Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

22
Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in Personality Tests A Proposed New Method and Practical Guidelines for Data Analysis Tunca, Burak Published in: The Wiley Handbook of Personality Assessment DOI: 10.1002/9781119173489.ch25 2016 Document Version: Peer reviewed version (aka post-print) Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Tunca, B. (2016). Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in Personality Tests: A Proposed New Method and Practical Guidelines for Data Analysis. In U. Kumar (Ed.), The Wiley Handbook of Personality Assessment (pp. 346-360). Wiley-Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119173489.ch25 Total number of authors: 1 General rights Unless other specific re-use rights are stated the following general rights apply: Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain • You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal Read more about Creative commons licenses: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/ Take down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Download date: 27. Oct. 2021

Transcript of Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

Page 1: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

LUND UNIVERSITY

PO Box 117221 00 Lund+46 46-222 00 00

Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in Personality Tests

A Proposed New Method and Practical Guidelines for Data AnalysisTunca, Burak

Published in:The Wiley Handbook of Personality Assessment

DOI:10.1002/9781119173489.ch25

2016

Document Version:Peer reviewed version (aka post-print)

Link to publication

Citation for published version (APA):Tunca, B. (2016). Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in Personality Tests: A ProposedNew Method and Practical Guidelines for Data Analysis. In U. Kumar (Ed.), The Wiley Handbook of PersonalityAssessment (pp. 346-360). Wiley-Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119173489.ch25

Total number of authors:1

General rightsUnless other specific re-use rights are stated the following general rights apply:Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authorsand/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by thelegal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private studyor research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain • You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal

Read more about Creative commons licenses: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/Take down policyIf you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will removeaccess to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

Download date: 27. Oct. 2021

Page 2: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

Running Head: USING JOHNSON-NEYMAN PROCEDURE TO DETECT ITEM BIAS 1

Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in Personality Tests:

A Proposed New Method and Practical Guidelines for Data Analysis

Burak Tunca

University of Agder, Norway

Author Note

This is the author’s version of the following book chapter: Tunca, B. (2016)

Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in Personality Tests, in The

Wiley Handbook of Personality Assessment (ed U. Kumar), John Wiley & Sons, Ltd,

Chichester, UK., which has been published in final form at

http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9781119173489.ch25/summary. This article

may be used for non-commercial purposes in accordance with Wiley Terms and

Conditions for Self-Archiving.

Page 3: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

USING JOHNSON-NEYMAN PROCEDURE TO DETECT ITEM BIAS 2

Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in Personality Tests:

A Proposed New Method and Practical Guidelines for Data Analysis

Personality researchers are often interested in examining trait differences between

groups. For instance, are men more assertive than women? Or, are the Americans more

impulsive than the Chinese? The common practice in answering such questions is first to

administer the same personality scale to members of each group, and then to compare groups’

scores on the scale. Validity of such comparisons, however, rests on the assumption that the scale

items are not biased: Respondents with different group memberships understand and interpret the

scale items in a similar manner. If this assumption is violated, the validity of the results becomes

questionable. Personality researchers have long been warned against this potential problem in

between-group comparisons (e.g., Smith, 2002; Thissen, Steinberg, & Gerrard, 1986).

The significance of this problem has also led to development of various statistical

techniques to detect biased items (for reviews, see Reynolds, 2000; Zumbo, 2007). In the current

chapter, I first review one of these techniques known as the analysis of variance (ANOVA)

procedure (van de Vijver and Leung, 1997), which has been widely used in personality research

(e.g., Caprara, Barbaranelli, Bermúdez, Maslach, & Ruch, 2000; Ramírez-Esparza, Gosling,

Benet-Martínez, Potter, & Pennebaker, 2006; Vecchione, Alessandri, & Barbaranelli, 2012).

