A form of long-lasting, learning-related synaptic ... · PDF fileA form of long-lasting,...

6
A form of long-lasting, learning-related synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus induced by heterosynaptic low-frequency pairing Yan-You Huang, Christopher Pittenger*, and Eric R. Kandel Center for Neurobiology and Behavior, Howard Hughes Medical Institute, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, 1051 Riverside Drive, New York, NY 10032 Contributed by Eric R. Kandel, November 10, 2003 The late, transcription- and translation-dependent phase of long- term synaptic potentiation (L-LTP) at the Schaffer collateral syn- apse of the hippocampus is an experimental model of the synaptic plasticity underlying long-lasting memory formation. L-LTP is typ- ically induced by homosynaptic tetanic stimulation; but associative forms of learning are likely to require the heterosynaptic pairing of stimuli. Here we describe L-LTP elicited by such heterosynaptic pairing at the Schaffer collateral synapse in mice. We find that repeated stimulation of one pathway at low frequency (0.2 Hz), which does not by itself induce synaptic potentiation, will produce long-lasting synaptic plasticity when paired with a brief condition- ing burst applied to an independent afferent pathway. The induc- tion of heterosynaptic L-LTP is associative and critically depends on the precise time interval of pairing: simultaneous, conjunctional pairing induces L-LTP; in contrast, delayed pairing induces short- lasting early-phase LTP. Heterosynaptically induced early-phase LTP could be depotentiated by repeatedly presenting unpaired test stimuli, whereas L-LTP could not. This heterosynaptically induced L-LTP requires PKA and protein synthesis. In addition, heterosyn- aptically induced L-LTP is impaired in transgenic mice that express KCREB (a dominant negative inhibitor of adenosine 35-cyclic monophosphate response element-binding protein-mediated tran- scription) in the hippocampus. These mice have previously been shown to be impaired in spatial memory but have normal L-LTP as induced by a conventional homosynaptic tetanic protocol. These data suggest that at least in some instances this L-LTP-inducing protocol may better model behaviorally relevant information stor- age and the in vivo mechanisms underlying long-lasting memories. L ong-lasting synaptic change is widely believed to underlie many forms of long-lasting memory (1), but it is increasingly clear that a synapse can support mechanistically distinct forms of long-lasting synaptic plasticity. For example, distinct forms of late-phase long-term synaptic potentiation (L-LTP) at the Schaf- fer collateral synapse of the hippocampus differ in their depen- dence on brain-derived neurotrophic factor (2, 3) and perhaps on their dependence on the transcription factor adenosine 35- cyclic monophosphate response element-binding protein (CREB) (4). In light of this observation, it is critically important to determine which forms of experimentally induced synaptic plasticity best recapitulate the endogenous mechanisms of in- formation storage recruited in the hippocampus and in other structures by different forms of learning. LTP can be separated into at least two temporal phases: early-phase LTP (E-LTP), which is independent of macromo- lecular synthesis and decays to baseline within 1–3 h of induction, and L-LTP, which requires transcription and translation and can persist for hours or days. Most studies of plasticity induce both E-LTP and L-LTP homosynaptically by patterned stimulation of a single input pathway (Fig. 1 A2). E-LTP is typically induced by a single high-frequency tetanus (100 Hz, 1S), whereas L-LTP is typically induced by three to four such tetanic trains (5, 6). We developed a protocol to induce transcription-dependent L-LTP by using heterosynaptic pairing (two input pathways, Fig. 1 A2). The resulting plasticity has several characteristics that make it an attractive model for learning-related synaptic plasticity. In particular, we find that the timing between the paired stimuli critically regulates the duration of synaptic plasticity. This observation mirrors the importance of timing in CS–US pairing in behavioral studies and is consistent with the heterosynaptic rules for the long-term memory and mem- ory-associated synaptic plasticity, which emerged from exper- iments in Aplysia (7, 8). Materials and Methods Transverse slices (400 m) of acutely dissected mouse hippocam- pus were prepared from C57B6 mice aged 4–8 weeks. In KCREB mice, slices from the dorsal hippocampus of littermate control and KCREB transgenic mice were used (4). Slices were maintained at 28°C in an interface chamber with continuous perfusion of artificial cerebrospinal fluid (124 mM NaCl4 mM MgSO 4 4 mM KCl1.0 mM Na 2 HPO 4 4 mM CaCl 2 26 mM NaHCO 3 10 mM D-glucose) at 1.5–2 mlmin, bubbled with 95% O 2 5% CO 2 . In all experiments, the GABAergic inhibitor picrotoxin (20 M) was present in the perfusion solution throughout the experiment. Experiments were started at least 2 h after the slice dissection. Extracellular recordings were made by using artificial cerebrospinal fluid glass electrodes (1–3 M). Field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs) were recorded in the Schaffer collateral–CA1 pathway of the hippocampus. Stimuli were delivered through bipolar stainless steel electrodes. Two stimulating electrodes were placed on either side of the recording electrode in stratum radiatum (Fig. 1 A). Stim 1 was used as the test pathway, and Stim 2 was used as the conditioning pathway. The two afferent pathways were considered indepen- dent if no paired-pulse facilitation occurred when two stimuli were coupled in a 50-ms interval. For the measurement of the baseline synaptic response, 0.033-Hz (0.05-ms pulse duration) stimulation was used. In two pathway experiments, Stim 1 and Stim 2 were delivered with the same frequency (0.033 Hz) 10 S apart except during pairing. In other experiment, baseline EPSPs were sampled only in the test pathway (Stim 1) and Stim 2 was turned on only during the pairing. Results Pairing Single Volleys with Brief Conditioning Bursts in an Indepen- dent Pathway Induces Heterosynaptic L-LTP. We induced long- lasting synaptic potentiation in mouse hippocampal slices by Abbreviations: L-LTP, late phase of long-term synaptic potentiation; E-LTP, early-phase LTP; CREB, adenosine 35-cyclic monophosphate response element-binding protein; STDP, spike timing-dependent synaptic plasticity; NMDA, N-methyl-D-aspartate; EPSP, excitatory postsynaptic potential; APV, 2-amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid; VGCC, voltage-gated calcium channels. *Present address: Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, 25 Park Street, New Haven, CT 06519. To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected]. © 2004 by The National Academy of Sciences of the USA www.pnas.orgcgidoi10.1073pnas.2237201100 PNAS January 20, 2004 vol. 101 no. 3 859 – 864 NEUROSCIENCE

