wp-10-03

download wp-10-03

of 36

Transcript of wp-10-03

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    1/36

    Explaining Organizational Citizenship Behavior:

    A Critical Review of the Social Exchange Perspective

    by

    Tiffany Schroeder

    WP-10-03

    CopyrightDepartment of Organizational BehaviorWeatherhead School of Management

    Case Western Reserve UniversityCleveland OH 44106-7235

    e-mail: [email protected]

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    2/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 2

    Explaining Organizational Citizenship Behavior:

    A Critical Review of the Social Exchange Perspective

    Tiffany Schroeder

    Case Western Reserve University

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    3/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 3

    Abstract

    Social Exchange has been widely cited as an explanation for why employees might act as good

    organizational citizens, proposing that individuals perform OCB as part of a desire to maintain

    equitable and favorable workplace relationships that extend beyond the benefits of the more

    impersonal contractual agreements. Although this perspective has been given much credit in the

    study of OCB, I argue that a lack of clarity around the underlying assumptions and connection to

    motives for OCB, in combination with a dearth of attention to alternative explanations for

    findings around justice, have thwarted a critical examination of this explanation to employee

    citizenship. In this paper I attempt to clarify the social exchange explanation by outlining several

    additional assumptions underlying this approach to OCB, and then use these assumptions to

    highlight empirical and conceptual gaps. Finally, I consider the value of the social exchange

    perspective for future research on OCB.

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    4/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 4

    Researchers have been studying organizational citizenship behavior (OCB) for over 30

    years now, with insights into antecedents and motives guided primarily through the lens of social

    exchange theory (Blau, 1964). Although other researchers have also explored OCB from intra-

    individual perspectives (e.g., Grant & Mayer, 2009; Motowidlo et al., 1997) and through an

    impression management framework (Bolino, 1999), this paper is limited to the OCB research

    based on social exchange theory. A reliance on social exchange as an explanatory mechanism

    has led us to discovery of several important antecedents of citizenship: relationships between

    perceptions of justice, equity, relationship quality, as well as several important consequences

    including enhanced productivity, lower turnover, and higher customer satisfaction (Podsakoff,

    MacKenzie, Paine, & Bachrach, 2000). But despite its apparent popularity and role in guiding

    the study of organizational citizenship to date, some scholars have begun to question reliance on

    social exchange as the dominant explanatory framework of citizenship behavior (Bolino,

    Turnley, & Niehoff, 2004; Rioux & Penner, 2001; Snape & Redman, 2010; Zellars & Tepper,

    2003).

    In this review I start by giving a brief history as to how and why scholars became

    interested in applying social exchange to OCB. I then provide a more in-depth look at the major

    tenets of the perspective, and the findings attributed to it. Building on previous accounts of social

    exchange, I propose several key unexamined assumptions, discuss areas where more work is

    needed, and give examples of alternative explanations for extant findings. In the last section I

    revisit the perspective in terms of its proposed significance and provide suggestions for future

    research.

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    5/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 5

    The concept of OCB originally grew out of Dennis Organs musings into explanations for

    the apparent non-relationship between job satisfaction and job performance (Organ, 1988).

    Organizational citizenship behavior was eventually proposed as an alternative form of

    performance, differentiated from traditional performance on the basis of its relative freedom

    from situational and ability constraints (p.70). Essentially, the upper boundary of task

    performance is largely limited by a persons knowledge, skills, and ability, and the lower

    boundary is limited by the fear of losing ones job. This means the individual performing the job

    does not have a great deal of room to vary in performance based on their satisfaction with the

    context. In contrast, helping a coworker does not necessarily depend on expertise in helping, and

    because doing so is not typically tied to a persons job description that person may decide not to

    perform the behavior at all. In this sense, citizenship has more freedom to vary than task

    performance, and should be comparatively more likely to vary with a persons cognitive or

    affective appraisal(i.e., job satisfaction) of the workplace (Organ, 1988).

    Researchers started out exploring the role between job satisfaction and OCB, but the

    focus soon shifted to fairness. The reasoning here was that satisfaction and its close relative,

    commitment, were reflections of the employees attitudestowardthe organization, rather than

    direct insights on how they perceive the organization to be acting toward them (Moorman,

    Niehoff, & Organ, 1993). While satisfaction and commitment might be influenced by both

    cognitions and mood, fairness- thought to be a component of satisfaction- represented a

    conscious calculation of the environment. The calculative nature of the cognitive judgment was

    thought to be less influenced by stable dispositional factors and less prone to situational

    fluctuation than other types of attitudes. Therefore, fairness perceptions represented a potentially

    more useful target of study than other attitudinal forms, in terms of both providing a window of

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    6/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 6

    insight into employees cognitions and in foreshadowing potential receptivity to managerial

    interventions (Organ, 1988; Organ & Konovsky, 1989). This cognitive approach fit in line nicely

    with the emerging social exchange explanation.

    Social exchange provided a mechanism for the intuitive link between attitudes and

    performance. The idea was that many acts in the workplace are not strictly regulated by

    contractual obligations, but through a more implicit and discretionary exchange of resources,

    including those more social in nature (Blau, 1964). According to this perspective employees

    might perform OCBs out of a sense of obligation to return any number of perceived material or

    social benefits they have gained from the organization (Organ, Podsakoff. & MacKenzie, 2006).

    Social exchange has received a great deal of credit during the last three decades for linking

    employee attitudes and OCB performance, yet as Cropanzano and Mitchell (2005) point out,

    many ambiguities remain. The precise motive prompting employees to perform OCBs and

    logistics of the process are not always clear, as detailed in the following section.

    The Social Exchange Perspective

    As Cropanzano & Michell (2005) point out, social exchange can refer to a type of

    transaction, but has typically been conceptualized as a type of relationshipby OCB theorists

    (e.g., Organ, 1988). In this view, organizations function partially through mutually desirable

    relationships in which parties give and receive a variety of benefits- including socio-emotional

    benefits. When trust has been developed to a critical level, employees can engage in behaviors

    beyond the minimum requirement, trusting that they will not be taken advantage of, but rather

    their needs will be met through this ongoing relationship. In this light, social exchange does not

    explicitly specify motives beyond the desire to maintain the exchange relationship.

