Visual Cognition-in 1814 words! - Charles Brand

download Visual Cognition-in 1814 words! - Charles Brand

of 8

Transcript of Visual Cognition-in 1814 words! - Charles Brand

  • 8/8/2019 Visual Cognition-in 1814 words! - Charles Brand

    1/8

    ASSIGNMENT REPORT FORM

    Student ID

    58117156

    Tutor ID

    Name:

    Date Student sent

    assignment:06/12

    /2010Date Received by

    tutor:Date Sent to

    Oscail:Overall Mark Obtained:

    Module:PSY 4 Group: 1 TMA No:Summary of Performance*

    Performance Components BandsExcellent

    (H1)

    Marks

    range:

    70-100%

    Very

    Good

    (H2.1)

    Marks

    range:

    60-69%

    Good

    (H2.2)

    Marks

    range:

    50-59%

    Fair

    (H3)

    Marks

    range:

    40-49%

    Weak

    Marks

    range:

    35-39%

    Poor

    Marks

    range:

    below

    35%

    Not

    applicable

    Attention to assignment task

    Analysis

    StructureUse of sources

    References

    Introduction

    ConclusionSpelling/Grammar

    Presentation (Style)* This table facilitates the assessment of your performance in selected components of your assignment, and is

    designed to alert you to the general areas of strength and/ or in need of improvement in your work. Please note that

    the components are not equal in terms of contribution to your overall mark. Attention to Assignment Task,

    Analysis and Structure are the three most important criteria for assessment. Please note that the total mark

    indicated is based on an evaluation of your overall performance in the set assignment.

  • 8/8/2019 Visual Cognition-in 1814 words! - Charles Brand

    2/8

    S UMMARY COMMENTS

    ADVICE FOR FUTURE ASSIGNMENTS

    ANNOTATED F EEDBACK

    (Refer to Assignment for the sections relating to the following comments)

    1.

    2.

  • 8/8/2019 Visual Cognition-in 1814 words! - Charles Brand

    3/8

    In recent years, the study of cognitive psychology has become influenced by neuroscience and neuropsychology. What

    has this type of research told us about our capacity for attention, or visual perception?

    Introduction

    Cognitive psychology is generally agreed to be concerned with the scientific study of processes in

    the mind that help us to make sense of our external world. It is also concerned with how these processes

    function and how we appropriate responses to our world (Solso, 2001; Eysenck & Keane, 2005).

    Cognitive science is a multi-disciplinary science that includes areas of computer science, artificial

    intelligence, philosophy, linguistics and neuroscience (Wylie & McMahon, 2010). Within the neuroscientific

    area are; neuropsychology, cognitive neuropsychology and cognitive psychology. Cognitive

    neuropsychology is the closest relative to cognitive psychology as it developed from a merging of

    neuroscience and cognitive psychology (Solso, 2001 p.38). Cognitive neuropsychologists model the

    workings of a normal brain and compare this to the functionality of an impaired brain. Neuropsychological

    research conclusions can have a direct influence upon the field of cognitive psychology; they can thus be

    though of as complementary disciplines. (Ellis & Young, 1996, p.24).

    Getting the Bigger Picture

    While cognitive psychology approaches this examination by way of observation (Quinlan &

    Dyson, 2008), the neuropsychological approach consists of observing the physiological activity in the

    brain during cognitive tasks. One of the most influential technological areas to have had a direct impact

    on the mutual advancement of disciplines interested in neurological and neuropsychological cognitive

    function is that of neurophysiological sensing. The lesion method is widely used in neurological research

    and Damasio (2000, p. 77) defines the purpose of a lesion research as relating a specific type of

    behaviour within a neuropsychological experiment with the site of the lesion. While of use to the cognitive

    neuropsychologist in terms of correlating physiological location and elements of cognitive function, lesion

    based studies do not tell much about the cognitive processes at work (Ellis & Young, 1996; Groome et al,

    1999). Lesion studies have however given significant weight to the assertion that; functional brain

    deficits cannot necessarily be attributed to particular regions of the brain where lesions are located

    (Steinberg & Mio, 2008). This is of importance, as it highlights the fact that that in order for the brain

    (from a neuropsychological perspective) to be able to perform functions of the mind (from a cognitive

    psychological perspective) such as problem solving, abstraction, language acquisition and visual

    interpretation, etc. we must acknowledge that many disparate physical parts of the brain are involved

    (Damasio, 2000, p. 78; Steinberg & Mio, 2008, p. 207).

  • 8/8/2019 Visual Cognition-in 1814 words! - Charles Brand

    4/8

    The goal of neurophysiological sensing is to represent areas of elevated glucose and oxygen

    levels as well as increased blood flow. All cognitive activity requires increased metabolic activity, so by

    providing an accurate representation of this increase we can view particular cognitive processes occurring

    at specific locations in the brain (Solso, 2000, p. 56). Consequently, we can see neurophysiologicalimaging is useful for determining functional as well as structural information.

