Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating...

50

Transcript of Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating...

Page 1: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,
Page 2: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

2 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

About the authors

Cambridge Cognition is a leading global provider of computerized cognitive tests

combining neuroscience and technology to optimize mental health through life.

CANTAB, has become the worlds most validated and comprehensive cognitive

assessment system, used to conduct sensitive language-independent

touchscreen tests in pharmaceutical clinical trials and research projects for over 30 years.

UK office

Cambridge Cognition

Tunbridge Court

Bottisham

Cambridge

CB25 9TU United Kingdom

Copyright

The copyright in all material published in this handbook by Cambridge

Cognition, including all portions of the text, graphics and the selection and

arrangement of the material is owned by Cambridge Cognition, unless otherwise

indicated.

All rights reserved. The Material may not be reproduced or distributed, in whole

or in part, without prior written permission of Cambridge Cognition. However,

reproduction in part, by non-profit, research or educational institutions for their

own use is permitted if proper credit is given, with full citation, and copyright is

acknowledged. Any other reproduction or distribution, in whatever form and by

whatever media, is expressly prohibited without the prior written consent of

Cambridge Cognition. For further information, please contact:

[email protected]

+44 (0)1223 810 700

Page 3: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

1 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

Contents

Introduction .................................................................................. 2

An overview of CANTAB .................................................................. 2

Familiarisation .............................................................................. 5

Motor Screening Test (MOT) ........................................................... 5

Psychomotor speed ....................................................................... 6

Reaction Time (RTI) ....................................................................... 7

Attention ..................................................................................... 11

Rapid Visual Information Processing (RVP) ...................................... 11

Episodic memory ......................................................................... 17

Paired Associates Learning (PAL) ................................................... 18

Delayed match to sample (DMS) ................................................... 25

Working memory and executive function .................................... 30

Spatial Working Memory (SWM) .................................................... 31

Cognitive testing: test recommendation by disease .................... 38

Cognitive testing: quality consideration ...................................... 39

Study practicalities ..................................................................... 41

Data analysis considerations ....................................................... 43

Page 4: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

2 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

Introduction The CANTAB Cognition Handbook offers a comprehensive introduction to computerized cognitive assessment with CANTAB. It starts with an overview of CANTAB software, and then is divided into a series of sections that focus on the key cognitive domains of psychomotor speed, attention, episodic memory, working

memory and executive function.

Each section starts by providing an overview of the theoretical background to the cognitive domain and how it is affected in neurodegenerative, psychiatric and neurological

disorders. Each section then describes and discusses the CANTAB test or tests that have been used to assess the cognitive domain, with a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies, studies of patients with Alzheimer’s disease

(AD), mild cognitive impairment (MCI), depression, schizophrenia and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) as well as data from neuroimaging research to provide a convergent overview of the use and

interpretation of the CANTAB tests.

The text concludes with general information on study design, study practicalities and data analysis considerations, making this a useful resource at all stages of studies using CANTAB. It is likely to be of interest not only to psychologists and neuroscientists, but also to

anyone with an interest in using theoretically motivated and empirically validated cognitive tests in their research.

An overview of CANTAB

The Cambridge Neuropsychological Test

Automated Battery (CANTAB) is composed of 25

tests and was created at the University of

Cambridge in the 1980s. CANTAB has been in

continuous development for over two decades.

CANTAB has established sensitivity to a large

number of drugs and disorders and has been

extensively validated. To date, CANTAB has

been cited in over 1500 peer-reviewed articles

(http://www.cantab.com/cantab/site/bibliograp

hy.acds).

CANTAB tests were designed to be independent

of language and culture, which makes them

ideal for use in multi-national studies. The

battery has been used in 50 countries in

academic research and in multi-national clinical

trials.

Due to the nature of the CANTAB tests, pre-

baseline participant training is not required,

which reduces the time taken up by cognitive

testing in your trial. There are instead built-in

practice phases at the start of tests that allow

participants to familiarize themselves with the

tasks. A simple motor screening test is also used

to familiarize participants with the system and

to ensure that they are able to follow

instructions and use the touch screen

accurately.

Most CANTAB tests are graded in difficulty and

as a result have been used to test highly

impaired patients as well as high-functioning

individuals. CANTAB tests have excellent test-

retest reliability and parallel forms to help guard

against learning effects over repeated testing

sessions.

This guide introduces you to the key cognitive

domains assessed by CANTAB and the measures

used to interpret the data. For each test, there

is a summary of some of the key findings

relating to imaging, pharmacological and clinical

studies. At the end of the manual we provide an

overview of general testing and data analysis

considerations when using CANTAB.

Cognitive domains

Sound understanding of the complexities of

cognitive function is needed in order to

overcome the challenge of reliable assessment.

IQ tests provide one way of measuring

intellectual functioning. However, despite being

undoubtedly useful and important, IQ tests only

provide a broad and static profile of what we

mean by ‘cognition’. Furthermore, some

intelligence tests, particularly those based on

verbal knowledge, are relatively insensitive to

impairments such as those seen in early

Alzheimer’s disease.

Page 5: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

3 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

Cognitive neuroscience now divides cognition

into different neural systems controlling, for

example, aspects of perception, attention,

short-term memory, working memory, long

term memory (e.g. facts and personal

episodes), language and executive functions,

such as planning, decision-making and impulse

control. These different processes work together

to gather, store and make sense of information

from the environment, and use these

representations to guide the production of

thoughts and behaviour.

Any comprehensive battery of cognitive tests

should provide an overview of these different

stages of behaviour, starting at the most basic.

For cognition to occur at all, there must be

evidence of normal sensory or motor

functioning; if the information does not reach

the brain or cannot be expressed by it, then

clearly cognitive functioning cannot be

accurately assessed.

Four main domains of function are assessed in

CANTAB: psychomotor speed (page 5),

attention (page 11), memory (page 17) and

executive function (page 30). Together, these

domains provide a comprehensive assessment

of different levels of cognitive processing.

Each of these cognitive domains relies on a

distinct but overlapping set of brain areas. They

are therefore differentially affected by age,

disease and pharmacological manipulation.

Combined with brain imaging methodologies,

many of the CANTAB tests have also been shown

to be useful for defining the specific network of

brain structures responsible for their

performance (e.g. the prefrontal cortex, parietal

lobe and striatum in the case of visuospatial

planning). Correspondingly, a failure on a task

may suggest some form of impairment in that

specific brain system.

Cognition in central nervous system (CNS) disease

For each section we provide a brief overview of

CANTAB data in a range of neurodegenerative,

psychiatric and neurological disorders, which

provide an indication of the sensitivity of the

tests to impairment in clinical populations and

sensitivity to treatment effects. The core

disorders on which we focus are:

AD/MCI

Depression

Schizophrenia

ADHD

AD/MCI

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and mild cognitive

impairment (MCI) are characterised by an

insidious onset with progressive cognitive

decline. The earliest cognitive impairment is

episodic memory. As the disease progresses,

cognitive deficits become more global and

include impairments in attention, semantic

memory and executive function (reasoning,

planning and cognitive flexibility). Cognitive

deficits reflect damage to the basal forebrain

resulting in loss of acetylcholine throughout the

cortex and especially the temporal cortex.

Cognitive decline is one of the clinical hallmarks

of MCI/AD. Cognition is consequently a key

outcome measure in any trials testing new

products aimed at halting or even preventing the

progression of AD pathology.

• Égerházi, A., Berecz, R., Bartók, E., & Degrell,

I. (2007). Automated Neuropsychological Test

Battery (CANTAB) in mild cognitive

impairment and in Alzheimer's disease.

Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and

Biological Psychiatry, 31(3), 746-751.

• Fowler, K.S. et al. (2002). Paired associate

performance in the early detection of DAT.

Journal of the International

Neuropsychological Society.

Depression

Major depressive disorder is characterised by

depressed mood and/or loss of interest or

pleasure in life activities, causing clinically

significant impairment in social life, work, or

other important areas of every day functioning.

Page 6: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

4 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

The disorder is typically associated with deficits

of varying severity across a range of cognitive

functions including attention, working memory

and executive function, particularly on tests that

require effortful processing. A further cognitive

hallmark of the disorder is a negative bias in the

interpretation of (and memory for) emotionally-

salient information. Some cognitive symptoms

persist even in remitted individuals while others

are present predominantly during acute phases

and may increase in severity with progression of

the illness.

• Rock, P.L., Roiser, J.P., Riedel, W.J. &

Blackwell, A. D. (2013). Cognitive impairment

in depression: a systematic review and meta-

analysis. Psychological Medicine

Schizophrenia

Schizophrenia is characterised by widespread

cognitive dysfunction (affecting attention, visual

and verbal memory, executive function and

social cognition) in addition to positive and

negative symptoms. Cognitive Impairment

Associated with Schizophrenia (CIAS) is an

important determinant of functional outcome

and is inadequately treated by antipsychotic

medication, and thus represents a major target

for novel therapeutics.

Barnett, J. H., Robbins, T. W., Leeson, V. C.,

Sahakian, B. J., Joyce, E. M., & Blackwell, A. D. (2010). Assessing cognitive function in clinical trials of schizophrenia. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(8), 1161-1177.

ADHD

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)

is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterised

by symptoms of inattention, hyperactivity,

and/or impulsivity that interfere with the

everyday functioning of affected patients. While

it predominantly affects children and

adolescents, some patients continue to

experience persistent symptoms at adulthood,

which often leads to high unemployment rates

and drug abuse if left untreated.

Understanding how ADHD and its treatment

affect cognitive function has clear implications

for the diagnosis and management of this

condition.

• Chamberlain, S. R., Robbins, T. W., Winder-

Rhodes, S., Müller, U., Sahakian, B. J.,

Blackwell, A. D., & Barnett, J. H. (2011).

Translational approaches to frontostriatal

dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder using a computerized

neuropsychological battery. Biological

psychiatry, 69(12), 1192-1203.

Cognitive testing in non-CNS disorders

There is a growing recognition that cognition is

a potentially important endpoint for researchers

interested in a range of diseases and

interventions which do not directly target the

central nervous system, but which nevertheless

may affect cognitive function.

Although the brain is quite well-protected from

many blood-borne chemicals by the blood-brain

barrier, many small molecules, including agents

that may be therapeutic for illnesses affecting

other organs, may be deleterious to brain

function. Examples include environmental

chemical drugs used to treat cancer, asthma,

high cholesterol, cardiovascular problems,

diabetes, and drugs that affect the immune

system.

Virtually all effective drugs have some ‘adverse

side-effects’. These can be the product of a

drug’s non-specific mechanisms of action or may

simply reflect effects at other receptors distal

from the therapeutic target. Many of these side

effects are not serious. Other effects, such as

sedation, represent a clear cost but have to be

set against the major benefits of medication.

However, other actions, which may be insidious,

can in the long-term be so disadvantageous that

the treatment has to be discontinued. All of

these issues have to be considered during the

initial screening of new compounds or trials

involving them, as well as research studies that

investigate the repurposing of compounds for

new indications.

Page 7: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

5 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

Disturbances of normal homeostasis can also

result in undesirable effects on cognition if the

supply of nutrients and oxygen required for

cognitive function is disrupted. Because of the

impact of potential treatments for non-CNS

disorders on cognition, there is increasing

interest in assessing cognitive function in these

areas. Desirable features of cognitive tests for

the purpose of testing the safety of compounds

include sensitivity to disease and

pharmacological manipulation. This makes

CANTAB well suited to such studies, this guide.

General references and introduction to CANTAB tests

• Clark, L., Boxer, O., Sahakian, B. J., & Bilder,

R. M. (2012). Research methods: cognitive

neuropsychological methods. Handbook of

clinical neurology, 106, 75.

• Levaux, M. N., Potvin, S., Sepehry, A. A.,

Sablier, J., Mendrek, A., & Stip, E. (2007).

Computerized assessment of cognition in

schizophrenia: promises and pitfalls of

CANTAB. European Psychiatry, 22(2), 104-

115.

• Parsey, C. M., & Schmitter-Edgecombe, M.

(2013). Applications of Technology in

Neuropsychological Assessment. The Clinical

Neuropsychologist, 27(8), 1328-1361.

• Robbins, TW, James, M, Owen, AM, Sahakian,

BJ, Lawrence, AD, McInnes, L & Rabbitt, PM

(1998) A Study of Performance on Tests from

the CANTAB battery Sensitive to Frontal Lobe

Dysfunction in a Large Sample of Normal

Volunteers: Implications for Theories of

Executive Functioning and Cognitive Aging.

Journal of the International

Neuropsychological Society, 4, 474-490, 1998

• Robbins, TW, James, T, Owen, AM, Sahakian,

BJ, McInnes, L & Rabbitt, PM (1994) CANTAB:

A Factor Analytic Study of a Large Sample of

Normal Elderly Volunteers. Dementia, 5, 266-

281.

Familiarisation

Motor Screening Test (MOT)

The MOT is used to perform familiarisation and

screening, and provides a simple reaction time

measure. The goal of the MOT is not primarily to

assess cognition but to provide a general

indication that psychomotor function is intact

enough to proceed with more sophisticated

cognitive testing.

On this test, a lack of drug or group effect should

be interpreted as indicating that any effects

identified on the rest of the CANTAB battery are

not confounded by general effects of the drug or

disease state on motor control or low level

perception.

In addition, it allows researchers to screen for

any problems with vision, movement or

language comprehension that may prevent the

participant from responding meaningfully to the

subsequent tasks. For this reason, it is

recommended that it be administered first.

Test Description

The Motor Screening test (MOT) is a task

designed to familiarise participants with the

touch screen and computer. The procedure

consists of a series of crosses shown in different

locations on the screen. The participant is asked

to touch each cross using the forefinger of the

dominant hand.

The test takes approximately 1 minute to

complete.

Psychomotor Speed

CANTAB Motor Screening Task

Page 8: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

6 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

The ability to receive, process and respond to

input from the environment in an efficient way

is essential for effective functioning, particularly

in situations such as driving when rapid decision

making and response are critical for safety. The

concept of psychomotor speed encompasses a

component of both physical movement and

mental processing speed. Slowing in either one

of these aspects can result in an increase of

reaction times. It has been suggested that

slower speed and processing underlie decreases

in cognitive functioning with age associated with

neurodegenerative processes.

One of the key features of Parkinson’s disease is

slowing: this encompasses both motor slowing

and cognitive slowing. Other conditions, such as

depression and traumatic brain injury (TBI), are

also associated with reductions in psychomotor

speed, as are some drug effects.

Importantly, interpreting performance on more

complex tests of psychomotor speed relies on

intact performance at this more basic level of

processing.

Key references: • Robbins, T. (2002). The 5-choice serial

reaction time task: behavioural pharmacology

and functional neurochemistry.

Psychopharmacology, 163(3-4), 362-380.

• Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-

speed theory of adult age differences in

cognition. Psychological review, 103(3), 403.

Page 9: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

7 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

Reaction Time Test (RTI)

Several CANTAB tests have an element of

processing speed, requiring participants to

respond as quickly as possible. RTI makes

response speed the focus of the task. The

structure of the task enables slowing in

movement to be dissociated from slowing in

cognitive processing. In addition, by increasing

the number of items the participant has to

monitor (from 1 to 5 circles), the cognitive load

of the task can be increased, thereby placing

heavier demands on attention.

Description of the test

RTI is a measure of simple and choice reaction

time, movement time and vigilance during

simple and 5-choice reaction time trials. This

task also permits measurement of

anticipatory/premature responding and

perseverative responding.