Transcript of A form of long-lasting, learning-related synaptic ... · PDF fileA form of long-lasting,...

A form of long-lasting, learning-related synapticplasticity in the hippocampus induced byheterosynaptic low-frequency pairingYan-You Huang, Christopher Pittenger*, and Eric R. Kandel†

Center for Neurobiology and Behavior, Howard Hughes Medical Institute, College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, 1051 Riverside Drive,New York, NY 10032

Contributed by Eric R. Kandel, November 10, 2003

The late, transcription- and translation-dependent phase of long-term synaptic potentiation (L-LTP) at the Schaffer collateral syn-apse of the hippocampus is an experimental model of the synapticplasticity underlying long-lasting memory formation. L-LTP is typ-ically induced by homosynaptic tetanic stimulation; but associativeforms of learning are likely to require the heterosynaptic pairing ofstimuli. Here we describe L-LTP elicited by such heterosynapticpairing at the Schaffer collateral synapse in mice. We find thatrepeated stimulation of one pathway at low frequency (0.2 Hz),which does not by itself induce synaptic potentiation, will producelong-lasting synaptic plasticity when paired with a brief condition-ing burst applied to an independent afferent pathway. The induc-tion of heterosynaptic L-LTP is associative and critically depends onthe precise time interval of pairing: simultaneous, conjunctionalpairing induces L-LTP; in contrast, delayed pairing induces short-lasting early-phase LTP. Heterosynaptically induced early-phaseLTP could be depotentiated by repeatedly presenting unpaired teststimuli, whereas L-LTP could not. This heterosynaptically inducedL-LTP requires PKA and protein synthesis. In addition, heterosyn-aptically induced L-LTP is impaired in transgenic mice that expressKCREB (a dominant negative inhibitor of adenosine 3�5�-cyclicmonophosphate response element-binding protein-mediated tran-scription) in the hippocampus. These mice have previously beenshown to be impaired in spatial memory but have normal L-LTP asinduced by a conventional homosynaptic tetanic protocol. Thesedata suggest that at least in some instances this L-LTP-inducingprotocol may better model behaviorally relevant information stor-age and the in vivo mechanisms underlying long-lasting memories.

Long-lasting synaptic change is widely believed to underliemany forms of long-lasting memory (1), but it is increasingly

clear that a synapse can support mechanistically distinct forms oflong-lasting synaptic plasticity. For example, distinct forms oflate-phase long-term synaptic potentiation (L-LTP) at the Schaf-fer collateral synapse of the hippocampus differ in their depen-dence on brain-derived neurotrophic factor (2, 3) and perhaps ontheir dependence on the transcription factor adenosine 3�5�-cyclic monophosphate response element-binding protein(CREB) (4). In light of this observation, it is critically importantto determine which forms of experimentally induced synapticplasticity best recapitulate the endogenous mechanisms of in-formation storage recruited in the hippocampus and in otherstructures by different forms of learning.

