Tuning to the significant: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of visual...

13
Please cite this article in press as: Markovic J, et al. Tuning to the significant: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of visual perception and memory. Behav Brain Res (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.018 ARTICLE IN PRESS G Model BBR 8596 1–13 Behavioural Brain Research xxx (2013) xxx–xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Brain Research j ourna l h om epage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr Review Tuning to the significant: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of visual perception and memory Jelena Markovic a , Adam K. Anderson b,d , Rebecca M. Todd c,Q1 a University of British Columbia, Department of Philosophy, Canada b University of Toronto, Department of Psychology, Canada Q2 c University of British Columbia, Department of Psychology, Canada d Cornell University, Department of Human Development, United States h i g h l i g h t s Emotionally arousing events reach awareness more easily than more mundane events. Emotionally salient events are also perceived and remembered more vividly. We present the Biased Attention via Norepinephrine (BANE) model of affect-biased attention (ABA). BANE draws on genetic, neuromodulatory, neural and behavioural evidence to account for ABA. a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Received 26 June 2013 Received in revised form 10 November 2013 Accepted 12 November 2013 Available online xxx a b s t r a c t Emotionally arousing events reach awareness more easily and evoke greater visual cortex activation than more mundane events. Recent studies have shown that they are also perceived more vividly and that emotionally enhanced perceptual vividness predicts memory vividness. We propose that affect-biased attention (ABA) selective attention to emotionally salient events is an endogenous attentional system tuned by an individual’s history of reward and punishment. We present the Biased Attention via Nore- pinephrine (BANE) model, which unifies genetic, neuromodulatory, neural and behavioural evidence to account for ABA. We review evidence supporting BANE’s proposal that a key mechanism of ABA is locus coeruleus–norepinephrine (LC–NE) activity, which interacts with activity in hubs of affective salience net- works to modulate visual cortex activation and heighten the subjective vividness of emotionally salient stimuli. We further review literature on biased competition and look at initial evidence for its potential as a neural mechanism behind ABA. We also review evidence supporting the role of the LC–NE system as a driving force of ABA. Finally, we review individual differences in ABA and memory including differ- ences in sensitivity to stimulus category and valence. We focus on differences arising from a variant of the ADRA2b gene, which codes for the alpha2b adrenoreceptor as a way of investigating influences of NE Q4 availability on ABA in humans. © 2013 Published by Elsevier B.V. Contents 1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00 2. Terminology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00 3. Caveat . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00 4. Affective salience enhances visual perception and memory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00 4.1. Affective salience enhances the subjective quality of perception and memory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00 5. Potential neural pathways and mechanisms underlying affect-biased attention . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00 5.1. Neuroanatomical pathways mediating ABA . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00 5.2. Biased competition as a mechanism of ABA . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00 5.3. Acquisition of affective biases . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00 Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 647 284 0634; fax: +1 647 284 0634. Q3 E-mail addresses: [email protected], [email protected] (R.M. Todd). 0166-4328/$ see front matter © 2013 Published by Elsevier B.V. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.018 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33

Transcript of Tuning to the significant: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of visual...

Page 1: Tuning to the significant: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of visual perception and memory

G

B

R

Ta

JQ1

a

bQ2c

d

h

••••

a

ARR1AA

Q4

C

Q3

0h

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22

23

24

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

32

33

ARTICLE IN PRESS Model

BR 8596 1–13

Behavioural Brain Research xxx (2013) xxx– xxx

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Behavioural Brain Research

j ourna l h om epage: www.elsev ier .com/ locate /bbr

eview

uning to the significant: Neural and genetic processes underlyingffective enhancement of visual perception and memory

elena Markovica, Adam K. Andersonb,d, Rebecca M. Toddc,∗

University of British Columbia, Department of Philosophy, CanadaUniversity of Toronto, Department of Psychology, CanadaUniversity of British Columbia, Department of Psychology, CanadaCornell University, Department of Human Development, United States

i g h l i g h t s

Emotionally arousing events reach awareness more easily than more mundane events.Emotionally salient events are also perceived and remembered more vividly.We present the Biased Attention via Norepinephrine (BANE) model of affect-biased attention (ABA).BANE draws on genetic, neuromodulatory, neural and behavioural evidence to account for ABA.

r t i c l e i n f o

rticle history:eceived 26 June 2013eceived in revised form0 November 2013ccepted 12 November 2013vailable online xxx

a b s t r a c t

Emotionally arousing events reach awareness more easily and evoke greater visual cortex activation thanmore mundane events. Recent studies have shown that they are also perceived more vividly and thatemotionally enhanced perceptual vividness predicts memory vividness. We propose that affect-biasedattention (ABA) – selective attention to emotionally salient events – is an endogenous attentional systemtuned by an individual’s history of reward and punishment. We present the Biased Attention via Nore-pinephrine (BANE) model, which unifies genetic, neuromodulatory, neural and behavioural evidence toaccount for ABA. We review evidence supporting BANE’s proposal that a key mechanism of ABA is locuscoeruleus–norepinephrine (LC–NE) activity, which interacts with activity in hubs of affective salience net-works to modulate visual cortex activation and heighten the subjective vividness of emotionally salient

stimuli. We further review literature on biased competition and look at initial evidence for its potentialas a neural mechanism behind ABA. We also review evidence supporting the role of the LC–NE systemas a driving force of ABA. Finally, we review individual differences in ABA and memory including differ-ences in sensitivity to stimulus category and valence. We focus on differences arising from a variant of the ADRA2b gene, which codes for the alpha2b adrenoreceptor as a way of investigating influences of NEavailability on ABA in humans.

© 2013 Published by Elsevier B.V.

ontents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 002. Terminology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 003. Caveat . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 004. Affective salience enhances visual perception and memory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00

4.1. Affective salience enhances the subjective quality of perception and memory. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 005. Potential neural pathways and mechanisms underlying affect-biased attention . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00

Please cite this article in press as: Markovic J, et al. Tuning to the significanvisual perception and memory. Behav Brain Res (2013), http://dx.doi.org/1

5.1. Neuroanatomical pathways mediating ABA . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

5.2. Biased competition as a mechanism of ABA . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

5.3. Acquisition of affective biases . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 647 284 0634; fax: +1 647 284 0634.E-mail addresses: [email protected], [email protected] (R.M. Todd).

166-4328/$ – see front matter © 2013 Published by Elsevier B.V.ttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.018

t: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of0.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.018

. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00

. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00

Page 2: Tuning to the significant: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of visual perception and memory

ARTICLE IN PRESSG Model

BBR 8596 1–13

2 J. Markovic et al. / Behavioural Brain Research xxx (2013) xxx– xxx

6. The role of norepinephrine in affect-biased attention and memory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 006.1. The role of norepinephrine in ABA and memory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00

7. Individual differences in NE influence on affect-biased attention and memory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 007.1. Individual differences in ABA . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 007.2. Genetic influences on ABA . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00

8. Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 009. Future directions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00

. . . . . .

1

Q5vcksspi

sare‘s

dbptaogtbsastmgaioorTiieuae

pcwratNte

34

35

36

37

38

39

40

41

42

43

44

45

46

47

48

49

50

51

52

53

54

55

56

57

58

59

60

61

62

63

64

65

66

67

68

69

70

71

72

73

74

75

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

83

84

85

86

87

88

89

90

91

92

93

94

95

96

97

98

99

100

101

102

103

104

105

106

107

108

109

110

111

112

113

114

115

116

117

118

119

120

121

122

123

124

125

126

127

128

129

130

131

132

133

134

135

References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

. Introduction

Emotionally arousing events are experienced with a heightenedividness, and emotionally compelling objects in the environmentapture the eye as we navigate the world. It is, of course, wellnown that we continuously filter incoming sensory information,electively allocating attention to what is important to us anduppressing distracting or irrelevant information. Yet the neuralrocesses involved in attentional biases towards affectively signif-

cant aspects of the world remain relatively underspecified.There is a well-established literature documenting the ten-

ion between ‘top-down’ and ‘bottom-up’ processes in modulatingttention (for review see [1]). In this literature, ‘top-down’efers to effortful attentional processes mediated by frontopari-tal attentional networks and tuned to short-term goals, whereas

bottom-up’ refers to attentional capture by ‘objectively’ salienttimuli such as bright colours, motion, and high contrast [2–4].

Alongside other challenges to the original top-down/bottom-upistinction [5–7], we have argued that attention is also modulatedy longer-term subjective goals of increasing pleasure and avoidingain [8]. Such long-term goals can tune visual attention habituallyo emotionally significant, or affectively salient, stimuli such as anttractive person, an angry face, or a gruesome scene. Based onbservations that the amygdala and other brain regions key in tag-ing salience modulate visual cortex activation in a manner similaro the way frontoparietal regions do [9] we propose that affect-iased attention (ABA), which tunes visual attention to affectivelyalient stimuli, is distinct from both ‘classic’ executive top-townnd bottom-up visual attention, and is at least in part circum-cribed by a different set of neural mechanisms (see also [18]). Inhis paper we will propose the Biased Attention via Norepinephrine

odel (BANE), a multilevel model incorporating neuromodulatory,enetic, imaging and behavioural levels of analysis implicated inffective biasing of attention and memory. BANE focuses on thenfluence of noradrenergic processes on activation patterns in hubsf the ‘anterior affective system’ [10], including the amygdala andrbitofrontal cortex, which in turn modulate activity in other brainegions implicated in affect-biased visual attention and memory.his system is responsible for directing attention to and heighten-ng the subjective vividness of perceived emotional events, whichn turn enhances memory vividness. In this paper we will reviewvidence for BANE, arguing that affect biased attention can mod-late visual cortex activity in a manner distinct from – althought times overlapping and/or interacting with – the frontoparietalxecutive network.

We will first review literature on ABA, and will then discussotential neural mechanisms underlying ABA, particularly biasedompetition, which facilitates the influence of frontoparietal net-orks on the visual cortices in selective attention. We will further

eview recent evidence that biased competition may underlie ABAs well. We will then look at the role of norepinephrine (NE) and

Please cite this article in press as: Markovic J, et al. Tuning to the significanvisual perception and memory. Behav Brain Res (2013), http://dx.doi.org/1

he locus coeruleus (LC) of the brainstem in ABA and memory.E is produced by LC neurons, which have widespread projec-

ions throughout the brain [11], and facilitates processing of salientvents [12–14]. We will then review evidence about individual

. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 00

differences in ABA and memory, focusing on individual differencesarising from a common variant of the ADRA2b gene coding for thealpha2b adrenoreceptor, which influences extracellular NE avail-ability. Finally we will present the BANE model in detail based onthe evidence previously discussed.

