A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony of Testi… · A Brief History of the Psychology of...

17
Current Psychological Reviews (1982), 2, 323-340 A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony SIEGFRIED LUDWIG SPORER Universitiit Erlangen-Niirnberg, FR Germany The historiography of psychology has largely ignored the history of applied aspects of its field. Moreover, contemporary legal psychologists have often overlooked previous related work. The present paper attempts partially to fill these gaps by providing a briel description of the history ol the psychology of testimony at the beginning of this century, particularly in central Europe. It is argued that in central Europe, in contrast to the United States and Britain, there existed a pervasive experimental psychology of testimony. This movement probably originated with Binet in France and Stern in Germany. However, it was especially the latter and his Iollowers who succeeded in institutionalizing a 'Psychologie der Aussage' that was widely discussed in legal circles at that time. Although the early studies have often been criticized for their methodological flaws and their negativistic one- sidedness the European movement did have some belated impact in that expert psychological testimony slowly started to be admitted before courts of law. It is of special interest to the contemporary researcher that many ol the critical issues raised by early legal scholars were quite sophisticated and remain as pertinent as ever to the experimental study of testimony. One of the most important areas in the new legat psychology is the psychology of evidence, in particular the psychology ol eyewitness testimony (ClilIord & Bull, 1978; Loftus, 1979; Yarmey, 1979; Tapp, 1980; Lob, 1981; Monahan & Loftus, 1982). It is also one of the oldest areas. In fact, as I hope this paper shows, contemporary psychology of testimony is in many respects a renaissance of the 'Psychologie der Aussage' that flourished at the beginning of this century. While contemporary researchers generally acknowledge these early beginnings, the major purpose ol their works (i.e., to review the more sophisticated recent research) does not allow space for a detailed analysis of historical roots. Thus most authors mention earlier work only in passing (e.g., Cliflord & Bull, 1978; Loftus, 1979; Yarmey, 1979; noteworthy exceptions are Rouke, 1957; Greer, 1971; Levine & Tapp, 1973; Lob, 1981). 0144-3895/82/0203-323 $02.00 © 1982 Praeger Publishers

Transcript of A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony of Testi… · A Brief History of the Psychology of...

Page 1: A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony of Testi… · A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony SIEGFRIED LUDWIG SPORER Universitiit Erlangen-Niirnberg, FR ... works

Current Psychological Reviews (1982), 2, 323-340

A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony

SIEGFRIED LUDWIG SPORER

Universitiit Erlangen-Niirnberg, FR Germany

The h is tor iography of psychology has l a rge ly ignored the his tory of appl ied a spec t s of its f i e ld . Moreover , c o n t e m p o r a r y legal psychologis ts have o f ten ove r looked previous r e l a t ed work. The present paper a t t e m p t s pa r t i a l ly to f i l l these gaps by providing a b r ie l descr ip t ion of the h is tory ol the psychology of t e s t imony at the

beginning of this cen tu ry , pa r t i cu la r ly in c e n t r a l Europe. It is argued tha t in cen t r a l Europe, in con t ras t to the Uni ted S ta tes and Br i ta in , t he r e ex i s t ed a

pe rvas ive e x p e r i m e n t a l psychology of t e s t i m o n y . This m o v e m e n t probably o r ig ina t ed with Binet in France and Stern in Germany. However , it was e spec ia l ly the l a t t e r and

his Io l lowers who succeeded in i n s t i t u t i ona l i z ing a 'Psychologie der Aussage ' tha t was widely discussed in legal c i rc les at tha t t i m e . Although the ear ly s tudies have of ten been c r i t i c i z ed for the i r me thodo log ica l f laws and the i r n e g a t i v i s t i c one- sidedness the European movemen t did have some be la ted impac t in tha t expe r t psychologica l t e s t imony slowly s t a r t ed to be admi t t ed before cour ts of law. It is of specia l i n t e r e s t to the con tempora ry r e sea r c h e r tha t many ol the c r i t i c a l issues raised by ear ly legal scholars were qui te soph i s t i ca ted and remain as pe r t i nen t as ever to the expe r imen t a l study of t e s t i m o n y .

One of the most impor t an t a reas in the new legat psychology is the psychology of e v i d e n c e , in pa r t i cu la r the psychology ol eyewi tness t e s t imony (Cl i l Io rd & Bull, 1978; Lof tus , 1979; Yarmey, 1979; Tapp, 1980; Lob, 1981; Monahan & Lof tus , 1982). It

is also one of the oldest a reas . In f a c t , as I hope this paper shows, c o n t e m p o r a r y psychology of t e s t imony is in many respec t s a r ena i s sance of the 'Psychologie der Aussage' tha t f lourished at the beginning of this c en tu ry . While c o n t e m p o r a r y r e sea rche r s genera l ly acknowledge these ea r ly beginnings , the major purpose ol the i r works ( i . e . , to review the more soph i s t i ca t ed r ecen t r e sea rch ) does not allow space for a de ta i l ed analysis of h i s to r ica l roo t s . Thus most authors ment ion ea r l i e r work only in passing ( e . g . , Cl i f lo rd & Bull, 1978; Lof tus , 1979; Yarmey , 1979; no t ewor thy except ions are Rouke, 1957; Greer , 1971; Levine & Tapp, 1973; Lob, 1981).

0144-3895/82/0203-323 $02.00 © 1982 Praeger Publishers

Page 2: A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony of Testi… · A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony SIEGFRIED LUDWIG SPORER Universitiit Erlangen-Niirnberg, FR ... works

324 S.L. Sporer

Textbook histories of psychology as well as monographs on various aspects of the history of psychology have s imi lar ly ignored such 'applied' aspects of their f ie ld (for a detai led discussion, see Sporer, 1981). To f i l l these histor iographic gaps, the present paper (1) searches for some of the roots of psychological th inking in the writings of s e l e c t e d legal scholars and the i r d i s t i l l a t ion in fo rma l rules of e v i d e n c e , (2) t r a c e s the beginnings of the psychology of t e s t imony in Europe , and (3) assesses its re la t ionship to the legal world, and (4) r e f l e c t s on some of the issues and problems that seem to have a f f e c t e d the psychology of t e s t i m o n y (and

legal psychology in genera l ) both past and p r e s e n t . The present paper does not discuss the history of the psychology of l ie detect ion in any deta i l , fo l lowing the current pract ice to exclude in tent ional distort ions from the psychology of test imony (cf. Stern, 1939; Wells, 1980). However, i t should be noted that this separation is probably h is tor ica l ly a r t i f i c i a l as previously this aspect was also widely discussed (e .g . , in the work on 'Tatbestandsdiagnostik'; cf . Sporer, 1981).

COMMOINISENSE PSYCHOLOGY AND THE LAW~ RULES OF EVIDENCE

'Above a l l , the ident i f i ca t ion procedure has to be preceded by a comprehensive interrogat ion of the witness, wherein he is to describe the character is t ic features which could fac i l i t a te recognit ion of the persons or objects to which his test imony or statements re fer . Thereaf ter , in the iden t i f i ca t ion procedure i tse l f , he is, whenever possible, to be confronted with several persons or objects resembling the one to be ident i f ied . He should be urged to point out, for example, the iden t i f i ed object, without hesi tat ion, and also to give the reasons why he had ident i f ied this one as the real one instead of any of the others . . . On the one hand, the invest igator has to take care, to the best of his ab i l i t y , to remove any changes that may have occurred in the object to be recognized and that may thus impair recognit ion- therefore, for example, he must not present the accused in his prison clothes, or with a distor t ing beard, etc. On the other hand, the invest igator must beware of drawing the witness's a t tent ion to the correct object through fac ia l expressions, gestures, or external signs that d i f fe ren t ia te the object in question from others'. (Henke, 1838, pp. 705-706; my t ranslat ion)

This quotat ion is from a handbook of cr iminal law and cr iminal pol i t ics wr i t ten about 150 years ago! It demonstrates the sophist icat ion and degree of psychological learning one may f ind in the history of legal wri t ings based on the exper ience, in tu i t ion and logic of legal scholars. The passage also addresses some issues and problems that , through the very fact of their h istor ic recurrence, are impor tant ones worthy of the contemporary researcher's a t ten t ion .