LTP can be separated into at least two temporal phases:early-phase LTP (E-LTP), which is independent of macromo-lecular synthesis and decays to baseline within 1–3 h of induction,and L-LTP, which requires transcription and translation and canpersist for hours or days. Most studies of plasticity induce bothE-LTP and L-LTP homosynaptically by patterned stimulation ofa single input pathway (Fig. 1 A2). E-LTP is typically induced bya single high-frequency tetanus (100 Hz, 1S), whereas L-LTPis typically induced by three to four such tetanic trains (5, 6).We developed a protocol to induce transcription-dependentL-LTP by using heterosynaptic pairing (two input pathways,

Fig. 1 A2). The resulting plasticity has several characteristicsthat make it an attractive model for learning-related synapticplasticity. In particular, we find that the timing between thepaired stimuli critically regulates the duration of synapticplasticity. This observation mirrors the importance of timingin CS–US pairing in behavioral studies and is consistent withthe heterosynaptic rules for the long-term memory and mem-ory-associated synaptic plasticity, which emerged from exper-iments in Aplysia (7, 8).

Materials and MethodsTransverse slices (400 �m) of acutely dissected mouse hippocam-pus were prepared from C57�B6 mice aged 4–8 weeks. InKCREB mice, slices from the dorsal hippocampus of littermatecontrol and KCREB transgenic mice were used (4). Slices weremaintained at 28°C in an interface chamber with continuousperfusion of artificial cerebrospinal f luid (124 mM NaCl�4 mMMgSO4�4 mM KCl�1.0 mM Na2HPO4�4 mM CaCl2�26 mMNaHCO3�10 mM D-glucose) at 1.5–2 ml�min, bubbled with 95%O2�5% CO2. In all experiments, the GABAergic inhibitorpicrotoxin (20 �M) was present in the perfusion solutionthroughout the experiment. Experiments were started at least 2 hafter the slice dissection. Extracellular recordings were made byusing artificial cerebrospinal f luid glass electrodes (1–3 M�).Field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs) were recordedin the Schaffer collateral–CA1 pathway of the hippocampus.Stimuli were delivered through bipolar stainless steel electrodes.Two stimulating electrodes were placed on either side of therecording electrode in stratum radiatum (Fig. 1 A). Stim 1 wasused as the test pathway, and Stim 2 was used as the conditioningpathway. The two afferent pathways were considered indepen-dent if no paired-pulse facilitation occurred when two stimuliwere coupled in a 50-ms interval. For the measurement of thebaseline synaptic response, 0.033-Hz (0.05-ms pulse duration)stimulation was used. In two pathway experiments, Stim 1 andStim 2 were delivered with the same frequency (0.033 Hz) 10 Sapart except during pairing. In other experiment, baseline EPSPswere sampled only in the test pathway (Stim 1) and Stim 2 wasturned on only during the pairing.

ResultsPairing Single Volleys with Brief Conditioning Bursts in an Indepen-dent Pathway Induces Heterosynaptic L-LTP. We induced long-lasting synaptic potentiation in mouse hippocampal slices by

Abbreviations: L-LTP, late phase of long-term synaptic potentiation; E-LTP, early-phase LTP;CREB, adenosine 3�5�-cyclic monophosphate response element-binding protein; STDP,spike timing-dependent synaptic plasticity; NMDA, N-methyl-D-aspartate; EPSP, excitatorypostsynaptic potential; APV, 2-amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid; VGCC, voltage-gatedcalcium channels.

*Present address: Department of Psychiatry, Yale University, 25 Park Street, New Haven,CT 06519.

†To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected].

© 2004 by The National Academy of Sciences of the USA

www.pnas.org�cgi�doi�10.1073�pnas.2237201100 PNAS � January 20, 2004 � vol. 101 � no. 3 � 859–864

NEU

ROSC

IEN

CE

pairing low-frequency stimulation of a test pathway (0.2 Hz) withbursts of stimuli, also delivered at low frequency, in a second,conditioning pathway (Fig. 1 A). Such paired stimulation of twodistinct afferent pathways has previously been used to induceE-LTP, as has pairing of low-frequency stimulation of a singleafferent pathway with depolarization of the postsynaptic cell

(9, 10). The magnitude and valence of such pairing-inducedplasticity sensitively depend on the precise relative timing of thepaired stimuli; by changing the interval between paired stimuli,different levels of E-LTP or synaptic depression (LTD) can beproduced with identical electrical stimuli. This phenomenon hasbeen termed spike timing-dependent plasticity (STDP) (11–17).