2. Terminology

Before we examine the literature, let us first clarify the key termsused in this paper. Salience is defined as the quality by which anaspect of the environment stands out relative to its surroundingsdue, perhaps, to its visual features (visual salience) or the goalsof the perceiver. For example, something may be visually salientbecause it is high in contrast or brightly coloured or high in motionin comparison with its surroundings. Because salience is a some-what circular term – some items catch our attention because theyare salient, and are salient because their qualities catch our atten-tion – we use the term in a manner that is descriptive rather thanexplanatory. As such, it can be a useful concept in that it allowsus to examine the properties that determine salience in a givencontext. Affective salience is the tendency of an item to stand outrelative to its neighbours due to an association between its seman-tic meaning and a history of emotional arousal [8]. Affect-biasedattention (ABA) is attention biased towards stimuli that are affec-tively salient because they have a developmental history of painand pleasure, approach and avoidance.

We have claimed that in affect-biased attention, motivationalgoals tune affective control settings, habitual ‘mental sets’ that areshaped by one’s history of emotionally arousing experiences [8].We suggest that, over time, affective control settings come to beapplied reflexively. Thus, whereas we may be tuned to stimuli thatare visually salient because evolution has tuned us to attend tomoving or high contrast aspects of the environment, we may besimilarly tuned to affectively salient stimuli because of our historyof emotional experience with them.

Building on the modulation hypothesis of McGaugh, Cahill andcolleagues [15,16], BANE proposes that ABA influences emotionalenhancement of memory. According to the modulation hypothe-sis, the effects of arousal on initial memory formation, or encoding,interact with the influence of arousal on longer-term memory con-solidation processes to bias memory for emotionally salient events.To encode an event is to process the relevant sensory informationinto a unified, coherent construct so that it may be remembered.Consolidation is divided into short- and long-term processes. Theformer is a set of molecular processes required for the creationand change of synaptic connections and occurs during the hoursafter the experience [17]. Long-term consolidation is the set of pro-cesses responsible for large-scale reorganization of neural memorysystems [17].

3. Caveat

t: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of0.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.018

Previous research has uncovered two functionally independentattentional systems in the cortex: a dorsal-frontoparietal networkinvolved in top-down selection of stimuli and responses which

136

137

138

Page 3: Tuning to the significant: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of visual perception and memory

IN PRESSG Model

B

l Brain Research xxx (2013) xxx– xxx 3

iabpovsrsrvari

4m

rtbplomt

utesmof

isafiFswSb

pr[ua[lisecp

ttuat

Fig. 1. Diagram of a dual-target rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) task used tomeasure the attentional blink. Participants were instructed to ignore words appear-ing in black and to report the identity of the targets appearing in green. The timelag between the first (T1) and second (T2) target was varied. When T2 is presented

139

140

141

142

143

144

145

146

147

148

149

150

151

152

153

154

155

156

157

158

159

160

161

162

163

164

165

166

167

168

169

170

171

172

173

174

175

176

177

178

179

180

181

182

183

184

185

186

187

188

189

190

191

192

193

194

195

196

197

198

199

200

201

202

203

204

205

206

207

208

209

210

211

212

213

214

215

216

217

218

219

220

221

222

223

224

225

226

227

228

229

230

231

232

233

234

235

236

237

238

ARTICLEBR 8596 1–13

J. Markovic et al. / Behavioura

nvolves the intraparietal cortex and superior frontal cortex and right-lateralized ventral-frontoparietal network sensitive to theottom-up salience of stimuli and which is centred on the tem-oroparietal cortex and inferior frontal cortex [2]. BANE is a modelf the ABA system. Though Corbetta and Shulman [2] state that theentral system is responsible for detecting behaviourally relevanttimuli, we should note that the ABA system is distinct from theight ventral-frontoparietal attentional system because it is respon-ible for directing attention to stimuli with an individual history ofeward and punishment. In contrast, Corbetta and Shulman’s rightentral frontoparietal system orients attention to objectively visu-lly salient, task relevant and unexpected stimuli (although someegions, such as lateral intraparietal cortex (LIP), may be key nodesn both affective salience and bottom-up salience systems).

. Affective salience enhances visual perception andemory

There is an extensive body of literature on ABA and its neu-al correlates, including how ABA interacts with classically definedop-down and bottom-up attentional systems, and a full review iseyond the scope of this paper (for reviews see [18,19]). In thisaper, following a brief overview of background research estab-

ishing prioritized processing of affective salience, and a review ofur own work revealing emotional enhancement of perceptual andnemonic vividness, we will focus on noradrenergic contributions

o ABA as illustrated by select studies.A large body of research has shown that affectively salient stim-

li elicit enhanced behavioural and neural processing comparedo more neutral stimuli. Emotional stimuli capture attention moreasily when they are at the threshold of awareness [20] and wheneveral stimuli are in competition for attention [21,22]. We are alsoore easily distracted by affectively salient stimuli when focusing

n another task [23,24]. Finally, we generally have better memoryor emotional than mundane events [25–29] (but see [30,31]).

At the neural level, affective salience has been strongly linked toncreased activity in sensory cortices. Neuroimaging studies havehown that affectively salient images evoke greater visual cortexctivation than mundane ones [32–36], an effect that is paralleledor affectively salient sounds in auditory cortex [37–39]. This effects found for social stimuli as well as emotionally arousing scenes:ace-specific regions of the fusiform cortex have been found tohow greater fMRI activation for fearful than neutral faces evenhen processing facial expression is not part of the task [40–42].

uch enhanced activation of fusiform cortex is also associated withetter detection of emotional faces [43,44].

Affectively salient stimuli also evoke enhanced event-relatedotentials (ERPs) at both early and late latencies, suggesting bothapid and extended prioritization of salient aspects of the world45,46]. Importantly, enhanced activity for affectively salient stim-li has been observed in very early ERP components which arelso sensitive to classic ‘top-down’ attention. These include the C147,48] a very early ERP generated by the striate cortex reflectingow-level visual features, and the P1 [49], a component primar-ly indexing extrastriate cortex activity [50]. Although there istill some controversy about the latency at which affective salientffects can be observed, these finding suggest that very early visualortex activation is sensitive to predictions/expectations related torior learning about affective salience.

One line of our own research has focused on enhanced percep-ual encoding of affectively salient stimuli as a marker of affectively

Please cite this article in press as: Markovic J, et al. Tuning to the significanvisual perception and memory. Behav Brain Res (2013), http://dx.doi.org/1

uned attentional sets. An experimental paradigm that has beenseful in indexing affective biases in attention is an emotional vari-nt of the attentional blink (AB) paradigm. In classic AB studies,wo target words are presented among a series of distractor stimuli

within 500 ms of T1, the attentional blink typically occurs. (For interpretation of thereferences to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version ofthe article.)

(Fig. 1). The attentional ‘blink’ itself is a phenomenon where par-ticipants are typically unable to report a target stimulus when it ispresented within ∼500 ms of a previous target in a rapid stream ofstimuli. According to one interpretation of the AB, the blink reflectsa failure to switch attentional sets from those tuned to the categoryof the T1 stimulus to those tuned to the T2 stimulus if it appearstoo quickly after T1, resulting in impaired perceptual awareness[51]. Anderson [22] used a version of the AB paradigm to examinewhether emotionally salient T2 stimuli are less subject to the atten-tional blink than neutral stimuli. The first experiment comparedAB for negatively valenced high-arousal words (e.g. “rape”), nega-tively valenced low-arousal words (e.g. “hurt”), and neutral words(e.g. “rule”). Results showed that negatively valenced high-arousalwords had a significantly smaller blink effect than negativelyvalenced low-arousal words, which themselves had a smaller ABeffect than neutral words. Thus, emotionally salient and negativelyvalenced words were easier to detect than neutral words or, inother words, that there was an emotional “sparing” of the blinkfor such words. The second study showed that this effect appliedto positively valenced target words as well, implying that what isimportant for detection of the stimuli is emotional arousal ratherthan valence. A further series of experiments ruled out potentialconfounds for the sparing of emotional words. In conclusion, theseexperiments revealed that when attentional resources are limited,emotionally salient stimuli are perceived more easily than neutralstimuli – a finding that may reflect more resilient attentional filtersfor affectively salient stimuli.

4.1. Affective salience enhances the subjective quality ofperception and memory

Another line of our research has focused on enhanced subjectiveexperience of perceptual and mnemonic vividness for affectivelysalient stimuli. While it was established that emotional eventsare typically (though not always) better remembered than mun-dane ones [52–54], it was not known whether emotional eventsare remembered more vividly because they are experienced asmore vivid in the first place. To investigate whether emotionalsalience influences the subjective experience of perceptual vivid-

t: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of0.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.018

ness, we employed an emotional version of a classic magnitudeestimation paradigm from psychophysics experiments of the 1950s[55,56]. In a classic magnitude estimation task, participants are pre-sented with a stimulus (e.g. a light or a tone) and are asked to

239

240

241

242

Page 4: Tuning to the significant: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of visual perception and memory

ARTICLE IN PRESSG Model

BBR 8596 1–13

4 J. Markovic et al. / Behavioural Brain Research xxx (2013) xxx– xxx

F scramt icipanf

caanntspTtttopieciov(npT

243

244

245

246

247

248

249

250

251

252

253

254

255

256

257

258

259

260

261

262

263

264

265

266

267

268

269

270

271

272

273

274

275

276

277

278

279

280

281

282

283

284

285

286

ig. 2. Task design for Noise Estimation experiment. A standard, created by phase

he target image overlaid with 10%, 15%, or 20% noise. Following image offset, partrom “a lot less noise” to “same as standard” to “a lot more noise.”

ompare the magnitude of the stimulus to a standard presentedt a constant magnitude. In our adaptation, emotionally salientnd neutral images, which were equated for contrast and lumi-ance, were overlaid with one of three levels of Gaussian visualoise and standards were created for each image by scramblinghe image so its contents were not recognizable and overlaying atandard level of noise (Fig. 2). Participants were asked to judge theroportion of noisiness of each image relative to a standard [57].his design allowed us to look at the subjective vividness of affec-ively salient relative to neutral images measured as the signal ofhe underlying image relative to the overlaid noise. Results showedhat participants were very accurate in estimating objective levelsf noise. Crucially, both positive and negative arousing images wereerceived as less noisy, or more perceptually vivid, than neutral

mages. Even after controlling for the objective characteristics ofach image, participants still rated positive and negative images asontaining lower levels of noise, suggesting that affectively salientmages are subjectively experienced as more vivid than mundanenes. Moreover, when we created a direct measure of perceptualividness by calculating the inverse of the noise estimation ratings

Please cite this article in press as: Markovic J, et al. Tuning to the significanvisual perception and memory. Behav Brain Res (2013), http://dx.doi.org/1

NE−1, a measure of how clearly or vividly the image signal under-eath the noise was perceived), we found that, image by image,erceptual vividness predicted ratings of emotional salience.his relationship remained after controlling for computational

bling the target image, was overlaid with 15% noise. The standard was followed byts moved a cursor on a scale to indicated NE for the image relative to the standard

measures of objective visual salience, such as colour, image com-plexity, and a composite measure of visual salience [58,59],indicating that affective and objective salience make dissociablecontributions to perceived vividness. We refer to this influenceof emotional salience on perceptual vividness as emotionallyenhanced vividness (EEV).