The historiographer Thucydides, l iv ing during the Golden Age of Greece in the f i f th century BC, already knew of the problems of eyewitness test imony, not ing a 'want of coincidence between accounts of the same occurrences by d i f fe rent eye witnesses, arising sometimes from imper fect memory, sometimes from undue pa r t i a l i t y for one side or the other' (Levine & Tapp, 1973, p. I088). Two mi l lennia la te r , Oerstett (1822) took i t for granted that ' i t is easily i n te l l i g ib le that the proof through witness test imony be, as a matter of course, fa l l i b le (an sich t r~gl ich sei) and that only necessity had dictated that i t be admit ted in al l states' (p. 629; my t rans la t ion) . Many other legal scholars have shared these concerns (e .g . , Kleinschrod, 1805; Mi t te rmaier , 1834; Brauer, 184q; Bentham, 1843; Gross, 189g; cf .

Page 3: A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony of Testi… · A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony SIEGFRIED LUDWIG SPORER Universitiit Erlangen-Niirnberg, FR ... works

History of the Psychology of Testimony 325

also Schne icke r t , 1904; Wigmore, 1909; Hel lwig , 1910; Undeutsch , 1967; Gree r , 1971; Sporer , 1981).

These various scholars a t t e m p t e d to pinpoint the sources of e r ror ( e . g . , poor

l ight ing , f r igh t ) and to account for them t h e o r e t i c a l l y ( e . g . , in t e rms of subs t i tu t ion of i n f e r ences for pe rcep t ions ; supp lemen ta t i on of the pe rce ived by

fan tasy ; see 8 r aue r , 1801, and Hel lwig, 1910). There were also numerous a t t e m p t s to es tabl ish c r i t e r i a for the admiss ib i l i ty of c e r t a i n groups of wi tnesses a l though

t h e r e was room for much dispute concern ing who should be d isqual i f ied as a wi tness . Most of these a t t e m p t s at c a t e g o r i z a t i o n r e f e r r e d to what we would today call

'person var iab les ' , such as age , gender , or being a c lose r e l a t i v e (c f . Gross, 1898; Undeutsch, 1967). Other var iables used to exc lude wi tnesses , or at l eas t to d e r o g a t e the i r c r ed ib i l i t y , r e f l e c t social s t i g m a t i z a t i o n processes of those t imes ( e . g . , being of Jewish f a i t h , being an adulteress, having been i n c a r c e r a t e d ) . Many exc lus ionary rules also conta ined i n t e r e s t i n g impl ic i t psychologica l assumpt ions ( e . g . , having t e s t i f i e d against a person once be fo re and t h e r e f o r e being his enemy; c f . c r i t i c a l l y , Kle inschrod, 1805).

Depending on the legal sys tem under cons ide ra t ion ( i . e . , adversa ry or

inquis i to r ia l ) many of these psychologica l assumptions over t ime have d i s t i l l ed , r e s p e c t i v e l y , into ' rules of ev idence ' in the common law (see Gree r , 1971) and

cod i f i ca t i ons in penal and c r imina l p rocedura l codes: t h e r e are numerous examples in Mi t te rmaie r ' s (1830) t r e a t i s e on ev idence which compares the German c r imina l procedures with the English and the French sys t ems . Hence t he r e were spec i f i c age and/or gender norms for being cons idered to be c o m p e t e n t to t e s t i f y , or to swear an oa th , that varied considerably f rom one count ry to another and over t ime (see M i t t e r m a i e r , 1830; Undeutsch, 1967).

It should be noted that many of the psychologica l assumptions inheren t in these rules lend t hems e l ve s to empi r ica l t e s t i n g . Much of this psychologica l knowledge has been c o l l e c t e d in Hans Gross's w ide ly - r ead 'Handbook for Examining 3ust ices ' (1893) and in his t ex tbook on c r imina l psychology (1898). With the a d v a n c e m e n t of ' s c i e n t i f i c psychology' at the turn of the c e n t u r y , psychologis ts deve loped some of the tools to t a ck l e these problems , besides providing some genera l knowledge regarding pe rcep tua l and memory processes tha t could have helped the legal world to understand be t te r the in t r icac ies of eyewitness tes t imony. The ex ten t to which the 'new psychology' a t tempted to meet this chal lenge is discussed be low.

THE BEGINNINGS OF THE PSYCHOLOGY OF TESTIMONY

Background

By 'new' ' s c i en t i f i c ' psychology we mean psychology as a l a b o ra to ry s c i e n c e as it

had developed in the l a t t e r half of the n ine t e e n th cen tu ry , focusing p r imar i ly on processes of sensa t ion , pe rcep t ion and memory ( c f . Boring, 1950; Murphy & Kovach , 1972; Watson, 1978; Leahey, 1980). There were also o ther forms of psychology in the n ine teen th c e n t u r y , largely inspired by phi losophical and medica l t r ad i t i ons , tha t

deal t with d iverse fo rens ic issues, including those of t e s t imony ( c f . Gross, 1898; Sporer , 1981), and the re was a la rge body of f i c t i o n , popu la r -p sycho log ica l , and

medica l wri t ings tha t r evea led s t rong s o c i o - c u l t u r a l biases agains t women, ch i ldren and other subgroups (c f . Undeutsch, 1967; Sporer , 1991). It is aga ins t this genera l cu l tura l background, as well as in compar ison with the various branches of psychology ava i l ab le at the t i m e , that the e m e r g e n c e of 'appl ied psychology ' ,

Page 4: A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony of Testi… · A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony SIEGFRIED LUDWIG SPORER Universitiit Erlangen-Niirnberg, FR ... works

326 S L Sporer

p a r t i c u l a r l y t h e ' p s y c h o l o g y of t e s t i m o n y ' , as an o f f s p r i n g of t h e new e x p e r i m e n t a l p s y c h o l o g y h a s to be u n d e r s t o o d .

F~rly Studies on the Psychology of Testimony

It would be d i f f i cu l t , and probably wrong from a sound histor iographic perspect ive, to single out a specif ic person and even more problemat ic to determine a specif ic date as the origin of the exper imental study of test imony. Occasional papers bore on issues or dealt with themes that la ter were to become centra l to the psychology of testimony (e .g . , on association and memory in Germany and I taly: cf. Gross, 1898; Wigmore, 1909), and one or the other author may even have noted potent ia l impl icat ions ol their work for the cr iminal just ice system.

For example, Cat te l l (1895) invest igated certain aspects of ' incidental memory' that are relevant to the psychology of test imony. He demonstrated the unre l iab i l i t y of casual observation by posit ing questions to students about things they had recent ly seen. Bolton (1896) picked up a suggestion of Cattel l 's to conduct this sort of invest igat ion with d i f fe rent classes of people. He repl icated and extended Cattel l 's findings of the general inaccuracy of recol lect ion that seemed to occur despite the confidence some of the subjects expressed in their observations. However, in spite of remarks by these researchers regarding the potent ia l u t i l i t y of their work for the cr iminal justice system, apparent ly no one fo l lowed up thei r suggestions.

From a completely d i f ferent vantage point , the Austrian examining just ice Hans Gross had become aware of the shortcomings of test imony and the di f ferences between individual witnesses. In his dai ly experience he claimed to have examined 'wel l over #5 000' (Gross, 190#). For a long t ime, Gross had employed 'witness tests' which he used to perform rout inely with witnesses in his cr iminal invest igat ions (e .g . , he had them estimate distance, t ime, the number of coins, the age of people$ and he tested their recognit ion of people); and he had also conducted invest igat ions on problems of perception, retent ion and reproduction (Gross, 189#, ci ted in Gross, 1907). Klaussmann (1899), a jur is t , also reported on a series of ingenious techniques to test perceptual, judgemental and recogni t ion abi l i t ies of witnesses. Par t icu lar ly interest ing are his suggestions f o r facia l recogni t ion tests. Gross's favour i te demonstration exper iment was a simple event (e .g . , pouring water in one of several glasses) on the detai ls of which he questioned students, astonishing them at the inaccuracies and errors that emerged (Gross, 1898). Gross also stressed the importance of the sc ient i f ic approach to these problems, praising especial ly Ebbinghaus (1885) for his sound approach (Gross, 1898). Large portions of Gross's (1895) textbook on cr iminal psychology were devoted to general and d i f fe ren t ia l aspects of the psychology of test imony, making him an important pioneer in an area that psychology proper had not yet systemat ica l ly invest igated.

