Color Preference and Familiarity in Performance on Brand ...€¦ · Then they wrote down five...

10
Perceptualand Motor Skills, 2008, 107,587-596. O Perceptual and Motor Skills 2008 COLOR PREFERENCE AND FAMILIARITY IN PERFORMANCE ON BRAND LOGO RECALL ' KUO-CHEN HUANG CHIN-CHIUAN LIN Department of Product Design Department of Business Administration Mzng Chuan Univer.rity Kun Shan Univer.rity SHU-YINC CHIANG Graduate Institute of Design National Taiwan University of Science and Technologv Summary.-Two experiments assessed effects of color preference and brand-logo familiarity on recall performance. Exp. 1 explored the color preferences, using a forced-choice technique, of 189 women and 63 men, Taiwanese college students ages 18 to 20 years (M= 19.4, SD= 1.5). The sequence of the three most preferred colors was white, light blue, and black and of the three least preferred colors was light orange, dark violet, and dark brown. Exp. 2 investigated the effects of color prefer- ence based on the results o f Exp. 1 and brand-logo familiarity on recall. A total o f 27 women and 21 men, Taiwanese college students ages 18 to 20 years (M= 19.2, SD= 1.2) participated. They memorized a list of 24 logos (four logos shown in six colors) and then performed sequential recall. Analyses showed color preference significantly affected recall accuracy. Accuracy for high color preference was significantly greater than that for low preferences. Results showed no significant effects o f brand-logo fa- miliarity or sex on accuracy. In addition, the interactive effect o f color preference and brand-logo familiarity on accuracy was significant. These results have implications for the design o f brand logos to create and sustain memory o f brand images. Logo creation is a basic element of branding and has the potential to express organizational characteristics (Van &el, Van den Ban, & Heijmans, 2001). It can be an important company asset which companies spend enor- mous amounts of time and money promoting (Rubel, 1994), and provides plenty of scope for differentiating the company from competitors (Argenti & Druckenmiller, 2003). Therefore, logos should be recognized as familiar, elicit a consensually held meaning in the target market, and evoke positive effects (Robertson, 1989). Despite the importance and widespread use of lo- gos, many evoke negative evaluations, are unrecognizable, and hurt the cor- porate image (Bird, 1992). Part of the reason for this poor performance might lie in the initial poor selection of logos (Henderson & Cote, 1998). Logo recognition, of great interest in the document and shape-matching domain (Chen, Leung, & Gao, 20031, occurs at two levels. First, consumers must remember seeing the logo. Second, logos must remind consumers of 'Address correspondence to Kuo-Chen Huang at 5, Teh-Ming Road, Gwei-Shan, Taoyuan 333, Taiwan or e-mail ([email protected]). DO1 10.2466/PMS.107.2.587-596

Transcript of Color Preference and Familiarity in Performance on Brand ...€¦ · Then they wrote down five...

Page 1: Color Preference and Familiarity in Performance on Brand ...€¦ · Then they wrote down five numbers of colors in order of greatest per- sonal preference and five numbers of colors

Perceptualand Motor Skills, 2008, 107,587-596. O Perceptual and Motor Skills 2008

COLOR PREFERENCE AND FAMILIARITY IN PERFORMANCE ON BRAND LOGO RECALL '

KUO-CHEN HUANG CHIN-CHIUAN LIN

Department of Product Design Department of Business Administration Mzng Chuan Univer.rity Kun Shan Univer.rity

SHU-YINC CHIANG

Graduate Institute of Design National Taiwan University of Science and Technologv

Summary.-Two experiments assessed effects o f color preference and brand-logo familiarity on recall performance. Exp. 1 explored the color preferences, using a forced-choice technique, o f 189 women and 63 men, Taiwanese college students ages 18 to 20 years ( M = 19.4, SD= 1.5). The sequence of the three most preferred colors was white, light blue, and black and of the three least preferred colors was light orange, dark violet, and dark brown. Exp. 2 investigated the effects o f color prefer- ence based on the results o f Exp. 1 and brand-logo familiarity on recall. A total o f 27 women and 21 men, Taiwanese college students ages 18 to 20 years ( M = 19.2, SD= 1.2) participated. They memorized a list o f 24 logos (four logos shown in six colors) and then performed sequential recall. Analyses showed color preference significantly affected recall accuracy. Accuracy for high color preference was significantly greater than that for low preferences. Results showed no significant effects o f brand-logo fa- miliarity or sex on accuracy. In addition, the interactive effect o f color preference and brand-logo familiarity on accuracy was significant. These results have implications for the design of brand logos to create and sustain memory o f brand images.