Fig. 1. Pairing of 0.2-Hz stimuli in a test pathway with conditioning bursts in an independent afferent pathway produces L-LTP at the Schaffer collateral synapse.(A1) Schematic representation of a transverse slice through the hippocampus, showing the positioning of two stimulating electrodes and a single recordingelectrode in stratum radiatum of the CA1 cell field. (A2) Schematic representation of a single CA1 pyramidal cell, showing the position of two stimulatingelectrodes and one recording electrode in stratum radiatum. (B) Test pulses alone do not alter synaptic strength. Sixty test pulses were delivered to test pathwayS1 at 0.2 Hz, with no stimulation in the conditioning pathway S2, as illustrated (Right). This produced no change in synaptic strength in either the test pathway(Top Right) or the simultaneously recorded conditioning pathway. Test stimuli produced simple postsynaptic EPSPs (Top Right, calibration, 20 ms, 2 mV).Independence of S1 and S2 was defined as the absence of paired-pulse facilitation of S1 by S2 with a 50-ms interval (data not shown). Stimulation in both pathwayswas at an intensity that produced 30–50% of the maximum postsynaptic response before any potentiation. (C) Asynchronous pairing of 60 test stimuli withconditioning bursts, as illustrated in Inset (S1–S2 interval, 100 ms) produced no potentiation in the test pathway (94 � 3% at 1 h after pairing, n � 5, Upper) butdid produce L-LTP in the simultaneously recorded conditioning pathway (162 � 12%, n � 5). Conditioning bursts produced a more complex EPSP (B Middle Right);asynchronous pairing produced both waveforms with no apparent summation of the two (B Bottom Right, calibration, 140 ms, 2 mV). (D) After a 30-min stablebaseline, conjunctional pairing, with an S1–S2 interval of 0.1 ms, produced L-LTP in both the test pathway (159 � 13% at 3 h, n � 5, Left) and the conditioningpathway (141 � 10%, n � 5, Right). This stimulation produced a more complex waveform, in which the S1 and S2 waveforms summated (Left Inset, calibrationbars, 2 mV, 30 ms).

860 � www.pnas.org�cgi�doi�10.1073�pnas.2237201100 Huang et al.

However, an STDP-like phenomenon has not previously beendemonstrated for protein synthesis-dependent long-lasting plas-ticity (L-LTP).

We stimulated two independent afferent pathways, a testpathway (S1) and a conditioning pathway (S2), in stratumradiatum of acute hippocampal slices and recorded the postsyn-aptic responses in a population of postsynaptic CA1 neurons(Fig. 1 A1). Test pulses delivered at 0.2 Hz to S1 (after a 30-minbaseline) produced no change in the synaptic strength of either

recorded pathway (Fig. 1B); these test pulses produced a brief,simple EPSP (Fig. 1B Top Right). When such test pulses wereasynchronously paired 60 times with brief conditioning bursts inthe second afferent pathway (the conditioning pathway, S2;conditioning bursts consisted of three pulses at 100 Hz; 100-msinterval between the S1 pulse and the first pulse in S2), theconditioning pathway was potentiated but the test pathway wasnot affected; that is, the two pathways remained independent ofone another (Fig. 1C). Conditioning bursts produced a longer

Fig. 2. Pairing-induced L-LTP depends on the NMDA receptor, PKA, and a brief S1–S2 interval. (A) Stimulation and pairing was repeated in the presence of theNMDA receptor blocker APV (50 �M). This did not affect the EPSP to test stimulus S1 but did truncate the late phase of response to conditioning burst S2 (LeftInset, S1 and S2 were presented asynchronously, with a 100-ms S1–S2 interval), indicating that this conditioning burst activates the NMDA receptor. The complexEPSP response to conjunctional pairing of S1 and S2 was both truncated and reduced in amplitude in the presence of APV (Right Inset, calibration, 2 mV, 50 ms).When the conjunctional pairing protocol was applied, as in Fig. 1A, APV eliminated the production of any early or L-LTP (96 � 4% at 100 min after pairing, n �6). (B) VGCCs inhibitor blocked the L-LTP. In the presence of inhibitor nifidipine (15 �M), the LTP at 1 h was not affected, but L-LTP at 3 h was significantly reduced(113 � 10%, n � 5 for each condition, P � 0.01). (C) Conjunctional pairing again produced L-LTP (upper graph; conditions identical to Fig. 1D; 168 � 10% baseline3 h after pairing; n � 6). Delayed pairing, with a S1-S2 interval of 20 ms during pairing but with otherwise identical stimulation, produced only E-LTP (lower graph;112 � 16% baseline 3 h after pairing; n � 6). (D) Both of these forms of LTP require PKA. The PKA inhibitor KT5720 (1 �M, perfusion for 2 h) truncated the L-LTPproduced by conjunctional pairing to E-LTP [upper, KT5720 (F), 93 � 5% 3 h after pairing; control (�), 168 � 11%; n � 7 for each condition; P � 0.01]. KT5720also reduced the E-LTP produced by delayed pairing [lower, KT5720 (�), 109 � 14% 40 min after pairing; control, 154 � 15%; n � 7 for each condition, P � 0.01].