The participant must hold down a button until a

yellow spot appears on the screen, at which

point they must touch the yellow spot as quickly

as they can. The spot appears in a single location

during the simple reaction time phase and in one

of five locations in the 5-choice reaction time

phase.

Core outcomes

Key

Outcome

Measures

Definition

Median

Simple or 5-

choice

reaction

time

The median duration between the

onset of the stimulus and the time at

which the subject released the

button. This measure is calculated

from correct, assessed trials in which

the stimulus could appear in one

location only / any one of five

locations

Median

Simple or 5-

choice

movement

time

The median time taken to touch the

stimulus after the button has been

released. This measure is calculated

from correct, assessed trials where

stimuli could appear in one location

only / any one of five locations

CANTAB Reaction Time test

Page 10: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

8 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

Brain systems

Reaction time and attention are dependent on a

network of brain areas including the prefrontal

and parietal lobes, and are also dependent on

neurotransmitter release from mid-brain

structures such as the nucleus accumbens and

ventral tegmental area.

Pharmacological manipulations

The CANTAB RTI task is a direct analogue of the

rodent 5-choice serial reaction time test (5-

CSRT), one of the most well-studied animal

behaviour paradigms. In the rat, 5-CSRT shows

sensitivity to discrete lesion sites in the

prefrontal cortex, to cholinergic lesions in basal

forebrain (e.g. McGaughy, Dalley, Morrison,

Everitt, & Robbins, 2002), and to several classes

of compound. In humans, the CANTAB RTI test

shows differential pharmacological sensitivity.

For example, clonidine, but not guanfacine,

impairs five-choice reaction time performance in

young healthy volunteers (Jäkälä et al, 1999).

Age effects

With increasing age there is a gradual but

significant decrease in processing speed (Figure

1).

Sensitivity to impairment in clinical populations

Dementia and MCI

Alzheimer’s Disease (AD)

Cholinergic dysfunction is evident in Alzheimer’s

disease. This led to the prediction that this task

would demonstrate impairment in AD, and this

has now been demonstrated. For instance,

Swainson (2001) compared performance on

CANTAB tests in AD to that of a non-demented,

clinical population (patients with depression)

and a group of patients with ‘questionable

dementia’ (QD). They found that both AD and

QD subjects were impaired on the memory

tests. However, only the AD patients showed

impairment in both accuracy and latency in the

RTI test. Decline on the mini-mental state exam

(MMSE) was also found to correlate significantly

with choice reaction time.

A more recent study has confirmed these

findings. In a study which compared

performance of AD patients to those with Mild

Cognitive Impairment (MCI) and normal

controls, Saunders and Summers (2010) found

that the AD group was significantly slower on

choice reaction time than the all the other

groups. Significant slowing was also seen on the

simple reaction time measure in amnestic MCI

(a-MCI) and AD groups, with the AD group also

being significantly slower than the a-MCI group.

A pharmacological study of the cholinesterase

inhibitor tacrine in AD patients found that while

there was no significant effect on tests of

memory (e.g. PAL or DMTS), there was a

significantly beneficial effect on accuracy and

latency in RTI performance (Sahakian et al.,

1993). This suggests that the deficits seen in AD

are attributable to the alterations in cholinergic

neurotransmission in this disease.

Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI)

The data from studies which have examined MCI

alongside AD and normal ageing (e.g. Swainson

2001; Saunders and Summers, 2010), reviewed

briefly above, suggest that there are no, or

limited, differences between controls and MCI

Figure 1: Age related impairments in reaction time. Data

are expressed as mean ± SEM. N=250

Page 11: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

9 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

patients in reaction time measures. This has

been confirmed by studies which have focused

on subtypes of MCI (e.g. Klekociuk & Summers

2014a, 2014b).

However, at longitudinal follow-up of 10 and 20

months, a-MCI groups showed significant

slowing in both simple and choice reaction time.

This significant slowing was not observed in

control or non-amnestic MCI participants

(Saunders and Summers, 2011). It is unclear

whether this increase in slowing with time

represents part of a trajectory to dementia in

this MCI group. However, such impairment

seems to occur relatively late compared to

deficits in episodic memory (e.g. PAL).

Psychiatric and mood disorders

Depression

Depressed individuals frequently report

cognitive slowing as a symptom. Despite this, a

recent meta-analysis of cognitive function in

depression showed that there was no significant

impairment in RTI in currently depressed

patients (Rock et al., 2013), although some

authors have reported an improvement in

psychomotor speed during remission (Egerhazi

et al., 2013).

Schizophrenia

Impairments in reaction time are frequently

observed in schizophrenia. These tend to be

marked and are present even in medication-

naïve cases with first-episode schizophrenia

(Fagerlund et al., 2004).

Deficits are still present, but are not as profound

in early-onset schizophrenia (Fagerlund et al.,

2006). Interestingly, a longitudinal study of

early onset-schizophrenia demonstrated

significant impairment in both reaction times

and movement times relative to controls.

However, over five year follow-up there was no

significant worsening in the severity of this

impairment (Jepsen et al., 2010), suggesting

that this is an aspect of cognition which is

differently affected in early onset schizophrenia,

and remains relatively stable in this group.

Neurological and movement disorders

ADHD

Attentional processing is a core deficit in ADHD.

Consistent with this, impairments have been

observed in children with ADHD. For instance,

Gau and Huang (2013) compared both children

with ADHD and their unaffected siblings to

typically developing controls. They found that

those with ADHD performed worse in the RTI

task after controlling for sex, age, co-morbidity,

parental educational levels and IQ.

Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI)

Processing speed and psychomotor slowing are

constantly reported in studies of TBI. Parry et al

(2004) reported that the mean performance of

their TBI group was significantly poorer than

that of their control group on all measures

derived from the RTI, including the simple

reaction time, simple movement time, 5-choice

reaction time and the 5-choice movement time.

Performance of both TBI and control participants

was correlated with metabolite concentration on

magnetic resonance spectroscopy, indicating a

relationship between indices of reaction time

and brain structural integrity.

Sterr at el. (2006) demonstrated that CANTAB

RTI was significantly impaired in patients with

symptomatic TBI relative to non-symptomatic

TBI patients and healthy controls, and that there

was a significant correlation between RTI speed

and accuracy and the severity of post-

concussion symptoms, suggesting that the task

is sensitive to clinically meaningful indices of TBI

severity.

Parkinson’s Disease (PD)

A 2004 study of RTI in PD patients found slowing

of movement in patients off medication, but not

on medication (Fern-Pollak 2004). However, an

earlier study examining RTI across a range of

disease severity found that deficits in reaction

time and accuracy increased as the severity of

PD increased (Riekkinen et al., 1998).

Page 12: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

10 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

Furthermore, withdrawal of dopaminergic

medication had a differential effect on reaction

time, depending on the severity of the

Parkinson’s disease, with those with the most

severe PD showing the largest effect on reaction

time. Choice reaction time performance has

been found to be a significant predictor of

freezing of gait, an important motor feature of

PD (Shine et al., 2012).

References • Egerhazi, A., Balla, P., Ritzl, A., Varga, Z.,

Frecska, E., & Berecz, R (2013). Automated

neuropsychological test battery in

depression–preliminary data.

Neuropsychopharmacol. Hung, 15, 5-11.

• Fagerlund, B., Mackeprang, T., Gade, A.,

Hemmingsen, R., & Glenthoj, B. Y. (2004).

Effects of low-dose risperidone and low-dose

zuclopenthixol on cognitive functions in first-

episode drug-naive schizophrenic

patients. CNS spectrums, 9, 364-374.

• Fagerlund, B., Pagsberg, A. K., &

Hemmingsen, R. P. (2006). Cognitive deficits

and levels of IQ in adolescent onset

schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders.

Schizophrenia research, 85(1), 30-39.

• Fern-Pollak, L., Whone, A. L., Brooks, D. J., &

Mehta, M. A. (2004). Cognitive and motor

effects of dopaminergic medication

withdrawal in Parkinson’s

disease. Neuropsychologia, 42(14), 1917-

1926.

• Gau, S. F., & Huang, W. L. (2014). Rapid

visual information processing as a cognitive

endophenotype of attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder. Psychological

medicine, 44(02), 435-446.

• Jakala P., Riekkinen M., Sirvio J., Koivisto E.,

Riekkinen P. JR, (1999) Clonidine, but not

guanfacine, impairs choice reaction time

performance in young healthy volunteers.

Neuropsychopharmacology, 21, 495-502

• Jepsen, J. R. M., Fagerlund, B., Pagsberg, A.

K., Christensen, A. M. R., Nordentoft, M., &

Mortensen, E. L. (2010). Deficient maturation

of aspects of attention and executive functions

in early onset schizophrenia. European child &

adolescent psychiatry, 19(10), 773-786. • Klekociuk, S. Z., & Summers, M. J. (2014a).

Exploring the validity of mild cognitive

impairment (MCI) subtypes: Multiple-domain

amnestic MCI is the only identifiable subtype

at longitudinal follow-up. Journal of Clinical

And Experimental Neuropsychology, 36(3),

290-301.

• Klekociuk, S. Z., & Summers, M. J. (2014b).

Lowered performance in working memory and

attentional sub‐processes are most prominent

in multi‐domain amnestic mild cognitive

impairment subtypes. Psychogeriatrics,

14(1), 63-71.

• McGaughy, J., Dalley, J. W., Morrison, C. H.,

Everitt, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2002).

Selective behavioral and neurochemical

effects of cholinergic lesions produced by

intrabasalis infusions of 192 IgG-saporin on

attentional performance in a five-choice serial

reaction time task. The Journal of

neuroscience, 22(5), 1905-1913.

• Parry, L., Shores, A., Rae, C., Kemp, A.,

Waugh, M. C., Chaseling, R., & Joy, P. (2004).

An investigation of neuronal integrity in

severe paediatric traumatic brain injury. Child

Neuropsychology, 10(4), 248-261.

• Riekkinen, M., Kejonen, K., Jakala, P.,

Soininen, H., & Riekkinen, P. (1998).

Reduction of noradrenaline impairs attention

and dopamine depletion slows responses in

Parkinson’s disease. European Journal of

Neuroscience, 10, 1449-1435.

• Rock, P. L., Roiser, J. P., Riedel, W. J., &

Blackwell, A. D. (2013). Cognitive impairment

in depression: a systematic review and meta-

analysis. Psychological medicine, 1-12.

• Sahakian, B. J., Owen, A. M., Morant, N. J.,

Eagger, S. A., Boddington, S., Crayton, L., ...

& Levy, R. (1993). Further analysis of the

cognitive effects of tetrahydroaminoacridine

(THA) in Alzheimer's disease: assessment of

attentional and mnemonic function using

CANTAB. Psychopharmacology,110(4), 395-

401.

• Saunders, N. L., & Summers, M. J. (2011).

Longitudinal deficits to attention, executive,

and working memory in subtypes of mild

cognitive impairment.

Neuropsychology, 25(2), 237.

• Saunders, N.J, & Summers M.J. (2010)

Attention and working memory deficits in mild

cognitive impairment. Journal of Clinical and

Experimental Neuropsychology 32(4), 350-

Page 13: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

11 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

357.

• Shine, J. M., Moore, S. T., Bolitho, S. J.,

Morris, T. R., Dilda, V., Naismith, S. L., &

Lewis, S. J. G. (2012). Assessing the utility of

freezing of gait questionnaires in Parkinson’s

disease. Parkinsonism & related

disorders, 18(1), 25-29.

• Sterr, A., Herron, K. A., Hayward, C., &

Montaldi, D. (2006). Are mild head injuries as

mild as we think? Neurobehavioral

concomitants of chronic post-concussion

syndrome. BMC neurology, 6(1), 7.

• Swainson, R., Hodges, J. R., Galton, C. J.,

Semple, J., Michael, A., Dunn, B. D., ... &

Sahakian, B. J. (2001). Early detection and

differential diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease

and depression with neuropsychological

tasks. Dementia and geriatric cognitive

disorders, 12(4), 265-280.

Attention

Attention can be seen as a gateway for

information into the other cognitive domains,

regulating the access of information from both

external (perceptual) and internal (memories,

knowledge) sources to later processing and

storage stages. The analogy that is frequently

used to describe attention is that of a spotlight

– an area or object where processing resources

are concentrated, sometimes at the expense of

items outside the focus of attention. Attention is

a limited resource which weakens as the number

of items over which it is shared increases.

Attention can also be characterised on the basis

of its temporal characteristics. When asked to

maintain attention over a long time, fatigue or

boredom sets in and accuracy can be

compromised. The capacity to resist distraction

and maintain the focus of attention in the face

of distracting information is referred to as

“selective attention”, while the ability to

maintain a steady level of attention over time is

referred to as “sustained attention”.

Attention depends on a broad network of areas

in the frontal and parietal lobes, including the

dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the anterior

cingulate. Frontal regions are thought to be

particularly critical in guiding the goal-directed

allocation of attentional resources. In addition,

subcortical structures such as the pulvinar and

the superior colliculus are involved in the shifting

of attention.

Consistent with this widespread anatomical

network, multiple neurochemical systems are

implicated. Noradrenaline, acetylcholine and

dopamine all interact in complex ways to

modulate the shifting, maintenance and control

of attention.

Disruption to the spatial characteristics of

attention can be most clearly seen in the

example of unilateral neglect following a stroke

that affects the parietal lobes. This results in a

“bias” away from one half of space or one half of

an object, such that the patient does not report

items in the neglected side. Disruption to the

temporal characteristics of attention is a

hallmark of ADHD, where mind-wandering and

poor classroom performance are attributed

particularly to deficits in sustained attention.

Key references • Fried, R., Hirshfeld-Becker, D., Petty, C.,

Batchelder, H., & Biederman, J. (2012). How

informative is the CANTAB to assess

executive functioning in children with ADHD?

A controlled study. Journal of attention

disorders. • Klinkenberg, I., Sambeth, A., & Blokland, A.

Brain. (2011). Acetylcholine and

attention. Behavioural research, 221(2), 430-

442.

• Raz, A., & Buhle, J. (2006). Typologies of

attentional networks. Nature Reviews

Neuroscience, 7(5), 367-379.

Rapid Visual Information Processing Test (RVP)

While the RTI task provides an estimate of

vigilance, RVP assesses more complex aspects

of attention. In addition to the requirement to

respond accurately and quickly to a target,

participants have to discriminate what is and is

Page 14: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

12 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

not a target in a rapidly changing stimulus array.

This heavily taxes sustained and selective

attention as well as processing speed.

The A-Prime (A’) outcome measure is based on

signal detection theory, and provides a single

index of how good a participant is at detecting a

target against ongoing distraction.

Description of the test

RVP is a test of continuous performance and

visual sustained attention. In a white box in the

centre of the screen, single digits appear in a

pseudo random order at a rate of 100 digits /

minute. Participants must detect a series of

target sequences (for example, 2,4,6; 4,6,8;

3,5,7) and register responses by touching

button. Nine target sequences appear every 100

numbers.

Core outcomes

Key Outcome

Measures

Definition

A-Prime A’ (A prime) is the signal detection measure of sensitivity to the target,

regardless of response tendency (the

expected range is 0.00 to 1.00; bad to good). In essence, this metric is a measure of how

good the subject is at detecting target sequences

Median latency The median response latency during assessment sequence

blocks where the subject responded correctly

CANTAB Rapid Visual Information Processing test

PFC

pSMA

ACC

PPC

Figure 2: Key anatomical and neurotransmitter

systems involved in attention and attentional control.

Attention depends on at least three separate yet

interacting neurotransmitter systems: noradrenaline

(blue), acetylcholine (yellow) and dopamine (green).