Next, I propose an alternative to the ANOVA method. The alternative method, which is based on

the Johnson-Neyman procedure (Johnson & Neyman, 1936), has the potential to overcome some

of the major weaknesses of the ANOVA procedure. I introduce the proposed method in a non-

technical manner and I present practical guidelines for data analysis using an add-on for

mainstream statistical software packages (PROCESS Macro; Hayes, 2013), so that researchers

Page 4: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

USING JOHNSON-NEYMAN PROCEDURE TO DETECT ITEM BIAS 3

who are inexperienced in item bias analysis can easily apply the Johnson-Neyman procedure to

their research.

What is Item Bias?

Item bias, also known as differential item functioning (DIF), refers to item level

anomalies within an instrument that can threaten the validity of group comparisons. In

psychology research, an item is considered to be biased when respondents with different group

memberships score differently on a survey item, while being at the same level of the latent trait

(Smith, 2002; van de Vijver & Leung, 2011). To illustrate, Santor, Ramsay, and Zuroff (1994)

examined gender level item bias in the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck, Ward, Mendelson,

Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961) and found that men and women, who were equally depressed,

responded differently to an item related to their perceived body image distortion: At all levels of

depression, women were more likely than men to report concerns about looking unattractive. The

authors concluded that the item was endorsed differently across gender groups, thus it was

biased, and scores for this item would lead to misleading results when comparing the degree of

depression between men and women.

Item bias exists in two forms: Uniform and non-uniform (van de Vijver & Leung, 1997).

Uniform bias manifests itself when there is a systematic difference between groups for an item

score across all score levels (see figure 26.1-a). This indicates that one group endorses the item

differently than the other group (either at a higher or lower level), regardless of their total score

on the latent trait. Non-uniform bias, on the other hand, is a less common form, and it occurs

when the item score differences between groups are not systematic across all score levels (see

figure 26.1-b). For example, an item may be more discriminating for one group at lower score

Page 5: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

USING JOHNSON-NEYMAN PROCEDURE TO DETECT ITEM BIAS 4

levels and more discriminating for the other group at higher score levels, which implies an

interaction effect of group membership and total score on the latent trait. Various statistical

techniques have been proposed to test for evidence of uniform and non-uniform item bias. While

item response theory (IRT) and logistic regression methods are commonly used to detect item

bias in dichotomous scores, the ANOVA procedure (van de Vijver and Leung, 1997) has been a

popular technique in examining item bias in numerical scores.

<FIGURE 26.1 HERE>

The ANOVA Procedure

To test for evidence of item bias in unidimensional scales, van de Vijver and Leung

(1997) introduced a procedure based on conditional analysis of variance (ANOVA). To illustrate

how the ANOVA procedure functions, consider a test instrument with ten items (item 1 to item

10) measured with a seven-point Likert-type scale (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree),

which was administered to members of two groups (group A and group B) to examine between-

group differences. The ANOVA procedure is centered on three variables (i.e., item, group, and

score level): Group and score level are categorical independent variables and item is a

continuous dependent variable.

Item is the dependent variable in the ANOVA procedure, and it refers to the item we

would like to examine for evidence of bias in the research instrument. In our hypothetical

example there are ten items (item 1 to item 10), and in the ANOVA procedure each item is

examined independently for evidence of bias. Group and score level are the independent

variables. Group refers to the groups that are compared in the study (e.g., gender or culture; in

Page 6: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

USING JOHNSON-NEYMAN PROCEDURE TO DETECT ITEM BIAS 5

our example group A and group B). The group variable is dummy coded in the dataset (e.g.,

group A = 0 and group B = 1).