Huang et al. PNAS � January 20, 2004 � vol. 101 � no. 3 � 861

NEU

ROSC

IEN

CE

and more complex EPSP (Fig. 1B Middle Right); asynchronouspresentation of S1 test pulses and S2 conditioning bursts pro-duced two distinct, nonoverlapping EPSPs (Fig. 1B BottomRight).

Delivering test pulses to the test pathway (S1) and bursts to theconditioning pathway (S2) separated by only 0.1 ms (conjunc-tional pairing) produced a complex EPSP (Fig. 1D Inset).Repeating this pairing 60 times produced long-lasting potenti-ation of both the test pathway (Fig. 1D Left) and the conditioningpathway (Fig. 1D Right). The robust potentiation of the testpathway is striking because this pathway never saw stimulationmore intense than the 0.2-Hz test pulses that, by themselves,produce no potentiation (Fig. 1B). Furthermore, the total elec-trical stimulation seen by the postsynaptic neurons is identical inasynchronous and conjunctional pairing; only the relative timingof test pulses and conditioning bursts differs between the twoconditions (compare Fig. 1C Upper with Fig. 1D Left). Thepotentiation produced in the test pathway by repeated conjunc-tional pairing lasts �3 h, demonstrating that such a heterosyn-aptic pairing protocol can produce L-LTP.

L-LTP Produced Heterosynaptically Depends on Activation of theN-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) Receptor, PKA, and Protein Synthesis.We found that L-LTP produced by pairing depends on theNMDA receptor. The NMDA blocker 2-amino-5-phosphonova-leric acid (APV) had no effect on the EPSP produced bystimulation of S1 but attenuated the more complex EPSPproduced by a conditioning burst in S2 (Fig. 2A Left Inset). APValso attenuated the EPSP produced in response to conjunctionalpairing (Fig. 2 A Right Inset). This shows that the postsynapticresponse to conditioning bursts in pathway S2 has a significantNMDA-dependent component and thus is likely to include

significant calcium influx. Pairing in the presence of APVproduced no E-LTP or L-LTP (Fig. 2 A).

STDP depends on both the NMDA receptor and voltage-gated calcium channels (VGCCs) (12–17). To test whether thiswas true of the L-LTP produced by conjunctional pairing, weperformed the pairing protocol in the presence of the VGCCinhibitor nifedipine (15 �M). This treatment did not affect LTPat 1 h, but it significantly reduced L-LTP at 3 h (Fig. 2B). Thus,pairing-induced L-LTP, like STDP, requires both the NMDAreceptor and VGCCs. Interestingly, only the late phase ofpairing-induced LTP requires VGCCs. This finding contrastswith STDP, in which E-LTP depends on VGCCs (12–16) butparallels the requirement for VGCCs of some forms of L-LTPproduced by homosynaptic tetanization (18).

We found that changing the interstimulus interval dramati-cally affected the plasticity produced by this protocol. Onceagain, repeated pairing (that is, 60 pairings of S1 test stimuli andS2 conditioning bursts with a 0.1-ms interval between the teststimulus and the first impulse of the conditioning burst) pro-duced robust L-LTP (Fig. 2C Upper, replicating Fig. 1D Left).However, when the interval between S1 and S2 was increased to20 ms (delayed pairing), potentiation of comparable initialmagnitude was produced but decayed to values indistinguishablefrom baseline within 90 min; that is, delayed pairing producedonly E-LTP (Fig. 2C Lower).

