Appraisals, Coping, and Stress in Breast Cancer Screening ...Resume Le modele devaluation cognitive...

14
Appraisals, Coping, and Stress in Breast Cancer Screening: A Longitudinal Investigation of Causal Structure LISA SWEET, Royal Ottawa Health Care Group JO ANNE SAVOIE and LOUISE LEMYRE, University of Ottawa Abstract The cognitive-appraisal model of Lazarus and Folkman (1984) is one of the most prominent theories of stress. As integral elements in this model, Appraisal and Coping have both been proposed as mediators of the stress re- sponse. The purpose of this study was to test the predic- tive power of the theoretical model based on two compet- ing formulations using structural equation modeling techniques. One conceptual model proposed that coping influences stress through appraisal; the other proposed that appraisals impact on stress via coping. Women (« = 506) undergoing breast cancer screening completed mea- sures of appraisal, coping, and stress. Support was found for both hypothesized causal structures. The implications of these findings are discussed in terms of Lazarus' theo- retical perspective. Resume Le modele devaluation cognitive de Lazarus et Folkman (1984) est une des principales theories sur le stress. Deux de ses elements propres, 1'evaluation et 1'adaptation, ont etc proposes comme mediateurs de la reaction au stress. La presente etude visait a tester 1'efficacite predictive du modele theorique d'apres deux formulations differentes, au moyen de techniques de modelisation par equation structurelle. Un modele conceptuel proposait que 1'adaptation influe sur le stress au moyen de 1'evaluation; 1'autre proposait que les evaluations influent sur le stress par 1'entremise de 1'adaptation. Les femmes (n =506) subissant un test de depistage du cancer du sein ont re- pondu a des questions visant a mesurer 1'evaluation, 1'adaptation et le stress. Les resultats corroborent les deux structures causales hypothetiques. Les consequences de ces conclusions sont exposees dans le cadre de la perspec- tive theorique de Lazarus. As a core element in many theoretical models, the topic of stress remains the target of much academic debate. One area of particular interest involves the relationship between stress and health. Despite evidence to support the relationship, many questions remain unanswered as to the correlates and predictors of stress (Aldwin, 1994; Folkman, Lazarus, Gruen, &DeLongis, 1986), and among them, the sequential pattern of influences. One of the most popular theories in the area of stress research, certainly in terms of citation, is the Cognitive Theory of Stress and Coping developed by Lazarus and Folkman (1984). For Lazarus and Folkman (1984), the interaction between the environment and the individual defines stress. Stress is experienced when demands from the environment exceed available resources, and the mediating processes of appraisal and coping therefore become crucial. Appraisal is also presented as a perpetual process, evolving in time as the individual re-appraises the stressor. Following a brief overview of the studies investigating the principle components of the cognitive- appraisal framework, two competing models are pre- sented and tested. The cognitive-appraisal model Lazarus and Folkman (1984) argue that cognitive apprais- als and coping are pivotal in our understanding of stress. More specifically, they present both these processes as mediators of the stress response (Folkman, 1984; Lazarus, 1993). Within this framework, cognitive appraisal is the "process of categorizing an encounter, and its various facets, with respect to its significance for well-being" (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984, p.31). According to Lazarus and Folkman (1984), appraisals are either primary, when the person evaluates the implications of the stressor, or secondary, when the evaluation entails what can be done to deal with the situation. Coping as a process involves some form of thought, action or feeling that is used, modified or eliminated to deal with an event that elicits some form of psychological stress (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). The relationship between coping and adaptation Attempts to develop a parsimonious taxonomy of coping Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 1999,31:4,240-253

Transcript of Appraisals, Coping, and Stress in Breast Cancer Screening ...Resume Le modele devaluation cognitive...

Page 1: Appraisals, Coping, and Stress in Breast Cancer Screening ...Resume Le modele devaluation cognitive de Lazarus et Folkman (1984) est une des principales theories sur le stress. Deux

Appraisals, Coping, and Stress in Breast Cancer Screening:A Longitudinal Investigation of Causal Structure

LISA SWEET, Royal Ottawa Health Care GroupJO ANNE SAVOIE and LOUISE LEMYRE, University of Ottawa

AbstractThe cognitive-appraisal model of Lazarus and Folkman(1984) is one of the most prominent theories of stress. Asintegral elements in this model, Appraisal and Copinghave both been proposed as mediators of the stress re-sponse. The purpose of this study was to test the predic-tive power of the theoretical model based on two compet-ing formulations using structural equation modelingtechniques. One conceptual model proposed that copinginfluences stress through appraisal; the other proposedthat appraisals impact on stress via coping. Women (« =506) undergoing breast cancer screening completed mea-sures of appraisal, coping, and stress. Support was foundfor both hypothesized causal structures. The implicationsof these findings are discussed in terms of Lazarus' theo-retical perspective.

ResumeLe modele devaluation cognitive de Lazarus et Folkman(1984) est une des principales theories sur le stress. Deuxde ses elements propres, 1'evaluation et 1'adaptation, ontetc proposes comme mediateurs de la reaction au stress.La presente etude visait a tester 1'efficacite predictive dumodele theorique d'apres deux formulations differentes,au moyen de techniques de modelisation par equationstructurelle. Un modele conceptuel proposait que1'adaptation influe sur le stress au moyen de 1'evaluation;1'autre proposait que les evaluations influent sur le stresspar 1'entremise de 1'adaptation. Les femmes (n = 506)subissant un test de depistage du cancer du sein ont re-pondu a des questions visant a mesurer 1'evaluation,1'adaptation et le stress. Les resultats corroborent les deuxstructures causales hypothetiques. Les consequences deces conclusions sont exposees dans le cadre de la perspec-tive theorique de Lazarus.

As a core element in many theoretical models, the topicof stress remains the target of much academic debate.One area of particular interest involves the relationship

between stress and health. Despite evidence to supportthe relationship, many questions remain unanswered asto the correlates and predictors of stress (Aldwin, 1994;Folkman, Lazarus, Gruen, & DeLongis, 1986), and amongthem, the sequential pattern of influences.

One of the most popular theories in the area of stressresearch, certainly in terms of citation, is the CognitiveTheory of Stress and Coping developed by Lazarus andFolkman (1984). For Lazarus and Folkman (1984), theinteraction between the environment and the individualdefines stress. Stress is experienced when demands fromthe environment exceed available resources, and themediating processes of appraisal and coping thereforebecome crucial. Appraisal is also presented as a perpetualprocess, evolving in time as the individual re-appraisesthe stressor. Following a brief overview of the studiesinvestigating the principle components of the cognitive-appraisal framework, two competing models are pre-sented and tested.

The cognitive-appraisal modelLazarus and Folkman (1984) argue that cognitive apprais-als and coping are pivotal in our understanding of stress.More specifically, they present both these processes asmediators of the stress response (Folkman, 1984; Lazarus,1993). Within this framework, cognitive appraisal is the"process of categorizing an encounter, and its variousfacets, with respect to its significance for well-being"(Lazarus & Folkman, 1984, p.31). According to Lazarusand Folkman (1984), appraisals are either primary, whenthe person evaluates the implications of the stressor, orsecondary, when the evaluation entails what can be doneto deal with the situation. Coping as a process involvessome form of thought, action or feeling that is used,modified or eliminated to deal with an event that elicitssome form of psychological stress (Lazarus & Folkman,1984).

The relationship between coping and adaptationAttempts to develop a parsimonious taxonomy of coping

Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 1999,31:4,240-253

Page 2: Appraisals, Coping, and Stress in Breast Cancer Screening ...Resume Le modele devaluation cognitive de Lazarus et Folkman (1984) est une des principales theories sur le stress. Deux

Cognitive-appraisal Model of Stress 241

strategies have led to several classifications of thesebehaviours, thoughts and feelings. The most commontaxonomies include dichotomous classifications such asemotion-focused and problem-focused (Folkman &Lazarus, 1980), engagement or disengagement (Tobin,Holroyd, Reynolds, & Wigal, 1989), and avoidance andapproach coping (Suls & Fletcher, 1985). Similarly, Endlerand Parker (1990, 1994) have identified three styles ofcoping: task oriented, emotion oriented, and avoidance-oriented.