Logo creation is a basic element of branding and has the potential to express organizational characteristics (Van &el, Van den Ban, & Heijmans, 2001). It can be an important company asset which companies spend enor- mous amounts of time and money promoting (Rubel, 1994), and provides plenty of scope for differentiating the company from competitors (Argenti & Druckenmiller, 2003). Therefore, logos should be recognized as familiar, elicit a consensually held meaning in the target market, and evoke positive effects (Robertson, 1989). Despite the importance and widespread use of lo- gos, many evoke negative evaluations, are unrecognizable, and hurt the cor- porate image (Bird, 1992). Part of the reason for this poor performance might lie in the initial poor selection of logos (Henderson & Cote, 1998).

Logo recognition, of great interest in the document and shape-matching domain (Chen, Leung, & Gao, 20031, occurs at two levels. First, consumers must remember seeing the logo. Second, logos must remind consumers of

'Address correspondence to Kuo-Chen Huang at 5, Teh-Ming Road, Gwei-Shan, Taoyuan 333, Taiwan or e-mail ([email protected]).

DO1 10.2466/PMS.107.2.587-596

Page 2: Color Preference and Familiarity in Performance on Brand ...€¦ · Then they wrote down five numbers of colors in order of greatest per- sonal preference and five numbers of colors

588 K-C. HUANG, ET AL.

the brand or company name. The former depends largely on design (Hender- son & Cote, 1998). Given exposure time, a more memorable design will be recognized more easily than a less memorable one. Enhancing recognition of logos might start with selecting a design easily recognized. Moreover, con- sumers primarily examine the sensory features of a logo such as color, shape, and size; they decipher the stimulus into categorical codes such as brand and select certain elements of the stimulus over others (Pieters & Warlop, 1999).

Colors attract attention, influence moods, and clarify and accentuate features of the environment (Teller & Bornstein, 1987) but also affect partic- ipant's short-term color memory (Bynum, Epps, & Kaya, 2006). Color plays an important role in marketing since it influences the creation of brand im- age (Tavassoli, 2001). Moreover, when people knew how brands were at-

- -

tempting to manipulate impression, people considered colors congruent with those impressions to be more appropriate (Bottomley & Doyle, 2006). A consistent use of print color can contribute to the maintenance of positive brand equity; especially for Chinese brand names can print color become an effective retrieval cue for meaning which may influence repetition of pur- chases (Tavassoli, 2001).

A classic study of color preference was by Eysenck (1941), who asked 30 college students to rank 10 colored papers using 12 rankings in order of preference. Analysis showed as much agreement among color preferences of people as among intelligence tests and their relation to a general factor of aesthetic appreciation. This general factor of preference for colors is bipolar and divides those who prefer saturated colors from those who prefer unsatu- rated colors. Also there is high agreement between the sexes as regards color preference.

Finally, color preference has discriminative power in identifying global psychopathology (French, 1996). Differences in color preferences and in col- or meanings across nationalities and cultures might clarify image perceptions of different brands or companies (Madden, Hewett, & Roth, 2000). Deep blue green, vivid green, and black were the most preferred colors in Japan, China, and Indonesia, whereas olive, dark gray, and gold were the least pre- ferred colors for people of these nations (Saito, 1996). Black was the pre- dominant choice in Senegal (D'Hondt & Vandewiele, 1983), whereas red was in the Netherlands (Wiegersma & de Kierck, 1984). In the USA, blue was the predominant choice (Silver, McCulley, Chambliss, Charles, Smith, Waddell, & Winfield, 1988).