These modulate the activity of regions involved in

attentional and cognitive controls, such as the

prefrontal cortex (PFC), anterior cingulate (ACC),

pre-supplementary motor area (pSMA) and posterior

parietal (PPC) regions.

Page 15: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

13 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

Brain systems

Performance on the RVP task is associated with

activation in a network of brain structures,

including the frontal and parietal lobes (Coull et

al., 1995). RVP activates the classic fronto-

parieto-occipital “attentional” network (Figure

3).

Pharmacology

RVP is sensitive to a range of sedating and

stimulant effects (see Chamberlain et al., 2011

for a systematic review). For example,

performance is worsened by diazepam (Coull et

al., 1995) and improved by nicotine (Sahakian

et al., 1989). Methylphenidate reduces response

latencies on this task in healthy young

volunteers (Elliott et al., 1997), but does not

affect performance in healthy elderly males

(Turner et al., 2003).

RVP is sensitive to monoamine manipulations,

including clonidine (Coull et al., 1995). Dietary

depletion of the dopamine precursor tyrosine

impaired RVP performance in recovered

depressed patients compared to patients treated

with a balanced amino acid mixture (Roiser et

al., 2005). While stimulants can increase the

rate of commission errors on this task,

methylphenidate has been found to improve

performance in adults with ADHD (Turner et al.,

2005). Nicotine improves performance in

healthy volunteers and patients with Alzheimer’s

disease (Jones et al., 1992).

Ageing

With age, there is a reduction in the speed and

accuracy of processing.

Sensitivity to impairment in clinical populations

AD and MCI

Alzheimer’s disease

A number of different studies have

demonstrated impairment in RVP in patients

with Alzheimer’s disease. Swainson et al (2001)

compared four groups of patients: controls,

patients with AD, a group with ‘questionable

dementia’ (QD) and a group of depressed

patients. Both AD and QD subjects showed

impairments in latency and accuracy on RVP. A

more recent study demonstrated that RVP

latency was significantly longer in AD compared

to controls. However, there were no significant

differences between MCI and AD (Saunders and

Summers, 2010).

Pharmacological studies have demonstrated

deficits in accuracy and latency in RVP at

baseline in AD patients relative to controls.

These deficits were ameliorated in a dose-

dependent manner by the administration of

nicotine (Sahakian et al., 1989; Jones et al.,

1992).

Mild cognitive impairment

Individuals with MCI have been found to have

significantly worse performance on the RVP task

compared to controls (Klekociuk & Summers

2014c; Saunders and Summers 2011), with

some differences between amnestic and non-

amnestic MCI in terms of latency and accuracy

indices (Klekociuk & Summers 2014b), and no

significant difference between MCI and early AD

(Saunders and Summers 2010).

In terms of differentiating MCI from normal

ageing, RVP latency and accuracy were two of

ten significant predictors which could be used to

correctly identify participants with MCI from

Occipital cortex

DLPFC / IFG

Parietal

cortex

Figure 3: Activation of a fronto-parietal network of areas

during performance of the RVP task (Coull et al., 1995)

Page 16: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

14 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

normal controls with a sensitivity of 80.0% and

a specificity of 87.9% (Klekociuk et al., 2014C).

However, longitudinal analysis has not

demonstrated a steady decline in performance

over a 10 and 20 month follow-up in patients

with MCI. Indeed, there is some evidence for

improved performance over time in some

individuals (Klekociuk & Summers 2014a).

Psychiatric and mood disorders

Depression

Patients with bipolar disorder are impaired on

RVP during both manic and remitted states

(Clark et al., 2001, 2002). In major depressive

disorder (MDD), some degree of sustained

attention impairment is present during

depressive phases, but these impairments

appear to recover completely during euthymia

(Clark et al., 2005).

A recent meta-analysis of studies of depressed

participants which have used CANTAB showed

that there was a significant deficit in RVP relative

to controls. This impairment was present even

in unmedicated and remitted participants (Rock

et al., 2013).

Schizophrenia

Deficits in patients with schizophrenia have been

reported in a number of studies. Prouteau et al.

(2004) explored the pattern of associations

between visual cognitive performance and

community functioning in patients with

Schizophrenia. They found that RVP

performance was associated with global

function, adjustment to living and behavioural

problems.

In addition to deficits in schizophrenia,

reductions in performance on CANTAB tests

including RVP were observed during the

prodromal phase of the disease (Bartók et al.,

2005). The authors concluded that deficits in

attention and cognition were present at the early

stages of development of psychosis and that

CANTAB might be a useful tool for early

detection in patients at risk of developing

schizophrenia.

Neurological and movement disorders

Traumatic brain injury

Numerous studies have shown that sustained

attention is impaired in patients with TBI (Chan,

2005; Malojcic et al., 2008; Parry et al., 2004).

For example, Salmond et al. (2005)

demonstrated that TBI patients exhibited

significantly longer response times and reduced

levels of target detection on CANTAB RVP

compared with healthy controls. The ecological

validity of the task is supported by findings such

as those of Sterr et al. (2006), who

demonstrated that performance on CANTAB RVP

was significantly associated with symptom

severity.

Multiple Sclerosis

Deficits in sustained attention are also observed

in MS, where participants with relapsing-

remitting MS (N=29) have slower response

latencies on the RVP (Roque et al., 2011).

ADHD

Attentional deficits, particularly the ability to

sustain attention, are a core deficit in ADHD.

Consistent with this, sustained attention

performance on RVP is significantly impaired in

ADHD (Turner et al., 2005, 2004). Gau and

Huang (2013) studied attentional performance

in typically developing controls, children with

ADHD and their unaffected siblings. They found

that, compared with controls, both children with

ADHD and their unaffected siblings had

significantly higher total misses and a lower

probability of hits in the RVP task. Deficits in

sustained attention were associated with a

longer duration of methylphenidate use and IQ,

but psychiatric co-morbidity and current use of

methylphenidate were not.

Page 17: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

15 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

References • Bartók, E., Berecz, R., Glaub, T., & Degrell, I.

(2005). Cognitive functions in prepsychotic

patients. Progress in Neuro-

Psychopharmacology and Biological

Psychiatry, 29(4), 621-625.

• Chamberlain, S. R., Robbins, T. W., Winder-

Rhodes, S., Müller, U., Sahakian, B. J.,

Blackwell, A. D., & Barnett, J. H. (2011).

Translational approaches to frontostriatal

dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder using a computerized

neuropsychological battery. Biological

psychiatry, 69(12), 1192-1203.

• Chan, R. C. (2005). Sustained attention in

patients with mild traumatic brain

injury. Clinical Rehabilitation, 19(2), 188-

193.

• Clark L., Iversen S. D. & Goodwin G. M.

(2001) A neuropsychological investigation of

prefrontal cortex involvement in acute mania.

American Journal of Psychiatry. 158, 1605-

1611

• Clark, L., Iversen, S.D. & Goodwin, G.M.

(2002) Sustained attention deficit in bipolar

disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry. 180,

313 -31

• Clark L., Kempton M. J., Scarna A., Grasby P.

M. & Goodwin G. M. (2005) Sustained

attention-deficit confirmed in euthymic bipolar

disorder but not in first-degree relatives of

bipolar patients or euthymic unipolar

depression. Biological Psychiatry. 57, 183-187

• Coull J.T., Middleton H.C., Robbins T.W.,

Sahakian B.J. (1995) Clonidine and diazepam

have differential effects on tests of attention

and learning, Psychopharmacology, 120, 322-

332

• Elliott R., Sahakian B.J., Matthews K.,

Bannerjea A., Rimmer J., Robbins T.W.

(1997) Effects of methylphenidate on spatial

working memory and planning in healthy

young adults, Psychopharmacology, 131,

196-206

• Gau, S. F., & Huang, W. L. (2013). Rapid

visual information processing as a cognitive

endophenotype of attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder. Psychological

medicine, 44(02), 435-446.

• Jones, G.M.M., Sahakian, B.J., Levy, R.,

Warburton, D.M., & Gray, J.A. (1992). Effects

of acute subcutaneous nicotine on attention,

information processing and short-term

memory in Alzheimer's disease.

Psychopharmacology, 108(4), 485-494.

• Klekociuk, S. Z., & Summers, M. J. (2014a).

Exploring the validity of mild cognitive

impairment (MCI) subtypes: Multiple-domain

amnestic MCI is the only identifiable subtype

at longitudinal follow-up. Journal of clinical

and experimental neuropsychology, 36(3),

290-301.

• Klekociuk, S. Z., & Summers, M. J. (2014b).

Lowered performance in working memory and

attentional sub‐processes are most prominent

in multi‐domain amnestic mild cognitive

impairment subtypes. Psychogeriatrics,

14(1), 63-71.

• Klekociuk, S. Z., & Summers, M. J. (2014c).

The learning profile of persistent mild

cognitive impairment (MCI): a potential

diagnostic marker of persistent amnestic MCI.

European Journal of Neurology, 21(3), 470-

e24.

• Malojcic, B., Mubrin, Z., Coric, B., Susnic, M.,

& Spilich, G. J. (2008). Consequences of mild

traumatic brain injury on information

processing assessed with attention and short-

term memory tasks. Journal of

neurotrauma,25(1), 30-37.

• Parry, L., Shores, A., Rae, C., Kemp, A.,

Waugh, M. C., Chaseling, R., & Joy, P. (2004).

An investigation of neuronal integrity in

severe paediatric traumatic brain injury. Child

Neuropsychology, 10(4), 248-261.

• Prouteau, A., Verdoux, H., Briand, C., Lesage,

A., Lalonde, P., Nicole, L., Reinharz, D. & Stip,

E. (2004). The crucial role of sustained

attention in community functioning in

outpatients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry

research, 129(2), 171-177.

• Rock, P. L., Roiser, J. P., Riedel, W. J., &

Blackwell, A. D. (2013). Cognitive impairment

in depression: a systematic review and meta-

analysis. Psychological medicine, 1-12.

• Roiser, J. P., McLean, A., Ogilvie, A. D.,

Blackwell, A. D., Bamber, D. J., Goodyer, I.,

Jones, P. B. & Sahakian, B. J. (2005). The

subjective and cognitive effects of acute

phenylalanine and tyrosine depletion in

patients recovered from depression.

Neuropsychopharmacology, 30 (4), 775-785.

• Roque, D. T., Teixeira, R. A. A., Zachi, E. C.,

& Ventura, D. F. (2011). The use of the

Page 18: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

16 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

Cambridge Neuropsychological Test

Automated Battery (CANTAB) in

neuropsychological assessment: application in

Brazilian research with control children and

adults with neurological disorders. Psychology

& Neuroscience, 4 (2), 255-265.

• Sahakian, B., Jones, G., Levy, R., Gray, J., &

Warburton, D. (1989). The effects of nicotine

on attention, information processing, and

short-term memory in patients with dementia

of the Alzheimer type. The British Journal of

Psychiatry, 154(6), 797-800.

• Salmond, C. H., Chatfield, D. A., Menon, D. K.,

Pickard, J. D., & Sahakian, B. J. (2005).

Cognitive sequelae of head injury:

involvement of basal forebrain and associated

structures. Brain, 128(1), 189-200.

• Saunders, N. L., & Summers, M. J. (2011).

Longitudinal deficits to attention, executive,

and working memory in subtypes of mild

cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology,

25(2), 237.

• Saunders, N.J, & Summers M.J. (2010)

Attention and working memory deficits in mild

cognitive impairment. Journal of Clinical and

Experimental Neuropsychology 32(4), 350-

357.

• Sterr, A., Herron, K. A., Hayward, C., &

Montaldi, D. (2006). Are mild head injuries as

mild as we think? Neurobehavioral

concomitants of chronic post-concussion

syndrome. BMC neurology, 6(1), 7.

• Swainson, R., Hodges, J. R., Galton, C. J.,

Semple, J., Michael, A., Dunn, B. D., Liddon,

J. L., Robbins, T. W. & Sahakian, B. J. (2001).

Early detection and differential diagnosis of

Alzheimer’s disease and depression with

neuropsychological tasks. Dementia and

geriatric cognitive disorders, 12(4), 265-280.

• Turner D.C., Robbins T.W., Clark L., Aron

A.R., Dowson J.H., Sahakian B.J.,

(2003) Relative lack of cognitive effects of

methylphenidate in elderly male

volunteers, Psychopharmacology, 168, 455-

464

• Turner, D. C., Blackwell, A. D., Dowson, J. H.,

McLean, A., & Sahakian, B. J. (2005).

Neurocognitive effects of methylphenidate in

adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder. Psychopharmacology, 178(2-3),

286-295.

• Turner, D. C., Clark, L., Dowson, J., Robbins,

T. W., & Sahakian, B. J. (2004). Modafinil

improves cognition and response inhibition in

adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder. Biological psychiatry, 55(10), 1031-

1040.

Page 19: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

17 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

Episodic Memory

Episodic memory refers to the ability to store

and retrieve information about specific events or

“episodes”. The material stored about each

event is a unique combination of items which

occur at a specific time and place. An example

is seeing somebody in the street and

remembering that they are Mike and that you

previously met him on New Year’s Eve at your

sister’s house. This unique combination of item

(Mike), place (your sister’s house) and time (a

specific New Year’s Eve) comprise an episode.

This type of memory is distinct from information

remembered without such a context, for

example our general knowledge or vocabulary,

which is accrued throughout life without being

“tagged” with a specific time and unique

context. This latter form of memory is termed

semantic memory.

Episodic memory is the basis for much of our

personal autobiographical memory and is crucial

for everyday life. Without the ability to store

material from our present and retrieve

information from the past, we become unable to

function. The devastating effects of Alzheimer’s

disease are in large part due to the erosion of

this form of memory, as the disease

progressively removes the capacity to form new

memories. This leads to disorientation in time

and place, and eventually to loss of even long-

stored memories such as the identity of family

and friends.

The neural basis of episodic memory has been

studied since the famous case in the 1950s of

Henry Molaison, or HM, a patient who underwent

surgical resection of the medial temporal lobes

and was rendered profoundly amnesic, unable to

store new information beyond a few minutes,

despite being still able to acquire new motor

skills and having otherwise intact language,

perceptual and cognitive skills. Since then, there

has been a large amount of research in animals

and patient populations, including imaging

studies, confirming the key role that medial

temporal lobe structures, particularly the

hippocampus, play in the formation and retrieval

of information. It is self-evident that these

processes of episodic memory do not occur in

isolation, but work in concert with and depend

on input from perceptual areas and strategic

coordination of resources from frontal regions

involved in executive control.

Decline in episodic memory is an unfortunate

characteristic of ageing. However, progressive

and severe loss of episodic memory is not

normal, and is considered a key feature of

Alzheimer’s disease. This is due to the fact that

Alzheimer’s pathology is first present in the

hippocampus and medial temporal lobe areas,

before inexorably progressing throughout the

brain to finally affect all areas of the cortex (see

Figure 4), at which point multiple, profound

deficits are seen in a wide range of cognitive

domains.

Figure 4: Smith et al., (2002) Progression of

Alzheimer's disease pathology. The earliest

pathological changes are present in the medial

temporal lobe, spreading at a later stage to other brain

areas, particularly those connected with the MTL,

resulting in more widespread cognitive changes.

Key references • Squire, L. R., & Zola, S. M. (1998). Episodic

memory, semantic memory, and amnesia.

Hippocampus, 8(3), 205-211.

• Tulving, E. (1985). Elements of episodic

memory.

• Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: from

mind to brain. Annual review of psychology,

53(1), 1-25.

• Smith, A. D. (2002). Imaging the progression

of Alzheimer pathology through the

brain. roceedings of the National Academy of

Sciences, 99(7), 4135-4137.