Authors have used different labels to describe whatare conceptually analogous types of coping behaviour.For instance, problem-focused, task-oriented, engage-ment and approach coping categories represent strategiessuch as problem solving and seeking social support,which are characterized as active and usually inferred asadaptive ways of dealing with a stressor. On the otherhand, emotion-focused, emotion-oriented, disengage-ment and avoidance coping strategies (e.g., denial ordistancing) draw attention away from the stressor. Itseems as though coping efforts that draw attention awayfrom the stressor can be considered avoidant-type effortswhereas those that channel efforts toward the stressor canbe regarded as approach-type strategies (Suls & Fletcher,1985).

Many investigators have studied the efficacy of copingin enhancing or preserving well-being. Despite agree-ment that specific coping can influence one's level ofwell-being, the strategies responsible for these fluctua-tions have varied from study to study. Findings suggestthat avoidant-type coping strategies (e.g., denial andwishful thinking) are positively correlated with levels ofpsychological and physical symptoms (Commerford,Gular, Orr, Reznikof, & O'Dowd, 1994; Dunkel-Schetter,Feinstein, Taylor, & Falke, 1992; Gass & Chang, 1989;Kenlder, Kessler, Heath, Neale, & Eaves, 1991). On theother hand, approach-type coping strategies have oftenbeen associated with decreased levels of symptoms(Dunkel-Schetter et al., 1992; Folkman & Lazarus, 1985).As such, keeping avoidance and approach strategies asdistinct forms of coping may help to examine the role ofdifferent strategies in psychological stress.

The relationship between appraisals and adaptationThere also exists considerable empirical evidence sup-porting the relationship between appraisals and adapta-tion. Researchers have studied the relationship betweena vast array of appraisals (e.g., including impact, impor-tance, negativity, threat, control or mastery or compe-tence, un/desirability, un/predictability, ambiguity, anduncertainty), and the person's physical or mental well-being (Folkman, Lazarus, Gruen, & DeLongis, 1986; Gall& Evans, 1987). On conceptual and empirical grounds,the apparently heterogeneous appraisals seem to reflect

three main cognitive dimensions: perceived impact,perceived mastery, and perceived uncertainty (Biron,Truchon, & Lemyre, 1992; Pillion, 1993; Karasawa, 1995;Lemyre, 1986; Lemyre & Tessier, 1988). Perceived impactrelates to the individual's evaluation that a stressoraffects his/her current life or future aspirations. Itincludes appraisals related to perceived threat, impor-tance, desirability, and negativity of the stressor. Per-ceived mastery, the notion that one can influence thecourse of the stressor, reflects the cognitive dimensions ofpersonal competence and control. Finally, perceiveduncertainty is defined as the person's perception of asituation as being ambiguous, unfamiliar or lacking ininformation. It includes cognitive appraisals such aspredictability, certainty, and familiarity (Lemyre, 1986).

Findings show that events perceived as stressfultypically comprise elements of impact and undesirability(Vinokur & Selzer, 1975). In contrast, thinking of an eventas an opportunity, instead of perceiving undesirableimpact, is associated with reduced emotional distress(Mishel & Sorenson, 1991). Overall, an enhanced sense ofpersonal control (mastery) is related to decreased stress(Felsten, 1991; Prince-Embury, 1992), as well as bettersomatic health (Folkman et al., 1986). In addition, eventsthat are perceived as uncertain are generally regarded asbeing quite stressful (Davis, 1990; Ham & Larson, 1990;Tomaka, Blascovich, Kelsey, & Leiten, 1993). This senseof uncertainty can lead to problems in adjustment(Christman, 1990). On a more global level, a situation thatis appraised negatively will be viewed as holding moreimpact and uncertainty with less opportunity for mas-tery.

The relationship between coping and appraisalsThe relationship between coping and appraisals has alsobeen empirically investigated. Because traditionalaccounts of the cognitive-appraisal model suggest thatappraisal of a situation precedes coping efforts, mostinvestigations have focused on the extent to whichcognitive appraisals are useful in the prediction of copingstrategies (e.g., Bjorck & Cohen, 1993; Davey, 1993;Valentiner, Holohan, & Moos, 1994). Studies haveconsistently shown that appraisals play an important rolein the moderation of various coping strategies (Carver &Scheier, 1994; Folkman & Lazarus, 1985; Stanton &Snider, 1993). For instance, coping strategies that involveapproaching the stressor (i.e., more active, problem-focused efforts) are often associated with appraisals ofperceived impact. When less mastery is perceived (i.e.,the situation is deemed less controllable),symptomatology may be increased if the person uses anapproach-type strategy (e.g., try to change the situation)(Forsyth & Compas, 1987). It has also been proposed thathigh degrees of uncertainty decrease the likelihood that

Page 3: Appraisals, Coping, and Stress in Breast Cancer Screening ...Resume Le modele devaluation cognitive de Lazarus et Folkman (1984) est une des principales theories sur le stress. Deux

242 Sweet, Savoie, and Lemyre

individuals will engage in direct action (i.e., they will relyless on approach-type strategies) (Mishel, 1981).

In Lazarus' more contemporary work on stress,however, coping is considered to be an antecedent toappraisal. That is, "...coping shapes emotion, as it doespsychological stress, by influencing the person-environ-ment relationship and how it is appraised" (Lazarus,1993, p.16). Even though debate remains about the detailsof the appraisal patterns and their link to emotions orother outcomes (Smith & Lazarus, 1993), the findingsreviewed above dearly indicate that coping and appraisalare somehow interrelated and that both are associatedwith psychological stress. For the most part, studies thatexamine the relationship between appraisal and copinghave tended to be correlational, making it difficult todisentangle the reciprocal effects of appraisal and coping.The direction of these relationships, therefore, remainsunclear.

There seem to be two distinct conceptual descriptionsof the relationships among coping, appraisal, and stress.The more traditional accounts suggest that copingmediates the effects of appraisals on stress. In contrast,more contemporary descriptions claim that appraisals arethe most proximate "cause" of emotions and that apprais-als mediate the effects of coping on stress. Most of theresearch done to date has been based on the traditionaldescription of the cognitive-appraisal model. That is,investigators have sought to understand how cognitiveappraisals can predict the choice of coping strategies,often neglecting the alternate formulation. As such, animportant initial step in disentangling these relationshipsis to contrast empirically both of these distinct formula-tions. Only then will it become possible to expand theconceptual model to include elements such as re-ap-praisal and the reciprocal relationship that this entails.Although a fully randomized experimental design is notreadily possible in a real-life setting, the use of a morepowerful statistical approach, such as Structural EquationModeling (SEM), could offer a solid empirical foundationon which to rest these theoretical models, especially ifbased on longitudinal data.

Accordingly, the objective of this study is to test therelations among the three principal variables of thecognitive-appraisal model, coping, appraisal, and stress,using SEM techniques. Although SEM cannot test formallycausal relationships, it can indicate support for causalmodels. The study was conducted in the context ofwomen undergoing breast cancer screening. Research hasshown a relationship between undergoing screening andincreased emotional distress (Wardle & Pope, 1992).However, there is minimal understanding of the mecha-nisms by which some individuals participating in breastscreening are distressed by the process while others arenot (Marteau, 1994). Therefore, it constitutes an ideal

Figure 1. Conceptual models of appraisal, coping, and stress: (A)Traditional model (upper diagram) and (B) Revised model (lowerdiagram).

setting for the study of appraisals, coping, and stress asone deals with a potentially severe health threat, an on-going process and a non-clinical sample.

Based on Lazarus and Folkman's (1984) theoreticalframework, two alternative conceptual models weretested. Our test of these models went beyond most workdone by others because our test used a longitudinaldesign. We examined appraisals, coping, and stressthroughout the screening (Time 1) and 20 days later(Time 2) once results were known. Thus, it is believedthat the results of this study can significantly enhance ourunderstanding of the relationships among appraisals,coping, and stress.