Unlike item and group, which are already present in the dataset, score level is a new

variable that must calculated by the researcher. This is done by first computing a total score

variable, which is simply the sum of all item scores (i.e., item 1 + item 2 + … + item 10) for each

respondent. Recall that the test instrument had ten items measured by a seven-point scale. If a

respondent selects 1 (strongly disagree) for all items, she will have the lowest possible total score

(i.e., 10 x 1 = 10). On the other hand, if she selects 7 (strongly agree) for all items, her total score

will be the maximum possible score (i.e., 10 x 7 = 70). Thus, each respondent’s total score on the

test instrument is a value between 10 and 70. Next, the total score variable is transformed into

the score level variable. Score level is a categorical variable that is created by splitting the

continuous total score variable into groups based on predetermined cut-points. The aim of this

procedure is to group respondents with similar total scores, so that groups are ranging from “low

total scores” to “high total scores”. According to van de Vijver and Leung (1997), the cut-points

should be determined in a way that each score level contains at least 50 respondents. If the

sample size is 500, for instance, there are eight or nine groups in the score level variable (for a

more detailed explanation of this procedure, see van de Vijver & Leung, 1997). It should be

noted that these cut-points, which designate score levels, are arbitrary.

Once the three variables (i.e., item, group, and score level) are ready, a conditional two-

way ANOVA is conducted to test for item bias. Conceptually, the ANOVA procedure tests the

null hypothesis that “item scores are not different between groups” across different score levels.

The ANOVA identifies three effects on the dependent variable item: The main effects of score

level and group, and the group x score level interaction effect. The main effect of score level is

Page 7: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

USING JOHNSON-NEYMAN PROCEDURE TO DETECT ITEM BIAS 6

of little interest to the researcher; respondents at higher score levels usually have higher score on

the item than respondents at lower score levels do, thus this main effect will often be significant.

The significance levels of the remaining two effects are, however, essential to the ANOVA

procedure.

When both the main effect of group and the group x score level interaction effect are

non-significant, the item under scrutiny is unbiased (van de Vijver & Leung, 1997). On the other

hand, a significant main effect of group and group x score level interaction effect indicate the

presence of uniform and non-uniform bias, respectively (van de Vijver & Leung, 1997). Given

our example, if respondents in Group A score higher (or lower) than respondents in Group B for

item 1 at all score levels, this uniform bias for item 1 will be evident as a significant main effect

of the group variable. Finally, if the differences between groups are not equivalent across all

score levels, this non-uniform bias will be evident as a significant group x score level interaction

effect.

The ANOVA procedure is easy to apply and interpret using mainstream statistical

software packages. It also enables visual examination of item bias with graphical displays.

Despite such advantages, the ANOVA procedure has two interrelated shortcomings: The need to

discretize a continuous variable (i.e., transforming the continuous total score variable into the

categorical score level variable) and the need for large sample sizes. Given that the higher the

number of score levels, the more sensitive the analysis of item bias (van de Vijver & Leung,

1997, 2011), large sample sizes become necessary for the ANOVA procedure to achieve the

“minimum 50 respondents per score level” rule of thumb and to retain the statistical power

reduced as a result of the discretization procedure. Large sample sizes may, however, inflate

significance levels, thereby making statistical significance testing less informative. Recognizing

Page 8: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

USING JOHNSON-NEYMAN PROCEDURE TO DETECT ITEM BIAS 7

this issue, Van de Vijver and Leung (2011) suggest using effect sizes instead of significance

values to detect biased items with the ANOVA procedure.

There is compelling evidence in the literature concerning the limitations of discretizing a

continuous variable based on arbitrary groups (e.g., Fitzsimons, 2008; Irwin & McClelland,

2003; MacCallum, Zhang, Preacher, & Rucker, 2002; Maxwell & Delaney, 1993). Taken

together, these studies conclude that the discretization procedure should be avoided as it may

result in reduced statistical power, loss of information, and misleading results. We can therefore

conclude that the main shortcoming of the ANOVA procedure stems from discretizing a

continuous variable to create a categorical alternative. Nonetheless, there are alternative methods

such as the Johnson-Neyman procedure (Johnson & Neyman, 1936), which allows conducting

similar analyses without discretizing continuous variables.