Paired Associates Learning (PAL)

Page 20: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

18 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

As described above, one of the hallmarks of

episodic memory is the binding of separate

pieces of information together, such as a person

and a specific time or place. This makes tests

which capture this feature particularly useful as

tests of episodic memory. PAL achieves this by

explicitly requiring the recall of a location which

was previously paired with an object.

Description of the test

The task consists of a number of stages that the

subject must complete in order. For each stage,

boxes are displayed on the screen. These boxes

open one at a time in a randomised order. One

or more of them will contain a pattern. The

patterns shown in the boxes are then displayed

one at a time in the middle of the screen, and

the subject must touch the box where the

pattern was originally located.

If the subject makes an error on a stage, the

patterns are re-presented to remind the

participant of their locations. For each stage, the

same patterns may be re-presented up to a set

number of times. When the participant gets all

the locations correct, they proceed to the next

stage. If they cannot complete a stage correctly,

the test terminates. At the easiest stages of PAL,

there is a single pattern to remember the

location of, and at the most difficult there are

eight patterns.

The patterns are designed to be difficult to

verbalise so that verbal rehearsal cannot be

used as a strategy. Their nonverbal nature also

allows the test to be used in international studies

without the need for stimuli translation.

Participants are allowed multiple attempts at

each trial, with automatic re-presentation of all

stimuli after each attempt until the stage is

passed or terminated. The test is adaptive so

that if a trial is not completed despite multiple

attempts, the test automatically terminates and

the error score calculated includes an

adjustment for errors at those stages that were

not attempted.

Administration time is approximately 8 minutes,

but depends on the performance of the

participant.

Core outcomes

Total errors (adjusted)

Definition: The number of times the subject

chose the incorrect box for a stimulus on

assessment problems but with an adjustment

for the estimated number of errors they would

have made on any problems, attempts & recalls

they did not reach due to failing or aborting the

test.

CANTAB Paired Associates Learning test

Page 21: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

19 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

Cognitive domains assessed

Paired associates learning is designed to assess

visual episodic memory and learning. Tests of

convergent validity have shown that there is a

modest correlation between the PAL and story

recall (-.19 immediate; -.21 delayed recall.

Smith et al., 2013). However, the verbal

component to story recall does limit the

comparability of these tasks. In terms of the

ecological validity of the measures, there is

strong correspondence between PAL and self-

reported memory problems (Swainson et al.,

2001), as well as other measures of memory.

Brain systems

Successful performance of the PAL test is

dependent on functional integrity of the

temporal lobe, particularly the entorhinal cortex,

but is also affected by resections of the frontal

lobe (Owen, Sahakian, Semple, Polkey, &

Robbins, 1995). Impairment on the CANTAB PAL

test correlates with hippocampal volume loss in

schizophrenia (Keri et al., 2012).

The PAL paradigm has been modified for use in

the MRI scanner, allowing direct assessment of

the brain areas involved in performance. This

has consistently shown involvement of the

medial temporal lobe, particularly the

hippocampus and parahippocamal regions (de

Rover et al., 2011; Owen et al., 1995; Figure 5).

De Rover et al. (2011) further showed that there

were differences in the pattern of hippocampal

activation during the PAL in MCI patients

compared to normal controls.

These data support the use of this measure as a

sensitive marker of hippocampal function.

Pharmacological data

The PAL task was designed to be a close

analogue to tasks used in both primate and

rodent research (e.g. Bussey et al., 2012). In

these models, performance has been found to

be dependent on the function of the

hippocampus which is fundamental to learning

associations between objects and locations.

The PAL task has been used in a range of

pharmacological studies, particularly those

targeting cholinergic systems implicated in

Alzheimer’s disease (e.g. scopolamine) in both

animals (e.g. Taffe et al, 2002) and humans. A

summary of the data from published

pharmacological studies in healthy participants

is presented below. These extensive data

demonstrate the sensitivity of these measures

to both pharmacological impairment and

improvement in performance. Of particular

interest is the presence of dose dependent

effects of scopolamine in healthy controls.

Ageing

Consistent with the early development of the

medial temporal lobes, episodic memory

improves rapidly in early childhood and remains

relatively stable through mid-life, before

worsening in later life (Figure 6).

Gender and education have also been found to

impact on error scores, such that women and

those with higher educational attainment

perform better on the task. However, there are

no significant interactions between age,

education and gender.

A

B

Figure 5: Significant bilateral hippocampal and

parahippocamal activation during the encoding (A) and

retrieval (B) of PAL in elderly subjects. Rover et al. (2011)

Page 22: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

20 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

Sensitivity to impairment in clinical populations

AD and MCI

Alzheimer’s disease

The medial temporal lobes are susceptible to

Alzheimer’s pathology, particularly early in the

course of the disease (Smith et al., 2002) and

problems with episodic memory can be the first

warning signs of AD. The sensitivity of PAL to

hippocampal function has led to the suggestion

that it may be used for the early and differential

diagnosis of AD (Blackwell et al., 2004; de Jager,

Lesk, Zhi, Marsico, & Chandler, 2008). Across

studies the sensitivity and specificity indices

were 0.94 and 0.91 respectively for detecting

AD compared to elderly non-demented controls.

The predictive ability for detecting incipient AD

in populations with mild cognitive impairment is

discussed below.

As would be expected, the rate of decline in PAL

performance in AD is greater than that seen on

average in MCI or healthy elderly populations

(Figure 7). For example, PAL total error score

(adjusted) increased by an average of 24 errors

over one year in patients with dementia of the

Alzheimer’s type (Fowler et al., 2002).

Mild cognitive impairment

Differentiating MCI from normal ageing is

challenging, since MCI represent a

heterogeneous group, with only certain

individuals progressing to Alzheimer's or

another underlying cognitive disease.

Nonetheless, PAL scores have a sensitivity of

0.83 and a specificity of 0.82 in differentiating

adults with MCI from healthy older adults

(Chandler et al., 2008). In a study at the

University of Tasmania, patients with amnestic

MCI showed poorer performance on PAL than

either controls or non-amnestic MCI patients at

baseline. When reassessed 10 months later,

patients with amnestic MCI had worsened by an

average of four errors at the six-pattern stage.

In contrast, PAL scores of patients with non-

amnestic MCI, who are at lower risk for

Alzheimer's disease, changed by less than one

error (Saunders & Summers, 2011).

An important issue in the study of MCI is

predicting which participants will go on to

develop AD and which will not. A meta-analysis

of 19 longitudinal studies has reported that

approximately 10% of MCI cases per year

progress to develop dementia (Bruscoli &

Lovestone, 2004). PAL has shown considerable

promise in this regard. Swainson et al. (2001)

examined the ability of PAL to distinguish AD

from controls and those with questionable

dementia (QD) over 24 months. At baseline,

both AD and QD participants were impaired on

PAL, and there was a significant correlation

between MMSE and PAL. PAL six-pattern error

score was able to classify group membership

with 98% accuracy. At baseline, those with QD

appeared to be split into two distinct groups:

one whose PAL scores resembled those with AD

and one that resembled control subjects. These

patients were followed up 6-12 months later, by

which time the QD subjects had split into two

groups: those whose performance had declined

similarly to AD, and those whose performance

had remained stable. By 24 months, all patients

with poor and deteriorating performance had

received a diagnosis of AD.

Figure 6: Age profile of PAL errors showing an increase in

errors with age in healthy participants

Page 23: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

21 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

Similar findings have been reported in a series

of studies by Fowler et al. (1995, 1997, 2002).

They demonstrate that PAL is able to predict

which participants with memory problems will go

on to have AD two years later. The authors

found that while participants with poor

performance and declining trajectories went on

to have a diagnosis of probable AD, those with

higher baseline scores and stable performance

did not (Fowler et al., 2002. See Figure 7). Using

PAL alongside a test of naming, patients with

relatively high baseline scores remained

dementia-free at 32 months, while all of those

impaired on both tests at baseline went on to

receive a diagnosis of probable AD (Blackwell et

al., 2004). Using the PAL alongside the

Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Evaluation (ACE),

Mitchell et al. (2009) obtained a sensitivity to

conversion to AD of 94%. This compared to

other cognitive tests, such as measures of

semantic memory and executive function, which

showed very low sensitivity to conversion,

ranging from 0-25%. These and similar findings

have led to the suggestion that PAL may be a

useful marker for the subsequent conversion of

MCI to AD (Blackwell et al., 2004; Sahakian et

al., 1988).

Recent studies examining the profile of

impairment in different subtypes of MCI have

found that participants with amnestic MCI (a-

MCI) show poorer performance than those with

non-amnestic MCI on PAL (Klekociuk &

Summers 2014a; Saunders & Summers 2011).

Together with other measures (including indices

from RVP, SWM and MTS), PAL scores

contributed significantly to the discrimination of

MCI cases from controls (Klekociuk & Summers,

2014c).

Non-Alzheimer’s dementia

Less common subtypes of dementia (and other

rarer neurodegenerative conditions) are

associated with a range of cognitive, behavioural

and functional deficits, and there are a number

of different clinical diagnostic criteria, of which

memory is a part. While memory assessment

can be helpful in earlier stages of assessment in

these diagnoses, it may not be a presenting

symptom until later stages of other disease

progression.

For Lewy Body Dementia (LBD), there is a study

showing that patients performed poorly on the

PAL task. Interestingly, in this study their

performance was more severely impaired than

patients with AD, which suggests that PAL may

have sensitivity to memory symptoms

associated with LBD (Galloway et al., 1992).

Fronto-temporal dementia (FTD) is often a

complex presentation and memory loss is not

typically a key feature in the early stages.

Nevertheless, from the few studies conducted,

there is some evidence that PAL performance is

impaired in the early stages of FTD presentation

(e.g. Deakin et al., 2003; Lee et al., 2003).

Figure 7: Paired associates learning decline over 24 months in individuals with questionable

dementia (Fowler, Saling, Conway, Semple, & Louis, 2002).

Page 24: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

22 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

Psychiatric and mood disorders

Depression

There is some debate regarding the sensitivity

of PAL to depression. Swainson et al. (2001)

showed that older adults with major unipolar

depression had PAL scores similar to age-

matched healthy controls, with little overlap

between these groups and patients with mild

Alzheimer’s disease. Similarly, Sweeney et al.

(2000) did not find a significant difference in PAL

performance between their young unipolar

depressed patients and healthy controls, but did

find that mixed/manic patients made more

errors than healthy subjects. O’Brien et al.

(1993) found that seasonal affective disorder

(SAD) patients were significantly impaired in

terms of number of trials to reach criterion on

PAL, compared both to healthy controls and to

their own performance once recovered.

A recent meta-analysis of studies using CANTAB

tests in depressed patients (Rock et al., 2013)

brought together these and similar studies and

revealed that, while PAL performance was

reduced in medicated currently depressed

patients, no significant change was seen in a

smaller number of studies of those not currently

medicated.

Schizophrenia

Patients with Schizophrenia show a level of

impairment similar to that seen in AD

(Gabrovska-Johnson et al., 2003). In addition,

there was a significant correlation between right

hemisphere ventricle size and performance on

the PAL in patients with Schizophrenia. Bartók

et al. (2005) compared the performance of the

patients to that of the CANTAB standardization

database. The performance of the prepsychotic

patients was significantly lower compared to the

healthy individuals on the PAL test (p<0.001),

as well as in tests of frontal lobe function.

Neurological and movement disorders

Multiple sclerosis

MS patients performed significantly worse than

controls on PAL. In addition, in MS patients

performance was associated with levels of

disease-specific markers (glutamate) in

hippocampus, thalamus and cingulate (Muhlert

et al., 2014). This again demonstrates the

sensitivity of the test to pathology of the

hippocampus.

Traumatic brain injury

TBI patients make more errors than controls on

the PAL test (Salmond, Chatfield, Menon,

Pickard, & Sahakian, 2005), and the degree of

impairment is associated with increased atrophy

in the hippocampal formation; this is measured

by white matter density (Salmond, Menon, et

al., 2006), again implicating this structure as a

key area mediating successful performance. A

later study examining changes in brain structure

outside the hippocampus found a statistically

significant negative correlation between

measures of white matter integrity in the corpus

callosum on diffusion weighted imaging and

memory performance (r = -0.588, P = .005)

(Holli et al., 2010).

References • Bartók, E., Berecz, R., Glaub, T., & Degrell, I.

(2005). Cognitive functions in prepsychotic

patients. Progress in Neuro-

Psychopharmacology and Biological

Psychiatry, 29, 621-625.

• Blackwell, A. D., Sahakian, B. J., Vesey, R.,

Semple, J. M., Robbins, T. W., & Hodges, J. R.

(2004). Detecting dementia: novel

neuropsychological markers of preclinical

Alzheimer's disease. Dementia and Geriatric

Cognitive Disorders, 17, 1-2.

• Bruscoli, M., & Lovestone, S. (2004). Is MCI

really just early dementia? A systematic

review of conversion studies. International

Psychogeriatrics, 16, 129-140.

• Bussey, T. J., Holmes, A., Lyon, L., Mar, A. C.,

McAllister, K. A. L., Nithianantharajah, J., ... &

Page 25: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

23 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

Saksida, L. M. (2012). New translational

assays for preclinical modelling of cognition in

schizophrenia: the touchscreen testing

method for mice and rats.

Neuropharmacology, 62(3), 1191-1203.

• Chandler, J. M., Marsico, M., Harper-Mozley,

L., Vogt, R., Peng, Y., Lesk, V., & de Jager, C.

(2008). P3-111: Cognitive assessment:

Discrimination of impairment and detection of

decline in Alzheimer's disease and mild

cognitive impairment. Alzheimer's &

Dementia, 4(4), T551-T552.

• de Jager, C. A., Lesk, V. E., Zhu, X., Marsico,

M., & Chandler, J. (2008). P3-120: Episodic

memory test constructs affect discrimination

between healthy elderly and cases with mild

cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease.

Alzheimer's & Dementia, 4(4), T554-T555.

• de Rover, M., Pironti, V. A., McCabe, J. A.,

Acosta-Cabronero, J., Arana, F. S., Morein-

Zamir, S., Hodges, J. R., Robbins, T. W.,

Fletcher, P. C., Nestor, P. J. & Sahakian, B. J.

(2011). Hippocampal dysfunction in patients

with mild cognitive impairment: a functional

neuroimaging study of a visuospatial paired

associates learning task.

Neuropsychologia, 49(7), 2060-2070.

• Deakin J.B., Aitken M.R., Dowson J.H.,

Robbins T.W., Sahakian B.J.,

(2003) Diazepam produces disinhibitory

cognitive effects in male

volunteers, Psychopharmacology, 173(1-2),

88-97

• Fowler KS, Saling MM, Conway EL, Semple JM,

Louis WJ (1995): Computerized delayed

matching to sample and paired associate

performance in the early detection of

dementia. Applied Neuropsychology 2: 72–78

• Fowler KS, Saling MM, Conway EL, Semple JM,

Louis WJ. (2002) Paired associate

performance in the early detection of DAT. J

Int Neuropsychol Soc. 8:58-71.

• Fowler, K. S., Saling, M. M., Conway, E. L.,

Semple, J. M., & Louis, W. J. (1997).

Computerized neuropsychological tests in the

early detection of dementia: prospective

findings. Journal of the International

Neuropsychological Society, 3(02), 139-146.

• Gabrovska-Johnson, V. S., Scott, M., Jeffries,

S., Thacker, N., Baldwin, R. C., Burns, A.,

Lewis, S. W. & Deakin, J. F. W. (2003). Right-

hemisphere encephalopathy in elderly

subjects with schizophrenia: evidence from

neuropsychological and brain imaging

studies. Psychopharmacology, 169, 367-375.