Several control studies were performed to rule out confound-ing explanations. To eliminate the possibility that noise ratingswere driven by differential deployment of overt attention, we usedeye tracking to control for differences in looking patterns. Wefound that emotional salience did predict patterns of overt atten-tion, with more fixations for affectively salient images; however,emotional salience predicted perceptual vividness after control-ling for number of fixations, and fixations did not statisticallymediate emotional salience. Thus, deployment of overt attentiondid not account for the influence of affective salience on noiseestimation ratings. The main effect of affective salience on noiseestimation ratings was sustained in experiments using grayscaleimages, images with lower levels of noise and a single presen-tation of each image, indicating that greater perceived vividness

t: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of0.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.018

for affectively salient images is not affected by image colour ordifferential effects arising from repetition of emotional images;rather, it is due to the emotional content of the images themselves Q6(Fig. 3).

287

288

289

290

Page 5: Tuning to the significant: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of visual perception and memory

ARTICLE IN PRESSG Model

BBR 8596 1–13

J. Markovic et al. / Behavioural Brain Research xxx (2013) xxx– xxx 5

Fig. 3. Noise estimation results. Arousing images were psychophysically scaled to contain less noise, i.e. were perceived as more perceptually vivid, despite equal levels ofo from

c imagei

rplPeaatPTsracp

scttadtl(taireaaiNeo

jstssE

291

292

293

294

295

296

297

298

299

300

301

302

303

304

305

306

307

308

309

310

311

312

313

314

315

316

317

318

319

320

321

322

323

324

325

326

327

328

329

330

331

332

333

334

335

336

337

338

339

340

341

342

343

344

345

346

347

348

349

350

351

352

353

354

355

356

357

358

359

360

361

362

363

364

365

366

367

368

369

370

371

372

373

374

bjective noise. The right side of the image illustrates a 15% decrement in noise levelontrolling for objective salience related to low-level featural characteristics of the

n left insula (left), LOC (right), and amygdala (bottom).

To see if the behavioural phenomenon of EEV reflected relativelyapid perceptual processes rather than later conceptual evaluativerocesses, we further examined the time course of ERP activity fol-

owing presentation of the images. We focused on the postsensory2, an early- to mid-latency positive peak measured at occipitallectrodes and implicated in object discrimination and enhancedttention to affectively salient images [60,61]. We found that P2mplitude was greatest for the least noisy images and impor-antly, that there was an effect of affective salience, with larger2 amplitudes for negative and positive versus neutral images.hat P2 amplitudes reflected objective perceptual vividness andubjective affective salience suggests that EEV involves relativelyapid perceptual processing and that emotionally salient imagesre perceived in the manner of objectively clearer images. Thisorresponds with the behavioural data indicating that participantserceive emotional images more vividly.

Finally, we employed fMRI to examine potential modulatoryources of EEV, to determine whether emotionally enhanced per-eption reflected enhanced visual cortex activation, and to examinehe relation between amygdala and visual cortex activation in rela-ion to EEV. We found that activations in the left amygdala as wells left lateral occipital cortex (LOC), which plays a role in objectiscrimination [62–64], and a region of left dorsal posterior insulahought to function as primary interoceptive cortex [65,66], modu-ated NE−1 for emotional images. Further analyses of co-activationPPI) found correlated activity between amygdala and visual cor-ex for affectively salient but not for neutral images. Statistically,mygdala activation mediated the influence of LOC and posteriornsula on EEV. These findings can be interpreted as reflecting theole of the amygdala in tagging affective salience, which in turn maynhance both the experience of seeing (reflected in LOC activation)nd gut feeling (reflected by posterior insula activation). Finally,ctivation in parietal and frontal regions which function as hubsn executive attentional networks was negatively correlated withE−1 suggesting that in this task there was a trade-off betweenxecutive attentional activity and amygdala-mediated modulationf ABA.

In short, we found that emotional salience modulates the sub-ective visual experience of seeing an image. Moreover, our resultsuggest that emotional salience modulates object-based atten-

Please cite this article in press as: Markovic J, et al. Tuning to the significanvisual perception and memory. Behav Brain Res (2013), http://dx.doi.org/1

ion, making a subjectively salient object appear more objectivelyalient. In this case, the amygdala, a hub of the anterior affectiveystem, accounted for enhanced visual cortex activation linked toEV in a manner that is consistent with the hypothesis that anterior

the left. (b) Image by image, emotional salience predicted perceptual vividness after. (c) fMRI activation parametrically modulated by emotionally enhanced vividness

affective networks modulate visual cortex activation similarly to,but dissociable from, frontoparietal networks. This result convergeswith electrophysiological findings that both affective salience [47]and state [67,68] modulate visual processing independently butsimilarly to executive top-down attention. Moreover, there was atradeoff between activation in anterior affective networks and fron-toparietal networks associated with top-down executive attention.Thus, in terms of executive attention, our results indicate that par-ticipants were not just attending more to affectively salient images– they were attending differently.

A second line of interest concerned whether EEV at the timeof encoding was related to memory vividness. Previous studieshave demonstrated that affectively salient images are typicallybetter recollected than neutral ones (e.g. [53] but see [69]). Morespecifically, participants show greater memory for the goal-relatedand emotionally salient aspects of images [70,71]. These effectsmay be due to differences at the time of perceptual processingbetween emotionally salient and neutral images. Emotional eventsare encoded more easily [22,32,44,72] and enhanced memory ofemotional images is associated with increased amygdala activa-tion [73,74] and high-level visual cortex activation [75,76] at thetime of encoding.

To test whether perceptual vividness at the time of encodingpredicts memory vividness we employed two memory tasks: acued recall task and a recognition memory task [57]. The cuedrecall study was performed 45 min after the completion of the noiseestimation task. Participants were given one-word cues that corre-sponded to one of the pictures seen in the noise estimation task andasked to provide a written description of the picture in as muchdetail as possible. Descriptions were rated for number of detailsrecalled from correctly remembered images, including thoughtsand emotions associated with the image. Participants recalled moredetails about affectively salient than neutral images, and inversenoise estimation was correlated with number of details recalledas well as associated thoughts and emotions. Thus, although par-ticipants were not more likely to recall an emotional image thana neutral one, it appears that the vividness with which we viewemotionally salient images modulates memory vividness as well.

In the recognition memory task, participants returned one weekafter performing the noise estimation task. They were shown all

t: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of0.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.018

of the images from the original task as well as unfamiliar imagesmatched for emotional salience, scene content and objective imagecharacteristics. Participants were asked to rate each image as oldor new and to rate the vividness of the memory. Again, NE−1

375

376

377

378

Page 6: Tuning to the significant: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of visual perception and memory

ING Model

B

6 l Brain

soosworvpsocpi

sttcntcpttTsi

sifisvdnab

5a

5

bisbelnumuspaovshc

c

379

380

381

382

383

384

385

386

387

388

389

390

391

392

393

394

395

396

397

398

399

400

401

402

403

404

405

406

407

408

409

410

411

412

413

414

415

416

417

418

419

420

421

422

423

424

425

426

427

428

429

430

431

432

433

434

435

436

437

438

439

440

441

442

443

444

445

446

447

448

449

450

451

452

453

454

455

456

457

458

459

460

461

462

463

464

465

466

467

468

469

470

471

472

473

474

475

476

477

478

479

480

481

482

483

484

485

486

487

488

489

490

491

492

493

494

495

496

497

498

499

500

ARTICLEBR 8596 1–13

J. Markovic et al. / Behavioura

ignificantly predicted memory vividness after controlling forbjective salience. Thus EEV contributes to some of the vividnessf emotional memory, though it is likely that post encoding con-olidation processes play a further role in memory vividness asell [77,78]. fMRI findings further revealed that the same regions

f amygdala and LOC that modulated EEV modulated subsequentatings of recognition memory vividness [79]; however memoryividness was uniquely modulated by additional activity in hip-ocampal and parahippocampal regions. These findings suggesthared neural substrates for the influence of emotional saliencen perceptual and mnemonic vividness, with amygdala and visualortex activation at encoding contributing to the experience of botherception and subsequent memory. However, memory vividness

s also predicted by unique patterns of neural activity.In summary, the noise estimation studies showed that affective

alience contributes not only to ease of detecting an image but alsoo the quality of one’s visual experience. Thus, not only do emo-ional images grab attention more easily, but we also see them morelearly. Affective salience is a distinct source of perceptual vivid-ess – contributing to an image’s vividness in a manner additionalo the image’s objective visual characteristics. The enhanced per-eptual vividness of emotional images is due to rapid perceptualrocessing rather than later conceptual processing. Furthermore,he enhanced processing received by affectively salient images athe time of perception trickles down to impact memory processes.he vividness of an image during perception and the emotionalalience of an image both contribute to the vividness of an imagen memory.

Thus, a large body of research has established that affectivelyalient stimuli enjoy prioritized attention and perceptual encod-ng, and elicit rapid and prioritized sensory enhancement. Our ownndings have established that they also enhance the vividness ofubjective perceptual experience, which in turn predicts memoryividness. Current research questions involve specifying in greateretail which aspects of an emotional stimulus influence attention,eural mechanisms underlying prioritization of affective stimulind how these may differ between individuals, and how affectiveiases are acquired through experience.

. Potential neural pathways and mechanisms underlyingffect-biased attention

.1. Neuroanatomical pathways mediating ABA

Previous research has established that feedback connectionsetween the amygdala and visual areas play an important role

n mediating enhancement of visual cortex activity for affectivelyalient stimuli [32,80–82]. There are bidirectional connectionsetween the amygdala and early visual areas in the striate andxtrastriate cortices [83,84]. Moreover, patients with amygdalaesions and an intact visual cortex lack the typically found enhancedeural response to affectively salient stimuli [80]. fMRI researchsing sophisticated analysis approaches such as dynamic causalodelling have provided functional evidence for amygdala mod-

lation of visual cortex when participants view affectively salienttimuli [85]. Yet although much research has focused on amygdalaathways, critics of an amygdalo-centric approach suggest that themygdala is not the sole hub of affective salience detection but isne hub among several participating in parallel cascades of acti-ations in networks mediating the influence of affectively salienttimuli on sensory processing [86,87]. Other regions that serve as

Please cite this article in press as: Markovic J, et al. Tuning to the significanvisual perception and memory. Behav Brain Res (2013), http://dx.doi.org/1

ubs in an “anterior affective system”, such as the orbitofrontalortex (OFC) are also potential modulators [10,44].