The Johnson-Neyman Procedure

The Johnson-Neyman procedure, introduced by Johnson and Neyman (1936) and

extended to multiple regression models by Preacher, Curran, and Bauer (2006), has been

proposed as an alternative to analysis of covariance techniques (D’Alonzo, 2004; Miyazaki &

Maier, 2005). Analogous to the ANOVA method, The Johnson-Neyman procedure is used for

examining the conditional effect of an independent variable on a dependent variable at different

values of another independent variable (i.e., a moderator). Unlike the ANOVA method, the

Johnson-Neyman procedure does not require any transformation of the continuous variables.

Instead of discretizing a continuous variable into groups, it tests the same null hypothesis that

“item scores are not different between groups” at all levels of the continuous variable and returns

a “region of significance” within which the scores between groups differ at a specified

Page 9: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

USING JOHNSON-NEYMAN PROCEDURE TO DETECT ITEM BIAS 8

significance level (e.g., p < .05). The region of significance can then be plotted for a visual

inspection of the results.

Based on its similarities with the ANOVA method, the Johnson-Neyman procedure can

easily be adopted to item-bias analysis. In the item bias analysis context, the Johnson-Neyman

procedure is also based on three main variables (i.e., item, group, and total score), which were

explained in the previous section. Note that the categorical score level variable is redundant for

the Johnson-Neyman procedure as the continuous total score variable can be used without any

transformation. These three variables can be entered into a moderated multiple regression

analysis as follows: Item is the dependent variable, whereas group, total score, and group x total

score interaction are the independent variables. The multiple regression analysis then provides an

output with two main effects (group and total score) and one interaction effect (group x total

score). As for the ANOVA method, the significant main effect of group indicates uniform bias

and the significant group x total score interaction effect indicates non-uniform bias. At this

point, the region of significance can also assist the researcher in determining biased items such

that high proportions of the sample within the significance region provide further evidence of

item bias.

The biggest advantage of the Johnson-Neyman procedure over the ANOVA method in

detecting item bias is that it accommodates the total score variable in the analysis as a

continuous variable and tests the conditional effect of group on item at all levels of the total

score variable. Testing all levels of the total score variable is more informative to the researcher,

because respondents’ total item scores are arbitrary and there are no meaningful values that can

be selected and analyzed independently (see also floodlight analysis by Spiller, Fitzsimons,

Lynch, & McClelland, 2013). For example, when conducting the Johnson-Neyman analysis with

Page 10: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

USING JOHNSON-NEYMAN PROCEDURE TO DETECT ITEM BIAS 9

the body mass index (BMI) as the moderator (e.g., Spiller et al., 2013), researchers may choose

to examine the moderator at specific meaningful values (e.g., overweight respondents; BMI >

25). The total score variable in the item bias analysis, however, does not have such focal

meaningful values; it therefore needs to be analyzed at all levels. Treating the total score variable

as continuous rather than discretizing it with arbitrary cut-points also avoids loss of statistical

power and possibility of spurious effects (West, Aiken, & Krull, 1996), which may be of concern

when the ANOVA method is used.

Although the Johnson-Neyman procedure is not a new development, its complexity and

lack of availability in statistical analysis programs has impeded its implementation. Today,

however, add-ons like PROCESS Macro (Hayes, 2013) enable researchers to conduct the

Johnson-Neyman analysis with ease using mainstream statistical packages (e.g., SPSS and SAS).

An Illustrative Example Using Conscientiousness

An illustration of the Johnson-Neyman procedure is presented here using a dataset that

was collected from a university student sample in Norway. 230 students (120 females and 110

males) responded to a brief conscientiousness measure (8 items; Saucier, 1994) using a 7-point

Likert-type scale (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree) as part of a battery of survey

questions. Our objective in this example is to examine item bias across gender groups. The scale

had satisfactory alpha levels (αtotal = .84, αmale = .83, αfemale = .83). Because the purpose here is to

illustrate the procedure, advanced unidimensionality tests were not conducted and the scale was

assumed to be unidimensional. Researchers are recommended to ensure their instruments’

unidimensionality prior to conducting item bias analysis.