• Galloway, P. H., Shagal, A., Cook, J. H.,

Ferrier, I. N., & Edwardson, J. A. (1992).

Visual pattern recognition memory and

learning deficits in senile dementias of

Alzheimer and Lewy body types. Dementia

and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 3(2), 101-

107.

• Holli, K. K., Wäljas, M., Harrison, L.,

Liimatainen, S., Luukkaala, T., Ryymin, P.,

Eskola, E., Soimakallio, S., Öhman, J. &

Dastidar, P. (2010). Mild traumatic brain

injury: tissue texture analysis correlated to

neuropsychological and DTI findings.

Academic radiology, 17(9), 1096-1102.

• Kéri, S., Szamosi, A., Benedek, G., &

Kelemen, O. (2012). How does the

hippocampal formation mediate memory for

stimuli processed by the magnocellular and

parvocellular visual pathways? Evidence from

the comparison of schizophrenia and amnestic

mild cognitive impairment

(aMCI).Neuropsychologia, 50, 3193-3199

• Klekociuk, S. Z., & Summers, M. J. (2014a).

Exploring the validity of mild cognitive

impairment (MCI) subtypes: Multiple-domain

amnestic MCI is the only identifiable subtype

at longitudinal follow-up. Journal of clinical

and experimental neuropsychology, 36, 290-

301.

• Klekociuk, S. Z., & Summers, M. J. (2014c).

The learning profile of persistent mild

cognitive impairment (MCI): a potential

diagnostic marker of persistent amnestic MCI.

European Journal of Neurology, 21, 470-e24.

• Lee, A. C., Rahman, S., Hodges, J. R.,

Sahakian, B. J., & Graham, K. S. (2003).

Associative and recognition memory for novel

objects in dementia: implications for

diagnosis. European Journal of

Neuroscience, 18(6), 1660-1670.

• Mitchell, J., Arnold, R., Dawson, K., Nestor, P.

J., & Hodges, J. R. (2009). Outcome in

subgroups of mild cognitive impairment (MCI)

is highly predictable using a simple

algorithm. Journal of neurology, 256(9),

1500-1509.

• Muhlert, N., Atzori, M., De Vita, E., Thomas,

D. L., Samson, R. S., Wheeler-Kingshott, C.

A., ... & Ciccarelli, O. (2014). Memory in

Page 26: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

24 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

multiple sclerosis is linked to glutamate

concentration in grey matter regions. Journal

of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry,

jnnp-2013.

• Muller U., Clark L., Lam M.L., Moore R.M.,

Murphy C.L., Richmond N.K., Sandhu R.S.,

Wilkins I.A., Menon D.K., Sahakian B.J.,

Robbins T.W., (2005) Lack of effects of

guanfacine on executive and memory

functions in healthy male volunteers.

Psychopharmacology, 182, 205-213

• O'Brien, J. T., Sahakian, B. J., & Checkley, S.

A. (1993). Cognitive impairments in patients

with seasonal affective disorder. The British

Journal of Psychiatry, 163(3), 338-343.

• Owen, A. M., Sahakian, B. J., Semple, J.,

Polkey, C. E. and Robbins, T. W. (1995) Visuo-

spatial short-term recognition memory and

learning after temporal lobe excision, frontal

lobe excision or amygdalo-hippocampectomy

in man. Neuropsychologia, 33, 1-24.

• Rock, P. L., Roiser, J. P., Riedel, W. J., &

Blackwell, A. D. (2013). Cognitive impairment

in depression: a systematic review and meta-

analysis. Psychological medicine, 1-12.

• Sahakian, B. J., Morris, R. G., Evenden, J. L.,

Heald, A., Levy, R., Philpot, M., & Robbins, T.

W. (1988). A comparative study of

visuospatial memory and learning in

Alzheimer-type dementia and Parkinson's

disease. Brain, 111(3), 695-718.

• Salmond, C. H., Chatfield, D. A., Menon, D. K.,

Pickard, J. D., & Sahakian, B. J. (2005).

Cognitive sequelae of head injury:

involvement of basal forebrain and associated

structures. Brain, 128(1), 189-200.

• Salmond, C. H., Menon, D. K., Chatfield, D. A.,

Williams, G. B., Pena, A., Sahakian, B. J., &

Pickard, J. D. (2006). Diffusion tensor imaging

in chronic head injury survivors: correlations

with learning and memory indices.

Neuroimage, 29(1), 117-124.

• Saunders, N. L., & Summers, M. J. (2011).

Longitudinal deficits to attention, executive,

and working memory in subtypes of mild

cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology,

25(2), 237.

• Smith, A. D. (2002). Imaging the progression

of Alzheimer pathology through the

brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of

Sciences, 99(7), 4135-4137

• Smith, P. J., Need, A. C., Cirulli, E. T., Chiba-

Falek, O., & Attix, D. K. (2013). A comparison

of the Cambridge Automated

Neuropsychological Test Battery ( CANTAB)

with “traditional” neuropsychological testing

instruments, Journal of clinical and

experimental neuropsychology, 35(3), 319-

328.

• Swainson, R., Hodges, J. R., Galton, C. J.,

Semple, J., Michael, A., Dunn, B. D., ... &

Sahakian, B. J. (2001). Early detection and

differential diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease

and depression with neuropsychological tasks.

Dementia and geriatric cognitive disorders,

12(4), 265-280.

• Sweeney, J. A., Kmiec, J. A., & Kupfer, D. J.

(2000). Neuropsychologic impairments in

bipolar and unipolar mood disorders on the

CANTAB neurocognitive battery. Biological

psychiatry, 48(7), 674-684.

• Taffe, M. A., Weed, M. R., Gutierrez, T., Davis,

S. A., & Gold, L. H. (2002). Differential

muscarinic and NMDA contributions to visuo-

spatial paired-associate learning in rhesus

monkeys. Psychopharmacology, 160(3), 253-

262.

Page 27: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

25 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

Delayed match to sample (DMS)

Delayed matching to sample is based on

paradigms developed for use in memory

research with non-human primates. These were

designed to tap memory processes in a non-

verbal manner and control for any deficits in

perception or attention which may be

responsible for decreases in performance. This

is achieved by having a simultaneous condition

where the participant matches simultaneously

presented stimuli to an identical item and an

array of visually similar items. Increasing the

delay between offset of the item to be

remembered and the onset of the choices makes

this task demanding of memory capacity, and

therefore sensitive to deficits of the medial

temporal lobe.

Description of the test

DMS is a test of simultaneous and delayed

matching to sample. This test assesses visual

matching ability and visual recognition memory.

Participants have to select from four similar

complex visual patters displayed at the bottom

of the screen the one that is the same as a

pattern displayed in the centre of the screen. In

some trials, the choice patterns at the bottom of

the screen and the sample pattern in the middle

of the screen are displayed simultaneously

whereas in others the choice patterns are only

shown after the sample pattern (delays of 0, 4

or 12 seconds between the presentation of the

sample pattern and the choice patterns).

Administration time: approximately 7 minutes

Core outcomes

Key Outcome

Measures

Definition

Percent correct The percentage of assessment trials

during which the subject selected the

correct box on their first box choice

Median Correct

latency

The median latency

from the available choices being displayed to the subject choosing the correct choice on assessment trials where the subject’s

first choice is correct

Probability of error given error

This measure reports the probability of an error occurring when the previous trial was responded to

incorrectly and is

used in calculations of A’ and B’’ (Note that this is incalculable if <1

errors made)

CANTAB Delayed Match to Sample test

Page 28: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

26 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

Cognitive domains assessed

A study looking at the convergent validity of the

CANTAB tests in a clinical sample confirmed that

there was a significant association between

delayed matching to sample and a pencil-and–

paper measure of the ability to remember and

match visual patterns (Torgersen et al., 2012).

This measure was not correlated with indices of

verbal memory or executive function.

Brain Systems

Many lesion studies in both humans and non-

human primates have indicated that delayed

matching to sample is primarily sensitive to

damage in the medial temporal lobes

(particularly hippocampus) and frontal lobes

(Sahgal and Iversen, 1978).

Imaging work has confirmed the contribution of

the medial temporal lobes and furthermore has

demonstrated that this contribution is delay-

dependent. Performance at longer delays is

more dependent on the medial temporal lobes.

At short delays, the task can be performed on

the basis of perceptual features, and therefore

is more dependent on visual processing areas,

such as the occipital lobes (Elliot and Dolan,

1999).

These findings have been more recently

confirmed in a study by Picchioni et al., (2007)

which supports the greater involvement of

medial temporal and frontal structures with

increasing delay periods (Figure 8).

Pharmacological manipulations

Matching to sample performance shows

established sensitivity to pharmacological

manipulations. Dose-dependent reductions in

performance were seen following administration

of scopolamine. Reductions in performance were

also seen following administration of the

adrenergic agonist clonidine. Conversely, small

but non-significant increases were seen

following administration of the

acetylcholinesterase inhibitor donepezil.

Administration of modafinil also produced a

modest increase in performance.

Figure 8: Picchioni et al., (2007). Increasing activation

in the anterior cingulate (upper panel) and in the

medial temporal lobe (lower panel) with increasing

maintenance delay across conditions (12,000 ms >

4,000 ms > simultaneous). Results are superimposed

onto a standard template brain.

Ageing

The effects of ageing on DMS are illustrated in

Figure 9 using published data in a large Chinese

sample. These data show a gradual decline with

age in performance on this task.

Figure 9: Age effects on DMS from Lee et al., (2013).

N=184

14

15

16

17

18

19

20s 30s 40s 50s >60

DM

S s

co

re

Age Category

Page 29: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

27 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

Sensitivity to impairment in clinical populations

AD and MCI

Alzheimer’s disease

As can be expected on the basis of the clear role

the medial temporal lobes play in AD,

performance on this memory task is affected

early in the course of AD and correlates with Mini

Mental State Examination (MMSE) performance

(Swainson et al., 2001). The memory profile of

AD patients shows a delay-dependent deficit. As

the delay period increased, and correspondingly

the medial temporal lobe demands increased,

performance of the AD patients became

increasingly poor (Abas et al., 1990).

Importantly, researchers have found that DMS

can aid in differentiating AD from MCI (Saunders

& Summers 2010) and depression in older adults

(Abas et al., 1990). Interestingly, in the study

group, although both groups were impaired in

the delayed MTS condition, AD patients made

more random distractor errors, and the

depressed patients’ degree of response slowing

was not related to task difficulty, unlike the

control group and the AD group, where reaction

times increased with task difficulty.

Mild cognitive impairment

Consistent with the memory impairments

characteristic of MCI, impairments in MTS

performance have been found in these patients

compared to controls (Klekociuk & Summers

2014 c), although of less severity than those

seen in AD (Swainson et al., 2001; Saunders &

Summers 2010).

There are some differences in performance in

different MCI subtypes, with non-amnestic MCI

showing relatively intact performance

(Klekociuk & Summers 2014 b).

Psychiatric and mood disorders

Depression

Depressed participants often report subjective

cognitive impairment, and differentiating

dementia and depression in older adults can be

challenging.

A recent meta-analysis showed that impairment

was observed in both unmedicated and

medicated patients who were currently

depressed (Rock et al., 2013). However, these

deficits were found to resolve after remission of

the depression.

Middle-aged adults with severe depression show

impairments both in accuracy and latency on

DMS (Elliott et al., 1996). Critically, DMS scores

show a moderate but significant correlation (r =

0.5) with clinical measures of depression such

as the Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating

Scale and the Clinical Interview for Depression

(Elliott et al., 1996).

Depressed individuals with bipolar I disorder

show normal performance in the simultaneous

condition that assesses perceptual ability

without memory demand, but show increasing

impairment as the delay period increases

(Rubinsztein et al., 2006). This effect has also

been shown in bipolar patients during euthymic

states (Rubinsztein et al., 2000).

An interesting feature of depressed individuals’

performance on this task is the significantly

increased likelihood of making an error after an

error, i.e. increased sensitivity to negative

feedback. This effect is seen to a significantly

greater extent in depressed individuals than in

patients with other neurocognitive disorders,

such as Parkinson’s disease and Schizophrenia,

or neurosurgical patients (Figure 10: Elliot et

al., 1996). This over-sensitivity to negative

feedback is an example of a negative bias in

information processing that is seen in

depression.

Page 30: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

28 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

With regards to the differentiation of AD and

depression in older adults, Abas et al. (1990)

found that elderly depressed patients were not

impaired on simultaneous matching to sample

but were as impaired as AD patients in accuracy

and reaction time on the DMS task. Both the

depressed and the AD patients also showed a

delay-dependent deficit in memory. Their

performance was different from that of AD

patients in two respects, however. Firstly, the

AD patients made significantly more random

distractor errors than the depressed patients or

the controls, with significantly fewer shape

distractor errors than colour distractor errors

(unlike the other groups, who made roughly

equal numbers of each error type). Secondly,

the depressed patients’ degree of response

slowing was not related to task difficulty,

whereas in the control group and the AD group,

reaction times increased with task difficulty.

Neurological and movement disorders

Traumatic brain injury

Consistent with the impact that TBI can have on

cognitive and attentional systems, Salmond et

al. (2005) showed that TBI patients make

significantly more errors and responded more

slowly relative to healthy controls. However,

Parry et al. (2004) found that scores were within

normal limits. The differences between the two

studies are probably accounted for by

differences in the severity of impairment in this

heterogeneous condition.

ADHD

There have been fewer studies examining

performance in ADHD on this task. However,

Gau and Huang (2014) found that, compared

with controls, participants with ADHD performed

worse in medial temporal lobe (MTS) tasks after

controlling for sex, age, co-morbidity, parental

educational levels and IQ. This may be

secondary to the attentional problems evident in

ADHD.

0.0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

Depressedpatients N=28

Parkinson'sdisease N=17

Schizophreniapatients N=11

Neurosurgicalpatients N=23

Healthycontrols N=22

Pro

bab

ility

of

erro

r af

ter

erro

r

Figure 10: probability of an error given an error in different patient groups. Journal of Neurology:

‘Abnormal response to negative feedback in unipolar depression: evidence for a diagnosis specific

impairment’ Elliott, Sahakian, Herrod, Robbins, Paykel (1996)

Page 31: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

29 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

References • Abas, M. A., Sahakian, B. J., & Levy, R.

(1990). Neuropsychological deficits and CT

scan changes in elderly

depressives. Psychological medicine, 20(03),

507-520.

• Elliott, R., & Dolan, R. J. (1999). Differential

neural responses during performance of

matching and nonmatching to sample tasks at

two delay intervals. The Journal of

Neuroscience, 19(12), 5066-5073.

• Elliott, R., Sahakian, B. J., McKay, A. P.,

Herrod, J. J., Robbins, T. W., & Paykel, E. S.

(1996). Neuropsychological impairments in

unipolar depression: the influence of

perceived failure on subsequent

performance. Psychological medicine, 26(05),

975-989.

• Gau, S. F., & Huang, W. L. (2014). Rapid

visual information processing as a cognitive

endophenotype of attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder. Psychological

medicine, 44(02), 435-446.

• Klekociuk, S. Z., & Summers, M. J. (2014b).

Lowered performance in working memory and

attentional sub‐processes are most prominent

in multi‐domain amnestic mild cognitive

impairment subtypes. Psychogeriatrics,

14(1), 63-71.

• Klekociuk, S. Z., & Summers, M. J. (2014c).

The learning profile of persistent mild

cognitive impairment (MCI): a potential

diagnostic marker of persistent amnestic MCI.