The first model (Model A), based on the more tradi-tional account of the cognitive-appraisal model, ispresented in Figure 1. As indicated by the paths in themodel, it is hypothesized that appraisals, measured interms of perceived impact, perceived mastery, andperceived uncertainty, will relate negatively onapproach-type coping, but positively on avoidance-typecoping. In other words, when the individual perceiveslittle mastery and evaluates the stressor as being uncer-tain and as having impact (a high level of threateningappraisal), she is more likely to engage in avoidant-typecoping and less likely to adopt approach-type copingstrategies. In turn, high use of approach-type coping ishypothesized to lead to low levels of psychological stress,whereas high levels of avoidance-type coping are ex-pected to result in elevated levels of psychological stress.

An alternative model (Model B), based on the morecontemporary revised formulation of Lazarus' theory,allows us to hypothesize that coping strategies

Page 4: Appraisals, Coping, and Stress in Breast Cancer Screening ...Resume Le modele devaluation cognitive de Lazarus et Folkman (1984) est une des principales theories sur le stress. Deux

Cognitive-appraisal Model of Stress 243

(approach-type and avoidance-type) will be directlyrelated to the appraisal of the stressor. Appraisals, inturn, will have an impact on the degree of stress reportedby the individual. Thus, as argued by Lazarus (1993), theeffect of coping on stress will be mediated by the person'sappraisal of the stressor, and no direct relation betweencoping efforts and stress is postulated. A diagram ofModel B is presented in Figure 1. It is hypothesized thatapproach-type coping is negatively associated withappraisal, whereas avoidance-type coping should have apositive association with appraisal. In other words,individuals who approach a stressor will subsequentlyreport more perceived mastery and less perceiveduncertainty and perceived impact (low levels of threaten-ing appraisal) than those who engage in more avoidantcoping efforts. In turn, high levels of perceived uncer-tainty and perceived impact, coupled with low perceivedmastery (high levels of threatening appraisal), are ex-pected to be associated with higher levels of psychologi-cal stress.

METHODParticipants

Participants (n = 826) were women aged 50 years andover who were recruited as they attended the OntarioBreast Screening Program (OBSP). To be eligible for theOBSP, women needed to be residents of the province ofOntario, have no history of breast malignancy, have nohistory of breast augmentation, and have no acute breastsymptoms. Women who had a breast lump or a benignbreast biopsy in the past were included in the sample.Women also had to be proficient in either official lan-guage (English or French). The majority of womenexpressed a preference for English (91%). Relying onstatistics provided by the OBSP, it was shown that thelanguage distribution of our sample was comparable tothat of the clientele seen at the OBSP (x2 = -53, p < .46).Anglophones and Francophones were not different ondemographic variables such as age and education (Wilks'= .99, p > .24), nor on income level (X2

(M7) = 1-62, p > .20).Multivariate analyses confirmed that Anglophone andFrancophone women were not different on appraisalsand stress at Time 1 (Wilks' = .99, p > .90) or at Time 2(Wilks',99, p > .70) (Sweet, 1998), nor were they differentin the use of stress and coping strategies at Time 1 (Wilks'= .86, p > .05) or Time 2 (Wilks' = .84, p < .06) (Savoie,1999). Therefore, data from Anglophones andFrancophones were pooled.

Information pertaining to screen status could have amajor effect on stress levels. Therefore, to ensure thatmodels were tested on homogeneous samples of normalresults and to avoid the potentially confounding effect ofan abnormal screen result, those women informed of anabnormal screen result (n = 70) were removed from the

data set, as were 36 cases for whom results were notknown at Time 2. An additional 158 cases could not beincluded in the analyses as they had not completedmeasures at both times. Such attrition is not uncommonin longitudinal investigations, especially since womenwere required to wait 20 days before completing thesecond evaluation. Consequently, the analyses were doneon a sample of 562 cases, which represented a highpercentage (68%) of the original sample. There were nosignificant differences between cases removed and thoseretained on age (t = .98, p > .33), education (t = .42, p >.15), marital status (x2

ao,788) = 5-21/ P < -80), or income (x2

(14/695) = 14.47, p<. 40).So that the two competing models could be tested on

independent samples, the remaining 562 women wererandomly divided into two subsamples using a table ofrandom numbers. Participants were aged between 50 and83 years (M = 60, so = 7.0). Of these, 73% were married orco-habitating with a partner, 11% were divorced, 6%were single, and 10% were widowed. Participantsreported an average of 14 years of education (so = 3.20)with a range of 7 years to 29 years.

MeasuresThe questionnaire used in the study incorporated ver-sions of three pre-validated psychological scales: thePsychological Stress Measure (PSM; Lemyre, Tessier, &Pillion, 1991), the Subjective Appraisal Rating Scale (SARS;Lemyre, 1986), as well as the COPE (Carver, Scheier, &Weintraub, 1989).

Psychological stress. Most researchers agree that stressis multifaceted and is often manifest at three levels:physical, behavioural, and cognitive (Lazarus & Folkman,1984; Lemyre, 1986; Lemyre et al., 1991; Selye, 1956).Psychological stress was assessed with the PsychologicalStress Measure, a self-report instrument which evaluatesthe subjective experience of feeling stressed over the lastfour or five days (Lemyre, 1986; Lemyre & Tessier, 1988;Lemyre et al., 1991). The instrument was originallydeveloped in French and has since been translated andvalidated in English. Both English and French versionshave been found to have similar psychometric profiles(Lemyre, Tessier, & Pillion, 1991). The PSM is designed foruse with a normal population and provides informationon the somatic, cognitive-affective, and behaviouralaspects of the subjects' perceptions of their stress state.Studies have shown the psychometric soundness of thisinstrument. Construct validity for the PSM was verified bydemonstrating its capacity to distinguish betweendifferential stress levels (Pillion, 1993; Lemyre, 1986).Concurrent validity between the PSM and salivary/serumimmunoglobulin has also been demonstrated (Pillion,Tessier, Tawadros, & Mouton, 1989). Findings show thatthe PSM has good convergent validity with the Brief

Page 5: Appraisals, Coping, and Stress in Breast Cancer Screening ...Resume Le modele devaluation cognitive de Lazarus et Folkman (1984) est une des principales theories sur le stress. Deux

244 Sweet, Savoie, and Lemyre

Symptom Inventory (r = .76), the Beck DepressionInventory (r = .75), and with Spielberger's anxiety scale(State subscale r = .73) (Dion, 1988; Lemyre et al., 1991).The developers also provide two short versions of thePSM. These short versions are highly correlated with theoriginal PSM (.98) (Lemyre et al., 1991). Both short ver-sions were found that have the same psychometricqualities as the original version (Lemyre, 1986). For theseshorter versions, test-retest reliability is moderatelysatisfactory at .56 and .65 over a six-month period forversions A and B, respectively (Lemyre, 1986; Lemyre etal., 1991).

Since the PSM provides information on the somatic,cognitive-affective, and behavioural aspects of the stressstate, three indicators of stress were created by combiningthe appropriate items. Using one of the validated abbre-viated versions of the PSM (Lemyre et al., 1991) a total ofeight items comprised the somatic indicator, nine itemscomprised the cognitive-affective indicator, and eightitems comprised the behavioural indicator of stress. Allscales were found to have high internal consistencies: .80for the somatic indicator, .78 for the behavioural indica-tor, and .90 for the cognitive indicator. Subscale meanswere used as indicators in the current model.1

Subjective appraisal ofstressors. Subjective appraisal ofthe stressors are measured using an abridged version ofthe Subjective Appraisal Rating Scale (SARS) (Lemyre,1986). This measure was originally developed in Frenchand was subsequently translated in English using thereverse-translation method. Both versions have beenfound to have similar psychometric qualities (Biron, 1992;Pillion, 1993). The original SARS consists of 10 items ratedfrom 1 to 8 (Likert scale) designed to assess subjects'appraisals of a specific stressful event in terms of negativeconsequences, positive consequences, loss, danger,failure, challenge, control, coping capability, unknown,and importance. Factor analyses have consistentlyextracted three factors: a) perceived impact, covering thenotions of severity, importance, and negativity; b)perceived uncertainty, comprising elements of unknownand unpredictability; and c) perceived mastery, repre-senting the dimensions of personal competence andcontrol (Biron et al., 1992; Pillion et al., 1989; Lemyre,1986). Together, these factors explain approximately 60%of the variance in appraisals (Pillion, 1993; Lemyre, 1986).The abridged version of the SARS used in this studyconsisted of three items pertaining to the perceivedimpact, perceived mastery and perceived uncertainty ofthe stressor.