Page 11: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

USING JOHNSON-NEYMAN PROCEDURE TO DETECT ITEM BIAS 10

The results of an independent samples t-test analysis showed significant differences for

various individual item scores and total scale mean score, such that females scored higher on trait

conscientiousness than males did (see table 26.1). An examination of effect sizes (Cohen’s d) in

table 26.1 shows that the magnitude of gender differences across items vary from small to

medium (Cohen, 1992). It is important to take note of this variation in the effect sizes, as it may

signal presence of item bias in the scale (Smith, 2002).

<TABLE 26.1 HERE>

To conduct the main item-bias analysis, first the total score variable (i.e., total) was

created. Recall that this variable is the sum of all item scores for each respondent. Hayes’s

PROCESS Macro for SPSS (Hayes, 2013) was used for data analysis (information regarding the

installation of the macro is available online at http://www.processmacro.org). Separate analyses

were conducted for each of the eight items in the scale. In the main PROCESS Macro dialogue

box, first the scale item of interest (e.g., careless) was entered into the outcome variable (Y) box.

Next, the dummy coded group variable (i.e., gender) was entered into the independent variable

(X) box. Finally, the total score variable (i.e., total) was entered into the moderator variables (M)

box. Model number 1 (the moderated regression model) was also selected in the main screen

menu. In the “options” menu, “mean center for products” option was selected. This option mean

centers the independent variables (X and M; gender and total) prior to computation of the

interaction variable (X x M; gender x total). Mean centering renders the regression coefficients

of the independent variables more meaningful (Hayes, 2013); given that our main interest lies in

interpreting the main effect of the group variable (in our case gender), mean centering must be

Page 12: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

USING JOHNSON-NEYMAN PROCEDURE TO DETECT ITEM BIAS 11

employed. Graphical representations of the results are useful in item bias analysis; hence, the

“generate data for plotting” option in the “options” menu was also selected. The final step was to

select the “Johnson-Neyman” option from the menu titled “conditioning”. Pressing the “OK”

button in the main dialogue box ran the regression model and generated the output.

The results of the item bias analyses are presented in table 26.2 and an example of

PROCESS Macro output is presented in table 26.3. Similar to the ANOVA procedure, we are

interested in the main effect of the group (gender) and the interaction effect (gender x total) to

examine uniform and non-uniform bias, respectively. The “model summary” section of the

output (see table 26.3) displays these effects. For the item “careless”, for example, we observe a

significant main effect of gender group (B = 0.43, t (226) = 3.47, p < .001) and a non-significant

interaction effect (B = -0.03, t (226) = -1.48, p = .141). These results indicate the presence of

uniform bias for the item “careless”.

<TABLE 26.2 AND 26.3 HERE>

In the Johnson-Neyman procedure, conclusions regarding biased items do not have to be

based solely on the statistical significance levels. As discussed previously, an advantage of the

Johnson-Neyman procedure is that it provides a “region of significance”, which informs us about

the amount of sample within the region where the conditional effect of group on item is

significant. The “Johnson-Neyman Technique” section of the output in table 26.3 provides us the

Johnson-Neyman point for the moderator (i.e., 5.42). An examination of the regression results in

this section reveals that gender group had a significant effect on item score for all cases with a

mean centered total score below 5.42. Of particular importance here is not only the Johnson-

Page 13: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

USING JOHNSON-NEYMAN PROCEDURE TO DETECT ITEM BIAS 12

Neyman point, but also the amount of sample present within the region of significance. For

example, if there are hardly any respondents who have scores below the Johnson-Neyman point,

we should be cautious in making item bias claims. The PROCESS Macro output provides this

necessary information. As seen on the “Johnson-Neyman Technique” section of the output in

table 26.3, about 76.5% of the cases in the data set had values below the Johnson-Neyman point

when the item “careless” was analyzed. On the other hand, when the item “disorganized” was

taken into consideration, there was again a significant effect of gender group, indicating presence

of uniform bias, but only 27% of the cases were within the significance region (see table 26.2).

