction. These are failure of the literal meaning, failure of the

    speakers meaning, and failure of the communicative effect (i.e. the partners refusal to

    accept a partners communication act). A failure may also involve a combination of two or allof these types. In order to achieve his or her communicative goal, and depending on the kind

    of failure that has occurred, a speaker may employ different repair strategies. A speaker may

    simply repeat whathe or shesaid (in the case of failure of the expression act), reformulate

    what he or she said (in the case of failure of the speakers meaning) or change the content ofwhat he or she said (in the case of failure of the communicative effect). This taxonomy

    allowed Bosco et al. (2006) to generate empirical hypotheses about the relative difficulty of

    recognizing and repairing different kinds of failure that were confirmed by empirical evidence

    obtained from 3- to 8-year-old children. In particular, it was found that it was easier for

    children to repair a failure of literal meaning than a failure of speakers meaning, whereasrepair of communicative effect was the most difficult.

    From a cognitiveanddevelopmental pragmaticsperspective it is useful to keep in mind

    childrens performance, since it can offer suggestions on the increasing difficulty in theproduction of a specific pragmatic task. Direct observations of children show that in the

    prelinguistic phase their recovery strategy is essentially to persist in repeating the failed

    communicative act (Alexander et al. 1997). Use of this strategy tends to decrease as the childgets older (Garvey 1984) and becomes capable of distinguishing different types of failure and

    of adopting a fitting repair strategy (Marcos 1991). During the second year of life children

    become able to perform two different kinds of verbal repair: repetition and modification. In

    particular, they just repeat their request when the adult replies with a neutral query. They give

    a more specific version of it in response to a specific query from the adult (Anselmi et al.

    1986) and reformulate it when the adult replies with a simple declarative comment (Wilcox

    and Webster 1980). Children also appear to adopt a repair strategy suitable to the type of

    failure that has occurred. When their mother misunderstands a request (rather than refuses to

    comply with it), children try to clarify it rather than simply repeat it (Marcos and Kornhaber-

    le Chanu 1992). Thus, while repetition appears to be the easiest strategy for recovery, the use

    of more sophisticated and appropriate strategies appears to be an early acquisition.

    A very early version of communication failure, rooted more in the failed realization of theinfants expectation of a certain action on the part of the mother than in the actual failure of acommunicative attempt on the part of the infant, has been claimed to play a role in the

    development oftheory of mind (ToM) and Gricean communication (Tirassa et al. 2006). In

    general, the possible relation between ToM and the ability to recognize and repair

    communication failures appears to be a particularly interesting topic. It has been suggested

    that when an agent be it a young child (Golinkoff 1986, 1993) or an adult (Feldman andKalmar 1996)repairs a communication failure, he or she usually adapts his or her strategyto take the partners perspective into account. In line with this hypothesis, children withautism, a condition which has been claimed to be associated with an impaired ToM (Baron-

    Cohen et al. 1985), appear to experience communication failures more frequently than their

    typically developing peers (see Keen 2003). However, Voldens (2004) study of the problem

    yielded ambiguous results. On the one hand, children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD)performed similarly to controls in responding to requests for clarification. They used more

    flexible and increasingly complex repair strategies according to whether they had received a

    neutral request, a request for clarification, or a semistructured prompt (Tell me in anotherway) from an interlocutor. On the other hand, ASD children also produced a greater numberof inappropriate replies than the controls.

    The symptomatology of schizophrenia, another psychiatric disorder, has been explained on

    the basis of a ToM impairment (Frith 1992). Children suffering from schizophrenia have been

    described as using self-initiated repair strategies like repetition, revision and fillers less

    frequently than normally developed children (Caplan et al. 1996). Adults suffering from the

    same disorder have been described as attempting to self-repair their messages inadequately

    during a referential communication task (Leudar et al. 1992). Docherty (2005) used the

    Communicative Disturbances Index (Docherty et al. 1996) to measure different kinds of

  • 7/29/2019 Communication Failure

    3/4

    failures during a natural conversation in a group of adult schizophrenic patients. These

    investigators found that these patients experienced more trouble than controls.

    To conclude, notwithstanding the importance of understanding communication failure,

    recent literature on the topic does not abound. Further studies are needed to understand more

    deeply the nature and role of the various cognitive processes involved in the ability to

    recognize and recover communication failures. The study of recoveries in relation to the type

    of failure that occurs appears to be a promising perspective.

    See also

    Discourse; Discourse analysis; Gestural communication; Inference; Inferential

    comprehension; Schizophrenic language.

    Suggestions for further reading

    Bazzanella, C. (ed.) (1996)Repetition in Dialogue, Tbingen: Niemeyer.

    Bosco, F.M., Bucciarelli, M. and Bara, B.G. (2006) Recognition and recovery of communicative failures: adevelopmentalperspective,Journal of Pragmatics, 38: 1398-1429.