Familiarity is an important factor in logo recognition. Familiar advertise- ments attract the greatest attention to a brand, which promotes brand mem- ory (Pieters, Warlop, & Wedel, 2002). Previous research (Arnell, Shapiro, & Sorensen, 1999; Buttle & Raymond, 2003) demonstrated that high familiarity

Page 3: Color Preference and Familiarity in Performance on Brand ...€¦ · Then they wrote down five numbers of colors in order of greatest per- sonal preference and five numbers of colors

BRAND LOGO RECALL 5 89

promotes faster or more efficient processing. Several studies of familiarity in visual processing suggested that familiarity increased naming speed (Bar & Biederman, 1998) and enhanced preference (Monahan, Murphy, & Zajonc, 2000). Thus, the question of what exposure a consumer requires to associate a logo with its brand name is important in understanding and use of visual representations of brands (Buttle & Westob~, 2006). Shapiro (1999) indi- cated that incidental exposure to an advertisement increased the likelihood that a product would subsequently be judged accept-able for inclusion in a set. Perfect and Heatherley (1997) showed there was no over-all preference for previously seen items, but there was an interaction for ratings. Post hoc testing indicated for memorability and distinctiveness, two statistically non- significant but interesting differences for logos in favour of previously seen items.

To summarize, color preference and logo familiarity are two factors which affect logo recall or recognition but have previously been studied sepa- rately. Here effects of these factors were investigated for simultaneous recall. The aim was to explore color preferences of Taiwanese college students and examine whether color preference and familiarity with a logo affect recall.

EXPERIMENT 1 The purpose of Exp. 1 was to ascertain which three colors were most

and least preferred. The two sets of color preferences were used as an inde- pendent variable in Exp. 2.

Method Subjects.-A total of 189 women and 63 men, college students in the

Department of Product Design of Ming Chuan University and ages 18 to 20 years (M= 19.4, SD= 1.5), were participants. All reported 16/20 corrected vi- sual acuity or better and were tested for color vision with the Ishihara Color Test. None showed color deficiencies. Participants received a reward of 100 new Taiwan dollars for their participation.

Stimulus material and design.--Based on the studies of Oota and Kawa- hara (1989), 21 colors were chosen for presentation. The CIELAB coordi- nates (L, a", b", AE) of the colors appears in Table 1. The 21 colors were

- -

printed on A3 paper in three rows of seven columns. The interrow and in- tercolumn space was 2 cm. Each color was numbered and occupied 4 x 4 cm. For each participant, the order (i.e., location) in which the colors were printed was randomized to balance order effects.

Task speczfication.-Groups of 20 participants were tested at one time in a quiet classroom in which the light level was approximately 320 lux. Ma- terial and one response paper were placed on each desk, then participants were greeted and given general instructions for the test. They were instruct- ed first to scan all 21 colors so they could compare the test colors.

Page 4: Color Preference and Familiarity in Performance on Brand ...€¦ · Then they wrote down five numbers of colors in order of greatest per- sonal preference and five numbers of colors

K-C. IIUANG, ET AL

TABLE 1 CIELAB COORDINATES (L, a*, b*, AE) OF 21 COLORS USED IN EXP. 1

No. Color L a $: b" AE

1 Light yellow 88.0 -11.1 57.1 71.0 2 Bright yellow 86.9 -11.3 85.8 99.7 3 Soil yellow 54.1 42.8 48.4 82.0 4 Light orange 71.3 16.2 29.1 48.3 5 Orange 53.2 44.6 38.1 76.2 6 Red brown 40.0 45.2 12.5 71.8 7 Light red 64.8 35.4 - 4.6 43.3 8 Red 45.6 57.2 30.3 83.3 9 Dark brown 27.6 20.2 4.7 69.1

10 Light violet 68.8 14.7 -24.4 29.0 11 Violet 32.8 29.0 -31.8 67.9 12 Dark violet 28.5 27.8 - 8.2 68.7 13 Light blue 80.9 - 7.8 -23.1 19.1 14 Blue 55.1 -28.2 -36.7 55.8 15 Cyan 35.8 4.9 -42.8 64.3 16 Light green 79.2 -21.8 46.2 64.7 17 Green 63.6 -38.8 46.7 77.7 18 Dark green 48.5 -56.3 24.4 82.2 19 White 92.3 2.8 -12.3 0.0 20 Gray 53.2 - 1.9 - 5.3 40.1 21 Black 18.7 0.6 - 1.1 74.5

--

Then they wrote down five numbers of colors in order of greatest per- sonal preference and five numbers of colors in order of least personal pref- erence. These two preferences from first to fifth were coded from 5 to 1, re- spectively. Test colors were coded zero if not selected. Each participant spent approximately 10 min. on the test.