Researchers should be cautious in making conclusions about item bias in such cases where there

are insufficient amount of cases within the region of significance. Although there are no rules of

thumb, it is reasonable to expect more than half of the sample to be within the region of

significance to have substantial evidence of item bias.

The Johnson-Neyman region of significance can also be plotted graphically for visual

inspection of the results. The last section of the output in table 26.3 provides the syntax codes to

produce plots in SPSS. These codes can be executed using the SPSS syntax window. Examples

of plots for an unbiased item (practical) and an item with uniform bias (careless) are presented

in figure 26.2. Note that the regression lines for groups have a substantial overlap when the item

is unbiased (left), but there is a large gap between the regression lines when uniform bias is

present (right).

<FIGURE 26.2 HERE>

To test whether the results were stable in a smaller sample, item bias analyses were

repeated in a random subsample from the dataset (N = 100, 43 Females, 57 Males). The results

Page 14: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

USING JOHNSON-NEYMAN PROCEDURE TO DETECT ITEM BIAS 13

were nearly identical (see table 26.4): The significant effect of gender group was again evident

for items “systematic” and “careless”. The only difference was that the significant gender group

x total score interaction effect on item “systematic” did not emerge in the subsample analysis.

This finding was anticipated, given that detecting interactions in multiple regression analysis

generally require larger sample sizes (McClelland & Judd, 1993). Therefore, the Johnson-

Neyman procedure may not be reliable to detect non-uniform bias when the sample size is small.

Small sample sizes (e.g., N = 100) are nevertheless not ideal for item-bias analysis in general.

<TABLE 26.4 HERE>

Concluding Remarks

Methods are indispensable to theory development (Greenwald, 2012). Advancing item

bias methods and making them available to a wider audience will not only yield more valid

group comparisons and better refined measurement instruments, but also enhance theory

development through understanding of why specific items are biased across groups. Therefore,

as suggested by Zumbo (2007), new generation item bias methods should be made accessible to

researchers who are not measurement specialists. The current chapter is an attempt in this

direction.

The present research has introduced the Johnson-Neyman procedure as an item bias

analysis tool. Future research can advance the ideas presented here through simulation studies

where the ANOVA and the Johnson-Neyman procedures are compared with respect to their

sensitivity to detect biased items across different sample sizes. This will be a welcome

contribution to the literature on item bias analysis.

Page 15: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

USING JOHNSON-NEYMAN PROCEDURE TO DETECT ITEM BIAS 14

Acknowledgments

I am grateful to Professor Sigurd V. Troye for his valuable comments to an earlier draft

of this chapter.

Page 16: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

USING JOHNSON-NEYMAN PROCEDURE TO DETECT ITEM BIAS 15

References

Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for

measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4(6), 561-571.

Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Bermúdez, J., Maslach, C., & Ruch, W. (2000). Multivariate

methods for the comparison of factor structures in cross-cultural research: An illustration

with the Big Five questionnaire. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 31(4), 437-464.

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155-159.

D’Alonzo, K. T. (2004). The Johnson-Neyman Procedure as an alternative to ANCOVA.

Western Journal of Nursing Research, 26(7), 804-812.

Fitzsimons, G. J. (2008). Editorial: Death to dichotomizing. Journal of Consumer Research,

35(1), 5-8.

Greenwald, A. G. (2012). There is nothing so theoretical as a good method. Perspectives on

Psychological Science, 7(2), 99-108.

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis:

A Regression-Based Approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Irwin, J. R., & McClelland, G. H. (2003). Negative consequences of dichotomizing continuous

predictor variables. Journal of Marketing Research, 40(3), 366-371.

Johnson, P. O., & Neyman, J. (1936). Tests of certain linear hypotheses and their application to

some educational problems. Statistical Research Memoirs, 1, 57-93.

MacCallum, R. C., Zhang, S., Preacher, K. J., & Rucker, D. D. (2002). On the practice of

dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychological Methods, 7(1), 19-40.