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    7/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 7

    But researchers have given a few possible motives for engaging in OCB from this

    perspective, or for sustaining the relationship. Indeed, without an explanation as to why

    employees might be motivated to engage in such an exchange, the perspective would not offer

    much in the way of explanation or prediction of behavior. Concerning motives on the

    employees parts, there have generally been two similar, yet distinct, lines of reasoning within

    the social exchange perspective for why people might perform OCBs. First, when people

    perceive that they are treated fairly, the norm of reciprocity says that they should reciprocate

    (Blau, 1964). So people perform OCBs in order to give back to the organization, or

    organizational leader. This explanation is typically linked to group-value literature, placing

    heavy emphasis on the quality of the relationship, such that employees are guided to return

    benefits in order to maintain the social exchange relationship (Cropanzano & Mitchell, 2005;

    Cropanzano et al., 2001). The second line of reasoning is similar but slightly more proactive:

    Because Im treated fairly, I can act now, trusting that my contributions will be fairly rewarded

    over the long-term. Typically these explanations imply a trade-off such that the employee is

    sacrificing something in the short-term with the expectation that in the long-run they will be

    fairly compensated for their efforts (e.g., Organ & Moorman, 1991).

    Again, OCB researchers have typically not focused on disentangling motives when

    discussing social exchange. They have typically provided arguments similar to one of the above,

    or a combination of these, to explain why employees might partake in social exchange, but

    focused most attention on linking perceptions of justice to citizenship behavior. Whether or not

    OCBs are performed proactively or reactively, with or without motivation beyond sustaining the

    social exchange relationship, there are three core features defining social exchange according to

    Organ and colleagues (2006, p. 72):

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    8/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 8

    1. Voluntary actions of individuals that are motivated by the returnsthey areexpected to bring and typically in fact do bring from others (Blau, 1964, p. 91)

    2. The obligation by a party to reciprocate a benefit voluntarily rendered by someother party. However, the obligation is unspecified as to form degree, or time of

    reciprocation

    3. It depends on trust- that is, confidence that the other party will, in good time andin some appropriate manner and situation, reciprocate benefits, contributions, or

    favors; reciprocation cannot be enforced by appeal to third parties.

    These statements primarily encompass the motives of individual actions rather than the

    relationship itself. In the end, it seems, we cannot fully understand the relationship without

    understanding individual motives, and likewise cannot fully understand individual motives

    without appreciating the complex nature of benefits accrued through dynamic relationships. In

    reality, there are likely multiple benefits and motives underlying each action in social exchange

    (Fassina, Jones, & Uggerslev, 2008a), making it difficult to identify where and when the social

    exchange label may be applied to a set of transactions. In an effort to clarify this, I have

    identified some additional assumptions that seem necessary, but not sufficient, components of

    social exchange, starting with assumptions regarding the nature of exchanges:

    1. Dyadic: Both parties are actively involved as giver and receiver in the exchangeof non-contractual benefits.

    2. Non-contracted/non-explicit: Further, both similarly view benefits as (mostly)discretionary in order to qualify as, and maintain, social exchange (Cropanzano

    & Mitchell, 2005). It is the perceived discretion over form and timing that

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    9/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 9

    primarily otherwise would be economic exchange from social exchange (Organ et

    al., 2006)

    3. Frequent: Social exchanges should be ongoing and frequent in order to maintaina generalized sense of obligation toward one another, and trust in reciprocity.

    4. Observable: Both sides should be able to see something in order to feel anobligation to reciprocate

    5. Long-Term Stablility:Roughly equivalent exchange will occur over the long-term, but not necessarily in the short-term

    6.

    Benefits are Beneficial: In order to maintain social exchange, receiving parties

    must actually perceive that the discretionary acts are desirable.

    Although implied above, there are also a number of assumptions that pertain to the cognitions of

    the actor:

    1. Action is Desirable: Discretionary behavior is assumed to be received by theother graciously, and perceived as a benefit.

    2. Trust in Reciprocity: An actor must trust the other party to return the benefits.3. Discretionary Benefits: To sustain the social exchange relationship, the actor

    must believe beyond those they would expect from the contractual relationship.

    To summarize, social exchange entails an inherently long-term set of transactions in

    which parties exchange benefits on the basis of trusting organizational relationships. This has

    been used as a generalized explanation for why employees might engage in citizenship

    behaviors, although the exact motive may depend on the individual employee, as well as the

    degree to which the social exchange relationship represents a substantial benefit in its own right

    (Cropanzano & Mitchell, 2005). Regardless of what the employee perceives to be getting out of

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    10/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 10

    the relationship, the belief in just returns over the long-term should be enough to promote action

    that is not otherwise guaranteed a reward, namely, citizenship behavior. Social exchange, then,

    does not specify the exact benefit or motivation for employees engaging in this type of exchange

    aside from including socio-emotional benefits, as opposed to purely economic benefits. When

    viewed at the more general level the perspective presents no major problems, yet it seems that

    researchers are inconsistently inferring motivation. The assumptions I have listed above are not

    consistent with alldescriptions of social exchange (e.g., Fassina, Jones, & Uggerslev, 2008b, on

    p. 179, propose a social exchange mechanism not requiring perceptions of justice), but they

    provide a greater degree of precision and predictive power than that which has been previously

    afforded, and they should help researchers to be more mindful when inferring underlying

    motivation.

    Social Exchanges Impact on the Literature and Key Findings

    As I mentioned earlier, the social exchange explanation for OCB was developed

    alongside the view that cognitions about treatment by the organization would provide a cleaner

    and more efficient entrance into the link between employee attitudes and performance.

    Specifically, early scholars theorized that it was the belief in the organization as a microcosm of

    a just world (Organ & Moorman, 1993, p.8) that would be most important to subsequent

    OCBs, proposing that the belief frees the rational person from the hold of immediate returns to

    self and justifies contributions in the form of OCB (Organ & Moorman, p. 9). Interestingly,

    these scholars compared the development of organizational cognitions in the workers to the

    cognitive development of a child, explaining in the end that where they differ is that adults are

    more likely to place emphasis on the processes underlying reward distribution, because, unlike

    children, they understand the human intervention in the process. Before contributing in the form

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    11/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 11

    of OCB, then, adult workers should trust the people and processes in the system to consistently

    meet their needs.

    Much of the research from the social exchange perspective has thus focused not on the

    outcomes afforded through the exchange, but on indications of systemic, consistent fairness from

    the actors perspective. More specifically, this has led researchers to examine perceptions of

    fairness in terms of procedural justice- justice in terms of the people or systems determining how

    resources should be allocated. Other forms of justice have also been examined, and more

    recently have been distinguished according to the intended source of OCB (Fassina et al.,

    2008a). Findings centered on perceptions of justice, and to a lesser extent attitudes formed

    towardthe organization, have been the primary sources of data gathered to support social

    exchange as an explanation for OCB. It is outside the scope of this paper to present all of the

    research findings used to support the perspective, but here I present some of the key findings.