For the present study, the situation chosen as thetarget stressor to be appraised was the "risk of breast

1 Items of the Psychological Stress Measure are available from theauthors.

cancer." The items, formulated on conceptual grounds,were based on the broad cognitive dimensions discussedabove. The items were presented in the following man-ner: "To which degree do you perceive this situation ashaving an impact on your life currently," "To whichdegree do you perceive you feel mastery over thissituation currently," and "To which degree do youperceive uncertainty about this situation currently?"Items were rated on an 8-point Likert scale, ranging from"Not at all" (1) to "Extremely" (8). A high score on ap-praisals is indicative of more threatening appraisal (highimpact and uncertainty, with low mastery). Past studieshave shown test-retest reliability values of .83, .79, and.78 over a two-week delay for dimensions of perceivedimpact, mastery, and uncertainty (Pillion, 1993). Forindividual items of perceived impact, mastery, anduncertainty, 20-day test-retest values were .68, .59, and.41, respectively (Sweet, 1998). While it is recognized thatsome of these values are low, this is not unexpected whenusing single-item indices.

Coping. The COPE scale, developed by Carver andcolleagues (1989), was used to measure coping strategies.This instrument comprises 54 items rated on a 4-pointLikert scale ranging from "I do not do this at all" (1) to "Ido this a lot" (4). A total of 13 strategies are said to berepresented: active coping, planning, suppression ofcompeting activities, restraint coping, seeking socialsupport for instrumental reasons, seeking social supportfor emotional reasons, focusing on and venting emotions,behavioural disengagement, mental disengagement,positive reinterpretation and growth, denial, acceptance,and turning to religion. First-order factor analysesrevealed 11 distinct factors (active coping/planning,suppression of competing activities, restraint coping,seeking social support, positive reinterpretation, accep-tance, turning to religion, focus on emotions, denial,behavioural disengagement, mental disengagement).Second order factor analysis identified four higher-orderfactors (active coping/planning with suppression ofcompeting activities, seeking social support/focus onemotion, denial/disengagement, positive reinterpreta-tion/acceptance, and restraint) (Carver et al., 1989).According to Herman-Stahl and collaborators (1995),these coping indices can be conceptualized as approachand avoidance types of strategies.

Due to logistic constraints of the study, the question-naire had to be abridged. Consequently six scales whichrepresent commonly found strategies were chosen for thestudy (Carver et al., 1989; Folkman, 1984). A confirma-tory factor analysis (Fournier, 1996) of the six scalesrepresented good fit to the data (CFI = .96). However,high correlations among the factors suggested that thedata may be best represented by two factors. The clustersof correlations suggested that instrumental social support

Page 6: Appraisals, Coping, and Stress in Breast Cancer Screening ...Resume Le modele devaluation cognitive de Lazarus et Folkman (1984) est une des principales theories sur le stress. Deux

Cognitive-appraisal Model of Stress 245

TABLE 1Means and Standard Deviations for Appraisal, Coping, and Stress

Subscale Model A (n = 248)Mean (SD)

Model B (n = 258)Mean (SD)

Time 1 Time 2 Time 1 Time 2

AppraisalsIMPMASUNC

Coping StrategiesApproach ISS

ESSACPL

Avoidance DIBD

Psychological StressTotal score

3.08 (1.69)4.17 (1.96)3.53 (1.68)

2.20 (0.92)2.09 (0.99)2.52 (0.95)2.77 (2.14)1.54 (0.66)1.36 (0.51)

n/a

2.91 (1.56)4.64 (1.83)2.94 (1.54)

2.43 (0.95)2.22 (0.94)2.75 (0.94)2.81 (1.00)1.35 (0.53)1.27 (0.44)

63.24 (26.37)

3.00 (1.77)4.63 (1.95)3.46 (1.66)

2.09 (0.93)2.01 (0.94)2.40 (0.99)2.57 (1.09)1.46 (0.59)1.35 (0.50)

n/a

2.78 (1.78)4.90 (1.99)3.08 (1.73)

2.20 (1.00)2.03 (0.93)2.47 (0.98)2.53 (1.07)1.40 (0.60)1.32 (0.45)

64.02 (29.18)

Note. IMP = impact, MAS = mastery, UNC = uncertainy, ISS = instrumentalsocial support, ESS = emotional social support, AC = active coping, PL =planning, DI = denial, BD = behavioural disengagement; perceived masteryscores are interpreted in direction opposite to that of perceived impact andperceived uncertainty.

(2 items), emotional social support (3 items), planning (2items), and active coping (4 items) represented approach-type coping strategies, whereas denial (4 items) andbehavioural disengagement (3 items) reflect avoidant-type coping strategies. Mean inter-item correlationsranged from 0.29 (behavioural disengagement) to 0.79(planning and emotional social support), supporting theinternal consistency of the coping scales. According toCarver, et al. (1989), test-retest reliabilities over an eight-week period were also reported to be satisfactory,ranging from .54 (denial) to .77 (emotional social sup-port).2 The items from the COPE were translated for thisstudy using the reverse-translation method.

Procedure

As the study should not interfere or reduce compliancewith the efficiency of the screening program, women whohad made an appointment at the screening clinic wereinvited by the OBSP to present themselves about 30minutes earlier than their scheduled appointment. Uponarrival at the clinic, women read and signed the appropri-ate consent forms, then completed the questionnairesimmediately prior to their screening. The screeningconsisted of a clinical examination by a specially trainednurse, followed by a mammogram. While waiting to beseen by the nurse examiner, women completed the initial

2 Analyses and selected items from the COPE are available fromthe authors. The test-retest interval for the Carver et al. (1989)was eight weeks, which may explain the relatively low test-retestcorrelation for denial as a coping process.

questionnaire and were provided with the secondquestionnaire that was to be completed at home 20 dayslater after the screening, once their screen results wereknown. The official protocol of the clinic is to notifywomen of results from both the clinical breast exam andthe mammogram simultaneously in writing. Letters cantake anywhere from 3 to 14 days and sometimes longerto be delivered. The 20-day waiting period was chosen toensure that all women had indeed received their letter.As a reminder, women were called on the day they wereto complete and mail the second questionnaire. In all, 618(75%) women returned the second questionnaire.

Statistical analyses

Each conceptual model was tested in a three-step process.Firstly, preliminary analyses were conducted to screenthe data for violation of basic assumptions inherent tomultivariate analyses (Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996).Secondly, separate confirmatory factor analyses usingLisrel 8.12 (Joreskog & Sorbom, 1993) were conducted totest the adequacy of the measurement models. For eachof the two structural models, a CFA estimated the fit for atwo-factor solution using the six indicators of coping.Another CFA tested a one-factor solution for appraisalusing the three indicators of appraisal, and a third CFAtested a one-factor solution for stress using the threeindicators of stress. Finally, confirmatory factor analyticprocedures within the framework of covariance structureanalysis were used to test the hypothesized relationshipsbetween latent constructs. All analyses were based on thecovariance matrices and used the Maximum Likelihood

Page 7: Appraisals, Coping, and Stress in Breast Cancer Screening ...Resume Le modele devaluation cognitive de Lazarus et Folkman (1984) est une des principales theories sur le stress. Deux

246 Sweet, Savoie, and Lemyre

TABLE 2Lisrel Estimates for the Appraisal Measurement Models"

Indicators

IMP

MAS

UNCb

Model ATraditional

.44f = 8.27*

-.11t = -2.69*

1.42

ModelBRevised

.53t = 8.09*

-.31t = -5.07*

1.08

Note. IMP = perceived impact; MAS = perceived mastery;UNC = perceived uncertainty. Perceived mastery scoresare interpreted in direction opposite to that of perceivedimpact and perceived uncertainty.a Results represent completely standardized solution.b This parameter was fixed to 1.00, thereforeno t value is available.*p < .05.

estimation method.3

As recommended by researchers in this area (e.g.,Marsh & Hocevar, 1985), the evaluation of fit of themodel was based on many criteria, which considerstatistical, theoretical, and practical issues. Global assess-ments of fit are based on a) the chi-square likelihoodratio, b) the revised normal comparative fit index (CFI;Bentler, 1990), c) expected cross-validation index (ECVI;Cudeck & Brown, 1983) and d) the root mean squareerror of approximation (RMSEA).