Many forms of L-LTP and some forms of E-LTP (6, 19, 20)require the cAMP-dependent kinase PKA, which also is criticalin a variety of invertebrate model systems to relay the synapticstimuli required for long-lasting plasticity to nuclear events. Inaddition, disruption of PKA in genetically modified animalsdisrupts hippocampus-dependent learning and long-lasting al-terations in the hippocampal representation of space (6). It is

Fig. 3. Pairing-induced L-LTP depends on protein synthesis and on CREB-mediated transcription. (A) Conjunctional and delayed pairing were performed in thepresence of the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin (20 �M, applied 20 min before until 90 min after pairing). This truncated L-LTP produced by conjunctionalpairing [Upper, anisomycin (F), 118 � 18% baseline 3 h after pairing; control (�), 168 � 10%; n � 7 for each condition; P � 0.01]. However, anisomycin had noeffect on E-LTP produced by delayed pairing (Lower). (B) Pairing-induced L-LTP, but not high-frequency-induced L-LTP in the conditioning pathway, dependson CREB. Conjunctional pairing was induced in slices from dorsal hippocampus of KCREB-expressing transgenic mice (4) and littermate controls. Earlypotentiation in the test pathway was not altered in transgenic mice, but L-LTP was reduced [Upper, KCREB (�), 118 � 6% baseline 3 h after pairing; littermatecontrol (F), 150 � 11%; n � 8 for each genotype; P � 0.05]. L-LTP was normal in the conditioning pathway (Lower). (Inset) Representative EPSPs before and 3 hafter pairing in control (Left) and KCREB (Right) mice. (Calibration, 2 mV, 10 ms.)

862 � www.pnas.org�cgi�doi�10.1073�pnas.2237201100 Huang et al.

therefore likely that the forms of hippocampal plasticity under-lying long-term information storage require PKA. To seewhether this is true of pairing-induced L-LTP, we performed ourpairing and delayed-pairing protocols in the presence and ab-sence of the PKA inhibitor KT5720 (1 �M). We found that boththe L-LTP produced by conjunctional pairing and the E-LTPproduced by delayed pairing were attenuated by PKA blockade;pairing-induced L-LTP of longer than 1 h was entirely eliminated(Fig. 2D). Both these forms of potentiation thus require PKA. Ithas been suggested that E-LTP as conventionally produced bytetanization avoids such a dependence on PKA because itsaturates the elevation of cAMP and that gentler methods ofinduction are therefore more susceptible to PKA inhibition (19).By this hypothesis, the dependence of E-LTP as produced bydelayed pairing on PKA is therefore consistent with its beinginduced by less robust, and possibly more physiological, stimu-lation than the robust homosynaptic stimulation more conven-tionally used to produce tetanus-induced E-LTP.

The defining feature of L-LTP is its requirement for geneinduction and macromolecular synthesis. This requirement par-allels the known dependence of various forms of long-lastingmemory on macromolecular synthesis (21–23). To test thedependence of pairing-induced L-LTP on new protein produc-tion, we performed pairing in the presence and absence of theprotein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin (20 �M). We found thatanisomycin eliminated the late phase of pairing-induced L-LTP,although it did not affect the early phase (Fig. 3A Upper). Incontrast, E-LTP produced by delayed pairing was not affected byanisomycin (Fig. 3A Lower). This finding confirms the mecha-nistic distinction first made in Fig. 2 A, whereas delayed pairingproduces only E-LTP, a small change in interstimulus intervalcauses conjunctional pairing to produce L-LTP, as defined bothby duration and by pharmacological criteria.

Heterosynaptcally Induced L-LTP Requires CREB-Mediated Gene In-duction. Interference with the transcription factor CREB in areaCA1 of the hippocampus disrupts long-term hippocampus-dependent memory but not standard forms of hippocampallong-lasting potentiation (4). To examine whether disruption ofCREB-mediated gene induction would interfere with pairing-induced L-LTP, we examined LTP in slices from the dorsalhippocampus of transgenic mice expressing KCREB, a potentdominant inhibitor of CREB-family transcription factors, in areaCA1. We have shown that these mice are deficient in hippocam-pus-dependent memory tasks but not in homosynaptic tetanus-induced forms of L-LTP (4). We found that inhibition ofCREB-mediated transcription by KCREB significantly attenu-ated the L-LTP produced by conjunctional pairing (Fig. 3BUpper). In contrast, it did not affect the L-LTP produced inpathway S2, to which repeated conditioning bursts were applied(Fig. 3B Lower). This recapitulates the lack of effect of thistransgene on L-LTP produced by more robust synaptic stimu-lation and emphasizes that pairing-induced L-LTP is mechanis-tically distinct. Because KCREB-expressing animals have aproven deficit in long-term hippocampus-dependent memory,our observation supports the idea that pairing-induced L-LTPmay be a better model for the endogenous mechanisms of certainforms of long-term memory storage than homosynaptic, high-frequency protocols. Correlation between electrophysiology andother forms of learning will clearly be required to furthersubstantiate this hypothesis.