    Key Findings

    Perceptions of fair treatment appear to contribute to OCB performance beyond what can

    be accounted for by attitudes reflecting how the employee feels towardthe organization.

    Moorman (1991), for example, found that job fairness (procedural justice) accounted for

    variance in explaining OCB performance beyond what was accounted for by job satisfaction,

    implying that employees feelings toward the organization might be secondary to judgments of

    fair treatment in determining behaviors. Similarly, Moorman and colleagues (1993) found that

    when controlling for commitment and satisfaction, procedural justice carried additional variance

    in predicting three of the five OCB dimensions tested: courtesy, sportsmanship, and

    conscientiousness (but not altruism or civic virtue). On the other hand, Fassina and colleagues

    (2008b) found in a recent meta-analysis that job satisfaction also influences the performance of

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    12/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 12

    OCB beyond the effects of justice, suggesting that both perceptions of justice and feelings of

    satisfaction contribute to performance of OCB in slightly different ways. These findings suggest

    that social exchange is appropriate to the extent that employee cognitions of treatment by the

    employer, organization, or workgroup are related to their own behaviors are linked to an

    exchange for their own contributions to the relationship in the form of OCB.

    Konovsky and Pugh (1994) also contributed to our understanding of how social exchange

    works by their finding that feelings of trust mediate the relationship between procedural justice

    and OCB, but not between distributive justice and OCB. This, they explained, supports the social

    exchange perspective because perceptions of procedural fairness promote trust for reciprocation

    in future interactions, which is required in order for social exchange to occur. Distributive

    justice, on the other hand, is more likely indicative of economic exchange, where explicit

    agreements reduce ambiguities and the need to rely on judgments of trust. This and similar

    findings have been taken as indication that procedural fairness provides security over concerns

    about future treatment, allowing employees to engage in social exchange.1Overall, perceptions

    of justice appear to have a small effect on OCB, with global perceptions of justice (shared

    variance between distributive, procedural, and interactional) accounting for 2-3% of the variance

    in OCB (Fassina, Jones & Uggerslev, 2008a).

    Although not originally proposed as a part of the social exchange perspective, OCBs

    also appear to vary according to perceptions of justice specific to the target (i.e., the intended

    beneficiary of the OCB), such that interactional justice matters more for OCBs directed toward

    individuals, and procedural justice carries more weight for OCBs directed toward the

    organization (Lavalle et al., 2009; LePine, Erez & Johnson, 2002; Yoon & Suh, 2003). To the

    extent that perceptions of justice are important to the performance of OCB because they

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    13/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 13

    represent trust that OCBs will be reciprocated, target-specific effects are generally supportive of

    an underlying social exchange mechanism (Fassina et al., 2008a).

    To summarize, justice, satisfaction, trust, and commitment appear to play similar, yet

    slightly different roles as predictors of OCB (e.g., Fassina et al., 2008b; Moorman, 1991;

    Williams & Anderson, 1991; Yoon & Suh, 2003). Researchers have primarily taken relationships

    between procedural justice and OCB as support for the social exchange perspective. This line of

    thinking has led to some important insights in terms of identifying different types of justice,

    distinguishing between types of OCB and their targets, and positing explanations for these

    effects. One study even shows that OCB can be increased with a justice intervention (Skarlicki &

    Latham, 1997). Yet the research conducted from this perspective tends to explain little variance

    in terms of OCB performed, and appears to be capturing only part of the story. Below, I revisit

    assumptions of the social exchange perspective and use these to show where further investigation

    is needed.

    Revisiting the Notion of Social Exchange as an Explanation for Employee Citizenship: An

    Incomplete Story

    In terms of the assumptions listed earlier, many features of the social exchange relationship

    have been left unexamined by the extant research. In this section I discuss missing pieces of the

    puzzle that currently undermine support for the social exchange mechanism. I start by revisiting

    the assumptions underlying the exchange and actor cognitions, then I discuss where the evidence

    attained so far may be unreliable as a pure indication of social exchange.

    Evidence around Process Assumptions

    The first assumption underlying the exchange process was that it is dyadic in nature, with

    both parties actively involved as giver and receiver in the exchange of non-contractual benefits.

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    14/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 14

    Research thus far has typically been conducted from the employee side only, and even here only

    examining what the employee judges as the likelihood of reciprocation (implied from justice),

    and their contributions to the exchange. In order to truly determine whether or not the exchange

    is dyadic, we would need to be able to see how changes on either side would affect behavior on

    the other.

    The second assumption is that reciprocation is non-contracted or non-explicit, that is, the

    timing and form of reciprocation is left to discretion. Research indicates that many employees

    view OCBs as a part of the job (Morrison, 1994; Coyle-Shapiro et al., 2004), and early on

    scholars noted the importance of careful distinction between in-role effects and extra-role effects

    (Williams & Anderson, 1991), yet researchers in this sphere have typically not restricted analysis

    to OCBs that were indeed considered discretionary. We also do not know what, if any,

    benefits from the employers side are considered by either party to be discretionary.

    Social exchanges and accompanying interactions should also be frequent enough to

    maintain the generalized sense of obligation toward one another and trust in reciprocity. Because

    researchers have not typically inquired into the nature of the benefits perceived to have been

    given and received in the interaction, it makes it difficult to question the frequency at which they

    are exchanged. Further, it become harder to test the assumption that the benefits being exchanged

    are actually observable enough to provoke reciprocation in the form of continued social

    exchange.

    Social exchange relationships are more personal and reliant on trust for continuation over

    the long-term, yet researchers have failed to test whether these relationships are actually stable

    over long periods of time. Instead, the majority have relied on cross-sectional studies (Organ et

    al., 2006), which clearly falls short in capturing an explanation reliant on long-term relationships.

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    15/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 15

    Given the difficulty and resources involved in longitudinal studies a complementary alternative

    might be inquiry into how these relationships are built in the first place.