Louis William Stern and the q)sychologie der Aussage'

S t e r n and his f o l l o w e r s c a r r i e d ou t s o m e of t h e e a r l i e s t c o n t r o l l e d e x p e r i m e n t a l s t u d i e s on t h e p s y c h o l o g y of t e s t i m o n y ) and t h e y w e r e a m o n g s t t h e f i r s t e x p e r i m e n t a l p s y c h o l o g i s t s e x p l i c i t l y to s t r e s s t h e i m p o r t a n c e of t h e i r work to t h e law ( a n d o t h e r a r e a s such as p e d a g o g y , m e d i c i n e , and e v e n h i s t o r y ) . H o w e v e r , o n e of t h e

e x p e r i m e n t a l p a r a d i g m s S t e r n (1902) i n i t i a l l y e m p l o y e d was a d a p t e d f r o m A l f r e d B i n e t

Page 5: A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony of Testi… · A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony SIEGFRIED LUDWIG SPORER Universitiit Erlangen-Niirnberg, FR ... works

History of the Psychology of Testimony 327

• • • - - f l (1897, 1900) who had also noted, in 'La Suggest lb l l i te , the potent ia l impl icat ions of his experiments on suggest ib i l i ty (wi thout the use of hypnosis) for a 'science du temoi gnage':

'The questions that we are t reat ing here are so new that they shed l ight on some unnoticed, unexpected blind spots. I want to point out in passing the usefulness that could come from creating a pract ica l science of test imony by studying errors of memory, the means of recognizing them, and also ways of recognizing the signs of fact (or accuracy). This science is too important for i t not to be organized at some t ime or another'. (Binet, 1900, p. 285; t ranslat ion in Wolf, 1973~ pp. 108-109)

Binet's (1900) experiments, using elementary schoolchi ldren as subjects, w e r e

designed to invest igate the effects of various forms of questioning with d i f fe r ing degrees of suggest ib i l i ty . The results showed a considerable proport ion of wrong answers, and this led Binet to stress the indiv is ib le unity of question and answer (cf . Stern, 1902). In another series of exper iments, Binet invest igated the inf luence of a group of peers on test imony, f inding what social psychologists such as Asch were later to call 'conformity ' ef fects (cf . Haines & Vaughan, 1979). Although Binet did not fo l low through his ideas i t is worth noting that he envisaged a 'psychojudicial science' of a much more comprehensive scope, including the psychology of jurors and judges, than did Stern and his fo l lowers (Binet, 1905; Wolf, 1973).

In Berlin during 1901, Stern introduced his research programme for the 'Psychologie der Aussage': that is, the psychology of verbal report as it occurs in the law, in education, in psychiatry and even in history. (I prefer to leave the term 'Aussage' untranslated. It was used in its broadest sense, referr ing general ly to 'that funct ion which strives to bring to reproduct ion present or past rea l i t y through the ac t i v i t y of human consciousness' (Stern, 1903-1906, vol. l , p. l ; my t rans la t ion) . Thus, neither the term 'report ' (Whipple, 1909) nor ' test imony' seems a wholly adequate translat ion although I have used the la t te r term interchangeably, at least in the legal contex t . ) Of special theoret ica l interest was the exper imental study of reco l lect ion. Stern described his recol lect ion experiments in which subjects were to report on pictures af ter studying them ('Bildversuch', usually translated as 'picture test ' ) . The obtained findings led him, perhaps too hast i ly , to conclude with the often cited adage: 'Error- f ree recol lect ion is not the rule but the exception - and even the oath is no protect ion against deceptions of memory' (Stern, 1902, p. 327). Stern was quick to point out the pract ica l impl icat ions of his alarming findings. A more cautious evaluat ion of people's accounts seemed appropr iate, especial ly in a legal context . One possibi l i ty , Stern suggested, would be to have key witnesses examined by psychological experts (or psychological ly t rained jurists) to assess their character is t ic degree of recol lect ion ab i l i t y , even in 'normal' cases, given that the current methods had been developed fur ther and been made more re l iab le. It should be noted that Stern did not phrase his suggestions as demands, but rather formulated them as problem areas which he hoped further empir ica l invest igat ion could solve.

Stern also outl ined a gamut of factors to be invest igated, for example the effects of longer retent ion intervals, various types of questioning and of hearsay. Of special interest was his suggestion to study memory for events exper imenta l l y , ei ther presented as ~ilms or pictures or as wel l -rehearsed and programmed dramas, in order to be able to compare the contents of the report wi th that of the event (Stern~ 1902). The drama experiment ( 'Wirkl ichkeitsversuch', that is l i t e ra l l y

Page 6: A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony of Testi… · A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony SIEGFRIED LUDWIG SPORER Universitiit Erlangen-Niirnberg, FR ... works

328 S. L. Sporer

' real i ty exper iment ' , usually t ranslated as 'event test ') was f i rs t carr ied out in the same year in the cr iminal ists ' seminar of yon Liszt , the legal scholar and well-known cr iminologist at Berlin (von Liszt, 1902). Another lawyer , 3affa (1903), has described the Liszt drama exper iment , in which an audience was asked to report on a staged event they had seen, using ' free narrat ive ' . Some groups of par t ic ipants were interrogated af ter several days; a proport ion of these were also asked leading and suggestive questions. Results indicated a general super ior i ty in accuracy for the 'free narrat ive ' compared with the ' in terrogatory ' condi t ion, especial ly i f the la t ter was tainted by suggestion. Earl ier reports were more accurate than delayed ones.

In the following years 'reality experiments' were quite fashionable, both as

instruments of scientific investigations and as demonstration experiments in university law courses; even the participants of an interdisciplinary congress of

jurists, medical experts, psychiatrists and psychologists fell prey to the investigatory zeal of this new field (cf. Stern, 1903-1906). And during the

renaissance of the psychology of testimony this research paradigm, aided by the advances in film and video technology, is again frequently used (e.g., Buckhout,

197#; Marshall, 1980; cf . Wells, 1980). Stern's ca l l for an i n t e r d i s c i p l i n a r y approach to the p rob lem of

'Aussagepsychologie' received a response from many psychologists, jur ists, teachers, psychiatrists and others. A boom of empir ica l invest igat ions fo l lowed and they tackled a great var iety of issues (e .g . , the educabi l i ty of 'Aussagen', Borst, 1905; the ab i l i t y of a panel of judges and one of psychologists to arr ive at the ' t ruth' on the basis of witness test imony, Kobler, 1914; cf. Stern, 1908, and Sporer, 1981). These studies also employed a diverse array of methods, f rom laboratory and f ie ld experiments to case studies, and included quant i ta t ive and qua l i ta t i ve analyses of errors and confidence indices (cf . Lipmann, 1935).

Stern was also instrumental in the ins t i tu t iona l i za t ion of this new f ie ld in Germany. He coined the term 'angewandte Psychologie' ( 'applied psychology'; Stern, 1903-1906; Dorsch, 1963) and provided a new publ icat ion out let and a common forum for discussion for the numerous 'Aussage' studies in his 'Beitr~ige zur Psychologie der Aussage' (Stern, 1903-1906). In 1907, the 'Beitr/ ige' were transformed and expanded into the 'Zei tschr i f t f i i r angewandte Psychologie und psychologische Sammelforschung' under the editorship of Stern and Lipmann. Otto Lipmann became an important f igure in the movement of applied psychology, and succeeded Stern as a direc tor of the ' Ins t i tu t f~]r Angewandte Psychologie ' in 1916. A prol i f ic w r i t e r , Lipmann also published ex tens ive ly on the psychology of t e s t imony : for example , on the 'psychology of the lie' (Lipmann & P lau t , 1927) and on methodologica l issues in the psychology of t es t imony (Lipmann, 1935).

FURTHER DE~r.LOPMENTS

Stern and Lipmann were not the only psychologists interested in the psychology of test imony. Other researchers picked up these ideas in Germany and elsewhere, the most notable being Karl Marbe at Wi3rzburg, Edouard Claparede in Switzer land, and Hugo Mtinsterberg in the USA (cf . Dorsch, 1963). In addi t ion, Wigmore (1909) mentioned that in Russia, India and even in Chile some interest was shown. Binet in France, however, seemed less successful in establishing the psychology of test imony and expanding i t into a 'psychojudicial science' (Binet, 1905; Wolf, 1973).