European Journal of Neurology, 21(3), 470-

e24.

• Lee, A., Archer, J., Wong, C. K. Y., Chen, S.

H. A., & Qiu, A. (2013). Age-Related Decline

in Associative Learning in Healthy Chinese

Adults. PloS one, 8(11), e80648.

• Parry, L., Shores, A., Rae, C., Kemp, A.,

Waugh, M. C., Chaseling, R., & Joy, P. (2004).

An investigation of neuronal integrity in

severe paediatric traumatic brain injury. Child

Neuropsychology, 10(4), 248-261.

• Picchioni, M., Matthiasson, P., Broome, M.,

Giampietro, V., Brammer, M., Mathes, B.,

Fletcher, P., Williams, S. & McGuire, P.

(2007). Medial temporal lobe activity at

recognition increases with the duration of

mnemonic delay during an object working

memory task. Human brain mapping, 28(11),

1235-1250.

• Rock, P. L., Roiser, J. P., Riedel, W. J., &

Blackwell, A. D. (2013). Cognitive impairment

in depression: a systematic review and meta-

analysis. Psychological medicine, 1-12.

• Rubinsztein, J. S., Michael, A., Paykel, E. S.,

& Sahakian, B. J. (2000). Cognitive

impairment in remission in bipolar affective

disorder. Psychological medicine, 30(05),

1025-1036.

• Rubinsztein, J. S., Michael, A., Underwood, B.

R., Tempest, M., & Sahakian, B. J. (2006).

Impaired cognition and decision-making in

bipolar depression but no ‘affective bias’

evident. Psychological Medicine, 36(05), 629-

639.

• Sahgal, A., & Iversen, S. D. (1978). The

effects of foveal prestriate and inferotemporal

lesions on matching to sample behaviour in

monkeys. Neuropsychologia, 16(4), 391-406.

• Salmond, C.H., Chatfield, D.A., Menon, D.K.,

Pickard, J.D., & Sahakian, B.J. (2005).

Cognitive sequelae of head injury:

involvement of basal forebrain and associated

structures. Brain, 128(1), 189-200.

• Saunders, N.J, & Summers M.J. (2010)

Attention and working memory deficits in mild

cognitive impairment. Journal of Clinical and

Experimental Neuropsychology 32(4), 350-

357.

• Swainson, R., Hodges, J. R., Galton, C. J.,

Semple, J., Michael, A., Dunn, B. D., Iddon, J.

L., Robbins, T. W. & Sahakian, B. J. (2001).

Early detection and differential diagnosis of

Alzheimer’s disease and depression with

neuropsychological tasks. Dementia and

geriatric cognitive disorders, 12(4), 265-280.

• Torgersen, J., Flaatten, H., Engelsen, B. A., &

Gramstad, A. (2012). Clinical validation of

Cambridge neuropsychological test

automated battery in a Norwegian epilepsy

population. Journal of Behavioral and Brain

Science, 2, 108.

Page 32: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

30 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

Working memory and executive function

Working memory can be defined as the ability to

hold material in mind while that material is

actively processed. This cognitive component is

critical to many everyday activities such as

mental arithmetic and following multi-step

instructions and directions. A network of brain

areas is involved in these kinds of tasks,

consisting of parietal, temporal and prefrontal

cortex. As a key cognitive skill dependent on a

network of areas, it is impaired in a range of

developmental and degenerative disorders,

particularly those impacting on the prefrontal

cortex and dopaminergic neurotransmission,

such as schizophrenia and ADHD.

Executive function is perhaps the most complex

and multifaceted cognitive domain. It is

comprised of multiple sub-processes, only some

of which will be addressed here. Broadly,

executive function allocates attentional

resources and coordinates the work of other

cognitive systems to guide action to fulfil goals.

An example of this can be seen in shopping for

food at the supermarket: the shopper has to

formulate a goal, which is to buy what they

need, employ their (episodic) memory of where

things are to navigate through the supermarket,

find what they need on crowded shelves of

similar items (selective attention), keep track of

what they have bought so far (working

memory). They have to do all this while resisting

the urge to buy items which would blow the

budget, and behave in socially appropriately

ways to their fellow shoppers and staff. At the

end of all this, they have to find where they

parked their car (episodic memory again).

The coordination of these individually complex

tasks, planning, strategic thinking and inhibitory

control are all key aspects of executive function.

These components depend on the frontal lobes,

with different sub-divisions playing a role in

distinct aspects. Damage to the frontal lobes,

following head injury, for example, is associated

with impairments of executive function, which

can have a devastating effect on the ability of

individuals to perform in cognitively demanding

or unstructured real-life situations, and to make

decisions. Deficits are also seen in conditions

such as schizophrenia which also affect the pre-

frontal cortex.

Key references • Baddeley, Alan D., and Graham Hitch.

"Working memory." Psychology of learning

and motivation 8 (1974): 47-89.

• D'Esposito, M., Detre, J. A., Alsop, D. C.,

Shin, R. K., Atlas, S., & Grossman, M.

(1995). The neural basis of the central

executive system of working memory.

Nature, 378(6554), 279-281.

Page 33: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

31 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

Spatial Working Memory (SWM)

The Spatial Working Memory test taxes the key

aspects of working memory: storage of

information over short periods of time and the

updating and manipulation of that information to

guide action. It also provides a measurement of

strategy use, thereby indexing executive

function in addition to working memory

capacity.

Description of the test

SWM measures the ability to retain spatial

information and manipulate it in working

memory. It is a self-ordered task that also

assesses the use of strategy.

Participants must search for blue tokens by

touching the coloured boxes to open them. The

critical instruction is that the participant must

not return to a box where a token has previously

been found. The task becomes more difficult as

the number of boxes increases.

Core outcomes

Key Outcome

Measures

Definition

Between errors The total number of times the subject revisits a box in which a token has

previously been found in the same

problem (calculated for assessed problems only)

Strategy For assessed problems with six boxes or more, the number of distinct boxes used by the subject to begin a new search for a

token, within the same problem

CANTAB Spatial Working Memory test

Page 34: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

32 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

Brain systems

Data from patients with brain lesions

demonstrates that SWM performance is

impaired by damage to the prefrontal cortex,

especially the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex

(Manes et al., 2002; Owen, Downes, Sahakian,

Polkey, & Robbins, 1990).

Versions of the task adapted for use in brain

scanning techniques have been used to shed

further light on the neural substrates mediating

SWM task performance. Positron emission

topography (PET) indicated that the dorsolateral

and mid-ventrolateral prefrontal cortices are

particularly recruited during task performance

(Figure 11: Mehta et al., 2000).

Figure 11. Control participants performing this task

show activation in the VLPFC and DLPFC

Pharmacology

Numerous studies have suggested that SWM

performance is sensitive to dopamine

manipulation. For example, SWM is impaired by

acute tyrosine depletion (Harmer, McTavish,

Clark, Goodwin, & Cowen, 2001), and improved

by D2 agonist administration (Mehta et al.,

2001). Furthermore, the stimulant

methylphenidate significantly improves SWM

performance in healthy volunteers as well as

various patient groups (Kempton et al., 1999;

Mehta et al., 2000; Turner et al., 2005). By

integrating imaging and pharmacological

approaches, it was found that dorsolateral

prefrontal cortex activation during SWM

performance is dependent on dopamine

modulation (Mehta et al., 2000).

This task has been used in a number of

pharmacological studies of patients. For

example, adults with ADHD show significantly

improved performance on SWM following

administration of methylphenidate but not

modafinil (Kempton et al., 1999; Turner et al.,

2005).

Ageing

Consistent with the protracted development of

the prefrontal cortex thorough childhood and

adolescence and its sensitivity to normal ageing,

performance on the SWM task shows both clear

developmental and ageing effects particularly in

the between-search errors index (Figure 12).

Figure 12: normative trajectory of performance in the

SWM task. Higher scores indicate worse performance

Sensitivity to impairment in clinical populations

Dementia and MCI

Alzheimer’s Disease

In addition to affecting episodic memory, AD

affects working memory, which is dependent on

frontal and temporal lobe structures. Patients

have been found to perform worse than healthy

controls on spatial working memory (Sahgal et

al., 1992). The pattern of performance is such

that there is a steep decrease in performance as

task difficulty increases in both AD and controls,

Page 35: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

33 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

although AD performance is worse than that of

controls at all stages of the task (Figure 13). This

highlights the suitability of this test even in

cases with mild dementia, as performance does

not reach floor levels early on in the task.

Figure 13: Sahgal et al., 1992

Mild cognitive impairment

SWM contributes to discrimination of MCI from

normal ageing in combination with other

measures of memory, attention and executive

function (Klekociuk & Summers, Neurology,

2014).

Significant differences in working memory

capacity have been found between subgroups of

MCI, with those with a-MCI+ (amnestic MCI with

additional deficits in other domains) making

more errors than those with purely amnestic

MCI (Klekociuk & Summers 2014b; Saunders &

Summers 2011).

The strategy score also showed significant

differences between MCI sub-groups, with again

the a-MCI+ performing worse than the a-MCI

group. (Klekociuk & Summers 2014a). SWM

strategy scores were significantly higher at

baseline compared to 10 month and 20 month

follow-up periods (Saunders & Summers 2011).

Non-Alzheimer’s dementia

There is less data regarding the profile of

cognitive performance in other forms of

dementia. Nevertheless, there is some evidence

to suggest that working memory is impaired.

Rahman et al. (1999) used CANTAB to profile

the cognitive dysfunction of patients with frontal

variant fronto-temporal dementia (fvFTD).

Small groups of fvFTD and age-matched controls

(n=8 in each group) were compared on a

number of CANTAB tests: pattern and spatial

recognition, spatial span, spatial working

memory and set-shifting.

In this study, fvFTD subjects were relatively

unimpaired on the spatial working memory task,

although differences were found in more

complex CANTAB measures of executive

function.

However, Sahgal et al (1995) assessed

differences in cognitive profiles between

subjects with AD and those with dementia of the

Lewy Body type (DLB). They examined spatial

working memory in a small subset of their

sample (AD, n=8; DLB, n=8), and found a

differential pattern of errors. Control subjects

made the fewest between search errors,

followed by AD patients, and DLB patients made

the greatest. For within-search errors, DLB

patients were significantly impaired in

comparison with AD and controls (these two

groups did not differ). This differential pattern of

impairment on spatial working memory is

thought to reflect dysfunctions in non-mnemonic

processes mediated by fronto-striatal circuits,

which are more severely damaged in DLB.

Psychiatric and mood disorders

Depression

A recent meta-analysis of studies of depressed

participants which have used CANTAB showed

that there was a significant deficit in spatial

working memory, relative to controls. This

impairment was present even in unmedicated

and remitted participants (Rock et al., 2013),

suggesting that this is potentially an important

cognitive variable in depression.

Page 36: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

34 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

Schizophrenia

Cognitive Impairment Associated with

Schizophrenia (CIAS) is an important

determinant of functional outcome, and working

memory impairment is a core component of this.

Impairments have been seen across the range

of chronicity and severity of schizophrenia.

In hospitalised patients with chronic

schizophrenia, Pantelis et al. (1997) found

deficits in SWM relative to healthy controls. In

addition, comparison between patients with

schizophrenia and those with frontal lobe lesions

showed equivalent impairments on spatial

working memory, with increased between-

search errors. Patients with schizophrenia were

unable to develop a systematic strategy to

complete this task, relying instead on a limited

visuospatial memory span.

Elliot et al. (1998) considered

neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenic

patients with preserved intellectual function (IQ

>90) in order to examine the specificity of their

cognitive impairment independent of global

decline. On Spatial Working Memory, the

patients showed impairment on both mnemonic

and strategic components of the task, in

contrast to temporal lobe patients (mnemonic

impairment only) and frontal lobe patients

(strategic impairment only) (Owen et al, 1995).

Thus the data presented in this study confirm

the many previous findings of mnemonic and

executive dysfunction in schizophrenia, but do

not suggest primacy of one over the other.

Deficits are present even earlier in the course of

the disease, before the cumulative effects of

medication have had an impact. Hutton et al.

(1998) researched executive function in first-

episode schizophrenia in 30 patients and 30

healthy volunteers matched for age and NART

IQ. Patients with schizophrenia showed the

greatest impairments on tests of executive

functioning, including SWM.

The potential importance of SWM in the

cognitive phenotype of schizophrenia is

highlighted in a study of patients before the

onset of diagnosed schizophrenia. Wood et al.

(2003) explored the relationship between

working memory and negative symptoms in

patients who had been referred to the Personal

Assessment and Crisis Evaluation Clinic,

Melbourne, Australia, as being at high risk of

developing psychosis. There were 38 high risk

patients (9 of which later become psychotic at

least 12 months from baseline assessment) and

49 healthy controls. The high risk group

performed significantly more poorly than the

healthy controls, and those who went on to

develop psychosis performed more poorly than

those who did not, but this did not reach

significance. However, for the group that went

on to develop psychosis, there was a significant

association between their SWM errors score and

their total score on the Scale for Assessment of

Negative Symptoms.

Neurological and movement disorders

Parkinson’s Disease

SWM performance is impaired in Parkinson’s

disease (Owen et al., 1997, 1992). This

impairment is thought to be driven by

dopaminergic dysfunction in PD. L-dopa

withdrawal impairs SWM task performance in

patients with Parkinson’s disease (

Page 37: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

35 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

Figure 14: Lange et al., 1992). Neuroimaging

evidence suggests that the beneficial effect of L-

dopa is mediated by improved efficiency in the

dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Cools, Stefanova,

R. Barker, Robbins, & Owen, 2002).

Figure 14: L-dopa withdrawal impairs spatial working

memory in patients with Parkinson’s disease (Lange et

al., 1992).

Multiple sclerosis

SWM performance has been found to be

impaired in patients with multiple sclerosis

relative to controls (

Figure 15: Foong et al., 2000; Foong et al.,

1997). Furthermore, performance at the more

difficult 6-box and 8-box stages of this task

correlated significantly with frontal lobe lesion

load determined using MRI in patients with

multiple sclerosis (Foong et al., 1997), again

indicating the sensitivity of this measure to

frontal lobe impairments.

Figure 15: Patients with multiple sclerosis show

impaired spatial working memory relative to controls

at all levels of difficulty (Foong et al., 1997).

Traumatic Brain Injury

Sterr et al. (2006) showed a trend for poorer

CANTAB SWM performance in patients with

symptomatic TBI relative to non-symptomatic

TBI patients and healthy controls. Performance

on CANTAB SWM was significantly associated

with symptom severity. A study by McAllister et

al. (2001) showed that TBI patients

demonstrate different activation patterns of

working memory circuitry on fMRI relative to

healthy controls. A further review by McAllister

(2006) suggests that patients with TBI have

problems in the activation and allocation of

working memory, and that these may be

secondary to reduced catecholaminergic

function.

ADHD

Both individual studies (e.g. (Kempton et al.,

1999; McLean et al., 2004), and meta-analysis

(Chamberlain et al., 2011), have shown

extensive impairments in spatial working

memory in ADHD. These are evident both in

terms of between-search errors and strategy

scores.

Methylphenidate significantly improves

performance on SWM in children and adults with

ADHD as well as in healthy volunteers (Kempton

et al., 1999; Mehta et al., 2004, 2000; Turner et

al., 2005).

Off

On

Page 38: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

36 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

References • Chamberlain, S. R., Robbins, T. W., Winder-

Rhodes, S., Müller, U., Sahakian, B. J.,

Blackwell, A. D., & Barnett, J. H. (2011).