Maxwell, S. E., & Delaney, H. D. (1993). Bivariate median splits and spurious statistical

significance. Psychological Bulletin, 113(1), 181-190.

Page 17: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

USING JOHNSON-NEYMAN PROCEDURE TO DETECT ITEM BIAS 16

McClelland, G. H., & Judd, C. M. (1993). Statistical difficulties of detecting interactions and

moderator effects. Psychological Bulletin, 114(2), 376-390.

Miyazaki, Y., & Maier, K. S. (2005). Johnson–Neyman type technique in Hierarchical Linear

Models. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 30(3), 233-259.

Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational tools for probing interactions

in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of

Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 31(4), 437-448.

Ramírez-Esparza, N., Gosling, S. D., Benet-Martínez, V., Potter, J. P., & Pennebaker, J. W.

(2006). Do bilinguals have two personalities? A special case of cultural frame switching.

Journal of Research in Personality, 40(2), 99-120.

Reynolds, C. R. (2000). Methods for detecting and evaluating cultural bias in neuropsychological

tests. In E. Fletcher-Janzen, T. Strickland & C. R. Reynolds (Eds.), Handbook of Cross-

Cultural Neuropsychology (pp. 249-285). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum

Publishers.

Santor, D. A., Ramsay, J., & Zuroff, D. C. (1994). Nonparametric item analyses of the Beck

Depression Inventory: Evaluating gender item bias and response option weights.

Psychological Assessment, 6(3), 255-270.

Saucier, G. (1994). Mini-markers: A brief version of Goldberg's unipolar Big-Five markers.

Journal of Personality Assessment, 63(3), 506-516.

Smith, L. L. (2002). On the usefulness of item bias analysis to personality psychology.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(6), 754-763.

Page 18: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

USING JOHNSON-NEYMAN PROCEDURE TO DETECT ITEM BIAS 17

Spiller, S. A., Fitzsimons, G. J., Lynch, J. G., & McClelland, G. H. (2013). Spotlights,

floodlights, and the magic number zero: Simple effects tests in moderated regression.

Journal of Marketing Research, 50(2), 277-288.

Thissen, D., Steinberg, L., & Gerrard, M. (1986). Beyond group-mean differences: The concept

of item bias. Psychological Bulletin, 99(1), 118-128.

van de Vijver, F., & Leung, K. (1997). Methods and Data Analysis for Cross-Cultural Research.

Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

van de Vijver, F., & Leung, K. (2011). Equivalence and bias: A review of concepts, models, and

data analytic procedures. In D. Matsumoto & F. van de Vijver (Eds.), Cross-Cultural

Research Methods in Psychology (pp. 17-45). New York, NY: Cambridge University

Press.

Vecchione, M., Alessandri, G., & Barbaranelli, C. (2012). The Five Factor Model in personnel

selection: Measurement equivalence between applicant and non-applicant groups.

Personality and Individual Differences, 52(4), 503-508.

West, S. G., Aiken, L. S., & Krull, J. L. (1996). Experimental personality designs: Analyzing

categorical by continuous variable interactions. Journal of Personality, 64(1), 1-48.

Zumbo, B. D. (2007). Three generations of DIF analyses: Considering where it has been, where

it is now, and where it is going. Language Assessment Quarterly, 4(2), 223-233.

Table 26.1

Page 19: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

USING JOHNSON-NEYMAN PROCEDURE TO DETECT ITEM BIAS 18

Descriptive Statistics and Independent Samples T-Test Results for the Conscientiousness Scale

Gender

Females

(N = 120)

Males

(N = 110) t p Cohen’s d

Organized 5.23 (1.28) 4.55 (1.41) 3.83 <.001 0.51 Efficient 5.05 (0.96) 4.86 (1.26) 1.26 .206 0.17 Systematic 5.29 (1.16) 4.51 (1.39) 4.65 <.001 0.62 Practical 5.02 (1.14) 4.79 (1.26) 1.42 .157 0.19 Disorganized (R) 5.59 (1.14) 4.90 (1.38) 4.14 <.001 0.55 Sloppy (R) 5.55 (1.08) 5.31 (1.10) 1.68 .094 0.22 Inefficient (R) 5.62 (1.22) 5.31 (1.19) 1.93 .055 0.26 Careless (R) 5.51 (1.28) 5.54 (1.23) -0.17 .866 0.02 Conscientiousness (Scale) 5.36 (0.78) 4.97 (0.87) 3.54 <.001 0.47 Alpha Coefficient α = .83 α = .83

Note. (R) denotes reverse-coded item.