As proposed by Byrne (1991), a number of criteriawere also taken into account in judging the worth ofindividual parameters; these include statistical signifi-cance as indicated by the LISREL t-values, goodness-of-fitbased on the normalized residual values and modifica-tion indices.

For Model A, appraisals from the initial assessment(i.e., Time 1, prior to screen) were used to predict thecoping efforts reported at the second assessment (i.e.,Time 2, post screen), which were related to stress at Time2. For Model B, coping at Time 1 was related to appraisalsat Time 2, which were related to stress at Time 2. Stress asthe outcome was measured at the same time for bothmodels so that the potential impact of the timing of thescreen result was uniform across both models.

RESULTSData screening

Data from each subsample were screened independently.Cases with more than 10% missing data were excluded,while mean substitution was the strategy used in caseswith less than 10% missing data. Looking first atsubsample A (n = 279), a preliminary screen of the datarevealed that all values were reasonable and withinexpected limits. No univariate outliers were identified.Using Mahalanobis distances, 12 multivariate cases were

3 All covariance matrices are available from the authors.

identified as outliers and were thus deleted from thedataset. This resulted in a final sample size of 248 partici-pants for subsample A. As for subsample B, 20 cases withmore than 10% missing data were deleted. Once again,no univariate outliers were identified and only onemultivariate outlier was identified which resulted in asample of 258 cases. When compared, the two resultingsubsamples were not different from one another on age(F<i,528) = -001, p > .95), education (F(1/5M) = .68, p > .40),marital status (x^zj = 7.18, p > .20), or income (X2

(5,473) =

5.86, p > .55). As recommended by Muthen and Kaplan(1985), the mean skewness and kurtosis of the measuredvariables were within limits for both subsamples. Thus,the data were considered as normally distributed. Forboth subsamples, a visual inspection on the plotted rawpredicted values versus the raw residuals suggested thatthe relationship between the independent and dependentvariables was linear, and supported the assumption ofmultivariate normality. The means and standard devia-tions on coping, appraisal, and stress are presented forboth subsamples in Table 1.

Structural equation modelingAn important preliminary step in the analysis of fulllatent variable models is to test the validity of the mea-surement model before making any attempt to evaluatethe structural portion of the model. Accordingly, CFAprocedures are used in testing the validity of indicatorvariables.

Traditional Model:Appraisal influences stress through coping

Establishing the measurement model. In the present case,CFAS were conducted for indicator variables derived fromthe three measures used in the study. The first CFA modelhypothesized a priori that responses to the abridged SARScould be explained by one factor named "threateningappraisals." The initial one-factor model of appraisalrepresented a high degree of fit to the data with x2

(1) = 0.0(p > .05). As shown in Table 2, the estimated parameterswere all statistically significant. Perceived impact andperceived uncertainty were both positively associatedwith appraisal, whereas perceived mastery was nega-tively associated with this factor.

The second CFA model hypothesized a priori thatresponses to the abridged COPE could be explained bytwo factors named "approach-type coping" and"avoidant-type" coping (i.e., instrumental social support,emotional social support, active coping, and planningwould load together on one factor representingapproach-type coping, whereas denial and behaviouraldisengagement would load together on a second factorrepresenting avoidant-type coping). The initial two-factormodel represented satisfactory fit to the data on the basis

Page 8: Appraisals, Coping, and Stress in Breast Cancer Screening ...Resume Le modele devaluation cognitive de Lazarus et Folkman (1984) est une des principales theories sur le stress. Deux

Cognitive-appraisal Model of Stress 247

TABLE 3Lisrel Estimates for the Coping Measurement Models"

Model ATraditional

Model BRevised

Indicators Approach-type Avoidant-type Approach-type Avoidant-type

ISS

ESS

AC

PL"

DI

BDb

.80t = 17.64*

.72f = 14.34*

.94t = 25.01*

.93

.81t = 15.46*

.87t = 17.07*

.86t = 16.76*

.84

.97 .61t = 2.12* t = 2.82*

.48 .64

Note. ISS = instrumental social support; ESS = emotional social support; AC = active coping;PL = planning; DI = denial; BD = behavioural disengagement.* Results represent completely standardized solution.b These parameters were fixed to 1.00, thus no t values are available.*p < .05.

of statistical criteria (x2(8) = 46.42, p < .05). Examination of

the CFI (.95) is indicative of an adequate fit to the data ata practical level. As seen in Table 3, all paths leadingfrom indicators to the latent constructs are statisticallysignificant.

The third CFA model hypothesized a priori thatresponses to the Psychological Stress Measure could beexplained by one factor named "psychological stress." Theone-factor model of psychological stress represented ahigh degree of fit to the data on the basis of statisticalcriteria (x2

a) = 0.0, p > .05). As indicated in Table 4, allestimated parameters had statistically significant f-valuesin the initial model and were positively associated withstress.

Testing the hypothesized causal structure of Model A. Thenext step involved testing the structural paths betweenthe latent constructs. Figure 1 presents the hypothesizedconceptual traditional model (Model A) to be tested. Asshown, it is hypothesized that appraisals are directlyrelated to both avoidant- and approach-type copingstrategies, which in turn, impact on psychological stress.Figure 2 shows that one indicator for each latent con-struct was fixed to 1.0 for model identification. A total of28 parameters were to be estimated. With 78 datapoints[(12(13)/2 = 78)], the initial full structural model wasconducted with 50 degrees of freedom and was over-identified.

Examination of the CFI of .95 indicated that the modelrepresented an adequate fit to the data at a practical level.Furthermore, the RMSEA (0.08) also suggested an accept-able fit. See Table 5 for a summary of specifications andfit statistics.

Individual indicators of misfit were also examined.One parameter representing the relationship between

approach-type coping and psychological stress(Gamma (U)) was not significant (t = -.42, p > .05). Toestablish a more parsimonious model, this structural pathwas deleted. The difference in x2 (Ax2

(1) = 0.17, p > .05)was non-significant and therefore, removal of the param-eter did not significantly change the fit of the model. Inthis model, the fit was satisfactory (x2

(52) = 130.96, p < .05,CFI = .95, RMSEA = 0.08, ECVI = .75). All estimated parame-ters within the model were significant. Appraisals werepositively and significantly related to approach- (t = 2.52,p < .05) and avoidant-type (t = 3.46, p < .05) coping.Avoidant coping was positively associated with stress (t= 3.51, p < .05). The reliability of the indicators rangedfrom R2 = .02 for perceived mastery to R2 = .89 for activecoping. The very low reliability for perceived mastery isdiscussed later. Disturbance terms for approach coping,avoidance coping, and psychological stress were gener-ally quite elevated (.96, .86, & .89, respectively) (seeFigure 2).

Revised Model: Coping influences stress through appraisalEstablishing the measurement model. As for Model A, herealso a CFA model hypothesized that indicators of copingcould be explained by two factors and was found to havesatisfactory fit (x2

(8) = 64-75/ P < -01)- T^16 comparison fitindex (CFI = .93) was above the critical value with nosizeable standardized residuals and represented apsychometrically reasonable fit to the data. As seen inTable 3, individual parameters were significantly andpositively associated with avoidant-type coping efforts oravoidant-type coping efforts in a similar fashion as forModel A.

Another CFA model applied to indicators of appraisal.The hypothesized one-factor appraisal model was found

Page 9: Appraisals, Coping, and Stress in Breast Cancer Screening ...Resume Le modele devaluation cognitive de Lazarus et Folkman (1984) est une des principales theories sur le stress. Deux

248 Sweet, Savoie, and Lemyre

to have good fit with x2<i) = 0.00 (p < .05). Individualparameters were significant and related in a similarmanner to appraisal.