Page 7: A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony of Testi… · A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony SIEGFRIED LUDWIG SPORER Universitiit Erlangen-Niirnberg, FR ... works

History of the Psychology of Testimony 329

The f i rst decade of the psychology of test imony brought for th a large amount of exper imental research by psychologists, physicians, psychiatr ists, pedagogues and even jurists. General factors such as retent ion in terva l , exc i tement , form of questioning, inf luence of suggestion and oath, as wel l as d i f fe ren t ia l factors such as gender, indiv idual di f ferences in suggest ib i l i ty , and developmental aspects of remembrance for pictures, events and verbal mater ia l (rumour, hearsay) were meticulously studied and analysed in both qua l i ta t i ve and quant i ta t ive ways. Even the p o s s i b i l i t y of i m p r o v i n g the f i d e l i t y of )Aussagen' th rough t r a i n i n g ( 'Erziehbarkeit ' ; Borst, 1905; Oppenheim, 1906; Breuking, 1910) was invest igated exper imenta l ly ) indicat ing a shif t from the in i t i a l negat iv is t ic out look towards a more balanced appreciat ion of test imony (Stern, 1903-1906) vol . 1, p. 539).

Of special interest is an early a t tempt by Kobler ( I9 l# ) to compare the capabi l i ty of a tr ibunal of psychology=trained laypersons with one composed of 'real ' judges to reconstruct the 'facts' of an event on the basis of witness test imony. The results of this 'simulated t r ia l ' suggested that the two tr ibunals arr ived at s impl i f ied and somewhat distorted versions of the )truth ) , based on a par t ia l l y fau l ty test imony, and reached comparable results in verdict and damages compensated. In two other exper iments, for the f i rs t t ime short si lent f i lms were used to maximize exper imental control over the presentat ion of br ief events to be reported (Muscio) 1916; Vieweg, 1921). It would be impossible to summarize here the scores of studies carr ied out in psychological laborator ies, school classrooms, law courses and s c i e n t i f i c meet ings dur ing those ear l y years and pub l ished in psychological , medical , educational and legal journals (cf . Stern, 1908, 1911) 1913a; Whipple, 1909, 1910, 1911, 1912, 1913, 191~) 1915, 1917).

The f i rs t book=length monographs on forensic psychology tha t introduced the new psychology of test imony to a broader readership appeared at the beginning of the century ( e . g . , MEnsterberg, 1908; Reichel , 1910; Sti~hr) 1911; Marbe, 1913; Varendonck, 191¢). Even some int roductory textbooks of psychology devoted space to this new area ( e . g . , Braunshausen, 1915). Although research ac t iv i ty declined and s h i f t e d in emphas i s a f t e r the o r i g i n a l e n t h u s i a s m had waned , by the end of the 1920s a substant ia l body of knowledge) enriched by first=hand p rac t i ca l experiences before the courts , had accumulated (see the re fe rences de ta i led in Stern) 1908, 1911, 1913a, 1926; Vieweg, 1921; Schrenk, 1922; Lipmann, 1925, 1935; Gorphe, 1927; Hellwig, 1927; Kuhlmann, 1929; Slesinger & Pilpel , 1929; M6nkemSller, 1930; Plaut , 1931; Undeutsch, 1967) Arntzen, 1970; Sporer, 1981).

Reception by the Law and Expert Psychological Testimony

From the very beginning, researchers and pract i t ioners al ike were eager to point out the impl icat ions of their findings for a proper evaluat ion of eyewitness test imony. Specific procedural reforms for the handling of witness test imony during p re - t r i a l investigat ions by the police and the examining just ice, as well as for the t r i a l , were proposed (e .g . , Schneickert, 1904; Lipmann, 1905; Stern, 1905), and the admission and consultat ion of psychological experts were variously demanded (e .g . , by Stern, 1902; Lipmann, 1905; MLinsterberg, 1908).

The react ion of the legal profession was a c r i t i ca l one, but i t would be wrong to construe it as one of blank re ject ion, an impression one might derive from reading Wigmore's (1909) sat i r ica l cr i t ique of M~nsterberg's provocat ive 'On the Witness Stand' (190g) which contemporary researchers have, in my opinion erroneously, taken as representat ive of the legal psychology movement of those days.

Page 8: A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony of Testi… · A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony SIEGFRIED LUDWIG SPORER Universitiit Erlangen-Niirnberg, FR ... works

330 S. L. Sporer

The fact is that such notable legal scholars as Gross, Liszt, Aschaffenburg, Radbruch and Wigmore welcomed these new approaches to the study of test imony, and many act ive ly part ic ipated in them: for example, by having ' rea l i ty experiments' conducted in their classes. Their cr i t ic isms were to the point but construct ive, emphasizing Stern's (1902) postulate of 'closeness to l i fe ' ('Lebensn~ihe') as a pr inciple for exper imentat ion and thus ant ic ipat ing many arguments about the 'external va l id i ty ' of laboratory experiments as we would cal l i t today (Loh, 19gl; Monahan & Loftus, 1982).

The cr i t ic isms and suggestions were of a substantive nature and of ten entai led highly specif ic methodological issues that revealed a high degree o£ psychological sophist icat ion on the part of lawyers. For example, lawyers general ly favoured event over picture tests (e .g . , Gross, 1903; 3af fa, 1903), and stressed the importance of focusing more on indiv idual di f ferences and idiosyncrasies of specif ic witnesses (e .g . , Gross, 1903, 1904) rather than relying on findings based on group averages (e .g . , Wigmore, 1909). In several studies conducted by jurists, the inf luence of a t tent ion and the degree of emot ional exc i tement on the ab i l i t y to perceive and remember was recognized (e .g . , 3affa, 1903; Radbruch, 1906; Kobler, 19It+).

Methodological ly , the dist inct ion between test imonia l errors regarding 'central ' and 'peripheral ' detai ls (in today's terminology) was also elucidated by 3affa (1903) and by Wreschner (1903), who argued that such a d is t inct ion had to be made on empir ical grounds, and not through Stern's (1902) method who a pr ior i and in tu i t i ve ly had assigned a double weight to errors in ' important ' detai ls.

These examples i l lus t ra te that there existed a sophist icated in terd isc ip l inary dialogue between psychology and the law at the beginning of this century. They also demonstrate the interest that at least some members of the legal profession showed in the advancement of psychology and its appl icat ions to the law. However, the legal profession was re luctant , i f not v io lent ly opposed, to rel inquish control over the actual evaluat ion of witnesses which they considered their own province. While they were open to the theoret ica l developments in psychology and while they recognized the importance of psychological t ra in ing for jurists (e .g . , Gottschaik, 1906; Reichel, 1910; Fr iedr ich, 1911; Mi t te rma ier , 1912; Stern, 1913b), they general ly did not want psychologists as experts to conduct psychological witness examinat ions and experiments in their courtrooms (e .g . , Gottschalk, 1906). Even Gross c lear ly distinguished between ' theoret ical ' exper iments, which psychologists were to carry out to fur ther knowledge on the psychology of test imony and which psychologists should be called upon to discuss as expert advisers, and 'pract ical experiments' (Gross, 1903), namely witness examinat ions that should remain in the hands of examining justices (Gross, 1907).

Cons ider ing the wide a t t e n t i o n tha t the psychology of t e s t i m o n y received at that t ime, formal response by the leg is la t ive and by the judic iary was s low. A commiss ion for the re fo rm of the code o£ c r i m i n a l p rocedure

('Strafprozessordnung') largely bypassed the suggestions and demands by the 'Aussage' psychologists although one could not say that i t had ignored these ef for ts al together (Schneickert, 1906). S imi lar ly , the judic iary was i n i t i a l l y re luctant to admit psychologists as experts on test imony to the courtroom (Stern, 1926; Undeutsch, 1954, 1967). However, defence lawyers (and also medical experts) adopted the pr inciple of the new 'Aussagepsychologie' and f requent ly usurped them to thei r advantage (Undeutsch, 1954). Probably the f i rs t t ime that expert 'psychological ' testimony on the evaluat ion of witness test imony was heard before a court of law was in the Berchtold murder t r ia l in Munich in 1896 (Schrenk-Notzing, 1897). Two psychiatr ists, Schrenk-Notzing and Grashey, at tempted to demonstrate before the

Page 9: A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony of Testi… · A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony SIEGFRIED LUDWIG SPORER Universitiit Erlangen-Niirnberg, FR ... works

History of the Psychology of Testimony 331

court that the press, through its act ive par t ic ipat ion in the chase for the murderer and its biased report ing) had exerted suggestive inf luences and re t roac t ive memory distort ions ( 'rf ickwirkende Erinnerungsfglschung'). Schrenk-Notzing's ([897) expert test imony in this widely discussed case stressed the signi f icance of suggestion on test imony which he wanted to put on record although he had no doubt regarding Berchtold's gu i l t .