Pessoa and Adolphs further argue that visual information is pro-essed by multiple parallel channels, and that the cortex plays a

PRESS Research xxx (2013) xxx– xxx

large part in filtering visual information [87]. They propose that akey region for ABA is the pulvinar – especially its medial nucleus.The pulvinar receives visual input from the superior colliculus,retina and striate and extrastriate visual cortices, and its medialnucleus may be responsible for determining the behavioural rel-evance of a stimulus due to its connection with amygdala aswell as multiple cortical regions such as the OFC, cingulate cor-tex, insula and parietal regions [87]. These authors suggest thatthe amygdala is a “convergence zone” for information relevant toobject processing. The importance of the amygdala for ABA comesfrom its broad connectivity to other subcortical regions and to thecortex. The amygdala not only receives visual information fromhigher-order visual association cortices in the anterior temporallobe [84], it also has many connections to the cortex includingmedial, orbital and lateral regions of the prefrontal cortex [88].Thus, the amygdala impacts visual processing through both ofthese (direct and indirect) connections to the visual cortex. Anotherrecent model emphasizes the role of NE in amygdala entrainmentof widespread network co-activation in response to salient events[89]. In this paper we further emphasize the role of the LC/NEsystem in modulating specific neuronal mechanisms of selectiveattention in visual cortex in interaction with the anterior affectivesystem.

5.2. Biased competition as a mechanism of ABA

Neural mechanisms underlying modulation of the visual cor-tex by regions tagging affective salience are as yet underspecified;however, a potential mechanism is biased-competition, since thisis a well-mapped mechanism underlying executive influences onvisual attention. In biased-competition models of visual attention,top-down ‘attentional control settings’ bias attention to featuresof the environment that are relevant to one’s goals. Biased com-petition has been characterized in terms of three principles:competition, i.e. the brain systems that represent visual informa-tion are competitive and a gain in processing for one stimuluscomes at the cost of inhibition of activation tuned to other stimuli;control, i.e. there are mechanisms to allocate increased weight to acertain stimulus, and integration, i.e. when competition is resolvedin favour of a certain stimulus in one system this stimulus will gaindominance in other systems as well [90].

Beck and Kastner have reviewed evidence that stimuli competefor representation throughout the visual cortex and that competi-tion can be biased for spatial location as well as object features [91].For executive attentional biasing, frontoparietal networks modu-late visual cortex activation so that activity is enhanced in regionsresponsible for task-relevant stimuli and activation is suppressedin regions responsible for competing stimuli. For instance singlecell recordings of monkey visual cortex have shown that when amonkey attends to one of two competing stimuli within a neu-ron’s receptive field (RF), responses to the pair of stimuli in areasV2, V4 and MT are weighted to the attended stimuli, i.e. they aresimilar to response given to the attended stimulus presented alone[92–94]. Such findings are supported by fMRI studies on humans,which show increased activation in V4 and TEO in situations ofcompetition [95]. Thus in directing their attention, subjects wereable to enhance processing of one stimulus and suppress process-ing of competing stimuli. Beck and Kastner [90] review evidence ofexecutive modulation via the frontoparietal cortex based on spa-tial location and stimulus features, though they note that top-downmodulation is also possible based on memory or emotional mech-anisms.

t: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of0.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.018

We posit that activity in hubs of the anterior affective system(amygdala, orbitofrontal/ventromedial cortices) as well as thelocus coeruleus similarly modulates visual cortex activity based onthe affective salience of the stimulus. This hypothesis is supported

501

502

503

504

Page 7: Tuning to the significant: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of visual perception and memory

ING Model

B

l Brain

batsakas

ucufmorpucmTiatpcoabatwc

bstrtarbo(tagoiwbaaftwfclcaddtpA

responses to neighbouring frequencies while sparing response tothe best frequency [111]. It is also important for sensory gating,

505

506

507

508

509

510

511

512

513

514

515

516

517

518

519

520

521

522

523

524

525

526

527

528

529

530

531

532

533

534

535

536

537

538

539

540

541

542

543

544

545

546

547

548

549

550

551

552

553

554

555

556

557

558

559

560

561

562

563

564

565

566

567

568

569

570

571

572

573

574

575

576

577

578

579

580

581

582

583

584

585

586

587

588

589

590

591

592

593

594

595

596

597

598

599

600

601

602

603

604

605

606

607

608

609

610

611

612

613

614

615

616

617

618

619

620

621

622

623

624

625

626

627

628

629

ARTICLEBR 8596 1–13

J. Markovic et al. / Behavioura

y recent findings in non-human primates that visual cortexctivation is modulated by stimulus reward value in the same wayhat it is modulated by executive attention [96,97]. The BANE modeluggests that findings related to reward value may extend to over-ll stimulus salience, and further propose that connections withey salience hubs modulate visual cortex activation for emotion-lly relevant stimuli while suppressing activation for competingtimuli.

The notion of affect-biased competition is still somewhat spec-lative, but recent work provides preliminary evidence of biasedompetition in visual cortex based on the affective salience of stim-li. EEG evidence of rapid primary visual cortex modulation byacial expressions is consistent with findings of biased competition

odulated by executive attention [47]. A study taking advantagef high temporal and spatial resolution of magnetic encephalog-aphy (MEG) [10] further employed dynamic causal modelling toredict MEG differentiation of affectively salient from neutral stim-li and found evidence for a top-down model that included bothortical and subcortical pathways allowing for rapid top-downodulation of visual processing by the anterior affective system.

he orbitofrontal/ventromedial prefrontal cortices play a key rolen this model. Convergent research further suggest that the anteriorffective system plays a key role in maintaining affective con-rol settings by maintaining templates for salient items based onast experience that function as a ‘predictive set’ that enhancesontext-dependent visual processing of salient stimuli [98]. More-ver, OFC/VMPFc activations based on implicitly learned stimulusssociations between facial features and personality traits haveeen found to predict subsequent inferotemporal activations [99],gain suggesting a key role for ventral prefrontal cortex in main-aining pre-existing templates linked to stimulus salience – or whate have called affective control settings – that modulate visual

ortex activity.In an innovative study specifically examining patterns of affect-

iased competition in visual cortex, Wieser et al. [100] employedteady state visually evoked potentials (ssVEPs) to examine pat-erns of ABA related to trait anxiety. The ssVEP is an oscillatory EEGesponse to flickering stimuli whose oscillatory frequency matcheshat of the driving stimulus. ssVEPs are useful indices of attentionalllocation, as ssVEP amplitude is linked to allocation of attentionesources to the driving stimulus, and it can be modulated byoth ‘bottom-up’ sensory processing and ‘top-down’ modulationf sensory activity. In this study participants viewed Gabor patchesgratings) which were superimposed over pictures of angry, neu-ral and happy faces [100]. Gabor patches and faces each oscillatedt a different frequency so that ssVEPs for each could be distin-uished. Participants were asked to detect changes in the directionf the grating of the Gabor patches, a task which required direct-ng attention away from the underlying face stimuli. Participants

ere selected for either high or low social anxiety (HSA and LSA)ased on a preliminary questionnaire. The study found that ssVEPmplitudes for Gabor patches were attenuated by angry faces rel-tive to neutral and happy faces for HSA individuals and by happyaces relative to angry and neutral faces for LSA individuals. Fur-hermore, the highest cost for processing of Gabor patches occurredhen the underlying face was angry for HSA individuals and happy

or LSA individuals. This evidence suggests that affective saliencean operate according to mechanisms of biased competition simi-ar to those that have been well mapped for visual attention, sinceompetition from the face stimuli resulted in diminished resourcellocation to the Gabor patch. Moreover, this study is an elegantemonstration that individuals may differ in patterns of ABA toifferently valenced stimuli. Thus, preliminary research suggestshat, at the level of neuronal populations, processes of biased com-

Please cite this article in press as: Markovic J, et al. Tuning to the significanvisual perception and memory. Behav Brain Res (2013), http://dx.doi.org/1

etition, potentially tuned via Hebbian learning, may subserveBA.

PRESS Research xxx (2013) xxx– xxx 7

5.3. Acquisition of affective biases

The question of the learning processes by which such biasesare acquired and sensory systems are tuned is essentially a devel-opmental question, as many things become salient over repeatedexperience in infancy, childhood, and beyond [8]. On a shorter timescale, such questions can be tractably addressed in the laboratoryin sessions where salience is learned through conditioning. We canthink about the process of conditioning in terms of the creationand tuning of affective control settings which track the stimuli thathave proved a significant source of punishment and reward. Humanconditioning studies have revealed that associative learning mech-anisms play a key role in acquisition of ABA. Convergent researchsuggests that learning history continuously retunes neuronal sen-sitivity to the features of salient stimuli, and that this effect can beobserved in early stages of visual processing. This may occur boththrough re-entrant activation of visual cortex from other regionsin affective salience networks, including the amygdala, OFC and LC,as well as increased local neuronal sensitivity in early visual cortex– processes that may operate at different time scales (for thor-ough review see [86]). Again, ssVEPs have been used effectively toindex enhancement of neuronal population of responses in specificlearning contexts. Recent evidence suggests that sensory tuningto the salience of conditioned stimuli is mediated by implicitlyacquired Hebbian mechanisms of temporally coordinated neuronalactivity, rather than explicit expectations, again suggesting someindependence from executive modulation of attention [101]. Alongsimilar lines, future research can examine the role of other learn-ing processes, such as vicarious learning, in the acquisition of ABA.Developmental research can address the question of whether thereare sensitive time windows in early life during which affective asso-ciations may be more easily acquired or changed.

6. The role of norepinephrine in affect-biased attentionand memory

6.1. The role of norepinephrine in ABA and memory

A further question concerns neuromodulatory influences onneuronal activity linked to ABA. A comprehensive body of researchon LC–NE activity indicates a potentially key role for this neuro-chemical system in driving aspects of ABA. Non-human animalstudies have found that motivationally relevant stimuli elicit LCresponse [for review see [14,13], and LC–NE activity has beenshown to directly modulate visual cortex activation [102]. More-over, NE activity in the amygdala is important for recruiting andcoordinating the brain regions that direct attention to emotionallysalient events [103,104]. Let us elaborate further on this evidence.

The LC is structurally well positioned to facilitate ABA. It receivesinputs from the central nucleus of the amygdala [12] as well asventral prefrontal regions important for stimulus evaluation anddecision-making (for review see [105]) facilitating tuning of LCactivity to what is motivationally relevant. The LC also projects toregions of the thalamus and visual cortex [106], allowing for rapidtuning of sensory responses.

A wide body of evidence suggests that LC neurons facilitateresponses to the behavioural and biological relevance of a stim-ulus [13], regardless of stimulus valence [12], while suppressingthose to less relevant stimuli. Arousing stimuli elicit phasic LC acti-vation resulting in release of NE [107–110]. Released NE may tunetarget neurons by improving their signal-to-noise ratio, inhibiting

t: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of0.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.018

allowing silent neurons to become responsive to relevant stimuli[13]. In non-human animal studies, increased extracellular NE has

630

631

Page 8: Tuning to the significant: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of visual perception and memory

ING Model

B

8 l Brain

bettasusattr

sistb

nsBdmts

NNvhacpt[hmtmthsatba

eoto[brb[reeaTaesa

632

633

634

635

636

637

638

639

640

641

642

643

644

645

646

647

648

649

650

651

652

653

654

655

656

657

658

659

660

661

662

663

664

665

666

667

668

669

670

671

672

673

674

675

676

677

678

679

680

681

682

683

684

685

686

687

688

689

690

691

692

693

694

695

696

697

698

699

700

701

702

703

704

705

706

707

708

709

710

711

712

713

714

715

716

717

718

719

720

721

722

723

724

725

726

727

728

729

730

731

732

733

734

735

736

737

738

739

740

741

742

743

744

745

746

747

748

749

750

751

752

753

754

ARTICLEBR 8596 1–13

J. Markovic et al. / Behavioura

een shown to decrease spontaneous firing while leaving intactvoked response to sensory stimulation in somatosensory, olfac-ory and auditory pathways [112–114]. For example, NE appliedo auditory neurons in awake monkeys, who were presented with

series of conspecific vocalizations, resulted in a decrease inpontaneous activity but a spared response to the auditory stim-lus [115]. NE also improves spike timing and rhythmicity inomatosensory and olfactory neurons, suggesting that it provides

basis for encoding and perceptual accuracy [116–120]. All ofhese studies reinforce the view that the LC–NE system is sensitiveo the behavioural relevance of stimuli and influences perceptualesponses.