The last CFA model revealed a one-factor appraisalmodel of stress to have good fit with x2

(]) = 0.00 (p < .05)with individual parameters all being significant. As seenin Table 4, somatic, cognitive-affective, and behaviouralaspects were all positively related to stress.

Testing the hypothesized causal structure of Model B. Asshown in Figure 1, the hypothesized structural modelpostulated a priori that approach and avoidant-typecoping efforts would be directly related to appraisal andthat appraisal would impact on stress. The 28 parameterswere to be estimated with 12 observed variables. As therewere [12(12+1 )]/2 = 78 data points, the model was over-identified and was tested with 50 degrees of freedom.

Goodness-of-fit statistics are presented in Table 5.Turning first to the comparative fit index (CFI = .93), wefound that it provided evidence of a fairly well-fittingmodel. Since there were no outstanding modificationindex values suggestive of model misfit, no furtherconsideration was given to the inclusion of additionalparameters.

In reviewing the structural parameter estimates for thefull structural model, we can see that one parameter isnot significant. Specifically, approach-type coping wasnot found to be related to appraisal (Beta (3/1), t = .67, p >.05). In the interest of parsimony, a final model of stresswas estimated with this structural path deleted from themodel.

Estimation of the final model resulted in an overall x2

value of 159.70 (p < .05), with a CFI value of .94 and anRMSEA of .09. Although there is a slight increase in the x2

value, the important aspect of this change in model fit isthat the x2 difference between the two models is notsignificant (see Table 5). Based on the ECVI (.83), it seemsthat the final model has a better potential for replication.All hypothesized paths were significant and thus impor-tant to the model. More specifically, avoidant copingbehaviours were positively and significantly associatedwith appraisal (t = 2.45, p < .05), and appraisal waspositively associated with stress (t = 4.25, p < .05). Noadditional paths were identified as essential componentsof the causal structure. The reliability indicators rangedfrom R2 = .13 for perceived mastery (MAS) to R2 = .91 forcognitive aspects of stress (COGN). As with Model A, thereliability for the perceived mastery item was rather lowand this issue is addressed in the discussion. Disturbanceterms were rather high (.95 for threatening appraisal and.89 for psychological stress) (see Figure 3).

DISCUSSION

Using data from women undergoing breast cancerscreening with normal results, the objective of this study

TABLE 4Lisrel Estimates for the Psychological Stress MeasurementModels4

Indicators

COG

BEH

SOM"

Model ATraditional

.90t = 22.43*

.86t = 20.20*

.91

Model BRevised

.95t = 28.80*

.86t = 22.37*

.92

Note. SOM = somatic indicator of stress; COG = cognitiveindicator of stress; BEH = behavioural indicator of stress.' Results represent completely standardized solution.b This parameter was fixed to 1.00, thus no t value isavailable.*p < .05.

was to test the "causal" relations among the three princi-ple variables of the cognitive-appraisal model, coping,appraisal, and stress, using structural equation modelingtechniques. Two competing structural models werepresented. In the first, more traditional model, it waspostulated that appraisal would influence coping effortswhich, in turn, would impact on the stress response. Inthe second, revised model, it was hypothesized thatcoping efforts would be directly related to the appraisalof the stressor, which in turn, would have an impact onthe degree of stress reported by individuals.

A first step entailed testing the adequacy of themeasurement models. No problems with the measure-ment models of each of the two conceptual models wereidentified. Turning our attention to the structural portionof the models, we found that both models yielded arelatively high degree of fit. These findings partiallysupported both formulations of Lazarus' model of stress.

Nonetheless, the traditional model of stress seemed toyield a marginally better fit as demonstrated by a stron-ger CFI index. The RMSEA value was also lower, suggest-ing that this model more closely approximated thepopulation correlation matrix. In addition, the ECVlsuggested that this model was more likely than therevised model to be cross-validated in another sample. Assuch, it would seem that appraisals influence copingstrategies which then determine the degree of stressexperienced. These findings must, however, by cross-validated before any firm conclusions can be drawn.Furthermore, both models yielded a high level of fit and,therefore, both formulations merit some discussion.

Regarding the first model, the predicted paths fromappraisal to both types of coping were significant. Whenwomen perceived the stressor more negatively, theymade more of an effort to cope with the situation,whether it be by approaching it or avoiding it. The pathfrom avoidant-type coping to stress was also significant.

Page 10: Appraisals, Coping, and Stress in Breast Cancer Screening ...Resume Le modele devaluation cognitive de Lazarus et Folkman (1984) est une des principales theories sur le stress. Deux

Cognitive-appraisal Model of Stress 249

.32

UNC*

^

.35

JISS

.«o\

/

9 J*s

.49

JESS

.72V.

.11

IAC

.94 .93

>— ^vVApproach \coping J-

.14

1PL*

.9.89

DI BD*

I I.56 .53

SOM* COG BEH

! ! 1.18 .19 .26

Figure 2. Full structural model (Traditional) with completely standardized estimates. IMP =perceived impact, MAS = perceived mastery, UNC = perceived uncertainty, ISS = instrumental socialsupport, ESS = emotional social suppport, AC = active coping, PL = planning, DI = denial, BD =behavioural disengagement, SOM = somatic, COG = cognitive, BEH = behavioural, * = parameter fixedto 1.0.

Thus, yielding to more avoidance led to an increasein stress.

As predicted in the second model, avoidant-typestrategies were positively associated with threateningappraisal. Thus, as individuals focused attention awayfrom the source of stress or their reactions to it (Suls &Fletcher, 1985), they appraised the situation more nega-tively (i.e., high perceived impact, high perceived uncer-tainty, and low perceived mastery). The relationshipbetween appraisal and stress was also significant. There-fore, a negative perception of the event led to increasedpsychological stress.

Certain paths in the models were not significant.Based on the traditional conceptualization, it had beenhypothesized that approach-type coping would beassociated with reduced stress. This relationship was notsignificant. Inconsistencies in the direction of the relationsof approach-type coping to stress have been noted inprevious studies. It has also been suggested that the useof approach-type strategies in the context of an uncon-trollable stressor are less likely to be adaptive (Coyne &Downey, 1991; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). The highprevalence of breast cancer, coupled with no possibilityof action until full diagnosis, may leave women feelinghelpless in the face of potentially receiving an abnormalscreen, resulting in the nonsignificant path betweenapproach-type coping and stress.

The lack of association between approach strategiesand stress may also be explained by the natural context

in which the study was conducted. Some women indi-cated that they tried to regard screening as a routineprocedure and they expressed some difficulty in under-standing how a person could actively cope with some-thing as abstract as the risk of breast cancer.

In the revised model, the effect of approach-typecoping on appraisal was not significant. One possibleexplanation for this lack of association stems fromfindings suggesting a strong relationship between activecoping behaviours and event controllability. Researchershave found that individuals are more active in situationsthat are appraised as controllable (Terry, 1994; Vitaliano,DeWolf, Maiuro, Russo, & Katon, 1990). In fact, denial isa common strategy used when a situation is perceived ashaving a high degree of threat and uncontrollability(Buntrock & Reddy, 1992); when there is nothing con-structive left to be done, avoidant-type strategies maytake precedence (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Perhapsonce women arrived at the clinic, they perceived the riskof breast cancer as being out of their control. Conse-quently, they may have preferred to focus attention awayfrom the stressor at hand.