    Schegloff, E.A. (1992) Repair after next turn: the last structurally provided defense of intersubjectivity in

    conversation, American Journal of Sociology, 97: 1295-1345.

    Bibliography

    Airenti, G., Bara, B.G. and Colombetti, M. (1993) Failures, exploitations and deceits in communication, Journalof Pragmatics, 20: 30326.

    Alexander, D., Wetherby, A. and Prizant, B. (1997) The emergence of repair strategies in infants and

    toddlers,Seminars in Speech and Language, 18: 197-212.

    Anselmi, D., Tomasello, M. and Acunzo, M. (1986) Young childrens responses to neutral and specific contingent

    queries,Journal of Child Language, 14: 13544.Bara, B.G. (2008) Cognitive Pragmatics, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, in press.

    Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A.M. and Frith, U. (1985) Does the autistic child have a theory of mind?, Cognition, 21:

    37-46.

    Bosco, F.M., Bucciarelli, M. and Bara, B.G. (2006) Recognition and recovery of communicative failures: adevelopmental perspective,Journal of Pragmatics, 38: 1398-1429.

    Caplan, R., Guthrie, D. and Komo, S. (1996) Conversational repair in schizophrenic and normal

    children,Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35: 950-58.Dascal, M. (1985) The relevance of misunderstanding, in M. Dascal (ed.)Dialogue: An Interdisciplinary

    Approach, Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Docherty, N.M. (2005) Cognitive impairments and disordered speech in schizophrenia: thought disorder,disorganization and communicative failure perspectives,Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114: 269-78.

    Docherty, N.M., DeRosa, M. and Andreasen, N.C. (1996) Communication disturbances in schizophrenia andmania,Archives of General Psychiatry, 53: 358-64.

    Feldman, C.F. and Kalmar, D. (1996) You can't step in the same river twice: repair and repetition in dialogue, inC. Bazzanella (ed.)Repetition in Dialogue, Tbingen: Niemeyer.

    Fox, H. and Jasperson, R. (1995) A syntactic exploration of repair in English conversation, in P.W. Davis(ed.)Alternative Linguistics:Descriptive and Theoretical Modes, Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Friedland, D. and Miller N. (1998) Conversation analysis of communication breakdown after closed head

    injury,Brain Injury, 1: 1-14.Frith, C.D. (1992) The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia, Hove, UK and Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence

    Erlbaum Associates.

    Garvey, C. (1984) Childrens Talk, New York: Fontana.

    Golinkoff, R.M. (1986) I beg your pardon?: the preverbal negotiation of failed messages, Journal of ChildLanguage, 13: 45576.

    Golinkoff, R.M. (1993) When is communication a meeting of minds, Journal of Child Language, 20: 199-207.

    Grice, H.P. (1957) Meaning,The Philosophical Review, 67: 37788.

    Grice, H.P. (1989) Studies in the Way of Words, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Keen, D. (2003) Communicative repair strategies and problem behaviours of children with autism, InternationalJournal of Disabilities, Development and Education, 50: 53-63.

  • 7/29/2019 Communication Failure

    4/4

    Leudar, I., Thomas, P. and Johnston, M. (1992) Self repair in dialogues of schizophrenics: effects of

    hallucinations and negative symptoms,Brain and Language, 43: 487-511.Marcos, H. and Kornhaber-le Chanu, M. (1992) Learning how to insist and how to clarify in the second

    year,International Journal of Behavioral Development, 3: 359-77.

    Marcos, H. (1991) Reformulating requests at 18 months: gestures, vocalizations and words, First Language, 11:36175.

    Robinson, J.D. (2006) Managing trouble responsibility and relationships during conversational

    repair,Communication Monographs, 73: 137-61.Schegloff, E.A. (1997a) Third turn repair, in G.R. Guy, C. Feagin, D. Schiffrin & J. Baugh (eds) Towards a

    Social Science of Language 2, Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Schegloff, E.A. (1997b) Practices and actions: boundary cases of other-initiated repair,Discourse Processes, 23:

    499-545.

    Schegloff, E.A. (2004) On dispensability,Research on Language and Social Interaction, 37: 95-149.Schegloff, E.A., Jefferson, G. and Sacks, H. (1977) The preference for self-correction in the organization of repair

    in conversation,Language, 53: 361-82.

    Tirassa, M., Bosco, F.M. and Colle, L. (2006) Rethinking the ontogeny of mindreading, Consciousness andCognition, 15: 197-217.

    Volden, J. (2004) Conversational repair in speakers with autism spectrum disorder,International Journal ofLanguage and Communication Disorders, 39: 171-89.

    Weigand, E. (1999) Misunderstanding: the standard case, Journal of Pragmatics, 31: 76385.

    Wilcox, J. and Webster, E.J. (1980) Early discourse behavior: an analysis of childrens responses to listener feed -

    back,Child Development, 51: 112025.Zahn, C. (1984) A reexamination of conversational repair, Communication Monographs, 51: 56-66.