LC activity is also important in associative learning of what isalient. LC neurons fire in response to direct reward and pun-shment, and subsequently to any stimuli associated with thealient event [13]. NE modulation of long-term changes in synap-ic strength and gene transcription allow this system to guideehaviour based on stimulus salience within a given context [12].

Thus, the LC–NE system has the functional and anatomical con-ections needed to facilitate ABA. LC activity is driven by affectivelyalient stimuli and is capable of modulating visual cortex activation.ased on this evidence, BANE posits LC–NE activity as an importantriving force behind ABA. One hypothesis that emerges from thisodel is that, in humans, LC–NE activity modulates biased compe-

ition in the visual cortex, biasing processing of affectively salienttimuli (Fig. 5).

According to the modulation hypothesis [121] the influence ofE linked to arousal at encoding interacts with the influence ofE on more sustained consolidation processes, resulting in moreivid memories for emotionally salient stimuli. In this regard, non-uman animal studies have implicated NE in memory consolidationnd the formation of long-term memories [122]. The amygdaloidomplex influences memory consolidation processes in the hip-ocampus, caudate nucleus and other regions. It is also a keyarget site for the LC–NE system, possessing many NE receptors106]. Cahill and McGaugh [16] provide evidence that NE, stressormones, and the amygdala are part of an endogenous memoryodulating system, which influences recall based on the emo-

ional meaning of a stimulus. Noradrenergic activity is implicated inemory modulation, since infusion of adrenergic antagonists into

he amygdala eliminates memory modulation effects [123]. Non-uman animal studies provide further evidence that modulation oftress hormones influencing consolidation are mediated by beta-drenergic activity in the amygdala [16]. For instance, lesions ofhe amygdala and stria terminalis (a major amygdala output path)lock the memory-enhancing effects of adrenaline, glucocorticoids,nd drugs that affect the opiate and gabaergic systems [124,125].

The basolateral amygdala (BLA) plays a key role modulating theffects of other neurotransmitters and stress-released hormonesn memory consolidation [104,16]. Selective post-training inac-ivation of the BLA induces retrograde amnesia [126] and lesionsf the BLA block stress-hormone induced memory enhancement125]. BLA activation can also modulate synaptic plasticity in otherrain regions key for memory consolidation [15]. Moreover, theole of BLA in modulation of emotional memory consolidation haseen found to be in part mediated by alpha(2)-adrenoreceptors127]. In humans, NE activity has been found to play a role ineconfiguring brain network activity both during and subsequent toxposure to a stressor, suggesting that noradrenergic modulation ofncoding and memory occurs via reorganization of large-scale co-ctivation between regions sensitive to affective salience [85,89].hus, not only is NE activity key for ABA for salient stimuli within

Please cite this article in press as: Markovic J, et al. Tuning to the significanvisual perception and memory. Behav Brain Res (2013), http://dx.doi.org/1

ny given context, but noradrenergic activity in the amygdala atncoding may interact with NE activity implicated in memory con-olidation processes, ensuring that events tagged as most salientre not only more vividly perceived but better remembered. The

PRESS Research xxx (2013) xxx– xxx

BANE model further hypothesizes that noradrenergic activity mod-ulates behavioural and neural correlates of emotionally enhancedperceptual and mnemonic vividness found in our previous studies(Fig. 5).

7. Individual differences in NE influence on affect-biasedattention and memory

7.1. Individual differences in ABA

Individuals differ both in the capacity for ABA and memoryand in the relative salience of different categories of stimulus – inparticular stimulus valence. In the study of individual differences,attentional biases are typically not measured by indices of ABA aswe have defined it in terms of affectively biased attentional setsthat pre-tune visual attention prior to encountering stimuli. Rather,biases are measured primarily by indices of difficulty in disengag-ing spatial attention from the location of affectively salient stimuliafter they have been presented [128] – as a kind of attentional“stickiness,” or failure of executive control processes.

Behavioural and ERP studies of attentional biases indicate thatindividuals with temperamental anxiety show greater attentionalstickiness to threatening stimuli than non-anxious individuals[129–131]. Attentional bias for threatening stimuli is also asso-ciated with lower threshold for amygdala activation to threat[130,132]. Recent reviews have summarized current research onthe relation between threat-bias and anxiety [133], including anexamination of the time course of responses to threat-related stim-uli in attentional bias [134], as well as biases associated withpersonality measures [135]. In the other direction, biases towardspositive stimuli have been linked to extraversion [136]. In addi-tion to being linked with traits, attentional biases can be learnedthrough conditioning and are associated with trauma [137–139].For instance, individuals with PTSD showed increased perceptualand amygdala sensitivity to stimuli associated with the trauma[140,141].

Individual differences in attentional biases have been observedearly in development, and can influence behavioural outcomes.Attentional stickiness to the location of threatening stimuli inchildren with temperamental inhibition has been found to pre-dict whether they would show social withdrawal behaviour atage five [142]. Such biases can be reinforced by experience overthe course of development. For instance, children with a shortversion of the 5HTTLPR (serotonin-transporter-linked polymorphicregion) in SLC6A4, the serotonin transporter gene, which is asso-ciated with temperamental fearfulness and amygdala sensitivityto threat [143,144], are more likely to have a family environmentthat emphasizes threat stimuli, thus exacerbating the underlyingtrait [145,146]. On the other hand, sensitivity to negative stimulican be attenuated by the ability to shift attention. Children highin negative affect and effortful control – a trait which includes theability to volitionally focus and shift attention – do not show theattentional bias to threat displayed by children with negative affectand low effortful control [147]. Clinical research has explored thepossibility of improving anxiety symptoms by training attention.Attentional Bias Modification (ABM) uses a cueing task to train par-ticipants’ attentional biases by placing targets more frequently atthe location of neutral than negative stimuli. ABM has diminishedattentional bias towards negatively valenced stimuli and reducedanxiety scores in clinical and non-clinical populations [148] as well

t: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of0.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.018

as children [149]. That training attentional biasing has an effecton anxiety scores suggests that attentional biases may be partiallyresponsible for producing anxiety symptoms. However, it shouldbe noted that ABM research is in its preliminary stages, effect sizes

755

756

757

758

Page 9: Tuning to the significant: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of visual perception and memory

IN PRESSG Model

B

l Brain Research xxx (2013) xxx– xxx 9

ao

7

omarbdrN

wtaaagstc

eTbvFtiegtoafdaoWpasN

avs�Niigatcimfmdwnc

Fig. 4. ADRA2b influence on the attentional blink as measured by accuracy at Lag1, when the second target word directly follows the first and the attentional blinkeffect is greatest. Whereas both ADRA2b deletion carriers and non-carriers showedthe typical ‘emotional sparing,’ or greater accuracy for affectively salient words,only deletion carriers showed an additional sparing over non-carriers, suggesting

759

760

761

762

763

764

765

766

767

768

769

770

771

772

773

774

775

776

777

778

779

780

781

782

783

784

785

786

787

788

789

790

791

792

793

794

795

796

797

798

799

800

801

802

803

804

805

806

807

808

809

810

811

812

813

814

815

816

817

818

819

820

821

822

823

824

825

826

827

828

829

830

831

832

833

834

835

836

837

838

839

840

841

842

843

844

845

846

847

848

849

850

851

852

853

854

855

856

857

858

859

ARTICLEBR 8596 1–13

J. Markovic et al. / Behavioura

re small, and replication studies are needed to confirm the effectf ABM on subsequent outcomes and emotional responses [150].

.2. Genetic influences on ABA

Now that we have reviewed the role of NE in ABA and mem-ry, we turn to genetic variations linked to NE availability thatay partly underlie these individual differences. A deletion vari-

nt of the ADRA2b gene coding the inhibitory noradrenergic �2Beceptor is missing 9 base pairs, which impairs receptor regulationy G protein-coupled receptor kinase leading to a loss of receptoresensitization [151]. As inhibitory activity is itself inhibited, car-ying the deletion variant results in higher levels of extracellularE availability.

A recent seminal study linked the ADRA2b deletion variant,hich was previously associated with vasoconstriction, to emo-

ional memory [152]. In this study, participants viewed neutral andffectively salient images and performed a free recall task shortlyfterward. Whereas all participants showed greater recall for therousing images, ADRA2b deletion carriers showed a significantlyreater emotional memory enhancement in the laboratory. Theame study found that survivors of Rwandan genocide who carriedhe deletion variant were more vulnerable to intrusive memoriesharacteristic of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD).

A further question concerns whether the ADRA2b influencesmotional memory by enhancing encoding, consolidation, or both.he hypothesis that ADRA2b plays a role in perception is supportedy evidence that deletion carriers show increased amygdala acti-ation to negative arousing images relative to non-carriers [153].urthermore, in a study using the emotional attentional blink, par-icipants given reboxetine (a selective NE reuptake inhibitor whichncreases the amount of available NE) showed a smaller blink formotional stimuli – a greater emotional sparing – than participantsiven a placebo [154]. In a recent study, we used an emotional atten-ional blink paradigm to directly examine the influence of ADRA2bn affective biases in perception in a large sample of healthy youngdults [155]. The study employed the AB task to investigate dif-erences in ABA for positive, negative, and neutral words betweeneletion variant carriers and non-carriers while controlling for sexs well as individual differences in trait neuroticism, working mem-ry, and other genes potentially implicated in attentional biases.hereas all participants showed the classic emotional sparing for

ositive and negative over neutral words, deletion carriers showed further sparing for negative over positive words (Fig. 4). Thus, thistudy showed that ADRA2b affects visual encoding, suggesting thatE has an important role in ABA.