Table 26.2

Moderated Regression Analysis Results (N = 230)

Items B SE B t p Sample within the Johnson-Neyman

significance region (%) Organized Gender -0.20 0.12 -1.67 .096

– Gender x Total Score 0.01 0.02 0.80 .422

Efficient Gender 0.12 0.12 0.93 .351

– Gender x Total Score 0.01 0.02 0.34 .736

Systematic Gender -0.31 0.11 -2.85 .005

64.3% Gender x Total Score 0.03 0.01 2.18 .030

Practical Gender 0.03 0.15 0.19 .851

– Gender x Total Score -0.01 0.02 -0.27 .789

Disorganized (R) Gender -0.27 0.13 -2.09 .038

27% Gender x Total Score 0.01 0.02 0.74 .462

Sloppy (R) Gender 0.12 0.11 1.09 .278

– Gender x Total Score 0.01 0.02 0.14 .891

Inefficient (R) Gender 0.09 0.12 0.71 .478

– Gender x Total Score -0.03 0.02 -1.81 .071

Careless (R) Gender 0.43 0.12 3.47 .001

76.5% Gender x Total Score -0.03 0.02 -1.48 .141

Note. (R) denotes reverse-coded item.

Table 26.3

Example PROCESS Macro Output for the Johnson-Neyman Analysis of the Item “Careless”

Page 20: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

USING JOHNSON-NEYMAN PROCEDURE TO DETECT ITEM BIAS 19

Table 26.4

Moderated Regression Analysis Results from a Subsample (N = 100)

Items B SE B t p Sample within the Johnson-Neyman

significance region (%) Organized

Page 21: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

USING JOHNSON-NEYMAN PROCEDURE TO DETECT ITEM BIAS 20

Gender -0.17 0.17 -1.03 .308 –

Gender x Total Score 0.03 0.03 1.05 .296

Efficient Gender 0.29 0.19 1.57 .119

– Gender x Total Score 0.01 0.03 0.14 .889

Systematic Gender -0.43 0.16 -2.67 .009

69% Gender x Total Score 0.02 0.02 1.03 .304

Practical Gender -0.18 0.20 -0.88 .382

– Gender x Total Score -0.01 0.03 -0.55 .580

Disorganized (R) Gender -0.21 0.19 -1.11 .271

– Gender x Total Score 0.01 0.02 0.47 .641

Sloppy (R) Gender 0.16 0.17 0.92 .360

– Gender x Total Score -0.01 0.02 -0.11 .910

Inefficient (R) Gender 0.02 0.18 0.11 .909

– Gender x Total Score -0.02 0.02 -0.89 .382

Careless (R) Gender 0.52 0.18 2.86 .005

68% Gender x Total Score -0.03 0.03 -1.01 .312

Note. (R) denotes reverse-coded item.

Figure 26.1. Hypothetical examples of an item with (a) uniform bias, (b) non-uniform bias, and

(c) no bias (adapted from van Vijver & Leung, 2011).

Page 22: Using the Johnson-Neyman Procedure to Detect Item Bias in ...

USING JOHNSON-NEYMAN PROCEDURE TO DETECT ITEM BIAS 21

Figure 26.2. Example graphical displays of the Johnson-Neyman analysis results (unbiased item

to the left and biased item to the right). The Johnson-Neyman region of significance is shaded in

gray. (R) denotes reverse-coded item. Source: Author.