Around 1903 or 1904) Stern probably became the f i rs t psychologist to tes t i fy as an expert regarding the truthfulness of the depositions of an adolescent boy who supposedly had been sexual ly molested (Stern, 1926). From a comparat ive analysis of the successive depositions of the boy Stern concluded that the later statements were more l ike ly to have been a product of suggestive questioning than recol lect ions of true experiences. This type of case) involv ing some form of sexual abuse of chi ldren or adolescents (mostly gir ls) by a teacher, pr iest, re la t ive or stranger) became the prototype of the case in which psychologists' test imony was called upon. One of the best known is Amand van Puyenbroeck's murder t r ia l in Belgium in which Varendonck, a psychologist, presented results of experiments he had speci f ica l ly conducted with schoolchildren who were similar to the key witnesses, employing questions par t icu lar ly pert inent to the ones used in the interrogat ions (Varendonck, 1911- 1912; of . Whipple, 1913; Stern, 1926; Rouke, 1957). This case, as many others in which psychologists test i f ied on behalf of the defence, resulted in an acqui t ta l of the defendant (cI . Stern, 1926). However, the goal of these psychologists was not only to help defendants (in fac t , Stern (1926) deplored the one-sidedness of cases they were admit ted to) but also to act as advocates for the chi ldren and so save them from the agony of repeated in ter rogat ions.

Once confronted with the intr icacies of courtroom rout ine psychologists also became more aware of the judicial needs and ways of th inking. It also seems that this exposure to courtroom rea l i ty opened up new avenues and ways of knowledge for the psychology of test imony, leading to an increased appreciat ion of case studies ('Kasuistik'; cf . Lipmann, 1935). By the beginning of the 1930s, a series of case col lect ions had accumulated (e .g . , Stern, 1926; cf . Lipmann, 1933) which, along with the monographs summarizing the exper imenta l work, amounted to a substantial body of knowledge on the psychology of test imony.

Udo Undeutsch (1954, 1967), who later became one of the leading author i t ies on the psychology of testimony in Germany, has provided us with an excel lent summary of the history of expert psychological test imony in Germany. He has referred to the developments in Europe described so far as the ' f i rst phase' in the history of the psychology of test imony fo l lowed by a decl ine in research and publ icat ion ac t i v i t y for about the next two decades. His work has also shown that the re formatory ef for ts by the 'Aussage' psychologists had some belated impact on the Jaw of cr iminal procedures and on the decisions of German supreme courts. During the 1920s some state ordinances met the suggestions put for th by Stern and his fo l lowers ( repr inted in Stern, 1926), and the use of psychological experts became gradual ly required through higher court decisions for the evaluat ion of non-adult witnesses in the 1930s and 19q0s, and for adult witnesses in the 1950s.

It should be noted that the focus of the expert test imony in Germany was, and st i l l is, directed at the evaluat ion of indiv idual witnesses and their test imonies, unlike the si tuat ion in the USA where psychological experts today tes t i fy only to general factors that may, or may not, have af fected the test imony of a witness (Loftus, 1979). However, the question about whether the expert is to tes t i fy to the c red ib i l i t y of a witness in general or to the c red ib i l i t y of the detai ls of the test imony given has remained controversial (Liebel & yon UsJar, 1975; Wegener,

Page 10: A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony of Testi… · A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony SIEGFRIED LUDWIG SPORER Universitiit Erlangen-Niirnberg, FR ... works

332 S L. Sporer

1981). It is also s t r iking that this approach has led to a much more ba lanced eva lua t ion of witness t e s t imony , which is also evident in Stern 's l a t e r wr i t ings ( e . g . , S t e r n , 1930, 1939), c o m p a r e d wi th the r a t h e r n e g a t i v i s t i c o u t l o o k c h a r a c t e r i s t i c of the early days of the psychology of t e s t i m o n y . Undeutsch (195t~, 1967) has argued that this nega t iv i s t i c view was or iginal ly brought about by the one- sided goals of the exper ts and the one-s ided se lec t ion of cases . Original ly exper ts were called by the defence only (Stern , 1926i M6nkemiSller, 1930) whereas l a t e r i t was considered the duty of the s t a t e ( the so -ca l l ed 'Aufkl~rungspf l ich t ' by judge and prosecut ing a t to rney) to make use of psychological exper ts to gua ran t ee the ' f inding of t ru th ' . Other reasons for the or ig ina l ly nega t ive tone may have lain in the socio-cul tural biases against women and chi ldren which prevai led at that t ime, as well as in fau l ty methodology which was pr imar i ly designed to bring out the l imi ta t ions in eyewitnesses' performance (cf . Undeutsch, 1967; Wegener, 1981).

However, i t should also be pointed out that in central Europe this move away from laboratory and f ie ld exper imentat ion towards 'praxis', namely the diagnostic of indiv idual cases, has led to a stagnation in the~)retical advancements as well as to unnecessary restr ict ions on the scope of topics that ' forensic psychologists' have tackled. For instance, most contemporary monographs in German on the psychology of test imony (e .g . , Undeutsch, 1967; Arntzen, 1970) almost exclusively discuss and general ize from the evaluat ion of non-adult witnesses in sexual of fence cases. This selection bias, dictated by the needs of the legal profession, has most l ike ly also led to another kind of one-sidedness that s imi lar ly should be avoided. :]udicial

pract ice has enslaved 'forensic psychology' as a subsidiary science from which i t has to emancipate i tsel f again.

Developments in English-speaking Countries

The present review has pr imar i ly focused on the history of the psychology of t e s t imony in cen t ra l Europe, and especia l ly in Germany . As other authors have inves t iga ted the developments in Engl ish-speaking coun t r i e s , mainly in the USA, I summarize them only br ief ly here (see Rouke, 1957; Gree t , 1971; Levine & Tapp, 1973~ Anas tas i , 1979i Loh, 19gl).

Para l le l to the psychological knowledge inhe ren t in the rules of c r imina l procedure and legal commenta r i e s in cen t ra l Europe, there ex i s ted , probably even more pronounced and e labora ted in the common law of Br i ta in and the USA~ ' rules of evidence ' tha t summarized cen tu ry - long psychological learn ings on issues of t e s t imony and witness c red ib i l i ty (for examples see Bentham, 1843; Wigmore, 1909; Greer , 1971). But it is M~nsterberg's 'On the Witness Stand' (1908) tha t is f requent ly , and incor rec t ly , considered to mark the star t ing point for legal psychology. Mfinsterberg's provocat ive book comprised a series of essays published in the preceding year in popular magazines. It contained many useful but overs impl i f ied suggestions for applying the 'new psychology' to test imony and other c r ime- re la ted issues, drawing heavi ly on the European work wi thout giving specif ic references. The essays were not wr i t ten in a technical fashion but rather to popularize the ideas of a legal psychology among the general public, hoping that public opinion would exert pressure on the legal profession 'to turn the at tent ion of serious men to an absurdly neglected f ield' (M{Jnsterberg, 1908, p. 9).

However, the way MUnsterberg went about sel l ing his ideas was doomed to fa i lu re . His boisterous approach surely shows one way not to proceed i f one wants to convince an audience of the points one is t ry ing to make. To cal l the legal profession

Page 11: A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony of Testi… · A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony SIEGFRIED LUDWIG SPORER Universitiit Erlangen-Niirnberg, FR ... works

History of the Psychology of Testimony 333

'obdurate' (p. 9), 'completely sat isf ied with the most haphazard methods ol common prejudice and ignorance' (p. t~t~), 'unaware' (p. (~6), and 'slow to learn' (p. 63), etc. was an inappropriate way to try winning over t radi t ion-conscious members of the legal profession. This sledge-hammer approach was more l ike ly to create resistance in the legal circles being challenged. And the reply was awesome. John Wigmore (1909), Dean of the Chicago Law School, who himself had carr ied out ' test imonia l and verdict experiments' (Greer, 1971), launched a vehement, sa t i r i ca l , counter -a t tack on M~Jnsterberg's assertions. His lucid analysis contained many cr i t ic isms that have remained pert inent even to present research on the psychology of test imony (cf . Loh, 19gl). For example, he cr i t ic ized the legal naivety with which psychologists approached the legal system in which they wished to play a part and he noted that over-hasty general izat ions from exper imental results based on group means were no sound basis on which one could assess errors of indiv idual witnesses.