    To declare an exchange a social exchange, parties must believe that the resources being

    exchanged are discretionary, but also desirable or beneficial. Cropanzano & Mitchell (2005)

    propose that the key is a match in perceptions of the relationship or the exchange from the two

    sides. That is, in order for social exchange to take place successfully both parties must indeed

    believe that the exchange is a social one and feel they are somehow benefitting from this more

    personal relationship- but some employees or employers might prefer reciprocity in the form of

    economic exchange, or the agreed-upon transactions. As the research currently stands, it is

    difficult to distinguish whether the parties involved actually prefer social to economic exchange,

    or categorize these in the same way. Bergeron (2007) notes that there is likely a trade-off to

    OCBs, and they may take away from important task behavior. OCBs directed toward the

    organization or supervisor then might not always be kindly received. If no desire exists for

    exchanges beyond those made explicit, or if actions are performed out of perceived necessity

    rather than discretion, we cannot confidently label this as a social exchange. In short, this

    assumption must be tested if we are to label an exchange as social.

    Evidence around Cognitions of the Actor

    The first assumption here was that the actors contribution is primarily beneficial or

    desirable to the other party, and will be received graciously. We are currently lacking evidence

    that employees performing social exchange behaviors such as OCBs actually believe they are

    primarily benefitting the other party. That is, we might wish to confirm that people perform

    OCBs primarily because they are deemed valuable to some other person or entity, rather than

    because they are directly rewarding to the party performing the action. This distinction becomes

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    16/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 16

    especially key considering recent evidence by Bolino, Grant, and Harvey (in press)

    demonstrating that OCB directed toward the organization can actually directly benefit the

    individual by strengthening feelings of self-worth. Interestingly, this was true only for

    individuals motivated by prosocial values, or concern for the organization, while those who

    performed OCBs for impression management reasons (including the desire to build up future

    returns) made a trade-off such that feelings of self-worth were actually lowered by the

    performance of OCBs. The point here we can no longer assume that the other as the

    immediate beneficiary if individuals can gain direct reward with or without accompanied

    benefits from the other.

    Trust in reciprocity is another assumed cognition of the actor. This is the assumption that

    has received the most attention and support, but without controlling for other motives we cannot

    assume that this should represent social exchange. I will return to this point in the next section.

    The final assumption around actor cognition in the social exchange perspective is that the

    actor must believe the benefits they receive are in fact discretionary, rather than something they

    are entitled to as a part of the contracted exchange (behaviors expected in exchange for the

    paycheck). We need to determine what behaviors employees might consider discretionary on

    the part of employers and/or supervisors. Without this we cannot truly say that a perceived

    obligation to reciprocate in the form of OCB exists. For example, is fair and respectful treatment

    between human beings a right, or aprivilege? If a privilege, then employees might see this as a

    discretionary behavior which they ought to return in some fashion. This can easily be considered

    within the framework of social exchange. But if respectful treatment is a right then we are still

    left wondering what employees perceive to be getting out of the exchange relationship. On a

    related note we might ask: When employees do things like take time away from their regular

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    17/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 17

    work to help on an unexpected project, or intentionally portray the organization in a positive

    light during their off-hours- are these things that are considered a rightof the employer, or a

    privilege? Research has found that perceptions of role breadth vary between individuals and

    across job levels, and employees performance of OCB partially depends on whether or not they

    perceive it to be a part of their job (Coyle-Shapiro et al., 2004; Morrison, 1994). If employees

    perceive these behaviors to be a part of their job, this might be better considered under the

    economic exchange framework- especially because researchers typically infer social exchange

    from the perspective of the employee only. Alternatively, we might choose to measure perceived

    relationship. The trouble here is that not all are in agreement that some specific type of

    relationship underlies social exchange. On the other hand, one of the few explicit assumptions

    stated by theorists is that the relationship provokes a sense of obligation. To test this assumption,

    then, researchers should attempt to determine whether that obligation, if unfulfilled, leads to a

    reduction of discretionary behavior.

    Social exchange is based on the premise that perceptions of fairness or justice strengthen

    relationships and trust over time, and the norm of reciprocity present in these relationships holds

    that people will feel obligated to return any good deeds. In order to go above and beyond their

    level of required work, then, people should perceive that the organization or leader is meeting or

    exceeding that which is owed to them as an employee. Strictly speaking, some might say that

    which is owed to an employee is a paycheck. Others might say that employees are deserving of

    respect, and a lack of respect indicates a lack of justice or fairness. Still others might say that

    employees should be given the tools they need to adequately do the work theyve been hired to

    do, and that fairness is achieved when they are given access to these tools. The point here is that

    just as understanding of an employers expectations will vary from employee to employee, so

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    18/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 18

    will employee expectations for what is owed to them as a part of the contractual relationship,

    therefore researchers should exercise caution in labeling types of exchange without

    understanding employee expectations.

    Justice as an Indicator of Social Exchange

    Researchers of OCB have often relied on justice-especially procedural justice- as a proxy

    for social exchange. Despite some inconsistencies in effect sizes and covariances, perceptions of

    justice have emerged consistently as a predictor of employee OCB. Again, the thought here is

    that perceptions of fairness in interactions and procedures now reassures employees that they can

    trust they will be taken care of in the future, allowing them to contribute as they see fit, without

    needing to see immediate rewards. Given the evidence accumulated, we might infer that as far as

    these types of justice go, social exchange is occurring. In this section I aim to show why even

    this inference may be premature.

    As I pointed out earlier there are multiple motivations that might be inferred from the

    social exchange literature. Likewise, there are several reasons employees might rely on a sense

    of justice or trust in the relationship before deciding to contribute in the form of OCB. The sense

    of psychological safety underlying judgments of trust or justice frees employees from the

    restriction of fear, but is not a motivation in its own right because it must be accompanied by

    some other motive in order to lead to action. Building on Organ and colleagues (2006) emphasis

    around feelings of obligation as a key to the exchange process, psychological safety would not be

    sufficient because it would simply allow OCB to occur without the fear that one might be taken

    advantage of, not imply a sense of obligation to initiate the behavior. Nor would it necessarily

    ensure the belief that actions in the form of OCB will produce some personally desirable

    outcome. Despite some scholars beliefs that OCB research has tended to assume self-serving

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    19/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 19

    motives (e.g., Bolino, Turnley, & Niehoff, 2004), a closer examination of the social exchange

    explanation reveals that, according to this perspective, employees ultimately act in order to

    receive some sort of benefit in return. Justice is key to this perspective then because it ensures

    that benefits will in fact be reciprocated.

    Looking at the various operationalizations of justice, though, one might get the sense that

    there are additional reasons this might be important for an employees discretionary action.