Performance measure.-Two groups of subjective color preferences were collected from each participant, most and least preferable. Means and stan- dard deviations of colors were calculated.

Results and Discussion The three most preferred colors were white (M=4.8, SD= .6), light blue

(M=4.1, SD= 1.01, and black (M=3.6, SD= 1.4); the three least preferred were light orange (M=4.3, SD= l.O), dark violet (M=4.3, SD= 1.3), and dark brown (M=4.0, SD= 1.5). The former three colors were coded as of high preference, and the latter three as of low preference.

Exp. 1 yielded two groups of color preferences used to address the magnitude of the interactive effect of color preference and the brand-logo familiarity on recall. The method of McCabe, Robertson, and Smith's study (2005), in which participants were asked to remember the color of each of a short series of Stroop color-words of varying set size (two through six) was

Page 5: Color Preference and Familiarity in Performance on Brand ...€¦ · Then they wrote down five numbers of colors in order of greatest per- sonal preference and five numbers of colors

BRAND LOGO RECALL 591

used. Recall of the colors of the words in serial order allowed assessment of the effect of age on Stroop interference.

Method Subjects.- College students, 27 women and 21 men ages 18 to 20 years

(M= 19.2, SD= 1.2), were recruited via an advertisement posted at the uni- versity. Participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were test- ed for color vision using the Ishihara Color Test. None had color deficien- cies. A reward for their participation depended upon the number of accu- rate responses.

Stzinulus material and design.-Independent variables were color prefer- ence and brand-logo familiarity, chosen from Exp. 1. High and low color preferences at each level were three colors. The high level contained white, light blue, and black; the low level contained light orange, dark violet, and dark brown. Levels of brand-logo familiarity were also high and low. In the high level, the two logos were McDonald's and Coca-Cola (Fig. I ) , which are familiar to Taiwanese teenagers and young adults. In the low level, the two logos were Geng-E Company (GEST) and Shan-Yi Company (SYGM; Fig. I), which had not yet been registered in the Intellectual Property Of- fice, Ministry of Economic Affairs, R.O.C.

FIG. 1. Some examples of colors and familiarity of logos for Exp. 2. The two levels of fa- miliarity of logos were (a) and (b) lower familiarity, and ( c ) and (d) higher familiarity.

A 2 (two levels of color preference multiplied by three colors for each level of color preference) x 2 (two levels of brand-logo familiarity multiplied by two logos for each level of brand-logo familiarity) repeated-measures de- sign was used. These 24 stimuli (logos) were printed in a list as two col- umns. A number was placed at the top left for each logo. Each logo was printed at a size of 6 x 6 cm. For each participant, the order in which the logos were presented was randomized to balance out order effects.

Participants were asked to memorize all 24 logos and their numbers in the list so they couId write their answers on a response card. Twenty-four response cards (see Fig. 2), each printed with four logos, one number, and six colored squares at the bottom of each card, were designed for each par- ticipant. Each response card was a square of 6 x 6 cm. Each participant was rewarded with 20 new Taiwan dollars for each correct response, which pro- vided motivation for participation.

Page 6: Color Preference and Familiarity in Performance on Brand ...€¦ · Then they wrote down five numbers of colors in order of greatest per- sonal preference and five numbers of colors

5 92 K-C. HUANG, ET AL.

FIG. 2. An example of #1 response card. The locations of these four logos were random. The six colorful squares were fixed, their colors from left to right were light blue, white, black, dark brown, light orange, and dark violet, respectively.