Before concluding, various strengths and limitationsof this study merit consideration. This study was basedon a relatively large sample size and was conducted inthe context of a naturalistic setting. Moreover, the use ofa longitudinal design allowed us to examine more clearlythe sequence of events outlined in the cognitive-appraisalframework. In terms of limitations, it must first be

Page 11: Appraisals, Coping, and Stress in Breast Cancer Screening ...Resume Le modele devaluation cognitive de Lazarus et Folkman (1984) est une des principales theories sur le stress. Deux

250 Sweet, Savoie, and Lemyre

TABLE 5Summary of Specifications and Fit Statistics for Tested Models of Stress

Model ParameterDeleted

Model A - Traditional1 Initial —2 Final Gamma(i ,,

Model B - Revised1 Initial —2 Final Beta(w)

X2

130.79130.96

159.27159.70

df

5051

5051

CFI

.95

.95

.93

.94

ECVI

.76

.75

.84

.83

RMSEA Ax2 Adf

.08 - -

.08 0.17 1

.09 - -

.09 0.43 1

P

ns

ns

Note, ns = non-significant.

recognized that structural equation modeling techniquescannot establish causal relationships between variables.Instead, they are used to evaluate the relative fit ofvarious hypothesized causal models to existing data. Inaddition, even though considerable structural equationmodeling research is done using such a limited amountof indicators, generally, the use of at least three indicatorsper construct is recommended (Hayduk, 1987). Futurestudies should include a third indicator of avoidance-type coping.

Second, appraisal indicators (i.e., perceived impact,mastery, and uncertainty) were each assessed using asingle item. Even though multiple items would bepreferable, the squared multiple correlations for per-ceived impact and perceived uncertainty indicated thatthese items were in fact, reliable. The low squaredmultiple correlation for perceived mastery suggests,however, that this particular item may not reliablyrepresent the underlying construct of appraisal. Thismay, at least in part, be explained by the overlap betweenappraisals of mastery or control and coping efforts notedby various researchers (e.g., Dewe, 1991; Folkman &Lazarus, 1985). However, a more likely explanation is thefact that single items were used to assess each appraisaldimension.

Third, although the structural models were character-ized by a high degree of fit, the disturbance terms for thedependent variables suggest that a significant amount ofvariance in both appraisal and stress was left unex-plained. There are therefore other elements, not consid-ered in these models, that may help to explain variationsin appraisal and stress.

Next, our findings were based on data from self-reports. It is appropriate to recognize the various limita-tions associated with the use of such measures. The useof objective measures (i.e., investigator-based interviewprotocols and ratings) such as those of Brown and Harris(1978) in tandem with subjective reports would enhancethe validity of the findings.

Finally, it is important to remember that this studywas conducted within a very specific context, that ofbreast cancer screening. We argue that appraisals, coping,

and stress should be investigated in the context of anatural stressor that is readily experienced by a largenumber of homogeneous individuals. However, thespecific stressful situation chosen for this study mayhinder the extent to which our results can be generalized.It should also be acknowledged that women wereinformed of their normal results between the first andsecond measures, which may have impacted upon stresslevels. Nonetheless, the paths between appraisal, coping,and stress remain significant. Moreover, the exclusion ofwomen with abnormal findings prevented us frominvestigating whether or not the process would bedifferent for women whose screen results brought araised and sustained level of uncertainty and threat.Further replication of these findings using differentsamples is clearly needed.

To conclude, the results from this study support theimportance of both appraisals and coping in the experi-ence of stress. As described in Lazarus and Folkman's(1984) theoretical model, coping behaviours and ap-praisal are both important to a person's adaptation.Findings suggested that appraisals could influencecoping strategies which, in turn, would relate to the stresslevel. However, given the fact that both models ofappraisal leading to coping and reversibly coping leadingto appraisal, yielded a high degree of fit, more cross-validation studies are needed to better understand thecomplex and dynamic interrelationship between theseconcepts. In fact, there may be a need to develop a newalternative model that encompasses both traditional andrevised views. Such as a model would better reflect theevaluation process by acknowledging the possible bi-directional nature among these concepts. This model canthen be tested on data from many time points, allowingthe investigation of how the process unfolds and how astressor is re-evaluated over time.

This study was carried out with the financial support ofthe Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council. Wewish to express our gratitude to the Ontario Breast Screen-ing Program, all staff members involved in helping uswith this project, and the women who shared their experi-

Page 12: Appraisals, Coping, and Stress in Breast Cancer Screening ...Resume Le modele devaluation cognitive de Lazarus et Folkman (1984) est une des principales theories sur le stress. Deux

Cognitive-appraisal Model of Stress 251

.34 .24 .26 .29

J J I JJL _L _rSOM*| COG BEH

.73

Figure 3. Full structural model (Revised) with completely standardized estimates. IMP = perceivedimpact, MAS = perceived mastery, UNC = perceived uncertainty, ISS = instrumental social support,ESS = emotional social suppport, AC = active coping, PL = planning, DI = denial, BD = behavioraldisengagement, SOM = somatic, COG = cognitive, BEH = behavioral, * = parameter fixed to 1.0.

ences with us. Correspondence concerning this articleshould be addressed to Dr. Lisa Sweet, Royal OttawaHealth Care Group, 1145 Carling Ave., Ottawa, OntarioKlz 7K4 Canada (E-mail: [email protected]).

References

Aldwin, CM. (1994). Stress, coping, and development: Anintegrative perspective. New York: Guilford Press.

Bentler, P. (1990). Comparative fit indices in structuralmodels. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238-246.

Biron. C, Truchon, M., & Lemyre, L. (1992). Investigatorand respondent evaluations of stressors:Psychometrical considerations. Unpublished manu-script, Universite Laval, Quebec.

Bjorck, J.P., & Cohen, L. H. (1993). Coping with threat,losses, and challenges. Journal of Social and ClinicalPsychology, 12,56-72.

Buntrock, C.N., & Reddy, D.M. (1992). Coping dispositionand the stress appraisal process: Impact of defensive-ness on emotional response to threat. Personality andIndividual Differences, 13,1223-1231.

Byrne, B.M. (1991). The Maslach Burnout Inventory: vali-dating factorial structure and invariance across inter-mediate, secondary, and university educators.Multivariate Behavioral Research, 26,583-605.

Carver, C.S., Pozo, C., Harris, S.D., Noriega, V., Scheier,M.F., Robinson, D.S., Ketcham, A.S., Moffat, F.L., &Clark, K.C. (1993). How coping mediates the effect of

optimism on distress: a study of women with earlystages of breast cancer. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 65, 375-390.

Carver, C.S., & Scheier, M.F. (1994). Situational copingand coping dispositions in a stressful encounter. Journalof Personality and Social Psychology, 66,184-195.

Carver, C.S., Scheier, M.F., & Weintraub, J.K. (1989). As-sessing coping strategies: A theoretically basedapproach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56,267-283.

Christman, NJ. (1990). Uncertainty and adjustment dur-ing radiotherapy. Nursing Research, 39,17-20.

Commerford, M.C., Gular, E., Orr, D.A., Reznikof, M., &O'Dowd, M.A. (1994). Coping and psychological dis-tress in women with HIV/AIDS. Journal of CommunityPsychology, 22, 224-230.

Coyne, J.C., & Downey, G. (1991). Social factors andpsychopathology: Stress, social support, and copingprocesses. Annual Review of Psychology, 42,401-425.

Cudeck, R., & Brown, M.W. (1983). Cross-validation ofcovariance structures. Multivariate Behavioral Research,18,147-167.

Davey, G.C.L. (1993). A comparison of three cognitiveappraisal strategies: the role of threat devaluation inproblem-focused coping. Personality and IndividualDifferences, 14,535-546.

Davis, L.L. (1990). Illness uncertainty, social support, andstress in recovering individuals and family caregivers.Applied Nursing Research, 3, 69-71.

Page 13: Appraisals, Coping, and Stress in Breast Cancer Screening ...Resume Le modele devaluation cognitive de Lazarus et Folkman (1984) est une des principales theories sur le stress. Deux

252 Sweet, Savoie, and Lemyre

Dewe, P. (1991). Primary appraisal, secondary appraisaland coping: Their role in stressful work encounters.Journal of Occupational Psychology, 64, 331-351.

Dion, G. (1988). Validation du concept de burnout. Memoirede maitrise. Quebec, Canada, Ecole de psychologic,Universite Laval.

Dunkel-Schetter, C, Feinstein, L.G., Taylor, S.E., & Falk,R.L. (1992). Patterns of coping with cancer. HealthPsychology, 11, 79-87.

Endler, N.S., & Parker, J.D.A. (1990). Multidimensionalassessment of coping: A critical evaluation. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 58, 844-854.