LC activity has two phases, ongoing oscillatory tonic activitynd stimulus-locked phasic activity [156]. The ADRA2b deletionariant influences activity at NE �-receptors, leading to their desen-itization and putative greater NE availability [151]. Whereas NE-receptor activity is linked with the influence of phasic release ofE on the attention blink task [157], the emotional sparing effect

n the attentional blink task is associated with a tonic increasen synaptic NE [154]. Our finding that deletion carriers showed areater emotional sparing in the AB task suggests that �-receptorctivity may be the mechanism behind emotional sparing, andhus ABA, via increased tonic levels of NE. This interpretation isonsistent with a study by Cousijn et al. [158]. In this study, partic-pants viewed either a violent movie (stress condition) or a neutral

ovie (non-stress condition) and then saw sets of dynamic fear-ul and happy faces. Amygdala activation was measured during the

ovie and face stimuli. Non-carriers showed an increased amyg-

Please cite this article in press as: Markovic J, et al. Tuning to the significanvisual perception and memory. Behav Brain Res (2013), http://dx.doi.org/1

ala response to emotional faces only in the non-stress condition,hereas deletion carriers had such a response in both the stress andon-stress conditions. As the authors suggest, while non-deletionarriers seemed to be hitting a ceiling in their amygdala response

860

biased perceptual encoding of negative stimuli is associated with higher levels ofextracellular NE.

to emotional stimuli, deletion carriers possess a further range ofactivation. We may speculate that, as deletion carriers have lessactivity at inhibitory alpha2b receptors, a sustained mood induc-tion may increase tonic NE levels in carriers only, whereas it mayinhibit them in non-carriers. The greater tonic NE in deletion carri-ers would then interact with phasic activity to increase amygdalaactivation in non-carriers.

In our study, ADRA2b deletion carriers showed greater ABAtowards negative stimuli, consistent with previous findings thatcarriers show enhanced amygdala activation during perception ofnegative stimuli [153]. An outstanding question concerns whetherdeletion carriers show enhanced ABA for negative stimuli becausethey find negative stimuli to be more salient than non-carriers, orwhether they show enhanced NE-driven activity for stimuli that aregenerally salient to carriers and non-carriers alike (after controllingfor key trait differences), since young adults in this age group showan overall bias for negative stimuli (e.g. [159]).

The enhanced perceptual processing of emotional events dis-played by deletion carriers may result in enhanced emotionalmemory via NE �-receptors in the amygdala. A further study exam-ined whether subjective ratings of affective experience at encodingpredicted the accuracy and confidence of subsequent memory[160]. The same group of healthy young adults rated positive,negative, and neutral scenes for level of emotional arousal and sub-sequently performed a surprise recognition memory task one weeklater. Results showed that, for negative images, subjective ratingsof arousal at encoding predicted better memory accuracy one weeklater in deletion carriers. In contrast, non-carriers showed poorermemory when they rated images as higher in arousal. ADRA2bdeletion carriers also demonstrated a stronger overall relationshipbetween the subjective arousal level of each image at encodingand memory confidence for the same image one week later [160].According to the modulation hypothesis, phasic arousal related toperceptual vividness interacts with more tonic arousal extendingbeyond initial encoding to further enhance memory consolidation[121]. Our finding that carriers of the deletion variant showed agreater association between arousal at encoding and memory foremotionally salient images suggests that, for these individuals, it

t: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of0.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.018

is precisely this NE-mediated relationship between encoding andpost-encoding processes that is enhanced – at least for moder-ately arousing events [see also [122]]. Of course, noradrenergic

861

862

863

Page 10: Tuning to the significant: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of visual perception and memory

ARTICLE IN PRESSG Model

BBR 8596 1–13

10 J. Markovic et al. / Behavioural Brain Research xxx (2013) xxx– xxx

Fig. 5. Key pathways emphasized by BANE model: Green dashed lines indicate NE pathways. Red lines indicate projections to the LC. Thicker lines indicate direct modulationof visual cortex activity in affect-biased attention. NE activity is implicated in both stimulus encoding and selective attention [13]. A salient stimulus activates LC neurons,which project widely to cortical and subcortical regions. LC neurons are highly sensitive to previously acquired associations between a stimulus and punishment or reward.Descending influences from amygdala (central nucleus) and ventral prefrontal cortices Aston-Jones et al., 2007) provide information about contextually determine relevance,Q10which can then modulate the pattern of LC firing accordingly. Activity from LC can modulate activity in visual cortex directly, facilitating gating and tuning of neuronal activitya l cortQ11a ry encl

ii[ufmmfmars

tengr

8

Laifbvpiis

864

865

866

867

868

869

870

871

872

873

874

875

876

877

878

879

880

881

882

883

884

885

886

887

888

889

890

891

892

893

894

895

896

897

898

899

900

901

902

903

904

905

906

907

908

909

910

911

912

913

914

915

916

917

918

919

920

921

nd enhancing perceptual acuity directly as well as via the amygdala and prefrontas prefrontal regions and in turn modulates hippocampus activity related to memoegend, the reader is referred to the web version of the article.)

nfluences on perception, encoding, and memory do not work insolation, but interact with the influence of other neuromodulators13,14]. Although we did not find any interactions with genes mod-lating serotonin or dopamine, the influence of NE at adrenoceptorsound in terminals of serotonergic and dopaminergic neurons [14]

ay also play a role in the influence of ADRA2b on encoding andemory. Moreover, other neurochemicals may play a role in dif-

erent aspects of attentional biases. For example, whereas NE mayodulate activity in brain regions associated with object-based

spects of affective salience, serotonin may influence activity inegion sensitive to spatial attention/contextual aspects of affectivealience linked to attentional stickiness.

Future fMRI research can use ADRA2b groupings to examinehe role of NE in neural and behavioural markers of emotionallynhanced perceptual vividness and its relation to memory vivid-ess, on post-encoding processes in humans as well as potentialene-gene interactions (epistasis) and interactions with life expe-ience.

. Summary

The BANE model’s core claim is that ABA is partly driven byC–NE processes, which interact with activity in hubs of anteriorffective system key for tagging affective salience and modulat-ng activity in visual cortices. BANE provides a unified explanationor multiple streams of data related to ABA. Building on a largeody of evidence indicating that emotional stimuli elicit enhancedisual processing compared to neutral stimuli, convergent evidence

Please cite this article in press as: Markovic J, et al. Tuning to the significanvisual perception and memory. Behav Brain Res (2013), http://dx.doi.org/1

oints to the amygdala and OFC as among the regions that aremportant for modulation of visual cortex in ABA. Pharmacolog-cal and non-human animal studies have revealed that affectivealience related activity of this network is partly driven by LC–NE

ices. The amygdala receives contextual information from the hippocampus as welloding and consolidation. (For interpretation of the references to color in this figure

activity. A body of evidence indicates LC activation is implicated intuning and gating of perceptual responses to salient stimuli, allow-ing for enhanced responses to what is already salient as well aslearning of new associations. Thus, NE activity in the amygdalamay play an important role for recruiting visual cortex activationassociated with ABA (Fig. 5).

The importance of NE in ABA and memory is supported bygenetics studies on ADRA2b, a polymorphism that affects levels ofextracellular NE. ADRA2b deletion carriers are more sensitive toemotionally salient stimuli. They show greater ABA for negativestimuli, show a stronger relation between subjective arousal andmemory, experience greater emotional enhancement of memory,and are more likely to suffer from intrusive traumatic memories.However, it should be noted that is possible that higher levels oftonic NE availability associated with the deletion variant increaseresponsiveness to salient stimuli in general, and not just thosethat are affectively significant. It will be important to investigatewhether deletion carriers are more sensitive to low-level visual fea-tures than non-carriers and control for any such differences wheninvestigating the influence of ADRA2b in ABA. It is also important tonote that genetics studies reveal only a correlational relationshipbetween NE activity and ABA. Further pharmacological studies ofNE demonstrating a causal role for NE systems in human ABA wouldsubstantially strengthen the model.

The BANE model is similar to – and substantially overlaps with –another recent model of ABA, the MAGiC model [18]. According tothe MAGiC model, emotional stimuli gain enhanced perceptual pro-cessing via multiple amplification mechanisms operating in parallel

t: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of0.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.018

(and not via a single top-down modulatory source). The MAGiCmodel emphasizes the key causal role the amygdala plays in adap-tive gain processes subserving ABA, and the relative independenceof the amygdala-centred affective attention system. Thus, it focuses

922

923

924

925

Page 11: Tuning to the significant: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of visual perception and memory

ING Model

B

l Brain

oaofpvnte

9

tmtasiugntcBeaio

ttesdge

ttawpArpuue

RQ7

Q8

Q9

926

927

928

929

930

931

932

933

934

935

936

937

938

939

940

941

942

943

944

945

946

947

948

949

950

951

952

953

954

955

956

957

958

959

960

961

962

963

964

965

966

967

968

969

970

971

972

973

974

975

976

977

978

979

980

981

982

983

984

985

986

987

988

989

990

991

992

993

994

995

996

997

998

999

1000

1001

1002

1003

1004

1005

1006

1007

1008

1009

1010

1011

1012

1013

1014

1015

1016

1017

1018

1019

1020

1021

1022

1023

1024

1025

1026

1027

1028

1029

1030

1031

1032

1033

1034

1035

1036

1037

1038

1039

1040

1041

1042

1043

1044

1045

1046

1047

1048

1049

1050

1051

1052

1053

1054

1055

1056

1057

1058

1059

1060

1061

1062

1063

1064

1065

1066

1067

1068

1069

1070

1071

1072

ARTICLEBR 8596 1–13

J. Markovic et al. / Behavioura

n the degree to which amygdala-centred systems can processffectively salient stimuli even when they are task-irrelevant [161]r when emotional information is presented outside the focus ofrontoparietal attention [80,162,163]. Like MAGiC, the BANE modelroposes that affectively salient stimuli bias perception through aentral attentional system – although according to BANE these doot necessarily operate independently from frontoparietal atten-ion (for a similar perspective see [164]). The BANE model furthermphasizes the additional role of the LC–NE system.

. Future directions

The BANE model makes a number of predictions that can beested by future research. First, we predict that NE availability

odulates individual differences in EEV, and that ADRA2b dele-ion carriers will show enhanced EEV supported by enhancedmygdala/ventromedial activation, which will in turn predict sub-equent memory vividness. Second, we predict that NE availabilitynfluences electrophysiological correlates of biased competitionnderlying ABA, and that ADRA2b deletion carriers will showreater evidence of biased competition for arousing stimuli thanon-carriers. Third, future research can use the ADRA2b genotypeo investigate noradrenergic contributions to post-encoding pro-esses associated with affectively enhanced memory. Fourth, theANE model can be integrated with computational models of influ-nce of affective salience on perceptual expectations that guidettention according to context [98,165,166]. Finally, pharmacolog-cal interventions can further probe causal effects of NE availabilityn ABA.

A longer term research programme can involve investigation ofhe role of NE in learning processes by which ABA develops overhe lifespan, and the influence of both genotype and life experi-nce in both normal and pathological patterns of ABA, extendingalience-based models of predictive coding to address ABA in aevelopmental context. Understanding the interaction betweenenotype and epigenetic changes due to specific types of experi-nce will be an important part of such a research programme.