Task specification.-Each group of 16 participants was tested in a quiet classroom in which the light level was about 320 lux. Before the test, materi- als and 24 response cards were placed on each participant's desk. They were told they would first memorize the list containing the 24 stimulus logos and their corresponding numbers in a 10-min. period. Once the memorization task was completed, they turned the list over and put it on the desk. They then were asked to respond to each card as quickly and accurately as possi- ble, regardless of the order of cards. In responding, participants made a cross on the logo and its color (a square), respectively, on each response card, de- pending upon their memories of the stimulus materials. Participants' re- sponses were coded as accurate when the brand logo and its color were both correct; otherwise, they were coded as errors. Participants took approximate- ly 20 min. to complete the procedure.

Performance measure.-The accuracy was recorded as the percent of correct responses for the 24 response cards and evaluated using analysis of variance in the SPSS software.

Results and Discussion The data were analyzed using a 2 (color preference) x 2 (brand-logo fa-

miliarity) mixed-model analysis of variance. The main effect of color prefer- ence on recall accuracy was significant (F,,4, = 26.9, p < .001, q: = 0.37; Table 2). Accuracy for high color preference was significantly greater than for low color preference; however, there was no significant effect of brand-logo fa-

Page 7: Color Preference and Familiarity in Performance on Brand ...€¦ · Then they wrote down five numbers of colors in order of greatest per- sonal preference and five numbers of colors

BRAND LOGO RECALL 5 93

TABLE 2 MEANS AND STANDARD ERRORS OF ACCURACY (%) FOR PARTICIPANTS' RESPONSES (n = 48)

Variable Accuracy M SE

Color Preference High 43.9 3.1 Low 30.7 2.0

Brand-logo Familiarity High 35.4 3.1 Low 39.2 2.3

Sex Men 38.7 3.4 Women 36.3 3 .O

miliarity on accuracy of recall (F,,4, = 1.3, p = .26, q i = O.Oj), and neither sig- nificant effect was associated with the sex of participants (Fl,4, = 0.3, p = .59, qpZ = 0.01). The interaction of color preference and brand-logo familiarity on recall accuracy was significant (F, ,4, = 20.3, p < .001, q i = 0.3 1; Fig. 3). Per- cent of recall accuracy was significantly higher for the high color-preference condition ( M = 52.1) than for the low color-preference condition ( M = 26.4) under the low brand-logo familiarity (F,,4, = 43 .O, p < .001, T J ~ = 0.48). No dif- ference in recall accuracy was significant between the high color-preference condition ( M = 35.8) and the low color-preference condition (M = 35.0) if there was high brand-logo familiarity (Fl,4, = 0.1, p = 33 , qp2 = 0.01).

0 1 High Low

Familiarity of Brand Logo

FIG. 3. Accuracy as a function of color preference ( w low, high) and familiarity of brand logo

CONCLUSIONS The results of Exp. 1 ~rovided the three most preferred colors for Tai-

wanese college students (white, light blue, and black) and the three least pre-

Page 8: Color Preference and Familiarity in Performance on Brand ...€¦ · Then they wrote down five numbers of colors in order of greatest per- sonal preference and five numbers of colors

5 94 K-C. HUANG, ET AL.

ferred colors (light orange, dark violet, and dark brown). These results are similar to the conclusions of Dittmar (2OOI), who suggested that adults pre- fer the color blue over red, green, and yellow. However, results were incon- sistent with the studies of Saito (1996), who indicated that vivid green and dark gray were the most and least preferred colors for Chinese nationals, re- spectively. One possibility is that cultural factors and age might be associ- ated with differences in color preference. Another possibility is participants' background. Those in Exp. I had all been trained in design, so it is possible they preferred white and black because it is easier to develop an infinite number of incremental shades and tints through the mixing of one hue with black or white. That is, white and black colors go easily together with other colors in graphic design.

The results of Exp. 2 indicated that color preference affected recall ac- curacy; however, there was no significant effect of familiarity or sex on accu- racy of logo recall. Color can attract attention; thus, the stimulus logo de- signed with preferred colors might attract participants' attention. This means that the duration of participants' visual processing for logos designed using preferred colors is longer than that for logos designed using less preferred colors and may explain why logos designed using preferred colors showed increase in accuracy of recall and vice versa. Although results of Exp. 2 did not confirm the findings of Bynum, et al. (2006), hue categories of stimuli were different indicating orange might be a less correctly remembered color for both studies.