    Perhaps more importantly than representing the likelihood of reciprocation, justice holds its

    importance as an indicator that an employees effort will not be needlessly wasted. That is,

    respectful treatment and fair procedures might symbolize the employers desires to

    systematically make sound judgments in all areas. Therefore to the extent an employee believes

    the leaders are headed in the right direction, making the extra effort to help a colleague, put in

    extra effort, or speak up on the companys behalf can be viewed as worthwhile- these efforts are

    contributing to an organzation that is sure to use resources effectively.

    In both of these views, justice serves to reduce uncertainty around outcomes (Lind & van

    den Bos, 2002) - the difference is that in one case outcomes hold their importance in ensuring

    that personal needs will be met (the social exchange view), whereas in the other case employees

    are concerned more with the connection of their work to organizational outcomes. In the latter,

    an employees perception of expected reciprocity, or expected personal benefit, appears to be

    overshadowed by the direct benefits associated with perceived influence (e.g., Grant, 2007;

    Snape & Redman, 2010).

    Problems in Measuring Justice

    There also appear to be several problems with the way justice is currently measured, as

    well as the inferences based on this measurement. Many of the arguments for using justice

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    20/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 20

    measures come from Allan Linds early work (e.g., Lind & Taylor, 1988), but more recently he

    and a colleague have presented a compelling case that fairness may not be of fundamental

    importance in determining behavior, and in fact may only matter when the ability to trust is

    unclear (Lind & van den Bos, 2002) . This theory of uncertainty management is based on

    evidence that relationships between fairness and subsequent actions are stronger in cases when

    outcomes are unclear or when trust in an authority figure is not certain. They also proposed that

    if information from one type of justice is missing perceptions from another type of justice may

    be used to fill in the blanks (e.g., distributive justice for procedural justice). This is important for

    a number of reasons. First, it signals that using measurements of justice might not be appropriate

    in cases where trustworthiness is already well-established, and likewise, where trustworthiness is

    uncertain, justice may serve as a more sensitive measurement. Second, if fairness heuristics are

    used to inform ratings on the basis of whatever data is available, shared variance between the

    different types of justice are likely to be inflated. Recent meta-analytic findings have indeed

    suggested that shared variance between procedural, distributive, and interactional justice was

    greater than isolated justice effects (Fassina et al., 2008a).

    If justice measurements have problems independently, these only multiply when used in

    combination with similarly complex constructs. Skarlicki & Latham (1997) performed a quasi-

    experiment in which they tested an intervention designed to increase perceptions of justice. The

    intervention consisted of training one set of union leaders in how to increase their

    communication, respect, and openness toward union members. They then compared their

    members perceptions of justice and subsequent OCBs with members from control groups,

    finding that the intervention did positively impact both perceptions of justice and OCB

    performance in the training group. As noted earlier, this has been regarded as a key point of

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    21/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 21

    support in the social exchange OCB literature. This seems convincing on the surface, but looking

    deeper indicates other possible mechanisms acting on OCB. Much of the intervention was

    centered on teaching leaders how to give employees a chance to make their voices be heard, and

    the procedural justice questionnaire also asked employees questions about their ability to voice

    concerns. Further, the OCB measure tapped into members active participation in the union,

    which might be might also be thought of in terms of expressing ones voice. Rather than

    inferring social exchange, then, it seems more parsimonious to assume that the intervention

    aimed at increasing opportunities for union members to have their voices heard simply increased

    their perceived ability to voice concerns, which subsequently increased their active use of that

    voice. Greater attention to competing explanations such as this one are needed to guide us in

    identifying the mechanisms of social exchange and the performance of OCB.

    The Link between Justice and OCB

    Adding to the problems stemming from the measurements themselves, using supervisor

    ratings of employee behavior might be inflating the correlation between justice and OCB.

    According to the social exchange perspective employees contribute in the form of OCBs

    because they expect to receive some sort of reciprocation. Again, this relies on the assumption

    that the employer will notice and appreciate the behavior. Yet OCBs are by definition less

    likely to be enforceable job requirements than task behaviors (Organ, 1997), with the implicit

    reasoning being that they are more dependent on situation than are task behaviors, and therefore

    more challenging for a manager to observe and regulate in any systematic fashion.

    Bergeron (2007) proposed that individuals may differ in how visible their OCB is to

    supervisors and that through the performance appraisal process this visibility could moderate the

    relationship between OCB and individual career outcomes. If supervisors assessments of OCB

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    22/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 22

    are skewed in the performance appraisal process, they are probably also biased in research

    surveys asking for reports of subordinates citizenship behaviors. Just because a supervisor does

    not notice an employees OCB behaviors, it does not necessarily follow that the employee has

    not performed these behaviors. Because OCBs can be performed by any member of the

    organization and can be directed toward multiple targets, they are less likely to appear in some

    systematic fashion. Helping a fellow employee, for example, is only helpful if the fellow

    employee actually needs the help. It is unlikely that the fellow employee needs help on some

    regularly occurring basis- in fact, when individuals judge coworkers to be consistently needy

    they are actually less likely to help (Van der Vegt, Bunderson, & Oosterhof, 2006). In effect,

    OCBs are more likely than task behaviors to be performed on an as-needed basis, which may or

    may not catch the attention of the supervisor.

    The extent to which OCB is visible to a supervisor for a given employee may then

    depend on the frequency of interaction he or she has with that employee. Common sense tells us

    that the more two individuals interact, the better the chance one will have an opportunity to

    observe anothers behaviors, including those that are infrequent and situational. On the other

    hand, as two individuals interact less frequently, the chances for accurate observation of non-

    routine behaviors will decline exponentially. Thus a supervisors rating of OCB is more likely to

    be determined, at least in part, by the extent to which he or she interacts with a given employee.

    Researchers have used supervisors as the source in an effort to eliminate concern over

    common-method bias (Organ et al., 2006). The concern is that employees rating their perceived

    level of justice may similarly rate their levels of citizenship in order to reduce cognitive

    dissonance. It has been noted that supervisor ratings of OCB may not be as reliable (e.g., Organ

    et al., 2006), yet this may be a bigger issue than it appears on the surface. It is easy to see that

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    23/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 23

    supervisor ratings will not capture the full extent of an employees citizenship behaviors, but the

    hidden bias is that they may be picking up some employees behaviors more than others, and this

    may be systematically related to employees perceptions of justice. Aside from introducing error

    into the outcome of interest, though (OCB ratings), differences in interaction frequency might

    also influence employees perceptions of justice.