Translational approaches to frontostriatal

dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder using a computerized

neuropsychological battery. Biological

psychiatry, 69(12), 1192-1203.

• Cools, R., Stefanova, E., Barker, R. A.,

Robbins, T. W., & Owen, A. M. (2002).

Dopaminergic modulation of high‐level

cognition in Parkinson’s disease: the role of

the prefrontal cortex revealed by

PET. Brain, 125(3), 584-594.

• Elliott, R., McKenna, P. J., Robbins, T. W., &

Sahakian, B. I. (1998). Specific

neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenic

patients with preserved intellectual function.

Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 3(1), 45-69.

• Foong, J., Rozewicz, L., Chong, W. K.,

Thompson, A. J., Miller, D. H., & Ron, M. A.

(2000). A comparison of neuropsychological

deficits in primary and secondary progressive

multiple sclerosis. Journal of

neurology, 247(2), 97-101.

• Foong, J., Rozewicz, L., Quaghebeur, G.,

Davie, C. A., Kartsounis, L. D., Thompson, A.

J., Miller, D. H. & Ron, M. A. (1997). Executive

function in multiple sclerosis. The role of

frontal lobe pathology. Brain, 120(1), 15-26.

• Harmer C.J., McTavish S.F.B., Clark L.,

Goodwin G.M., Cowen P.J., (2001) Tyrosine

depletion attenuates dopamine function in

healthy volunteers, Psychopharmacology,

154, 105-111

• Hutton SB, Puri BK, Duncan L-J, Robbins TW,

Barnes TRE and Joyce EM (1998) Executive

function in first-episode schizophrenia.

Psychological Medicine, 28, 463-473.

• Kempton, S., Vance, A., Maruff, P., Luk, E.,

Costin, J., & Pantelis, C. (1999). Executive

function and attention deficit hyperactivity

disorder: stimulant medication and better

executive function performance in children.

Psychological medicine, 29(3), 527-538.

• Klekociuk, S. Z., & Summers, M. J. (2014a).

Exploring the validity of mild cognitive

impairment (MCI) subtypes: Multiple-domain

amnestic MCI is the only identifiable subtype

at longitudinal follow-up. Journal of Clinical

And Experimental Neuropsychology, 36(3),

290-301.

• Klekociuk, S. Z., & Summers, M. J. (2014b).

Lowered performance in working memory and

attentional sub‐processes are most prominent

in multi‐domain amnestic mild cognitive

impairment subtypes. Psychogeriatrics,

14(1), 63-71.

• Klekociuk, S. Z., & Summers, M. J. (2014c).

The learning profile of persistent mild

cognitive impairment (MCI): a potential

diagnostic marker of persistent amnestic MCI.

European Journal of Neurology, 21(3), 470-

e24.

• Lange, K. W., Robbins, T. W., Marsden, C. D.,

James, M., Owen, A. M., & Paul, G. M. (1992).

L-dopa withdrawal in Parkinson's disease

selectively impairs cognitive performance in

tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction.

Psychopharmacology, 107(2-3), 394-404.

• Manes, F., Sahakian, B., Clark, L., Rogers, R.,

Antoun, N., Aitken, M., & Robbins, T. (2002).

Decision‐making processes following damage

to the prefrontal cortex. Brain, 125(3), 624-

639.

• McAllister, T. W., Flashman, L. A., McDonald,

B. C., & Saykin, A. J. (2006). Mechanisms of

working memory dysfunction after mild and

moderate TBI: evidence from functional MRI

and neurogenetics. Journal of neurotrauma,

23(10), 1450-1467.

• McAllister, T. W., Sparling, M. B., Flashman,

L. A., Guerin, S. J., Mamourian, A. C., &

Saykin, A. J. (2001). Differential working

memory load effects after mild traumatic

brain injury. Neuroimage, 14(5), 1004-1012.

• McLean, A., Dowson, J., Toone, B., Young, S.,

Bazanis, E., Robbins, T. W., & Sahakian, B. J.

(2004). Characteristic neurocognitive profile

Page 39: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

37 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

associated with adult attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychological

medicine, 34(04), 681-692.

• Mehta, M. A., Goodyer, I. M., & Sahakian, B.

J. (2004). Methylphenidate improves working

memory and set‐shifting in AD/HD:

relationships to baseline memory

capacity. Journal of Child Psychology and

Psychiatry, 45(2), 293-305.

• Mehta, M. A., Owen, A. M., Sahakian, B. J.,

Mavaddat, N., Pickard, J. D., & Robbins, T. W.

(2000). Methylphenidate enhances working

memory by modulating discrete frontal and

parietal lobe regions in the human brain. J

Neurosci, 20(6), RC65.

• Mehta, M. A., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T.

W. (2001). Comparative psychopharmacology

of methylphenidate and related drugs in

human volunteers, patients with ADHD, and

experimental animals. Stimulant drugs and

ADHD: Basic and clinical neuroscience, 303-

331.

• Owen, A. M., Downes, J. J., Sahakian, B. J.,

Polkey, C. E., & Robbins, T. W. (1990).

Planning and spatial working memory

following frontal lobe lesions in man.

Neuropsychologia, 28(10), 1021-1034.

• Owen, A. M., Iddon, J. L., Hodges, J. R.,

Summers, B. A., & Robbins, T. W. (1997).

Spatial and non-spatial working memory at

different stages of Parkinson's

disease. Neuropsychologia, 35(4), 519-532.

• Owen, A. M., James, M., Leigh, P. N.,

Summers, B. A., Marsden, C. D., Quinn, N. P.,

Lange, K. W. & Robbins, T. W. (1992). Fronto-

striatal cognitive deficits at different stages of

Parkinson's disease. Brain, 115(6), 1727-

1751.

• Owen, A. M., Sahakian, B. J., Semple, J.,

Polkey, C. E. and Robbins, T. W. (1995) Visuo-

spatial short-term recognition memory and

learning after temporal lobe excision, frontal

lobe excision or amygdalo-hippocampectomy

in man. Neuropsychologia, 33, 1-24.

• Pantelis, C., Barnes, T.R.E., Nelson, H.E.,

Tanner, S., Weatherly, L., Owen, A.M. and

Robbins, T.W. (1997). Frontal-striatal

cognitive deficits in patients with chronic

schizophrenia. Brain, 120, 1823-1843.

• Rahman, S., Sahakian, B. J., Hodges, J. R.,

Rogers, R. D., & Robbins, T. W. (1999).

Specific cognitive deficits in mild frontal

variant frontotemporal

dementia. Brain, 122(8), 1469-1493.

• Rock, P. L., Roiser, J. P., Riedel, W. J., &

Blackwell, A. D. (2013). Cognitive impairment

in depression: a systematic review and meta-

analysis. Psychological medicine, 1-12.

• Sahgal, A., Galloway, P. H., McKeith, I. G.,

Lloyd, S., Cook, J. H., Ferrier, I. N., &

Edwardson, J. A. (1992). Matching-to-sample

deficits in patients with senile dementias of

the Alzheimer and Lewy body types. Archives

of neurology, 49(10), 1043-1046.

• Sahgal, A., McKeith, I. G., Galloway, P. H.,

Tasker, N., & Steckler, T. (1995). Do

differences in visuospatial ability between

senile dementias of the Alzheimer and Lewy

body types reflect differences solely in

mnemonic function?. Journal of clinical and

experimental neuropsychology, 17(1), 35-43.

• Saunders, N. L., & Summers, M. J. (2011).

Longitudinal deficits to attention, executive,

and working memory in subtypes of mild

cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology,

25(2), 237.

• Sterr, A., Herron, K. A., Hayward, C., &

Montaldi, D. (2006). Are mild head injuries as

mild as we think? Neurobehavioral

concomitants of chronic post-concussion

syndrome. BMC neurology, 6(1), 7.

• Turner, D. C., Blackwell, A. D., Dowson, J. H.,

McLean, A., & Sahakian, B. J. (2005).

Neurocognitive effects of methylphenidate in

adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder. Psychopharmacology, 178(2-3),

286-295.

• Wood, S.J., Pantelis, C., Proffitt, T., Phillips.

L.J., Stuart, G.W., Buchanan, J.-A, Mahony,

K., Brewer, W., Smith, D.J. and McGorry, P.D.

(2003). Spatial working memory ability is a

marker of risk-for-psychosis. Psychological

Medicine, 33, 1239-1247.

Page 40: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

38 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

Cognitive testing – test recommendation by disease

Page 41: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

39 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

Cognitive testing – test recommendation by disease

40 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

Page 42: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

40 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

Cognitive testing: quality consideration

Characterising cognition depends on the

reliability of the measures used. Reliability of

cognitive testing is increased by reducing

sources of error or bias in measurement. There

are several potential sources of error which

computerised cognitive testing address:

• Reducing errors of measurement. In

standardised pencil and paper tests, reliability

depends on highly skilled administrators to

accurately record responses, measure

reaction times and then code, record and

score the responses. Errors at each stage of

this process can impact reliability.

Computerised assessment interfaces directly

with the participant and automates these

processes, reducing potential errors.

• Lengthier tasks can increase reliability by

administering more trials. However, in

populations such as children, and those with

cognitive impairment, these additional trials

increase testing time and place additional

burdens on sustained attention. This may

paradoxically reduce reliability when

participants’ attention wanes or they tire.

Computerised assessment provides short

forms of tasks allowing appropriate testing

time for the study population.

• The ability to implement tasks in which the

condition or stimuli presented depends on

participants’ previous responses is a further

advantage, again reducing testing time, and

maintaining participant engagement.

• In populations such as Parkinson’s disease or

normal ageing, difficulties with motor control

can also introduce measurement error and

reduce participant’s ability to comply with

testing. The use of intuitive and well-designed

interfaces can facilitate this. Research has

shown that computerized cognitive testing is

acceptable to elderly participants, often being

better tolerated than pencil-and paper

assessments (Fillitt et al., 2008; Collerton et

al., 2008)

In addition to the reliability of measurement,

another key requirement of cognitive tests is

their sensitivity to impairment and to change

over time. Computerised cognitive testing can

optimise this in a number of ways:

• Floor or celling effects limit the ability to

detect change or differences in scores,

particularly in very able or impaired samples.

Adaptive algorithms, which alter presentation

of stimuli dependent on previous

performance, can prevent this.

• The provision of parallel forms and the

presentation of random stimuli to enable tasks

to be repeated over time can increase

sensitivity to longitudinal change by

minimising practice effects (Semple & Link,

1991; Louis et al., 1999).

Extensive published data from intervention

studies allows study-specific estimates of effect

size to be generated, which will ensure studies

are adequately powered to detect differences.

References • Collerton J, Collerton D, Arai Y, Barrass K,

Eccles M, Jagger C, McKeith I, Saxby BK,

Kirkwood T; Newcastle 85+ Study Core Team

(2007). A comparison of computerized and

pencil-and-paper tasks in assessing cognitive

function in community-dwelling older people

in the Newcastle 85+ Pilot Study. J Am Geriatr

Soc.; 55(10):1630-5.

• Fillitt HM, Simon ES, Doniger GM, Cummings

JL. Practicality of a computerized system for

cognitive assessment in the elderly.

Alzheimer's and Dementia. 2008;4:14–21

• Louis, W. J., Mander, A. G., Dawson, M.,

O'Callaghan, C., & Conway, E. L. (1999). Use

of computerized neuropsychological tests

(CANTAB) to assess cognitive effects of

antihypertensive drugs in the elderly. Journal

of hypertension, 17(12), 1813-1819.

• Semple, J., & Link, C. G. G. (1991). CANTAB

Parallel Battery: a suitable tool for

investigating drug effects on cognition. In Soc

Neurosci Abstr (Vol. 273, No. 2).

Page 43: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

41 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

Study Practicalities Does it matter if my subject is

colour blind?

Most of the tests can be administered if the

subject is colour blind. For example, PAL has

unique patterns that are independent of colour,

so scores on these tasks should not be affected.

In the DMS, however, subjects need to be able

to differentiate colours. However, the results

would only be affected in severe cases of colour

blindness, which are quite rare.

We would suggest doing a very quick screen for

colour blindness with a subject on the first

testing session using the Ishihara Colour Test.

Alternatively, the practice stages on each task

can be used to ensure the subjects can

differentiate the colours used.

Can I use CANTAB with people with limited cognitive functioning?

This depends on the level of impairment. The

advantage of CANTAB is that the majority of the

tasks are language and culture independent,

and the tasks have an intuitive game-like quality

ideal for testing in populations with limited

mental capabilities. The CANTAB tasks have

been used in individuals from specific disease

populations that are characterised by very

limited cognitive functioning, for example,

Alzheimer’s disease. The normative database

includes children as young as four and adults

over the age of 90.

Can I use CANTAB with people with physical disabilities?

This depends on the type and level of physical

disability. To determine whether a subject can

complete CANTAB, we recommend the

completion of the MOT task for screening

purposes. This task simply involves the subject

touching crosses that appear on the touch

screen. From performance on this task, we can

assess any problems with vision, motor control

or comprehension that may result in the subject

being unable to complete the remaining CANTAB

tests.

What if the subject refuses to finish the test?

The majority of CANTAB tests are graded in

difficulty and designed to terminate prematurely

when a subject reaches their cognitive limit. By

setting the subjects expectations and providing

reassurance and encouragement, a subject

should always be able to complete the tests.

Some examples of positive encouragement that

can be used to motivate a subject to continue

are as follows:

“Well done, this test is very hard, you are doing

well.”

“Not long to go; you have nearly finished this

test.”

If the subject refuses to complete the test,

despite encouragement, then please use the

abort function.

If a test is aborted, we would recommend you

record a comment at the end of the testing

session. The comment should explain the

situation, mention that the subject refused to

continue, any specific reasons why and which

particular tests were not attempted or aborted.

This is very useful when reviewing data.

Cambridge Cognition does not recommend that

aborted data is included for analysis, and our

normal internal procedure is to remove these

test runs.

What if I need to rerun the test?

If you needed to abort a test during a testing

session, e.g. because of a fire alarm, then you

should rerun the test as soon as possible.

Page 44: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

42 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

Can data from aborted tests be

used?

We would recommend that data from aborted

CANTAB test runs is excluded from the final

dataset and not include in the analysis.

The psychometric validity of data could be

significantly altered by aborting a test. To

provide consistency across sites and raters it is

extremely important that the CANTAB tests are

run in full. The majority of the tests are graded

in difficulty and designed to terminate

prematurely when a subject reaches their

cognitive limit, therefore there should be no

need to abort manually. For example, in the PAL

task, if the subject fails to recall all of the

locations correctly after a set number of

attempts the test will terminate. By following the

script, setting the subjects expectations and

providing reassurance and encouragement, it

should not be necessary to abort a test.

If an individual test is aborted in a test battery,

then all data from fully completed tests in the

session can still be included in the analysis.

Can I allow breaks during CANTAB sessions?

We would recommend you allow breaks during

the CANTAB sessions, depending on the length

of the testing period, providing that the breaks

are between tests and not during a test.

It is best practice to keep the breaks consistent

so that they occur at the same point in the

testing session for all subjects. However, this is

not always practical and additional breaks may

be given as and when required.

Can I allow food, drinks, and

cigarettes during CANTAB testing?

CANTAB tests are very sensitive, and differences

in CANTAB task performance have been

demonstrated from everyday substances, such

as the amount of caffeine contained in one cup

of tea. Cigarettes and certain foods have also

been demonstrated to influence performance.

Therefore we would advise that food, drinks and

cigarettes are not allowed during the CANTAB

testing period. Water may be given if needed.