Endler, N.S., & Parker, J.D.A. (1994). Assessment of multi-dimensional coping: Task, emotion and avoidancestrategies. Psychological Assessment, 6, 50-60.

Felsten, G. (1991). Influences of situation-specific masterybeliefs and satisfaction with social support on appraisalof stress. Psychological Reports, 69,483-495.

Pillion, L. (1993). Evaluation cognitive, etat de stress etimmunite lors d'un diagnostic de tumeur au sein. Unpub-lished doctoral dissertation. Universite Laval, Quebec.

Pillion, L., Tessier, R., Tawardos, E., & Mouton, C. (1989).Stress et immunite: Etude de validite d'une mesure destress psychologique. Canadian Psychology, 30, 30-38.

Folkman, S. (1984). Personal control and stress and copingprocesses: a theoretical analysis. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology, 46, 839-852.

Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R.S. (1980). An analysis of copingin a middle-aged community sample. Journal of Healthand Social Behavior, 21, 219-239.

Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R.S. (1985). If it changes it must bea process: Study of emotions and coping during threestages of a college examination. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology, 48,150-170.

Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R.S. (1988). Manual for the Ways ofCoping Questionnaire: Research edition. Palo Alto, CA:Consulting Psychologists Press.

Folkman, S., Lazarus, R.S., Gruen, R.J., DeLongis, A.(1986). Appraisal, coping, health status, and psycholog-ical symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-ogy, 50, 571-579.

Forsythe, C.J., & Compas, B.E. (1987). Interaction of cogni-tive appraisals of stressful events and coping: Testingthe goodness of fit hypothesis. Cognitive Therapy andResearch, 11 (4), 473-485.

Gall, T.L., & Evans, D.R. (1987). The dimensionality ofcognitive appraisal and its relationship to physical andpsychological well-being. The Journal of Psychology, 121,538-546.

Gass, K.A., & Chang, A.S. (1989). Appraisals of bereave-ment, coping, resources, and psychosocial health dys-function in widows and widowers. Nursing Research,38, 31-36.

Ham, M., & Larson, R. (1990). The cognitive moderation of

daily stress in early adolescence. American Journal ofCommunity Psychology, 18, 567-585.

Hayduk, L.A. (1987). Structural Equation Modeling withLisrel: Essentials and Advances. Baltimore, MD: JohnHopkins University Press.

Herman-Stahl, M.A., Stemmler, M., & Petersen, A.C.(1995). Approach and avoidant coping: implications foradolescent mental health. Journal of Youth and Adoles-cence, 24, 649-665.

Hu, L.T., & Bentler, P. (1995). Evaluating model fit. In R.H.Hoyle (Ed.), Structural Equation Modeling: Concepts,Issues, and Applications. London: Sage.

Irvin, M.J., & Logan, A.G. (1994). Is knowing your choles-terol number harmful? Journal of Clinical Epidemiology,47,131-145.

Joreskog, K.G., & Sorbom, D. (1993). Lisrel 8.12. Chicago,IL: Scientific Software.

Karasawa, K. (1995). Cognitive antecedents of emotions:Findings and future directions. Japanese PsychologicalResearch, 37,40-55.

Kendler, K.S., Kessler, R.C., Heath, A.C., Neale, M.C., &Eaves, L.J. (1991). Coping: A genetic epidemiologicalinvestigation. Psychological Medicine, 21, 337-346.

Lazarus, R.S. (1991). Cognition and motivation in emo-tion. American Psychologist, 46, 352-367.

Lazarus, R.S. (1993). From psychological stress to theemotions: A history of changing outlooks. AnnualReview of Psychology, 44,1-21.

Lazarus. R.S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, Appraisal andCoping. New York: Springer Publishing Company Inc.

Lemyre, L. (1986). Stress psychologique et apprehension cogni-tive. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, UniversiteLaval, Quebec.

Lemyre, L., & Tessier, R. (1988). Mesure de StressPsychologique (MSP): Se sentir stressee. Canadian Jour-nal of Behavioural Science, 20, 302-321.

Lemyre, L., Tessier, R., & Pillion, L. (1991). PsychologicalStress Measure: A translation. Quebec, PQ: UniversiteLaval.

Marsh, H.W., & Hocevar, D. (1985). Application of confir-matory factor analysis to the study of self-concept:first- and higher-order factor models and theirinvariance across groups. Psychological Bulletin, 97,562-582.

Marteau, T.M. (1994). Psychology and screening: Narrow-ing the gap between efficacy and effectiveness. BritishJournal of Clinical Psychology, 33,1-10.

Mishel, M.H. (1981). The measurement of uncertainty inillness. Nursing Research, 30(5), 258-263.

Mishel, M.H., & Sorenson, D.S. (1991). Uncertainty ingynecological cancer: A test of the mediating functionsof mastery and coping. Nursing Research, 40(3), 167-171.

Muthen, B., & Kaplan, D. (1985). A comparison of somemethodologies for the factor analysis of non-normal

Page 14: Appraisals, Coping, and Stress in Breast Cancer Screening ...Resume Le modele devaluation cognitive de Lazarus et Folkman (1984) est une des principales theories sur le stress. Deux

Cognitive-appraisal Model of Stress 253

Likert variables. British Journal of Mathematical andStatistical Psychology, 38,171-189.

Prince-Embury, S. (1992). Psychological symptoms asrelated to cognitive appraisals and demographic differ-ences among information seekers in the aftermath oftechnological disaster at Three Mile Island. Journal ofApplied Social Psychology, 22, 38-54.

Savoie, J. (1996). Confirmatory Factor Analysis of theCope Scale. Unpublished manuscript, University ofOttawa, Ottawa, ON.

Savoie, J. (1999). Contextual and Subjective Indices of CopingStrategies in Breast Cancer Screening: A LongitudinalInvestigation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Uni-versity of Ottawa, Ontario.

Selye, H. (1956). The stress of life. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Smith, C.A., & Lazarus, R.S. (1993). Appraisal compo-nents, core relational themes, and the emotions. Cogni-tion and Emotion, 3-4,233-269.

Stanton, A.L., & Snider, P.R. (1993). Coping with breastcancer diagnosis. Health Psychology, 12,16-23.

Suls, J., & Fletcher, B. (1985). The relative efficacy ofavoidant and non-avoidant coping strategies. A meta-analysis. Health Psychology, 4, 249-288.

Sweet, L. (1998). Stress, Subjective Appraisals, and Anticipa-tion in the Context of Breast Cancer Screening. Unpub-lished doctoral dissertation, University of Ottawa,Ottawa, ON.

Tabachnick, B.C., & Fidell, L.S. (1996). Using multivariatestatistics (3rd ed.). New York: Harper CollinsPublishers Inc.

Tenner, H., & Herzverger, S. (1985). Ways of Coping

Scale. In D.J. Keyser & R.C. Sweetland (Eds.), TestCritiques (Vol. 3, pp. 686-697). Kansas City, KA: TestCorporation of America.

Terry, D.J. (1994). Determinants of coping: The role ofstable and situational factors. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 66, 895-910.

Tobin, D.L., Holroyd, K.A., Reynolds, R.V., & Wigal, J.K.(1989). The hierarchical factor structure of the CopingStrategies Inventory. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 13,343-361.

Tomaka, J., Blascovich, J., Kelsey, R.M., & Leiten, C.L.(1993). Subjective, physiological, and behavioral effectsof threat and challenge appraisal. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology, 65, 248-260.

Valentiner, D.P., Holahan, CJ., & Moos, R.H. (1994).Social support, appraisals of event controllability, andcoping: An integrative model. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 66,1094-1102.

Vinokur, A., & Selzer, M.L. (1975). Desirable versus unde-sirable life events: their relationship to stress and men-tal distress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,32, 329-337.

Vitaliano, P.P., DeWolfe, D.J., Maiuro, R.D., Russo, J., &Katon, W. (1990). Appraised changeability of a stressoras a modifier of the relationship between coping anddepression: A test of the hypothesis of fit. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 59, 582-592.

Received January 16,1998Revised March 1,1999

Accepted April 19,1999