    Over time, fairness has been operationalized in a variety of ways, but these have

    typically contained some combination of two elements: the extent to which a person feels

    respected as a valuable member of the group, and the extent to which a person is kept in the loop

    and given a chance to speak up in procedural matters (Colquitt, 2001). An employee who

    interacts more frequently with the supervisor may be more likely to hear explanations for the

    supervisors decisions and be given a chance to respond. Further, this employee may feel more

    respected and valued as a group member as a result of this increased interaction, assuming the

    supervisor is not simply attempting to monitor or control the employees behavior. Frequency of

    interaction, it seems, could be biasing both employees justice ratings and supervisors ratings

    employees citizenship behaviors in the same direction. If this is the case, we have little reason to

    trust the extant data supporting the social exchange perspective.

    Rewards through Reciprocation vs. Rewards through Involvement

    Skewed measurements of justice might also be leading OCB researchers to erroneously

    attribute the mechanism of influence based on a flawed understanding of why justice really

    matters. As Moorman, Niehoff, & Organ (1993) point out, procedural justice taps into

    procedures that allow employees to exercise voice, which might intuitively be labeled under the

    self-interest model rather than the relational or group value model (Lind & Taylor, 1988).

    Yet a later finding by Lind and colleagues (1990) indicated that it was the procedure itself that

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    24/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 24

    invoked the fairness perceptions, not control over the outcome, leading to the conclusion that

    procedural justice holds its primary value in indicating a persons worth within the group. That

    is, according to these scholars procedural justice is important to people primarily because it

    ensures they will not be taken advantage of, or because they value involvement in the group as

    an end in itslf, rather than because they associate it with specific future rewards (Moorman et al.,

    1993).

    Cropanzano, Byrne, & Bobocel (2001) perhaps provide the most comprehensive discussion as to

    why justice matters, outlining three key perspectives: instrumental, relational/group value, and

    moral virtues. The instrumental approach, they explain, is a type of economic exchange, and is

    based on the idea that people have a need for control, and fair situations offer greater control

    over the long-term. This sense of control is not limited to outcomes, but can pertain to a sense of

    control over theprocessin the form of voice. The relational approach, on the other hand, is based

    on the idea that people need to feel a part of the group, and so to the extent that they are allowed

    to do so, they will perceive that they are being treated fairly. The difference between the

    instrumental and relational views, they explain, is the distinction between economic concerns

    (instrumental) and social concerns (relational). Finally, the moral virtues approach is

    conceptualized as a more general concern for the well-being of all people.

    On the surface it would seem that the instrumental view captures economic exchange,

    while the relational view captures social exchange, and therefore only the relational view would

    be pertinent in the application of social exchange. A person who values justice primarily for the

    sake of involvement in the process, or the satisfaction of the need for control might then tend to

    develop greater trust and grow closer to leaders or organizations that allow them to become more

    involved in the process. But then why would this person perform OCB? It is possible that the

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    25/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 25

    higher quality relationship developed then sparks a felt need to reciprocate in the form of

    additional contributions to the organization or leader, and the trust that future contributions will

    be recognized.

    On the other hand, it seems possible that the person who desires involvement and even

    perceives the opportunity for involvement as fair might also wish to be involved in activities

    beyond those that are restricted specifically to his or her job. OCBs can offer a form of process

    control because they offer greater discretion than do task behaviors (Organ et al., 2006). The

    point here is that when process control serves as the major determinant or motivator of justice

    perceptions, it could also provide the impetus for performing OCB. Research on OCB from the

    social exchange perspective has typically not attempted to distinguish between participants

    reasons for valuing fairness, and the motivation for performing OCB has typically been inferred

    from theoretical arguments. If process control serves to both shape perceptions of fairness and

    motivate discretionary behavior, then is the social exchange relationship (with a focus on the

    perceived obligation to reciprocate) really explaining citizenship behavior?

    While both the instrumental and group-value cases might be argued to fit within the realm of

    social exchange (Organ et al., 2006), it is worth considering other possible benefits of this form

    of justice such as the possibility that this sense of voice may be beneficial in its own right.

    Research in Self-Determination Theory attests to the fact that it is not the perception of control

    overoutcomesthat is what drives human behavior so much as it is the perception of choice in

    initiatingbehavior (Deci & Ryan, 2000). Therefore in addition to securing future fair treatment,

    having the freedom to speak out when needed might be rewarding to the extent that it satisfies

    the basic need for autonomy, or ones perceived control over initiating behavior.

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    26/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 26

    I point this out for two reasons. First, if the freedom to get involved in procedures can be

    satisfying in its own right, then the freedom to perform OCBs might also be satisfying in its own

    right. Second, if people care about this type of fairness because they see it as a basic human need

    rather than a group benefit, they may be more likely to view it as part of the employers

    obligation, rather than a benefit to be reciprocated or an indicator of status within the

    organization.

    Social Exchange vs. Economic Exchange

    The value of contributing through OCB in the social exchange relationship is not

    explicitly specified according to the perspective. This makes sense, as each employee will tend to

    differ in the value he or she attaches to various outcomes. Social exchange does tell us there

    should be some instrumental value attached to the action, at least to the extent that employees

    engage in the exchange in the hopes of receiving benefits from the other party. But

    instrumentality seems to carry different connotations for different researchers, with some

    distinguishing between OCBs performed as a part of social exchange and OCBs performed for

    instrumental reasons (e.g., Hui, Lam and Law, 2000). Expectations of reciprocation are

    important to both social exchange and economic exchange (Cropanzano & Mitchell, 2005), it

    seems that this reciprocation is just harder to pinpoint in social exchange: the obligation is

    unspecified as to form, degree, or time of reciprocation (Organ et al., 2006, p. 72). In this

    respect OCB is also instrumental from the social exchange perspective, but the expectations

    around return are typically not made explicit. Indeed if an employee wishes to repay some

    generosity from the supervisor, he or she might wait for the optimal time to do so, when help is

    actually needed, rather than attempting to return the favor immediately. If the supervisor had

    specified in advance the conditions for repayment, he or she may have been without help at the

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    27/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 27

    crucial time. In the same way, the return employees reap from their citizenship behaviors may

    take a variety of forms, but trust in the supervisor provides some confidence that the return will

    be appropriate.

    Does this mean that employees cannot have any image of the desired outcome? Hui and

    colleagues (2000) found that some employees performed instrumentally in order to gain a

    promotion. On the one hand, this might be considered social exchange, as benefits (OCBs) are

    being given in the hopes of some future, technically unspecified (no guaranteed link between

    OCB and promotion) return. But others might argue that this should not be considered social

    exchange, as employees tended to attach a specific return value to the OCBs (promotion), and

    discontinued the exchange once this value was received (they performed fewer OCBs after

    promotion). A return in the form of promotion was not made explicit by the employer or the

    employee at the time of helping- and the degree to which it was expected, as opposed to some

    other reward form, is unclear.