Finally, an important area for future research is to understandhe role of ABA in post-traumatic stress-disorder (PTSD) in conjunc-ion with research on the influences of genotype, life experience,nd traumatic event on neurophysiological processes associatedith generation and perpetuation of PTSD symptoms. For exam-le are pre-existing patterns of ABA a risk factor for PTSD? DoDRA2b deletion carriers show evidence of greater ABA for trauma-elated stimuli than non-carriers and do these patterns of ABAredict intrusive memory? Ultimately understanding mechanismsnderlying individual differences in ABA and memory can help usnderstand mechanisms underlying how they are shaped by lifexperience over development and in trauma.

eferences

[1] Cosmelli D. Attending to the stream of consciousness: a methodological chal-lenge. In: Aboitiz F, Cosmelli D, editors. From attention to goal directedbehavior. Berlin: Springer-Verlag; 2009. p. 83–103.

[2] Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven atten-tion in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2002;3:201–15.

[3] Kastner S, Ungerleider LG. The neural basis of biased competition in humanvisual cortex. Neuropsychologia 2001;39:1263–76.

[4] Serences JT, Yantis S. Spatially selective representations of voluntary andstimulus-driven attentional priority in human occipital, parietal, and frontalcortex. Cereb Cortex 2007;17:284–93.

[5] Awh E, Belopolsky AV, Theeuwes J. Top-down versus bottom-up attentionalcontrol: a failed theoretical dichotomy. Trends Cogn Sci 2012;16:437–43.

[6] Zhao J, Al-Aidroos N, Turk-Browne NB. Attention is spontaneously biased

Please cite this article in press as: Markovic J, et al. Tuning to the significanvisual perception and memory. Behav Brain Res (2013), http://dx.doi.org/1

toward regularities. Psychol Sci 2013;24:667–77.[7] Rauss K, Pourtois G. What is bottom-up and what is top-down in predictive

coding? Front Psychol 2013;4:276.[8] Todd RM, Cunningham WA, Anderson AK, Thompson E. Affect-biased atten-

tion as emotion regulation. Trends Cogn Sci 2012;16:365–72.

PRESS Research xxx (2013) xxx– xxx 11

[9] Pessoa L. The impact of emotion on cognition. In: Ochsner KN, Kosslyn SM,editors. The handbook of cognitive neuroscience. New York, NY: Oxford Uni-

versity Press; 2013 [in press].[10] Rudrauf D, David O, Lachaux JP, Kovach CK, Martinerie J, Renault B, et al.

Rapid interactions between the ventral visual stream and emotion-relatedstructures rely on a two-pathway architecture. J Neurosci 2008;28:2793–803.

[11] Jones BE, Halaris AE, McIlhany M, Moore RY. Ascending projections of thelocus coeruleus in the rat. I. Axonal transport in central noradrenaline neu-rons. Brain Res 1977;127:1–21.

[12] Berridge CW, Waterhouse BD. The locus coeruleus-noradrenergic system:modulation of behavioral state and state-dependent cognitive processes.Brain Res Brain Res Rev 2003;42:33–84.

[13] Sara SJ. The locus coeruleus and noradrenergic modulation of cognition. NatRev Neurosci 2009;10:211–23.

[14] Sara SJ, Bouret S. Orienting and reorienting: the locus coeruleus mediatescognition through arousal. Neuron 2012;76:130–41.

[15] McGaugh JL, McIntyre CK, Power AE. Amygdala modulation of memoryconsolidation: interaction with other brain systems. Neurobiol Learn Mem2002;78:539–52.

[16] Cahill L, McGaugh JL. Mechanisms of emotional arousal and lasting declarativememory. Trends Neurosci 1998;21:294–9.

[17] Frankland PW, Bontempi B. The organization of recent and remote memories.Nat Rev Neurosci 2005;6:119–30.

[18] Pourtois G, Schettino A, Vuilleumier P. Brain mechanisms for emotional influ-ences on perception and attention: what is magic and what is not. Biol Psychol2013;92:492–512.

[19] Dolcos F, Iordan AD, Dolcos S. Neural correlates of emotion-cognition interac-tions: a review of evidence from brain imaging investigations. J Cogn Psychol(Hove) 2011;23:669–94.

[20] Nielsen SL, Sarason IG. Emotion, personality, and selective attention. J PersSoc Psychol 1981;41:945–60.

[21] Soares JJ, Ohman A. Backward masking and skin conductance responses afterconditioning to nonfeared but fear-relevant stimuli in fearful subjects. Psy-chophysiology 1993;30:460–6.

[22] Anderson AK. Affective influences on the attentional dynamics supportingawareness. J Exp Psychol Gen 2005;134:258–81.

[23] Dolcos F, McCarthy G. Brain systems mediating cognitive interference byemotional distraction. J Neurosci 2006;26:2072–9.

[24] Dolcos F, Diaz-Granados P, Wang L, McCarthy G. Opposing influences ofemotional and non-emotional distracters upon sustained prefrontal cortexactivity during a delayed-response working memory task. Neuropsychologia2008;46:326–35.

[25] Dolcos F, LaBar KS, Cabeza R. Interaction between the amygdala and themedial temporal lobe memory system predicts better memory for emotionalevents. Neuron 2004;42:855–63.

[26] McGaugh JL. Memory reconsolidation hypothesis revived but restrained:theoretical comment on Biedenkapp and Rudy (2004). Behav Neurosci2004;118:1140–2.

[27] Phelps EA. Human emotion and memory: interactions of the amygdala andhippocampal complex. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2004;14:198–202.

[28] Kensinger EA, Corkin S. Effect of negative emotional content on workingmemory and long-term memory. Emotion 2003;3:378–93.

[29] Sharot T, Martorella EA, Delgado MR, Phelps EA. How personal experiencemodulates the neural circuitry of memories of September 11. Proc Natl AcadSci USA 2007;104:389–94.

[30] Todd RM, Mueller D, Palombo DJ, Robertson A, Eaton T, Freeman N, et al.Deletion variant in the ADRA2b gene enhances affective modulation of therelation between encoding and memory. J Neurobio Learn Mem 2013.

[31] Sharot T, Verfaellie M, Yonelinas AP. How emotion strengthens the rec-ollective experience: a time-dependent hippocampal process. PLoS ONE2007;2:e1068.

[32] Anderson AK, Phelps EA. Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhancedperception of emotionally salient events. Nature 2001;411:305–9.

[33] Bradley MM, Sabatinelli D, Lang PJ, Fitzsimmons JR, King W, Desai P. Activa-tion of the visual cortex in motivated attention. Behav Neurosci 2003;117:369–80.

[34] Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Fitzsimmons JR, Cuthbert BN, Scott JD, Moulder B, et al.Emotional arousal and activation of the visual cortex: an fMRI analysis. Psy-chophysiology 1998;35:199–210.

[35] Lane RD, Chua PM, Dolan RJ. Common effects of emotional valence, arousaland attention on neural activation during visual processing of pictures. Neu-ropsychologia 1999;37:989–97.

[36] Sabatinelli D, Bradley MM, Fitzsimmons JR, Lang PJ. Parallel amygdala andinferotemporal activation reflect emotional intensity and fear relevance. Neu-roimage 2005;24:1265–70.

[37] Grandjean D, Sander D, Pourtois G, Schwartz S, Seghier ML, Scherer KR, et al.The voices of wrath: brain responses to angry prosody in meaningless speech.Nat Neurosci 2005;8:145–6.

[38] Ethofer T, Bretscher J, Gschwind M, Kreifelts B, Wildgruber D, Vuilleu-mier P. Emotional voice areas: anatomic location, functional properties,and structural connections revealed by combined fMRI/DTI. Cereb Cortex

t: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of0.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.018

2012;22:191–200.[39] Ethofer T, Van De Ville D, Scherer K, Vuilleumier P. Decoding of emotional

information in voice-sensitive cortices. Curr Biol 2009;19:1028–33.[40] Morris JS, Ohman A, Dolan RJ. Conscious and unconscious emotional learning

in the human amygdala. Nature 1998;393:467–70.

1073

1074

1075

1076

1077

Page 12: Tuning to the significant: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of visual perception and memory

ING Model

B

1 l Brain

1078

1079

1080

1081

1082

1083

1084

1085

1086

1087

1088

1089

1090

1091

1092

1093

1094

1095

1096

1097

1098

1099

1100

1101

1102

1103

1104

1105

1106

1107

1108

1109

1110

1111

1112

1113

1114

1115

1116

1117

1118

1119

1120

1121

1122

1123

1124

1125

1126

1127

1128

1129

1130

1131

1132

1133

1134

1135

1136

1137

1138

1139

1140

1141

1142

1143

1144

1145

1146

1147

1148

1149

1150

1151

1152

1153

1154

1155

1156

1157

1158

1159

1160

1161

1162

1163

1164

1165

1166

1167

1168

1169

1170

1171

1172

1173

1174

1175

1176

1177

1178

1179

1180

1181

1182

1183

1184

1185

1186

1187

1188

1189

1190

1191

1192

1193

1194

1195

1196

1197

1198

1199

1200

1201

1202

1203

1204

1205

1206

1207

1208

1209

1210

1211

1212

1213

1214

1215

1216

1217

1218

1219

1220

1221

1222

1223

1224

1225

1226

1227

1228

1229

1230

1231

1232

1233

1234

1235

1236

1237

1238

1239

1240

1241

1242

1243

1244

ARTICLEBR 8596 1–13

2 J. Markovic et al. / Behavioura

[41] Critchley H, Daly E, Phillips M, Brammer M, Bullmore E, Williams S, et al.Explicit and implicit neural mechanisms for processing of social informationfrom facial expressions: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. HumBrain Mapp 2000;9:93–105.

[42] Vuilleumier P, Armony JL, Driver J, Dolan RJ. Effects of attention and emotionon face processing in the human brain: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron2001;30:829–41.

[43] Vuilleumier P, Armony JL, Clarke K, Husain M, Driver J, Dolan RJ. Neuralresponse to emotional faces with and without awareness: event-related fMRIin a parietal patient with visual extinction and spatial neglect. Neuropsy-chologia 2002;40:2156–66.

[44] Lim SL, Padmala S, Pessoa L. Segregating the significant from the mundane ona moment-to-moment basis via direct and indirect amygdala contributions.Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2009;106:16841–6.

[45] Sabatinelli D, Lang PJ, Keil A, Bradley MM. Emotional perception: correlation offunctional MRI and event-related potentials. Cereb Cortex 2007;17:1085–91.

[46] Schupp HT, Stockburger J, Codispoti M, Junghofer M, Weike AI, Hamm AO.Selective visual attention to emotion. J Neurosci 2007;27:1082–9.

[47] West GL, Anderson AA, Ferber S, Pratt J. Electrophysiological evidencefor biased competition in V1 for fear expressions. J Cogn Neurosci2011;23:3410–8.

[48] Rauss K, Schwartz S, Pourtois G. Top-down effects on early visual pro-cessing in humans: a predictive coding framework. Neurosci Biobehav Rev2011;35:1237–53.

[49] Pourtois G, Grandjean D, Sander D, Vuilleumier P. Electrophysiologicalcorrelates of rapid spatial orienting towards fearful faces. Cereb Cortex2004;14:619–33.