Page 45: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

43 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

Data analysis considerations

The analysis of cognitive variables raises a

variety of statistical issues, and the aim of this

section is to address some of these issues and

provide guidance when writing a statistical

analysis plan prior to the study. Naturally, it is

not expected that the recommendations made

below will be appropriate in all cases, and it is

strongly recommended that you seek specialist

statistical advice.

However, it is hoped that by following the

recommendations contained within this

document, statistical analyses performed on

CANTAB data will be both readily interpretable

and performed according to valid statistical

principles.

Checklist

This checklist gives a number of important

things that you should consider at the study

design stage:

• Have you considered sample size and

statistical power for the study?

• If you are planning to use composite

measures in the study, have you discussed

their construction with a statistician?

• Which factors do you plan to use as

covariates?

• Are you planning one-tailed or two-tailed

tests?

• How will you assess whether the assumptions

of parametric analysis have been met and

what are the alternative tests to be used

should these assumptions not be met?

• Have you discussed the procedures for dealing

with missing data with a statistician?

Power Calculations

The specific power calculation you need will

depend on whether you have a between-

subjects design (e.g. difference between two

independent means) or within-subjects design

(difference between two dependent means e.g.

from matched pairs).

We recommend that you power using two-

tailed testing with an alpha of 0.05 (probability

of false alarm) and a power of 0.8.

To help, below is a table of power calculations,

which is for between-subjects design. By

Cohen’s definition, an effect size of 0.2 is

considered small, an effect size of 0.5 is

considered medium and an effect size of 0.8 is

considered large.

Table 1: Power calculation table

Tails Power Alpha Effect size N1 N2

Two 0.80 0.05 0.2 394 394

Two 0.80 0.05 0.5 64 64

Two 0.80 0.05 0.8 26 26

Two 0.80 0.1 0.2 310 310

Two 0.80 0.1 0.5 51 51

Two 0.80 0.1 0.8 21 21

One 0.80 0.05 0.2 310 310

One 0.80 0.05 0.5 51 51

One 0.80 0.05 0.8 21 21

One 0.80 0.1 0.2 226 226

One 0.80 0.1 0.5 37 37

One 0.80 0.1 0.8 15 15

Data preparation

Before commencement of statistical analysis, it

is always recommended that the data are

inspected and, if necessary, transformed such

that appropriate statistical tests are employed

for a given cognitive measure. Note that it is

likely that different transformations will be

necessary for different measures, and in general

the procedures below ought to be performed on

a variable-by-variable basis.

Non-normality, skew and kurtosis

As discussed below, it is usually preferable to

employ parametric statistics for the analysis of

cognitive measures, since they provide a

number of advantages over non-parametric

statistics. Analysing raw data (e.g., percent

correct, reaction time) provides the most readily

interpretable results from parametric analysis.

Page 46: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

44 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

However, for many cognitive outcome variables

the raw data will be skewed, and therefore not

suitable for analysis with parametric tests,

which, assuming a relatively low number of data

points, only produce accurate results if data are

normally distributed. This is particularly

common for reaction time measures, where a

few slow subjects may lengthen the right hand

tail of the distribution (i.e. positive skew), or

accuracy measures on easy stages of tests

where most subjects perform very well but a few

inaccurate scores may lengthen the left hand tail

of the distribution (i.e. negative skew).

The preferred method to detect skew is to plot

the residuals from the statistical analysis, for

example using a box and-whisker plot, and the

statistician should use their judgment and

experience in this matter. However, since

skewed raw data will generally result in skewed

residuals, it may be possible to detect skew in

the raw data. Kurtosis is a measure of whether

the data are peaked or flat relative to a normal

distribution. Data sets with high kurtosis

(leptokurtic) tend to have a distinct peak near

the mean, decline rather rapidly, and have

heavy tails. Data sets with low kurtosis

(platykurtic) tend to have a flat top near the

mean rather than a sharp peak. A normal

distribution is said to be mesokurtic.

If significant skew or kurtosis is detected, a

transformation may render the data suitable for

parametric analysis. However, it is vital to

choose an appropriate transform for a given

outcome measure, since inappropriately

transforming data might either reduce

sensitivity or increase the chance of spurious

results.

The transformation is dependent on the nature

of the skew, and detailed advice should be

sought. As a general rule, for positively skewed

data a log transformation is recommended,

while a square transform may be able to correct

for negative skew. However, it should be noted

that transformation will change the nature of the

treatment estimates produced from parametric

analysis. For example, if a log transform has

been employed, the treatment estimates will

represent the ratio of one group’s score to that

of another group, and not differences in the

original cognitive measure.

Following transformation, the residuals from

statistical analysis and/or raw data should be

inspected to confirm that the skew has been

removed successfully. In some cases it may not

be possible to produce a normal distribution by

transformation, and non-parametric analyses

will be necessary (see below).

If using a regression to analyse data, it is the

normality of the residuals (in relation to

predictors) that is required, rather than

normality of the raw scores. In these cases, and

assuming an adequate sample size, it may be

that removing extreme outliers (i.e.

standardised residuals > +3 / -3) enables this

assumption to be met.

Including covariates in an analysis

In general, the covariates to be included in the

general linear model should be detailed in the

analysis plan prior to statistical analysis. It is

recommended that variables such as test site

and cognitive battery order are entered as

random-effects to conserve degrees of freedom

and improve sensitivity, while demographic

variables such as age, IQ and gender should be

entered as fixed-effects. In a repeated measures

design, if baseline scores are available for each

cognitive outcome variable of interest, baseline

score can also be entered as a fixed-effect.

Many studies, for example in pharmaceutical

trials, employ longitudinal designs, where

subjects are tested at repeated time-points

following the commencement of drug treatment.

In such cases, it is strongly recommended that

a repeated measures mixed-model is used. This

is the most sensitive method to determine

whether a drug starts to have a beneficial effect

at a particular time-point following treatment,

and can also be helpful in dealing with missing

data. Where appropriate, an autoregressive

function should be included to account for

correlations between measures taken at

successive time-points. It may also be useful to

include a drug treatment x time interaction term

Page 47: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

45 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

in the model. However, the exact nature of the

longitudinal model to be employed should be

discussed with a statistician. In some cases,

absolute differences between treatment groups

may provide the most meaningful interpretation

of the data. In other cases, change-from-

baseline scores may be most informative. In the

case where change-from-baseline score is

preferred, it is still recommended to include

baseline score as a covariate in the analysis to

provide maximum sensitivity. This may seem

counter-intuitive, since using a change-from-

baseline measure effectively already includes

the baseline term in the model. However, on

many cognitive outcome measures, it is possible

that both the change-from-baseline score and

the post-treatment score may covary with the

baseline score due to boundary effects. In such

cases, including the baseline score will improve

sensitivity by accounting for extra error variance

in the analysis.

Composite measures

A number of approaches have been taken with

respect to the generation of composite

measures. In general, it is advisable to discuss

the construction of composite measures prior to

the commencement of the study. Composite

measures can be useful, since they may provide

greater sensitivity than individual test results by

providing a more precise estimate of an

underlying cognitive process. Furthermore,

composite measures can reduce the number of

primary end-points in a trial, thus at least

partially circumventing the issue of multiple

comparisons. However, in some cases, a

particularly focused cognitive measure will prove

a more sensitive index than composite

measures.

If it is intended that composite measures are

employed, it is vital that the method of

calculating the composite measure is considered

prior to data collection. Furthermore, it is

strongly recommended that only those

composite measures based on published factor

analyses that have previously been validated

should be employed. If composite measures are

calculated without using reference to

established methods, the results may be difficult

to interpret, since a change in the composite

measure cannot be compared to other published

studies. This makes the magnitude of a given

change in a composite measure considerably

more difficult to interpret with respect to

previous literature. Factor analyses of CANTAB

scores in large datasets have been published

(e.g., Robbins et al., 1994; see also Harrison et

al., 2007 for non- CANTAB tests), and should

can be examined for guidance and to guide the

analysis strategy.

Test-retest reliability

Technically, reliability measurement is an

attempt to estimate the measurement error

attached to the use of a test or instrument.

When applied to psychological tests, reliability

most often relates to either the internal

consistency (do items in the test measure the

‘same thing’) or to temporal consistency. The

latter form is referred to as ‘Test-Retest

Reliability’ and is most often reported as the

degree of correlation between first test and

retest performance.

A number of studies have published test-retest

reliabilities in healthy volunteers and patients.

• Lowe and Rabbitt (1998) assessed 4-week

test-retest reliability from 162 healthy

volunteers:

• Delayed matching to sample (DMS) number

correct r=0.56

• Paired associates learning (PAL) first trial

memory score r=0.68, average trials to

success r=0.86

• Spatial working memory (SWM) total errors

r=0.68

Leeson and colleagues (2009) assessed test-

retest reliability over 1 and 2 years within 25

healthy volunteers and 104 patients with

schizophrenia:

• Spatial working memory (SWM) between

errors: healthy volunteer 56-week r=0.52,

66-week r=0.65; schizophrenia 74-week

r=0.62, 100-week r=0.62; strategy: healthy

volunteer 56-week r=0.54, 66-week r=0.60;

Page 48: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

46 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

schizophrenia 74-week r=0.40, 100-week

r=0.57

• Fowler and colleagues (1995) assessed 1-

month test-retest reliability in healthy

volunteers, patients with questionable

dementia and patients with Alzheimer’s

disease:

• Delayed matching to sample (DMS) number

correct (12-second delay): healthy volunteers

r=0.52, questionable dementia r=0.57,

Alzheimer’s disease r=0.84

• Paired associates learning (PAL) total errors:

healthy volunteers r=0.64, questionable

dementia r=0.71, Alzheimer’s disease r=0.88

Barnett and colleagues’ (2010) review outlines

3-month test-retest reliability in patients with

schizophrenia:

• Spatial working memory (SWM) between

errors 12-week r=0.65, 14-week r=0.83;

strategy 12-week r=0.75, 14-week r=0.75

Furthermore, Harrison and colleagues’

unpublished manuscript assessed test-retest

reliability over 1-8 weeks in 100 healthy

volunteers:

• Delayed matching to sample (DMS) number

correct r=0.50

• Paired associates learning (PAL) stages

completed r=0.87, total errors r=0.68

• Reaction time (RTI) simple RT r=0.54, 5-

choice RT r=0.57, movement time r=0.73

• Rapid visual information processing (RVP)

total hits r=0.64, latency r=0.64

• Spatial working memory (SWM) between

errors r=0.70, strategy r=0.63

Change in individual’s

performance - measurement

error or ‘real effect’?

One consequence of using a correlation

coefficient as a reliability measure is that a

uniform change in performance across a group

will yield a high correlation coefficient, even

though subjects may have improved or declined

by the same rate. With psychometric measures

there appears to be a high a priori expectation

that performance will necessarily improve with

repeated administrations of the test.

Improvements in performance as a result of

repeated exposure may be due to the adoption

of a strategy. In circumstances such as these,

changes in performance can be viewed as due to

a real effect, rather than simply due to

measurement error.

The foregoing discussion raises a number of

issues for examining the Test-retest reliability

(TRR) of the CANTAB system outcome

measures. The first challenge is to calculate TRR

correlation coefficients for each of the outcome

measures and then to determine whether retest

performance improves, deteriorates, or remains

the same when compared to first test scores.

Changes in group performance can be

determined by testing for the presence of a

significant change between test and retest.

However, most often one is seeking to

determine whether an individual’s change in

performance is due to a real effect or to

measurement error.

When assessing the performance of an

individual at retest, it is useful to know whether

any change associated with retest performance

is due to a real effect (e.g. due to physical

degeneration [a worsening of performance] or

due to practice or recovery [an improvement]).

The standard errors of prediction reported in

Table 2 can be used to determine this as they

can be used to calculate a confidence interval for

retest performance. If the retest performance

score falls within the upper and lower bounds of

this confidence interval, then retest change can

be judged to be due to measurement error.

Scores beyond the confidence interval bounds

are likely to be due to the influence of a real

effect. This decision is subject to the usual twin

statistical hazards of making type 1 and type 2

errors. When deciding whether retest change is

due to measurement error or a real effect, the

decision is biased toward rejecting the

hypothesis that the change is due to

measurement error. Consequently, we would

suggest that only scores falling within 66%

confidence intervals be interpreted as being

likely to be due to measurement error.

Page 49: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

47 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com ©Cambridge Cognition

A step-by-step procedure for determining

whether an individual’s retest change is due to

error or effect is described below.

Step 1

The vast majority of statistical techniques

assume that observations are independent.

Measures obtained from the same subject, even

on vastly different occasions, are clearly not

independent. For this reason, techniques have

been devised to allow for the calculation of ‘true

scores’ for which an allowance has been made

for measurement error and the score actually

obtained for a subject. Brophy (1986) describes

the following equation:

T = M + r(X-M)

where X is the obtained score, M is the mean of

the scores on the test and r is the reliability

coefficient of the test.

Step 2

Calculate the standard error of the ‘true’ score

obtained from step 1, using the appropriate SEP

from table 2 to plot the appropriate confidence

interval.

Step 3

Determine whether the retest scores falls within

or beyond the confidence interval calculated in

step 2 - scores falling within the CI can be

judged to be due to change consistent with

measurement error. Scores beyond the CI limits

are likely to be due to a real effect.

References • Barnett, J.H., Robbins, T.W., Leeson, V.C.,

Sahakian, B.J., Joyce, E.M., Blackwell, A.D.

(2010) Assessing cognitive function in clinical

trials of schizophrenia. Neuroscience and

biobehavioral reviews 34: 1161–77.

• Brophy, A.L. (1986) Confidence Intervals for

True Scores and Retest Scores on Clinical

Tests. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42(6),

989-991.

• Fowler, K.S., Saling, M.M., Conway, E.L.,

Semple, J.M., Louis, W.J. (1995).

Computerized delayed matching to sample

and paired associate performance in the early

detection of dementia. Applied

Neuropsychology 2: 72–78

• Harrison, J.E., Iddon, J.L., Stow, I.A.,

Blackwell, A.D., Jefferies, M.C., Shah, P.P.,

(2007). Cambridge Neuropsychological Test

Automated Battery (CANTAB): Test-Retest

Reliability Characteristics. Unpublished.

• Leeson, V. C., Robbins, T. W., Matheson, E.,

Hutton, S. B., Ron, M. A., Barnes, T. R., &

Joyce, E. M. (2009). Discrimination learning,

reversal, and set-shifting in first-episode

schizophrenia: stability over six years and

specific associations with medication type and

disorganization syndrome. Biological

psychiatry, 66(6), 586-593.

• Lowe C, Rabbitt P (1998) Test/re-test

reliability of the CANTAB and ISPOCD

neuropsychological batteries: theoretical and

practical issues. Cambridge

Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery.

International Study of Post-Operative

Cognitive Dysfunction. Neuropsychologia 36:

915–23

• Robbins, T.W., James, T., Owen, A.M.,

Sahakian, B.J., McInnes, L. & Rabbitt, P.M.

(1994) CANTAB: A Factor Analytic Study of a

Large Sample of Normal Elderly Volunteers.

Dementia, 5, 266-281.

Page 50: Cambridge Cognition 3 sector A4 mastheads · a succinct review of the existing literature relating to each CANTAB test included. Evidence is also integrated from pharmacological studies,

48 The CANTAB Cognition Handbook ©Cambridge Cognition

© Cambridge Cognition Limited 2018. All rights reserved.

www.cantab.com

UK office

Cambridge Cognition

Tunbridge Court

Bottisham

Cambridge

CB25 9TU United Kingdom

Cambridge Cognition

[email protected]

+44 (0)1223 810 700