It should be noted, however, that many of Wigmore's cr i t ic isms were directed against the 'associative method' of psychological 'Tatbestandsdiagnostik' ( l ie detect ion) , which even in Europe only few researchers considered s t i l l feasib le, and not against the col laborat ion of exper imenta l psychology with the law in general. The extensive bibl iography in his a r t i c le also attests to the fact that Wigmore, and probably many other legal scholars (c I . McCormick, [927), were general ly qui te interested in what psychology had to of fer but not in the form in which i t was avai lable at that t ime. The way Milnsterberg presented his arguments was perceived as 'yel low psychology' which the legal profession thought i t could do wi thout (Moorep 1907).

Thus i t seemed that legal psychology never real ly got of f the ground in the USA, nor in Great Br i tain (Greet, 1971; Loh, 1981). Later at tempts, this t ime by lawyers, to develop a legal psychology in the context of the ' legal real ist ' movement were also unsuccessful (c I . Loh, 1981). Occasional studies on the psychology of test imony in the USA and Br i ta in (e .g . , Muscio, 1916; Cady, 192t+; Marston, 1924; Slesinger & Pi lpel , 1929) never amounted to a 'movement' comparable with the one in central Europe; nor were the repeated proposals to wed the two discipl ines psychology and the law, ever consummated.

CONCLUSIONS

This review of the history of the psychology of test imony has at tempted to demonstrate that before the recent renaissance of the psychology-law in ter face there already existed at the beginning of this century in central Europe a smaller yet comparable movement to marry the two discipl ines. While the need to f i l l this gap in the historiography of applied psychology const i tutes a just i f iab le goal in i t se l f , the h is to ry of the psychology of t es t imony is also of i n t e r e s t to present researchers as i t highlights some problems that legal psychologists faced then and which their counterparts face today. The very fact of their h istor ical recurrence marks these issues as crucial ones that legal psychologists have to consider. It should, however, be noted that most of the developments described took place w i th in the context of an inquis i tor ia l legal system which fur ther complicates a t ransfer of these learnings to an adversary system of law.

One recurr ing issue is the degree of legal naivety with which psychologists 'ran to help', of ten being unaware of the century- long psychological learnings inherent in the legal l i t e ra tu re . Much of this psychological knowledge that has been d is t i l led from everyday courtroom experience lends i tse l f to empir ica l test ing and

Page 12: A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony of Testi… · A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony SIEGFRIED LUDWIG SPORER Universitiit Erlangen-Niirnberg, FR ... works

334 S. L. Sporer

provides a good start ing point for the legal psychologist; taking in to considerat ion where the law is coming from ensures 'relevance t and increases the l ike l ihood that psycho- lega l research is pe rce i ved as p e r t i n e n t to the lega l issues under discussion.

This should not be misunderstood to mean that psychology should give up its in tegr i ty and its unique perspective. The la t te r is also essential for providing new insights through psychological theor iz ing and for point ing out issues that otherwise may go unnoticed from a purely legal perspect ive. To meet these demands requires the ins t i tu t iona l iza t ion of a t ru ly in terd isc ip l inary dialogue that fac i l i ta tes the exchange of ideas. Stern and his fo l lowers recognized this and sought to establish a dialogue through the foundation of an in terd isc ip l inary journal and of research i n s t i t u t e s . They also publ ished in lega l journa ls and o f f e r e d courses and conferences on legal psychology. Perhaps the less pronounced role d i f f e ren t ia t i on among academic disciplines exist ing at that t ime made this dialogue across the i r boundaries easier. On the other hand 9 the numerous cr i t ic isms by jurists of the early work by the 'Aussage' exper imental is ts showed that these ef for ts were not yet suf f ic ient ly coordinated to meet the lawyers' needs. For example, pol ice o f f icers , judges and attorneys would have been great ly interested if psychologists could have provided them with foolproof methods to signal whenever a specif ic witness before them was te l l ing the truth and when not. Instead, they were being told that eyewitness test imony could be unrel iable, and tha t , for example, younger subjects, on average, made more errors of recol lect ion than older subjects. Although the early 'Aussage' e x p e r i m e n t a l i s t s did address the issue of i n d i v i d u a l d i f f e r e n c e s , apparently this was not enough to meet the pract ica l needs of the jur ists.

Another cr i t ic ism was one level led at the contr ived nature of some of the experiments, especial ly the 'picture tests'. What did lawyers care about what percentage of some hundred minor detai ls of an in t r i ca te and ar t is t ic p icture children could reproduce af ter calm, one-minute study of the picture when they were interested to know whether or not legal ly impor tant facts of a r ea l - l i f e event could be fa i th fu l l y recovered from a witness who had caught only glimpses of a f leet ing event while in extreme emot ional turmoi l or severe f r ight? It was lawyers who f i rs t employed the event methodology to the exper imenta l study of test imony, and its continued popular i ty has proved them r ight .

It was also lawyers who in i t i a ted the exper imenta l study of the impact of testimony: that is, they real ized that i t was not the correctness or falseness of testimony in the abstract which was of importance but rather whether judge or jury could discern between them and thus arr ive at an appropr iate decision. In other words, the crucial question for the jurist is whether i t is possible to achieve correct conclusions and decisions despite some par t ia l l y incorrect witness input. One way (and most contemporary German forensic psychologists would argue that i t is the only way) to explore this issue is by systemat ical ly observing the rout ine variat ions of testimony in their natural settings (e .g . , Undeutsch, 1967; Arntzen, [970). The histor ical shif t from the laboratory to courtroom (case) studies has started this trend. Are we to repeat the histor ical cycle? At last, the h is tor ica l pendulum that has swung between both types of approaches wi l l probably come to rest in the middle, indicat ing the necessity of both natura l is t ic and exper imenta l approaches and a str iv ing for general theories that account for f indings both in the laboratory and the f ie ld .

But even if psychologists had elaborated the most consistent and empi r ica l ly corroborated theories on the condit ions for the c red ib i l i t y of test imony and its assessment, i t would probably st i l l take some t ime before the courts and the

Page 13: A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony of Testi… · A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony SIEGFRIED LUDWIG SPORER Universitiit Erlangen-Niirnberg, FR ... works

History of the Psychology of Testimony 335

leg is lat ive incorporate them into thei r decision-making. Consider for a moment the fo l lowing analogy. If af ter decades in which exper imental psychology has made use of analysis of variance designs some group of stat ist ic ians arr ive at the conclusion that al l these studies are fraught with some heretofore unrecognized sources of error that could only be e l iminated if thei r new approach was fo l lowed, would we at once burn our books and journals, shut down our computers and fo l low the new stat is t ica l gospel? If not, how can legal psychologists expect that the legal profession wi l l at once change its wel l -establ ished ways of thinking (about test imony) that have served it wel l for so long.'? We must not expect immediate results, and we should be cautious and modest in our conclusions unt i l t ime shows whether our o f fer did in fact result in improvement. We must also beware of over- general izing our results and must point out the posit ive as well as the negat ive aspects of our findings to protect them from abuse.

Final ly , legal psychology has to understand i tsel f as a h is tor ica l ly grown product that has been shaped by external and internal h istor ical ' forces'. The psychology of test imony at the beginning of this century has been repeatedly described as 'negat iv ist ic ' , par t icu lar ly with regard to its assessment of the ( lack of) value of test imony by chi ldren and women. As this tendency re f lec ted socio- cul tural values prevalent at that t ime, legal psychologists have to become aware of possible biasing influences that may or may not a f fec t thei r theoriz ing or even their empir ica l data.

Today, as before, i t is usually the defence which calls psychologists in the USA and Bri tain to give expert psychological test imony. This form of selection bias may also restr ic t the impressions psychologists get from this l im i ted perspect ive. While an e f f e c t i v e ' c r ime con t ro l ' and a 'C i v i l L i b e r t i e s ' o r i e n t a t i o n need not necessarily be exclusive nor cont rad ic tory , social scientists should re f lec t on their personal predi lect ions before they render thei r services to one party or the other. However, nobody wi l l mind i f they dedicate their e f for ts to fur ther the f inding of truth and the promotion of just ice.