Exp. 2 yielded no significant effect of familiarity on accuracy of logo re- call. This result was inconsistent with the findings of Arnell, et al. (1999), Pieters, et al. (2002), and Buttle and Raymond (2003), who reported famil- iarity with an advertisement might promote brand memory directly and made processing more efficient. One possible reason for the inconsistency is the differences in task characteristics between Exp. 2 and the previous studies. For example, Pieters, et al. (2002) used infrared eye tracking to collect eye- fixation data in assessing memory for advertised brands, whereas Buttle and Raymond (2003) presented stimulus brands using the rapid serial visual pre- sentation method and asked participants to report brands they had seen. In Exp. 2, participants were first asked to memorize a list of 24 stimulus brand logos and then to recall the brand logos and colors. Therefore, participants had a longer delay for many material brand logos in Exp. 2 than in the two previous studies, implying that participants' memory traces of material logos might have decayed. Moreover, participants memorized 24 brand logos and their colors, a task which exceeds the limitations of short-term memory. Simi- lar reasons supported McCabe, et al. (2005), who suggested that dispropor- tionate increase in incongruent color-word errors resulted from memory load producing an increase in the overall number of operations required to main-

Page 9: Color Preference and Familiarity in Performance on Brand ...€¦ · Then they wrote down five numbers of colors in order of greatest per- sonal preference and five numbers of colors

BRAND LOGO RECALL 5 95

tain the representation of the to-be-remembered colors. In Exp. 2, delayed recall and the capacity limitations of participants' short-term memory might have led to the finding that brand-logo familiarity did not significantly affect recall accuracy. Although the reason sex did not affect brand-logo recall is unclear, this finding implies designers might be able to neglect sex as a fac- tor when designing brand logos to enhance recall.

Although brand-logo familiarity did not affect recall accuracy, there was a significant interaction of color preference with brand-logo familiarity. A simple effects analysis indicated that brand logos with high familiarity de- signed using a more or less preferred color did not affect accuracy of recall. However, brand logos of low familiarity designed using preferred colors en- hanced participants' accuracy of recall, and brand logos with low familiarity designed using less preferred colors reduced that accuracy. This implies that the brand logo might be designed using preferred colors to promote recall if participants were not familiar with the brand logo. In cases in which partici- pants are familiar with the brand logo, however, use of color would not nec- essarily affect recall.

These results have implications for graphics-based interface design. First, findings can be applied to the design of memorable brand logos by clearly distinguishing the roles of color preference and brand-logo familiar- ity. Second, brand logos can help observers recall their product during pur- chase or consumption and can enhance the formation of strong, favorable, and distinct brand associations. Such results may help designers be aware of the characteristics likely to be effective in design of a memorable brand logo. -

In addition, researchers have indicated that characteristics of brand logos possibly affect evaluation of product or service qualities. Therefore, effects of logo design on the evaluation of products and services in different indus- tries, including, for example, assessments of quality and price, yet need study.

REFERENCES

ARGENTI, P. A., & DRUCKENMILLER, B. (2003) Reputation and the corporate brand. Corporate Reputation Review, 6, 368-374.

ARNELL, K. M., SHAPIRO, K. L., & SORENSEN, R. E. (1999) Reduced repetition blindness for one's own name. Visual Cognition, 6, 609-635.

BAR, M., ~BIEDERMAN, I. (1998) Subliminal visual priming. Psychological Science, 9, 464-469. BIRD, L. (1992) Eye-catching logos all too often leave fuzzy images in minds of consumers.

The Wall Street Journal, 220 (1091, B-1. BOTTOMLEY, P. A., &DOYLE, J. R. (2006) The interactive effects of colors and products on per-

ceptions of brand logo appropriateness. Marketing Theory, 6, 63-83. BUTTLE, H., & RAYMOND, J. E. (2003) High familiarity enhances visual change detection for

face stimuli. Perception C Psychophysics, 65, 1296-1306. BUTTLE, H., &WESTOBY, N. (2006) Brand logo and name association: it's all in the name. Ap-

plied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 1181-1 194. BYNUM, C., EPPS, H. H., &&A, N. (2006) Color memory of university students: influence of

color experience and color characteristic. College Student lournal, 40, 824-831.