    Positron Emission Tomography (PET) brain scanning involves the introduction of minute amounts

    of radioactive material into the bloodstream which are tracked in the brain in order to identify areas of

    increased neural activity. Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) and Echo-Planar MRI (EPI) also provide a

    picture of the brain. Both utilise powerful magnets that align the hydrogen molecules found in water. The

    difference in densities between the hydrogen atoms and other tissue are then represented to build a

    graphical representation of the brain. Images produced by both MRI and EPI have been prone to blurring

    due to patient movement during the imaging process but this problem is currently being addressed (New

    Capabilities, 2006) and makes available previously unavailable cohorts of subjects for examination e.g.

    Parkinson's patients and children who are unable to remain stationary during the imaging process. As we

    have shown, lesion studies and imaging techniques provide cognitive scientists with powerful physical

    evidence that can be related, in particular ways, to behaviour and function deficit in humans. However,

    from a perspective of cognitive psychology this information must be regarded as partial in the overall

    view of behavioural cause and effect the map, but not the territory.

    Visual Perception - The Current Picture

    An early theoretical position on the nature of visual perception was known as intelligent

    perception; a precursor to constructive theory. Constructivist perception theory holds that perception

    takes place within an array of cognitive processes (Coren, Ward & Enns, 1994, p. 14). Rock (1981) found

    that the role of attention played a significant part in the ability of subjects to process a complex visual

    form. This integrative approach holds that several elements of our cognition (attention and visual

    perception) interact to construct our final percept.

    Palmer's work involved testing the parsing paradox i.e. whether we recognise the parts of our

    environment and build up a representation through our vision (bottom-up processing), or begin with a

    complete representation and progress to individually recognise the individual elements (top-down

    processing). This is described by Palmer as being part of the overall process of perceptual organisation

    (cited in Weiner et al. 2003, p. 179). The dichotomous processes of the parsing paradox have been

    theorised by Palmer to occur simultaneously, and as he demonstrated, context plays an important role

  • 8/8/2019 Visual Cognition-in 1814 words! - Charles Brand

    5/8

    during this interpretive process (Palmer, cited in Solso, 2000, p.121). He showed that in a simple line

    drawing, facial features (ear, nose eye and mouth) are easily recognised when shown in context of a

    complete facial representation but are less so when shown in isolation. However, once the simple line

    drawings of the isolated features are more elaborately represented they become more easily identifiablethus demonstrating the bottom-up and top-down principles at work within a contextual framework (cited

    in Solso, 2000, p.121).

    As the neurological load capacity and exact adherence requirements of template matching theory

    became more problematic (Solso, 2000), Biederman proposed a much more flexible and pragmatic

    approach. Intelligent assembly of his geons (geometrical ions) allows for the fabrication of complex

    shapes from a library of 24 simple and distinct forms (Solso, 2000, p.125). In terms of contextual

    influence, Biederman's experiment (cited in Weiner et al. 2003, p. 1999) showed that subjects who were

    presented with photographs containing randomly arranged objects took longer and were less accurate in

    their recognition. This demonstrates that we seek appropriateness when attending to objects within our

    visual field. This work along with that of Rock and Palmer all contributes to constructivist theories of

    visual perception; they are driven by the belief that complex higher cognitive processes are paramount in

    our attempts to accurately perceive the world.

    Direct perception conversely- holds that the stimulus contains all the information required to

    formulate our visual perception, and that the role of cognition and learning is negligible (Solso, 2000).

    Direct perception could be seen to be closely aligned with an ecologically based psychological outlook

    insofar as it stresses the connective properties between the environment and it's inhabitants. J.J. Gibson

    was a leading proponent of this ecological stance and he argued that it was our relationship with our

    environment as well as the available stimuli garnered by our senses that enabled us to form our percepts

    (Galotti, 2009). This relationship underlines an important aspect of Gibsonian thought; that of

    affordances. Best described by Michaels & Carello (1981) as the acts or behaviours permitted by objects,

    places and events (cited in Galotti 2009, p. 89) affordances are the properties of objects in our

    environment that are presented to our senses and guide our interaction and behaviour with those objects

    (Galotti, 2009). Therefore, according to Gibson the reason we do not swim in a chair and sit on a lake of

    water is because we pick up on the affordances these objects offer, hence our action and perception are

    inextricably linked.

    Another principle within direct perception is that of invariance, which holds that certain properties

    of a stimulant object remain fixed even though the image projected onto the retina may change

    constantly due to movement of the observer (Coren, Ward & Enns, 2000; Galotti, 2008). In the

  • 8/8/2019 Visual Cognition-in 1814 words! - Charles Brand

    6/8

    Gibsonian tradition this highlights the importance of the relational value between stimuli e.g. the same

    piece of music can be recognised even when played in two different keys because the main elements (the

    notes) have remained invariant. The influence of invariance upon visual perception was strongly

    supported by experiments conducted by Johansson (cited in Galotti, 2008, p.87). Lights were fitted to ahuman subject at particular points along the body and they were dressed in black and placed against a

    black background. Subjects were then videoed while performing a series of actions. Study participants

    instantly identified different actions from the video when they could only see the movement of the lights.