    It should be fairly obvious that returns are generally expected for good deeds that are

    performed, but the link between actions and specific outcomes might be better described as

    degrees along a continuum of expectation around contingency, rather than a presence of a

    contingent reward versus an ambiguous exchange of rewards. As other scholars have pointed

    out, this line can also become blurred by differing perspectives on what is considered part of the

    job (Coyle-Shapiro et al., 2004). This surfaces an important question that is not explicitly

    addressed by the social exchange perspective: The nature of the exchange (social or economic) is

    often difficult to distinguish, without knowledge of the underlying intents, values, and history

    between the two sides. Further, the perspective of one side relies on the reactions of the other,

    therefore exchange is unlikely to be accurately assessed at a single point in time.

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    28/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 28

    Re-evaluating the Reasons for the Social Exchange Framework

    I have reviewed empirical evidence in terms of social exchange and discussed areas

    where more research is needed. In this section I revisit the value of the social exchange

    perspective at a higher level by examining the framework in terms of its original purpose (Organ,

    1988; Organ et al., 2006): explaining the relationship between satisfaction (attitudes) and

    performance beyond that caused by constraints such as the relative lack of flexibility around

    changing the level of task behavior.

    We now know that OCBs are often rewarded as a part of performance evaluations, and

    linked to other individual career outcomes (Bergeron, 2007; Podsakoff et al., 2009). Even in the

    absence of explicit linkages between OCB and tangible outcomes, role breadth can make

    cognitive distinction between OCB and task behavior disappear. Given that the line between

    OCBs and task behavior is becoming increasingly blurry (Coyle-Shapiro et al., 2004), we must

    wonder whether there is truly any added flexibility around the performance of OCBs. Further,

    the unspoken pressures around discretionary behavior might be more binding than overtly

    imposed codes of conduct like formal descriptions of task behavior (Barker, 1993). One recent

    study indicated that about 75% of the participants felt strong pressure to engage in OCB (Vigoda,

    2007) and others have demonstrated the powerful role of workplace norms around OCB (Ehrhart

    & Naumann, 2004). If the variance around timing and form of OCB performance comes from

    real-time situational demands rather than an employees discretion, the social exchange

    perspective is not applicable.

    In addition to concerns about the volition involved in citizenship behavior, it is not clear

    that the link between attitudes and performance in the form of OCB is any stronger than the link

    between attitudes and task performance (Edwards, Bell, Arthur, Winfred, & Decuir, 2008). This

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    29/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 29

    might tell us that social exchange is a less viable explanation for OCB performance, it might

    reflect the problems associated with measurement of complex phenomena, or it might be

    indicative of an separate set of constraints around changing performance of OCB according to

    ones attitude. The first alternative creates serious doubts over the validity of the social exchange

    perspective, the second creates doubts over extant findings, and the third tells us that social

    exchange might not be important for the reasons originally proposed, but does not completely

    rule out its validity.

    Conclusion

    In this paper I have tried to highlight some of the ways we can begin to examine the assumptions

    underlying the social exchange approach. The purpose here is to both clarify research from the

    social exchange perspective, and to spark inquiry into other possibilities. At the core of social

    exchange is the understanding that in the absence of explicit rewards, humans give to others

    because they expect some benefit in return. This explanation is somewhat vague, and in this way

    it mirrors the perspective itself. The lack of clarity afforded by previous accounts makes it

    difficult to compare results, answer questions, and refine theory, which is needed in order to

    move forward in this area (Podsakoff et al., 2000). In order to determine more precise

    measurements, strong, explicit theoretical guidance will need to be at the forefront. This is

    fundamental to provide a common language and consensus for researchers to systematically

    examine the constructs of interest and build upon each others work (Pfeffer, 1993).

    In terms of application to OCB, researchers generally assume that it is not the specific

    benefit employees are receiving that is critical nor the primary motive driving their action, but

    rather it is the trust that reciprocation will prevail that is key. Yet without careful attention to the

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    30/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 30

    nature of expectations and motivations, we cannot rule out other explanations for the results we

    find. Are humans acting selfishly, selflessly, or are they exponentially benefitting both the self

    and the organization? Depending on the answer, we might intervene in slightly different ways

    (e.g., decrease barriers rather than trying to change employee perceptions).

    In conclusion, if we wish continue down the social exchange path, I would urge

    researchers to carefully consider and systematically test the assumptions and alternatives raised

    in this paper. On the other hand, given the small effect sizes and alternative explanations,

    perhaps his time might be better spent pursuing other, more promising, alternatives.

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    31/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 31

    References

    Barker, J. R. (1993). Tightening the iron cage: Concertive control in self-managing teams,

    Administrative Science Quarterly, 38: 408-437.

    Bergeron, D. M. (2007)The potential paradox of organizational citizenship behavior: Goodcitizens at what cost?Academy of Management Review, 32(4), 1078-1095.

    Blau, P. (1964).Exchange and power in social life. New York: Wiley.

    Bolino, M. C. (1999). Citizenship and impression management: Good soldiers or good actors?

    Academy of Management Review, 24, 8298.

    Bolino, M. C., Grant, A., & Harvey, J. (in press). Citizenship and self-worth: The role of

    citizenship motives

    Bolino, M. C., Turnley, W. H., & Niehoff, B. P. (2004). The other side of the story:

    Reexamining prevailing assumptions about organizational citizenship behavior. Human

    Resource Management Review, 14, 229-246.

    Colquitt, J. A. (2001). On the dimensionality of organizational justice: A construct validation of

    a measure.Journal of Applied Psychology, 86,386-400.DOI: 10.1037//0021-

    9010.86.3.386.

    Coyle-Shapiro, J. A-M., Kessler, I., & Purcell, J. (2004). Exploring organizizationally directed

    citizenship behavior: Reciprocity or its my job?Journal of Management Studies, 41,85-

    106.

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    32/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 32

    Cropanzano, R., Byrne, Z. S., Bobocel, D. R., & Rupp, D.E. (2001). Moral virtues, fairness

    heuristics, social entities, and other denizens of organizational justice.Journal of

    Vocational Behavior, 58,164-209.

    Cropanzano, R. & Mitchell, M. S. (2005). Social exchange theory: An interdisciplinary review.