Heterosynaptically Induced LTP Is Partially Depotentiated by Subse-quent Unpaired Test Stimuli. Many forms of synaptic potentiationcan be reversed, or depotentiated, by subsequent low-frequencystimulation. LTP produced by a 100-Hz tetanus is depotentiatedby subsequent stimulation at 1–5 Hz, if it is applied within 10 minof the induction of potentiation (24, 25). However, this and

similar examples use dramatically different stimulation for in-duction of LTP and its depotentiation. Because pairing-inducedLTP is produced by low-frequency stimulation in the testpathway, in conjunction with conditioning bursts in the condi-tioning pathway, we wondered whether constant stimulation ofthe test pathway, first with conditioning bursts and then alone,could lead first to the induction of potentiation and then to itsreversal. We found that when 60 pairings, by using the delayedprotocol (20-ms interstimulus), were immediately followed by 60unpaired stimuli to the test pathway at 0.2 Hz, the inducedpotentiation was rapidly depotentiated (Fig. 4A). In contrast, 60

Fig. 4. Pairing-induced E-LTP, but not L-LTP, can be depotentiated bysubsequent unpaired test stimuli. (A) Delayed pairing (20 ms) was immediatelyfollowed by 60 more unpaired test stimuli to pathway S1 at 0.2 Hz. Thistreatment truncated and reduced the E-LTP [depotentiated (F), 112 � 11% 40min after pairing; control, 147 � 9%; n � 7 for each condition; P � 0.05]. (Inset)Regular E-LTP (Left) and depotentiated E-LTP (Right) 40 min after pairing(calibration, 2 mV, 10 ms). (B) Depotentiation was ineffective 5 min afterdelayed pairing; 60 unpaired stimuli were delivered to the test pathway S1 5min after delayed pairing. E-LTP was not significantly reduced by this treat-ment (130 � 6% 40 min after pairing; control is the same as in A; n � 6; P � 0.5).(C) Conjunctional pairing-induced L-LTP cannot be depotentiated by thistreatment; 60 unpaired stimuli were applied to test pathway S1 immediatelyafter the induction of L-LTP by conjunctional pairing (0.1 ms). This treatmentdid not significantly reduce the LTP (n � 4 for each condition).

Huang et al. PNAS � January 20, 2004 � vol. 101 � no. 3 � 863

NEU

ROSC

IEN

CE

unpaired stimuli delivered 5 min after delayed pairing were farless effective at depotentiation (Fig. 4B), confirming that forpairing-induced potentiation, as for tetanus-induced potentia-tion, a critical window exists within which depotentiation ispossible. These data show that the same low-frequency synapticstimulation can both potentiate and depotentiate this synapse,depending on the co-occurrence of other synaptic activity in thepostsynaptic neuron. In contrast to the depotentiation of E-LTPproduced by delayed pairing, the potentiation induced by con-junctional pairing could not be depotentiated by subsequentunpaired test stimuli (Fig. 4C).

DiscussionBy using a modified STDP-like protocol to produce L-LTP, wehave described a previously univestigated form of experimentalplasticity that has several important characteristics. First, wehave shown that heterosynaptic pairing can produce translationand transcription-dependent L-LTP. Second, variations in theinterstimulus interval in our protocol control the duration ofsynaptic change. Finally, LTP induced by pairing can be, at leastpartially, depotentiated by the repeated application of unpairedtest stimuli immediately after pairing, rather than by the appli-cation of a dramatically different stimulation for depotentiationas has been described previously.

A particularly interesting and important characteristic ofpairing-induced plasticity is its heterosynaptic nature. Specifi-cally, the potentiation produced at one population of synapses(S1) depends critically on the activity at other, independentsynapses (S2). Such cooperativity between different synapses onthe same postsynaptic neuron may be important for associativememory; indeed, it is central to the idea of a Hebbian synapse(26). The ability to model this feature for the transcription andtranslation-dependent long-lasting synaptic plasticity in a simplein vitro preparation is an important experimental advance.

Might this experimental paradigm better recapitulate theendogenous mechanisms of memory storage? Several consider-ations suggest that it does. First, the stimulation used to induceL-LTP by the pairing protocol is far less intense, and thereforepossibly more physiological, than the tetanization conventionallyused to induce L-LTP. This is true even in comparison withtheta-burst protocols, because the stimulation at the potentiatedtest pathway (S1) consists simply of single pulses delivered at 0.2Hz. Second, the precise timing of stimulation to the two afferentpathways in this pairing protocol critically regulates the durationof LTP; as has been explored in the discussion of STDP, suchsensitive dependence on spike timing may be an importantcharacteristic of neural networks underlying associate learningand memory (11).