    This demonstrated the possibility that certain patterns of motion were susceptible to some degree of

    invariance. From a neuropsychological perspective, the concept of invariance in cognitive psychology is

    highly relevant. Common fundamental organisational structures between normal human subjects are the

    bedrock upon which modelling, the hypotheses for neurophysiological experimentation, is formed

    (Temple, 1997). Subsequently, both disciplines gain from this shared conceptualisation.

    The view that all required information for forming percepts is one shared by the computational

    theories of perception. Computational theory differs from direct perception however in that it posits that

    simple dimensions such as line endings, edges etc. can be pieced together and synthesised in order to

    present a conceptual picture of the world (Coren, Ward & Enns, 2008). Marr (cited by Galloti, 2009, p.77)

    posits that there are several sequential steps in the process of visual perception e.g. colour analysis,

    movement analysis etc. These steps, or modules, operate autonomously, without influence from other

    modules in terms of input and output and do not take regard of real-world knowledge . Thus the viewer

    builds up a partial representation from the arriving stimuli and this can be viewed as a bottom-up

    process. Once the building process reaches a certain point of completion; real-world knowledge or other

    subjective elements (top-down processes) are incorporated to present the final perceptual experience.

    Here we see echoes of neurophysiological rationale previously mentioned and posited by Damasio (2000)

    that concurs with a cognitive psychological approach to visual perception.

    Conclusion

    From a cognitive neurophysiological perspective, in the case of visual perception, we have shown

    that some of the approaches taken by cognitive psychology have more to offer than others. Gibson's

    direct perspective has been criticised for not being sharply enough defined in relation to the concepts of

    variance and affordances (Galotti, 2008), and without sufficiently defined concepts where do cognitive

    neurological scientists begin to investigate possible physiological associations? Computational

    perspectives have more credible common ground and share the concept of modularity in visual

  • 8/8/2019 Visual Cognition-in 1814 words! - Charles Brand

    7/8

    perception purported by Marr (cited by Ellis and Young, 1996, p.61), which complements a neurological

    approach insofar as it supports a more isomorphic hypothesis of brain function. What has become clear

    then is that there is a bi-directional flow of influence between the areas of cognitive neuroscience and

    cognitive psychology, and that attention can be demonstrably shown, through neuroscience, to have astrong bearing on our visual percept.

  • 8/8/2019 Visual Cognition-in 1814 words! - Charles Brand

    8/8

    References

    Coren, S., Ward, L.M., Enns, J.T. (1994). Sensation and Perception. Texas, U.S.A.:Harcourt Brace &

    Company

    Damasio, H. (2000). The Lesion Method in Cognitive Neuroscience. In F. Boller & J. Grafman Eds.Handbook of Neuropsychology: Volume 1 Section 1. Introduction. (2 nd ed.)(pp. 77-102)

    Amsterdam, Netherlands:Elsevier Science.

    Eysenck, M.W., Keane, M.T. (2005). Cognitive Psychology: A Student's Handbook. (5 th ed.). New

    York, U.S.A.:Psychology Press.

    Galotti, K.M. (2009). Cognitive Psychology: In and Out of the Laboratory. Belmount, California:Cengage

    Learning

    Groome, D., Dewart, H., Esgate, A., Gurney, K., Kemp, R., Towell, N. (1999). An introduction to

    cognitive psychology: processes and disorders. Cornwall, Great Britain:Psychology Press.

    Quinlan, P., Dyson, B. (2008). Cognitive Psychology . Lombarda, Italy:Pearson Education.

    Weiner, I.B., Healy, A.F., Proctor, R.W., Schinka J.A. (2003). Visual Perception of Objects. In. I.B.

    Weiner, A. F. Healy, D.K. Freedheim, & R.W. Proctor Eds. Handbook of Psychology:

    Experimental psychology Volume 4 (pp. 179-212) New Jersey, U.S.A.:John Wiley and Sons.

    Rock, I. (1981). The Effect of Inattention on Form Perception. Journal of Experimental

    Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, (7)2, 275-285.

    Rock, I. (1983). The Logic of Perception Massachusetts, U.S.A:M.I.T. Press.

    Solso, R.L. (2001). Cognitive Psychology . (6 th ed.). Massachusetts, U.S.A:Allyn & Bacon.

    Steinberg, R., Mio, J. (2008). Cognitive Psychology. Belmount, California:Cengage Learning

    St. Joseph's Hospital and Medical Center (2006, January 5). New Technology Enhances MRI Capabilities.

    ScienceDaily. Retrieved December

    http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2006/01/060105083855.htm

    Temple, C. (1997). Developmental cognitive neuropsychology. East Sussex, U.K.:Psychology Press

    Wylie, J., McMahon, C. (2010) Oscail Psychology 4:Cognitive Psychology Dublin, Ireland:Oscail.

    http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2006/01/060105083855.htmhttp://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2006/01/060105083855.htm