    Journal of Management, 31, 874-902. DOI: 10.1177/0149206305279602

    Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The what and why of goal pursuits: Human needs and

    the self-determination of behavior. PsychologicalInquiry, 11, 227268.

    Edwards, B. D., Bell, S. T., Arthur, J., Winfred, & Decuir, A. D. (2008). Relationships between

    facets of job satisfaction and task and contextual performance.Applied Psychology: An

    International Review, 57(3), 441-465. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2008.00328.x

    Ehrhart, M. G., & Naumann, S. E. (2004). Organizational citizenship behavior in work groups: A

    group norms approach.Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(6), 960-974.

    Fassina, N. E., Jones, D. A., & Uggerslev, K. L. (2008a). Meta-analytic tests of relationships

    between organizational justice and citizenship behavior: Testing agent-system and shared-

    variance models.Journal of Organizational Behavior, 29,805-828.DOI: 10.1002/job.494

    Fassina, N. E., Jones, D. A., & Uggerslev, K. L. (2008b). Relationship clean-up time: Using

    meta-analysis to clarify relationships among job satisfaction, perceived fairness, and

    citizenship behaviors. Journal of Management, 34,161-188.

    Grant, A. M. (2007). Relational job design and the motivation to make a prosocial difference.

    Academy of Management Review, 32,393-417.

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    33/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 33

    Grant, A. M., & Mayer, D. M. (2009). Good soldiers and good actors: Prosocial and impression

    management motives as interactive predictors of affiliative citizenship behaviors.Journal

    of Applied Psychology, 94(4), 900-912.

    Hui, C., Lam, S. S. K., & Law, K. K. S. (2000). Instrumental values of organizational citizenship

    behavior for promotion : A field quasi-experiment.Journal of Applied Psychology, 85,

    822-828.

    Konovsky, M. A., & Pugh, S. D. (1994). Citizenship behavior and social exchange.The

    Academy of Management Journal, 37(3), 656-669.

    Lavelle, J. J., Brockner, J., Kanovsky, M. A., Price, K. H., Henley, A. B., Taneja, A., & Vinekar,

    V. (2009). Commitment, procedural fairness, and organizational citizenship behavior: a

    multifoci analysis.Journal of Organizational Behavior, 30(3),337-357.

    LePine, J. A., Erez, A., & Johnson, D. E. (2002). The nature and dimensionality of

    organizational citizenship behavior: A critical review and meta-analysis.Journal of

    Applied Psychology, 87, 52-65.

    Lind, E. A., & Tyler, T. R. (1988). The social psychology of procedural justice.New York:

    Plenum Press.

    Lind, E. A. & van den Bos, K. (2002). When fairness works: Toward a general theory of

    uncertainty management.Research in Organizational Behavior, 24,181-223.

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    34/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 34

    Moorman, R. H. (1991). Relationship between organizational justice and organizational

    citizenship behaviors: Do fairness perceptions influence employee citizenship?Journal of

    Applied Psychology, 76,845-855.

    Moorman, R. H., Niehoff, B. P., & Organ, D. W. 1993. Treating employees fairly and

    organizational citizenship behaviors: Sorting the effects of job satisfaction, organizational

    commitment, and procedural justice.Employee Responsibilities andRights Journal, 6,

    209-225.

    Morrison, E. W. (1994). Role definitions and organizational citizenship behaviors: The

    importance of the employees' perspective.Academy ofManagement Journal, 37, 1543-

    1567.

    Motowidlo, S. J., Borman, W. C., & Schmit, M. J. (1997). A theory of individual differences in

    task and contextual performance.Human Performance, 10,71-83.

    Organ, D. W. (1988). Organizational citizenship behavior: The good soldier syndrome.

    Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.

    Organ, D. W. (1997). Organizational citizenship behavior: Its construct clean-up time. Human

    Performance, 10, 8597.

    Organ, D. W., & Konovsky, M. (1989). Cognitive versus affective determinants of

    organizational citizenship behavior.Journal of Applied Psychology, 74,157-164.

    Organ, D. W., & Moorman, R. H. (1993). Fairness and organizational citizenship behavior: What

    are the connections? Social Justice Research, 6,5-18.

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    35/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 35

    Organ, D. W., Podsakoff, P. M. and MacKenzie, S. B. (2006). Organizational Citizenship

    Behavior: Its Nature,Antecedents, and Consequences. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Pfeffer, J. (1993). Barriers to the advance of organizational science: Paradigm development as a

    dependent variable.Academy of Management Review, 18, 599-620

    Podsakoff, P.M., MacKenzie, S. B., Paine, J. B., & Bachrach, D. G. (2000). Organizational

    citizenship behaviors: A critical review of the theoretical and empirical literature and

    suggestions for future research. Journal of Management, 26(3),513-563.

    Podsakoff, N. P., Blume, B. D., Whiting, S. W., & Podsakoff, P. M. (2009). Individual- and

    organizational-level consequences of organizational citizenship behaviors: A meta-

    analysis.Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(1), 122-141

    Rioux, S. M., & Penner, L. A. (2001). The causes of organizational citizenship behavior: A

    motivational analysis.Journal of Applied Psychology, 86,13061314.

    Snape, E., & Redman, T. (in press). HRM Practices, organizational citizenship behaviour, and

    performance: A multi-level analysis. Journal of Management Studies.

    Skarlicki, D. P., & Latham, G. P. (1997). Leadership training in organizational justice to increase

    citizenship behavior within a labor union: A replication. Personnel Psychology, 50,617-

    633,

    Van der Vegt, G. S., Bunderson, J. S., & Oosterhof, A. (2006). Expertness diversity and

    interpersonal helping in teams: Why those who need the most help end up getting the least.

    Academy of Management Journal, 49,877-893.

  • 8/13/2019 wp-10-03

    36/36

    OCB and Social Exchange 36

    Williams, L. J., & Anderson, S. E. (1991). Job satisfaction and organizational commitment as

    predictors of organizational citizenship and in-role behaviors.Journal of Management, 17,

    601-617.

    Yoon, M. H., & Suh, J. (2003). Organizational citizenship behaviors and service quality as

    external effectiveness of contact employees.Journal of Business Research, 56(8), 597.

    doi:10.1016/S0148-2963(01)00290-9

    Zellars, K. L. and Tepper, B. J. (2003). Beyond social exchange: new directions for

    organizational citizenship behavior theory and research. In Martocchio, J. J. and Ferris, G.

    R. (Eds),Research in Personnel and Human Resources Management. Amsterdam:

    Elsevier JAI Press, 22, 395424.