NOTE AND ACKNOWLEDGEMENT

A longer version of this paper was the basis for a presentat ion to the Bri t ish Psychological Society, Welsh Branch, Internat ional Conference on Psychology and Law in Swansea, Wales, 3uly 1982, and is avai lable from the author on request.

The author grate fu l ly acknowledges the d i l igent help of A. 3. Chapman in adapting this paper.

REFERENCES

Anastasi , A. (1979). Fields of Applied Psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill. Arntzen, F. (1970). Psychologie der Zeugenaussage. Gbttingen: Hogrefe. Bentham, 3. (1843). Rationale of judicial evidence. In 3. Bowring ( e d . ) , The Works

of 3eremy Bentham. Volume 6. Edinburgh: Tai t . Binet, A. ( lg97). Description d'un objet . Anne~e Psychologique, 3, 296-332. Binet, A. (1900). La SuggestibilitY'. Paris: Schleicher Fr'~res. Binet, A. (1905). La science du t~moignage. Ann~'e Psychologique, I I , 128-136. Bolton, F.W. (1896). The accuracy of reco l lec t ion and observat ion. Psychological

Review, 3, 286-295.

Page 14: A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony of Testi… · A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony SIEGFRIED LUDWIG SPORER Universitiit Erlangen-Niirnberg, FR ... works

336 S. L. Sporer

Boring, E.G. (1950). A History of Experimental Psychology. New York: Appleton. Borst, M. (1905). Experimentelle Untersuchungen ~iber die Erziehbarkeit und die Treue

der Aussage. In L.W. Stern (ed.) , Beitr~ge zur Psychologie der Aussage. Volume 2. Leipzig: Barth.

Brauer, W. (1841). Uber die Unzul~issigkeit des direkten Zeugenbeweises. Annalen der deutschen und ausl~4ndischen Criminal-Rechtspflege. 14(1), 1-34.

Braunshausen, N. (1915). EinfOhrung in die experimentelle Psychologie. Leipzig: Teubner.

Breuking, H. (1910). 0ber die Erziehbarkeit der Aussage. Zeitschri f t fEr angewandte Psychologie, 3, 32-87.

Buckhout, R. (197#). Eyewitness testimony. Scientif ic American, 23I, 23-31. Cady) H.M. (1924). On the psychology of testimony. American 3ournal of Psychology,

35, 110-112. C a t t e l l , 3 .McK. (1895). Measurements of the a c c u r a c y of r e c o l l e c t i o n s . Sc i ence , New

Ser ies 2, 761-766. C l i f fo rd , B.R. & Bull, R. (1978). The Psychology of Person I d e n t i f i c a t i o n . London:

Rout ledge & Kegan Paul . Dorsch, F. (1963). Gesch ich te und P r o b l e m e der a n g e w a n d t e n Psycho log ie . Bern &

S t u t t g a r t : Huber . Ebbinghaus , H. (1855). 0be r das Ged~ch tn i s . Leipzig: Duncker & Humblo t . F r i e d r i c h , 3. (1911). Die No twend igke i t r e c h t s p s y c h o l o g i s c h e r F o r t b i l d u n g . Recht und

W i r t s c h a f t , 1, 7~-76. Gorphe , F. (1927). La c r i t i que du t~ 'moignage . Par is : Dal loz . G o t t s c h a l k , A. (1906). Zur Zeugenpsycho log ie . In L.W. Stern ( e d . ) , Beitr~(ge zur

Psycholog ie der Aussage . Volume 2. Leipzig: Bar th . Gree r , D.S. (1971). Anything but the t r u th? The r e l i a b i l i t y of t e s t i m o n y in c r i m i n a l

t r i a l s . Bri t ish 3ournal of Cr imino logy , 11, 131=154. Gross , H. (1893). Handbuch f(ir U n t e r s u c h u n g s r i c h t e r . Graz: Leuschner & Lubensky . Gross , H. (1898). C r i m i n a l p s y c h o l o g i e . Leipzig: Vogel. Gross , H. (1903). Das Wahrnehmungsproblem und der Zeuge im S t a f p r o z e s s . Archly fLir

S t r a f r e c h t und S t a fp roze s s , 49, 184-203. Gross , H. (1904). BiJcherbesprechungen yon Hans Gross . Archly f~ir K r i m i n a l -

Anthropolog ie und Kr imina l i s t i k , 16, 371-377. Gross , H. (1907). 10her Zeugenpr~ifung. Mona t s sch r i f t f~r K r i m i n a l p s y c h o l o g i e und

S t r a f r e c h t s r e f o r m , 3, 577-580. Haines , H. & Vaughan, G.M. (1979). Was 1898 a ' g r e a t da t e ' in the h i s to ry of

e x p e r i m e n t a l soc ia l psychology? 3ournal of t he His tory of t he Behav ioura l Sc iences , 15, 323-332.

Hel lwig , A. (1910). His to r i sches zur Aussagepsycho log i e . Archly fi~r K r i m i n a l - Anthropo log ie und Kr imina l i s t i k , 36, 323-341.

Hel lwig , A. (1927). Psychologie undVernehmungs t e c hn ik bei T a t b e s t a n d s e r m i t t l u n g e n . Berlin: Langensche id t .

Henke, E. (1838). Handbuch des C r i m i n a l r e c h t s und der C r i m i n a l p o l i t i k . Vie r te r Thei l . Berlin: Nico la i .

3a f fa , S. (1903). Ein psycho log i sches Exper imen t im k r i m i n a l i s t i s c h e n Seminar der Univers i t~ t Ber l in . In L.W Stern ( e d . ) , Be i t r~ge zur Psycho log ie der Aussage . Volume 1. Leipzig: Bar th .

Klaussman, A.O. (1899). Zeugen-Pr f i fung . Archly fi]r K r i m i n a l - A n t h r o p o l o g i e und Kr imina l i s t i k , 1, 39-60.

Kle inschrod , G .A. (1805). 0be t den Beweis durch Zeugen in pe in l i chen Sachen . Archly des K r i m i n a l r e c h t s , 6(3) , 34-65.

Page 15: A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony of Testi… · A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony SIEGFRIED LUDWIG SPORER Universitiit Erlangen-Niirnberg, FR ... works

History of the Psychology of Testimony 337

Kobler, F. (I914). Ein rechtspsychologisches Experiment. Zeitschri f t f[ir angewandte Psychologie, 8, 317-325.

Kuhlmann, A.F. (1929). A guide to material on crime and criminal justice. New York= Wilson.

Leahey, T.H. (1980). A History of Psychology. Englewood Cl i f fs , N3: Prentice Hail. Levine, F.3. & Tapp, 3.L. (1973). The psychology of criminal identification= the gap

from Wade to Kirby. University of Pennsylvania Law Review, 121, 1079-1131. Liebel, H. & Uslar) W. yon (1975). Forensische Psychologie. Stuttgart= Kohlhammer. Lipmann, O. (1905). Reformvorschl~ge zur Zeugenvernehmung vom Standpunkte des

Psychologen. Archiv I(ir Kriminal-Anthropologie und Kriminal ist ik, 20, 68-81. Lipmann, O. (1925). Gundzfige der Psychologie fur 3uristen. Leipzig: Barth. Lipmann, O. (1935). Methoden der Aussagepsychologie. In E. Abderhalden (ed.))

Handbuch der biologischen Arbeitsmethoden. Abteilung VI, Tell C, l i t 2, Band 3. Berlin: Urban and Schwarzenberg.

Lipmann, O. & Plaut, P. (eds.) (1927). Die L~Jge. Leipzig= Barth. Loftus, E.F. (1979). Eyewitness testimony. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Loh) W.D. (1981). Psycholegal research= past and present. Michigan Law Review, 79,

639-707. Marbe, K. (1913). Gundz~ge der Forensischen Psychologie. M~nchen: Beck. Marshall, 3. (1980). Psychology and Law in Conflict. Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-

Merrill. Marston, W.M. (192/*). Studies in testimony. 3ournal of Criminal Law and Criminology,

13, 3-31. McCormick , C .T . (1927). D e c e p t i o n - t e s t s and the law of e v i d e n c e . Ca l i f o rn i a Law

Review, 13, 48(~-304. M i t t e r m a i e r , C . 3 . A . (1834). Die Lehre vom Beweise im deu t schen S t r a f p r o z e s s e .