Third, the L-LTP produced by pairing requires several pro-cesses that have independently been shown to be required forhippocampus-dependent learning, including activation of theNMDA receptor, PKA signaling, and protein synthesis. Inparticular, pairing-induced L-LTP is impaired in transgenic micewith inhibited CREB and a deficit in long-lasting hippocampus-dependent memory; tetanus-induced L-LTP is normal in thesemice (as is LTP induced directly by conditioning bursts in theconditioning pathway, S2, in our experiments; Fig. 3B Bottom).This correlation suggests that, if the memory deficit in theseanimals is indeed due to a disruption of hippocampal synapticplasticity, pairing-induced L-LTP may be a better model for theimportant disrupted processes than are more conventional formsof L-LTP. Pairing-induced L-LTP merits close attention as a newin vitro model for the synaptic processes that underlie hippocam-pus-dependent learning and memory.

We thank Matthew Nolan for critically reading this manuscript. Thiswork was supported by the Howard Hughes Medical Institute, the NewYork State Psychiatric Institute, and National Institute of Mental HealthGrant 5P50-MH50733 (to E.R.K.).

1. Martin, S. J., Grimwood, P. D. & Morris, R. G. (2000) Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 23,649–711.

2. Kang, H, Welcher, A. A., Shelton, D. & Schuman, E. M. (1997) Neuron 19,653–664.

3. Patterson, S. L., Pittenger, C., Morozov, A., Martin, K. C., Scanlin, H., Drake,C. & Kandel, E. R. (2001) Neuron 32, 123–140.

4. Pittenger, C., Huang, Y.-Y., Paletzki, R. F., Bourtchouladze, R., Scanlin, H.,Vronskaya, S. & Kandel, E. R. (2002) Neuron 34, 447–462.

5. Huang, Y.-Y., Nguyen, P. V., Abel, T. & Kandel E. R. (1996) Learn. Mem. 3,74–85.

6. Abel, T., Nguyen P. V., Barad, M., Deuel, T. A., Kandel, E. R. & Bourtchou-ladze, R. (1997) Cell 88, 615–626.

7. Kandel, E. R. & Tauc, L. (1965) J. Physiol. 181, 1–27.8. Kandel, E. R. & Tauc, L. (1965) J. Physiol. 181, 28–47.9. Gustafsson, B. & Wigstrom H. (1986) J. Neurosci. 6, 1575–1582.

10. Gustafsson, B., Wigstrom, H., Abraham, W. C. & Huang, Y.-Y. (1987)J. Neurosci. 7, 774–780.

11. Bi, G.-Q. & Poo, M.-M. (2001) Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 139–166.12. Bi, G.-Q. and Poo, M.-M. (1998) J. Neurosci. 18, 10464–10472.

13. Markram, H., Lubke, J., Frotscher, M. & Sakmann, B. (1997) Science 275, 213–215.14. Froemke, R. C. & Dan, Y. (2002) Nature 416, 433–438.15. Feldman, D. E. (2000) Neuron 27, 45–56.16. Magee, J. C. & Johnston, D. (1997) Science 275, 209–213.17. Lin, Y.-W., Min, M.-Y., Chiu, T.-H. & Yang, H.-W. (2003) J. Neurosci. 23,

4173–4181.18. Impey, S, Mark, M., Villacres, E. C., Poser, S., Chavkin, C. & Storm, D. R.

(1996) Neuron 16, 973–982.19. Otmakhova, N. A., Otmakhov, N., Mortenson, L. H. & Lisman, J. E. (2000)

J. Neurosci. 20, 4446–4451.20. Huang, Y.-Y. & Kandel, E. R. (1994) Learn. Mem. 1, 74–82.21. Davis, H. P. & Squire, L. R. (1984) Psychol. Bull. 96, 518–559.22. Freeman, F. M., Rose, S. P. & Scholey, A. B. (1995) Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.

63, 291–295.23. Bourtchouladze, R., Abel, T., Berman, N., Gordon, R., Lapidus, K. & Kandel,

E. R. (1998) Learn. Mem. 5, 365–374.24. O’Dell, T. J. & Kandel, E. R. (1994) Learn. Mem. 1, 129–139.25. Huang, C. C. & Hsu, K. S. (2001) Rev. Neurosci. 12, 51–68.26. Hawkins, R. D., Kandel, E. R. & Sigelbaum, S. A. (1993) Annu. Rev. Neurosci.

16, 625–665.

864 � www.pnas.org�cgi�doi�10.1073�pnas.2237201100 Huang et al.