Page 10: Color Preference and Familiarity in Performance on Brand ...€¦ · Then they wrote down five numbers of colors in order of greatest per- sonal preference and five numbers of colors

596 K-C. HUANG, ET AL.

CHEN, J., LEUNG, M. K., &GAo, Y. (2003) Noisy logo recognition using line segment Hausdorff distance. Pattern Recognition, 36, 943-955.

D'HONDT, W., &VANDEWIELE, M. (1983) Colors and figures in Senegal. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 56, 971-978.

DITTMAR, M. (2001) Changing colour preferences with ageing: a comparative study on younger and older native Germans aged 19-90 years. Gerontology, 47, 219-226.

EYSENCK, H. J. (1941) A critical and experimental study of colour preferences. American Jour- nal of Psychology, 54, 385-394.

FRENCH, A. l? (1996) Color rejection revisited. American Journal ofForensic Psychology, 14, 73- 74.

HENDERSON, P. W., &COTE, J. A. (1998) Guidelines for selecting or modifying logos. Journal of Marketing, 62, 14-30.

MADDEN, T. J., HEWETT, K., & ROTH, M. S. (2000) Managing images in different cultures: a cross-national study of color meanings and preferences. Journal of International Market- ing, 8, 90-107.

MCCABE, D. P., ROBERTSON, C. L., &SMITH, A. D. (2005) Age differences in Stroop interfer- ence in working memory. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27, 633- 644.

MONAHAN, J. L., MURPHY, S. T., & ZAJONC, R. B. (2000) Subliminal mere exposure: specific, general, and affective effects. Psychological Science, 11, 462-466.

OOTA, A., & KAWAHARA, E. (1989) [Color and harmony]. Tokyo: Graphic-sha Publ. [in Japa- nese]

PERFECT, T. J., &HEATHERLEY, S. (1997) Preference for advertisements, logo, and names: effects of implicit memory. Psychological Reports, 80, 803-808.

PIETERS, R., & WARLOP, L. (1999) Visual attention during brand choice: the impact of time pressure and task motivation. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 16, 1-16.

PIETERS, R., WARLOP, L., &WEDEL, M. (2002) Breaking through the clutter: benefits of adver- tisement originality and familiarity for brand attention and memory. Management Science, 48, 765-781.

ROBERTSON, K. R. (1989) Strategically desirable brand name characteristics. Journal of Consum- er Marketing, 6, 61-71.

RUBEL, E. A. (1994) Trademarks and the press: a year in review. Editor & Publisher, 127, 6-26. SAITO, M. (1996) A comparative stud of color preferences in Japan, China and Indonesia,

with emphasis on the preference &r white. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 83, 115-128. SHAPIRO, S. (1999) When an ad's influence is beyond our conscious control: erceptual and

conceptual fluency effects caused by incidental ad exposure. Journal o P ~ o n s u m e r Re- search, 26, 16-36.

SILVER, N. C., MCCULLEY, W. L., CHAMBLISS, L. N., CHARLES, C. M., SMITH, A. A,, WADDELL, W. M., & WINFIELD, E. B. (1988) Sex and racial differences in color and number prefer- ences. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 66, 295-299.

TAVASSOLI, N. T. (2001) Color menlory and evaluations for alphabetic and logographic brand names. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 104-111.

TELLER, D., &BORNSTEIN, M. H. (1987) Infant color vision and color perception. In P. Salapa- tek & L. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of infant perception. Vol. 1. Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Pp. 185-236.

VAN RIEL, C. B. M., VAN DEN BAN, A., & HEIJMANS, E-J. (2001) The added value of corporate logos: an empirical study. European Journal of Marketing, 35, 428-440.

WIEGERSMA, S., &DE KIERCK, I. (1984) The 'blue phenomenon' is red in the Netherlands. Per- ceptual and Motor Skills, 59, 790.

Accepted August 19, 2008