D a r m s t a d t : Heyer . M i t t e r m a i e r , W. (1912). No twend igke i t und Durchf~Jhrbarkeit p sycho log i sche r Schulung

tier 3ur i s ten auf der Un ive r s i t~ t . Recht und W i r t s c h a f t , l , 340-343. Monahan, 3. & Lof tus , E .R. (1982). The psychology of l aw. Annual Review of

Psychology , 33, 441-473. Mbnkembl le r , O. (1930). Psycholog ie und P s y c h o p a t h o l o g i e der Aussage . He ide lbe rg :

Winter . Moore, C .C . (1907). Yellow psycho logy . Law Notes , I t , 12~-127. Mfinsterberg, H. (190g). On the wi tness s t and . New York= Doubleday , Page . Murphy, G. & Kovach, 3 .K. (1972). H i s to r i ca l In t roduc t ion to Modern Psycho logy . New

York= H a r c o u r t , Brace , 3ovanovich . Muscio, B. (1916). The in f luence of the form of a ques t ion . Bri t ish 3ournal of

Psychology, 8, 331-389. O e r s t e t t , 3 .H. (1822). Uber das No th rech t als ein e i n f l u s s r e i c he s Pr inzip der

Stairechtspflege. Neues Archly des C r i m i n a l r e c h t s , 5, 623-677. Oppenheim, R. (1906). 0be r die E rz i ehba rke i t der Aussage bei Schulk indern . In L.W.

Stern ( e d . ) , Bei t r~ge zur Psycholog ie der Aussage . Volume 2. Leipzig: Bar th . P1aut, P. (1931). Der Zeuge und se ine Aussage im S t r a f p r o z e s s . Leipz ig : Th ieme . Radbruch , G. (1906). Ein neuer Versuch zur Psycholog ie der Zeugenaussage . Archly f{Jr

K r i m i n a l - A n t h r o p o l o g i e und K r i m i n a l i s t i k , 23, 329-333. Re iche l , H. (1910). Ober Forens i sche Psycho log ie . M~nchen: Beck. Rouke, F . L . (1957). Psycho log ica l r e s e a r c h on prob lems of t e s t i m o n y . 3ournal of

Social Issues, 13(2), 50-59. S c h n e i c k e r t , H. (190t~). Die Zeugenvernehmung im Lich te der S t r a f p r o z e s s r e f o r m . In

L.W. Stern ( e d . ) , Beitr~Ige zur Psycho log ie der Aussage . Volume I . Leipzig:

Page 16: A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony of Testi… · A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony SIEGFRIED LUDWIG SPORER Universitiit Erlangen-Niirnberg, FR ... works

338 S. L. Sporer

Barth. Schneickert, H. (1906). Aus den Beratungen der Kommission f i i r die Reform des

Stafprozesses. Archly ffir Kriminal-Anthropologie und KrJminalistik, 24, J17-12~° Schrenk~ 3. (1922). Einf~ihrung in die Psychologie der Aussage. Leipzig; Quelle &

Meyer. Schrenk-Notzing, A. (1897). IJber Suggestion und Erinnerungsf~ilschung irn Berchtold-

Process. Zeitschrift ffir Hypnotismus, 59 118-1799 277-3069 307-331. Slesinger, D. & Pilpel, E.M. (1929). Legal psychology. Psychological Bullet in, 26,

677-692. Sporer, S.L. (19gl). Toward a Comprehensive History of Legal Psychology. Paper

presented to the Annual Convention of the American Psychological Associa t ion, Los Angeles.

Stern, L.W. (1902). Zur Psychologie der Aussage. Ze i t schr i f t fSr die gesamte Strafrechtswissenschaft, 22, 315-370.

Stern, L.W. (1903-1906). Beitr~ge zur Psychologie der Aussage. 2 volumes. Leipzig= Barth.

Stern, L.W. (1905). Leits~tze iJber die Bedeutung der Aussagepsychologie f(Jr das gerichtliche Veriahren. In L.W. Stern f e d . ) , Beitr~ge zur Psychologie der Aussage. Volume 2. Leipzig= Barth.

Stern L.W. (1908). Sammelbericht i~ber Psychologie der Aussage. Ze i t schr i f t for angewandte Psychologie, I, tt29-t~50.

Stern L.W. (1911). Bibliographie zur Psychologie der Aussage. Ze i t schr i f t f~ir angewandte Psychologie~ t~ 37g-3gl.

Stern L.W. (1913a). Psychologie der Aussage 1911-1913. Ze i t schr i f t ftir angewandte Psychologie~ 7, 577-596.

Stern L.W. (1913b). Die Psychologie und die Vorbildung der 3uristen. Ze i t schr i f t fi'w angewandte Psychologie, 7, 70-�it.

Stern L.W. (I926). 3ugendliche Zeugen in Si t t l ichkei tsprozesseno Leipzig; Quelle & Meyer.

Stern L.W. (1930). fiber psychologische Zeugenbegutachtung. Deutsche Medizinische Wochenschrift , 56, It~67-I~70.

Stern L.W. (1939). The psychology of tes t imony. 3ournal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 34, 3-20.

St6hr A. (19 l l ) . Psychologie der Aussage. Berlin= Puttkammer & M~hlbrecht. Tapp, 3.L. (l�g0). Psychological and policy perspectives on the law: reflections on

a decade. 3ournal of Social Issues, 36(2), 165-192. U n d e u t s c h , U. (195t~). Die En twick lung der g e r i c h t s p s y c h o l o g i s c h e n

Gutach te r t~ t igke i t . Gbttingen* Hogrefe. Undeutsch, U. (1967). Beurteilung der GlaubhaftJgkeit der Aussage. In U. Undeutsch

f e d . ) , Handbuch der Psychologie. Band I I . Forensische Psychologie. GSttingen= Hogrefe.

Varendonck, 3. (1911/12). Les temoignages d 'enfants dans un proces r e t en t i s s an t . Archives de psychologle, I I , 129-171.

Varendonck, 3. (191t~). La psychologie du temoignage. Gand-" Maison d'~'ditions et d ' impressions.

Vieweg, 3. (1921). Die Stufen der geis t igen Entwickelung. Ze i t schr i f t f{Jr angewandte Psychologie, lg~ 251-306.

yon Liszt, F. (1902). S t raf recht und Psychologie. Deutsche 3uris tenzei tung, 7~ 16- 18.

Watson, R.I . (1978). The Great Psychologists . Phi ladelphia, PA= Lippincot t .

Page 17: A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony of Testi… · A Brief History of the Psychology of Testimony SIEGFRIED LUDWIG SPORER Universitiit Erlangen-Niirnberg, FR ... works

History of the Psychology of Testimony 339

Wegener) H. ( l gg l ) . Einf~hrung in die Forensische Psychologie. Darmstadt: Wissenschaftliche Buchgeselischaft.

Wells, G.L. (J980). Eyewitness behavior: the Alberta Conference. Law and Human Behavior) 4) 237-242.

Whipple) G.M. (1909). The observer as reporter: a survey of the 'psychology of test imony'. Psychological Bul let in) 6) 153-170.

Whipple) G.M. (1910). Recent l i t e r a t u r e on the psychology of t e s t i m o n y . Psychological Bulletin, 7) 365-368.

Whipple, G.M. (1911). Psychology of tes t imony. Psychological Bulletin, g, 307-309. Whipple) G.M. (1912). Psychology of test imony and repor t . Psychological Bulletin, 9,

264-269. Whipple, G.M. (1913). Psychology of tes t imony and repor t . Psychological Bulletin,

10, 26g-26g. Whipple, G.M. (1914). Psychology of tes t imony and repor t . Psychological Bulletin,

II, 245-250. Whipple, G.M. (1915). Psychology of testimony. Psychological Bulletin, 12, 221-224. Whipple, G.M. (1917). Psychology of testimony. Psychological Bulletin, 14, 234-236. Wigmore, 3. (1909). Professor M~ansterberg and the psychology of testimony. Illinois

Law Review, 3, 399-445. Wolf, T.H. (1973). Alfred Binet. Chicago, [L: University of Chicago Press. Wreschner) A. (1903). Zur Psychologie der Aussage. Archiv fur die gesamte

Psychologie, I) lgg-lg3. Yarmey, A.D. (1979). The Psychology of Eyewitness Test imony. New York: Free

Press.

S l n E ~ LUDWIG ~=ORIE~ Psychologie, Universit~it Erlangen-NiJrnberg, Regensburger Strasse 160) 85 Ni]rnberg) FR Germany.