[50] Hillyard SA, Anllo-Vento L. Event-related brain potentials in the study of visualselective attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1998;95:781–7.

[51] Di Lollo V, Kawahara J, Shahab Ghorashi SM, Enns JT. The attentionalblink: resource depletion or temporary loss of control? Psychol Res2005;69:191–200.

[52] Cahill L, McGaugh JL. A novel demonstration of enhanced memory associatedwith emotional arousal. Conscious Cogn 1995;4:410–21.

[53] Ochsner KN. Are affective events richly recollected or simply familiar? Theexperience and process of recognizing feelings past. J Exp Psychol Gen2000;129:242–61.

[54] Brown R, Kulik S. Flashbulb memory. Cognition 1977;5:73–99.[55] Stevens SS. On the psychophysical law. Psychol Rev 1957;64:153–81.[56] Stevens SS. The direct estimation of sensory magnitudes—loudness. Am J

Psychol 1956;69:1–25.[57] Todd RM, Talmi D, Schmitz TW, Susskind J, Anderson AK. Psychophysical

and neural evidence for emotion-enhanced perceptual vividness. J Neuroci2012;32:11201–12.

[58] Itti L, Koch C. A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shiftsof visual attention. Vision Res 2000;40:1489–506.

[59] Itti L, Koch C. Computational modelling of visual attention. Nat Rev Neurosci2001;2:194–203.

[60] Carretie L, Hinojosa JA, Martin-Loeches M, Mercado F, Tapia M. Auto-matic attention to emotional stimuli: neural correlates. Hum Brain Mapp2004;22:290–9.

[61] Rousselet GA, Husk JS, Bennett PJ, Sekuler AB. Single-trial EEG dynamics ofobject and face visual processing. Neuroimage 2007;36:843–62.

[62] Grill-Spector K, Kushnir T, Hendler T, Edelman S, Itzchak Y, Malach R. Asequence of object-processing stages revealed by fMRI in the human occipitallobe. Hum Brain Mapp 1998;6:316–28.

[63] Kourtzi Z, Kanwisher N. Cortical regions involved in perceiving object shape.J Neurosci 2000;20:3310–8.

[64] Op de Beeck HP, Haushofer J, Kanwisher NG. Interpreting fMRI data: maps,modules and dimensions. Nat Rev Neurosci 2008;9:123–35.

[65] Craig AD. How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiologicalcondition of the body. Nat Rev Neurosci 2002;3:655–66.

[66] Craig AD. How do you feel—now? The anterior insula and human awareness.Nat Rev Neurosci 2009;10:59–70.

[67] Rossi V, Pourtois G. Negative affective state mimics effects of perceptual loadon spatial perception. Emotion 2012.

[68] Rossi V, Pourtois G. State-dependent attention modulation of human primaryvisual cortex: a high density ERP study. Neuroimage 2012;60:2365–78.

[69] Sharot T, Delgado MR, Phelps EA. How emotion enhances the feeling ofremembering. Nat Neurosci 2004;7:1376–80.

[70] Kensinger EA, Garoff-Eaton RJ, Schacter DL. Effects of emotion on memoryspecificity: memory trade-offs elicited by negative visually arousing stimuli.J Mem Lang 2007;56:575–91.

[71] Levine LJ, Edelstein RS. Emotion and memory narrowing: a review and goalrelevance approach. Cogn Emot 2009;23:833–75.

[72] De Martino B, Kalisch R, Rees G, Dolan RJ. Enhanced processing ofthreat stimuli under limited attentional resources. Cereb Cortex 2009;19:127–33.

[73] Canli T, Zhao Z, Brewer J, Gabrieli JD, Cahill L. Event-related activation inthe human amygdala associates with later memory for individual emotionalexperience. J Neurosci 2000;20:RC99.

Please cite this article in press as: Markovic J, et al. Tuning to the significanvisual perception and memory. Behav Brain Res (2013), http://dx.doi.org/1

[74] Hamann SB, Ely TD, Grafton ST, Kilts CD. Amygdala activity relatedto enhanced memory for pleasant and aversive stimuli. Nat Neurosci1999;2:289–93.

[75] Kensinger EA, Garoff-Eaton RJ, Schacter DL. How negative emotion enhancesthe visual specificity of a memory. J Cogn Neurosci 2007;19:1872–87.

PRESS Research xxx (2013) xxx– xxx

[76] Talmi D, Anderson AK, Riggs L, Caplan JB, Moscovitch M. Immediate memoryconsequences of the effect of emotion on attention to pictures. Learn Mem2008;15:172–82.

[77] Anderson AK, Wais PE, Gabrieli JD. Emotion enhances remembrance of neutralevents past. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2006;103:1599–604.

[78] McGaugh JL. Memory consolidation and the amygdala: a systems perspective.Trends Neurosci 2002;25:456.

[79] Todd RM, Schmitz TW, Susskind J, Anderson AK. Shared neural substratesof emotionally enhanced perceptual and mnemonic vividness. Front BehavNeurosci 2013;7:40.

[80] Vuilleumier P, Richardson MP, Armony JL, Driver J, Dolan RJ. Distant influ-ences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical activation during emotional faceprocessing. Nat Neurosci 2004;7:1271–8.

[81] Vuilleumier P, Driver J. Modulation of visual processing by attention and emo-tion: windows on causal interactions between human brain regions. PhilosTrans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2007;362:837–55.

[82] Furl N, Henson RN, Friston KJ, Calder AJ. Top-down control of visual responsesto fear by the amygdala. J Neurosci 2013;33:17435–43.

[83] Catani M, Jones DK, Donato R, Ffytche DH. Occipito-temporal connections inthe human brain. Brain 2003;126:2093–107.

[84] Amaral DG, Behniea H, Kelly JL. Topographic organization of projections fromthe amygdala to the visual cortex in the macaque monkey. Neuroscience2003;118:1099–120.

[85] van Marle HJ, Hermans EJ, Qin S, Fernandez G. Enhanced resting-state connec-tivity of amygdala in the immediate aftermath of acute psychological stress.Neuroimage 2010;53:348–54.

[86] Miskovic V, Keil A. Acquired fears reflected in cortical sensory processing: areview of electrophysiological studies of human classical conditioning. Psy-chophysiology 2012;49:1230–41.

[87] Pessoa L, Adolphs R. Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a ‘lowroad’ to ‘many roads’ of evaluating biological significance. Nat Rev Neurosci2010;11:773–83.

[88] Ghashghaei HT, Hilgetag CC, Barbas H. Sequence of information processingfor emotions based on the anatomic dialogue between prefrontal cortex andamygdala. Neuroimage 2007;34:905–23.

[89] Hermans EJ, van Marle HJ, Ossewaarde L, Henckens MJ, Qin S, van KesterenMT, et al. Stress-related noradrenergic activity prompts large-scale neuralnetwork reconfiguration. Science 2011;334:1151–3.

[90] Desimone R, Duncan J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. AnnRev Neurosci 1995;18:193–222.

[91] Beck DM, Kastner S. Top-down and bottom-up mechanisms in biasing com-petition in the human brain. Vis Res 2009;49:1154–65.

[92] Luck SJ, Chelazzi L, Hillyard SA, Desimone R. Neural mechanisms of spa-tial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. JNeurophys 1997;77:24–42.

[93] Recanzone GH, Wurtz RH. Effects of attention on MT and MST neuronal activ-ity during pursuit initiation. J Neurophys 2000;83:777–90.

[94] Reynolds JH, Chelazzi L, Desimone R. Competitive mechanisms subserveattention in macaque areas V2 and V4. J Neurosci 1999;19:1736–53.

[95] Kastner S, De Weerd P, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG. Mechanisms of directedattention in the human extrastriate cortex as revealed by functional MRI.Science 1998;282:108–11.

[96] Stanisor L, van der Togt C, Pennartz CMA, Roelfsema PR. A unified selectionsignal for attention and reward in primary visual cortex. Proc Biol Sci 2013.

[97] Serences JT, Saproo S. Population response profiles in early visual cortex arebiased in favor of more valuable stimuli. J Neurophys 2010;104:76–87.

[98] Summerfield C, Egner T, Greene M, Koechlin E, Mangels J, Hirsch J. Pre-dictive codes for forthcoming perception in the frontal cortex. Science2006;314:1311–4.

[99] Gamond L, George N, Lemarechal JD, Hugueville L, Adam C, Tallon-BaudryC. Early influence of prior experience on face perception. Neuroimage2011;54:1415–26.

[100] Wieser MJ, McTeague LM, Keil A. Competition effects of threatening faces insocial anxiety. Emotion 2012;12:1050–60.

[101] Miskovic V, Keil A. Perceiving threat in the face of safety: excitation and inhi-bition of conditioned fear in human visual cortex. J Neurosci 2013;33:72–8.

[102] Waterhouse BD, Azizi SA, Burne RA, Woodward DJ. Modulation of rat corticalarea 17 neuronal responses to moving visual stimuli during norepinephrineand serotonin microiontophoresis. Brain Res 1990;514:276–92.

[103] Gallagher M, Holland PC. The amygdala complex: multiple roles in associativelearning and attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1994;91:11771–6.

[104] Roozendaal B, McEwen BS, Chattarji S. Stress, memory and the amygdala. NatRev Neurosci 2009.

[105] Aston-Jones G, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of locuscoeruleus–norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance.Ann Rev Neurosci 2005;28:403–50.

[106] Jones BE, Moore RY. Ascending projections of the locus coeruleus in the rat.II. Autoradiographic study. Brain Res 1977;127:25–53.

[107] Aston-Jones G, Bloom FE. Norepinephrine-containing locus coeruleus neu-rons in behaving rats exhibit pronounced responses to non-noxiousenvironmental stimuli. J Neurosci 1981;1:887–900.

t: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of0.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.018

[108] Grant SJ, Aston-Jones G, Redmond Jr DE. Responses of primate locuscoeruleus neurons to simple and complex sensory stimuli. Brain Res Bull1988;21:401–10.

[109] Herve-Minvielle A, Sara SJ. Rapid habituation of auditory responses of locuscoeruleus cells in anaesthetized and awake rats. Neuroreport 1995;6:1363–8.

1245

1246

1247

1248

1249

Page 13: Tuning to the significant: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of visual perception and memory

ING Model

B

l Brain

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

1250

1251

1252

1253

1254

1255

1256

1257

1258

1259

1260

1261

1262

1263

1264

1265

1266

1267

1268

1269

1270

1271

1272

1273

1274

1275

1276

1277

1278

1279

1280

1281

1282

1283

1284

1285

1286

1287

1288

1289

1290

1291

1292

1293

1294

1295

1296

1297

1298

1299

1300

1301

1302

1303

1304

1305

1306

1307

1308

1309

1310

1311

1312

1313

1314

1315

1316

1317

1318

1319

1320

1321

1322

1323

1324

1325

1326

1327

1328

1329

1330

1331

1332

1333

1334

1335

1336

1337

1338

1339

1340

1341

1342

1343

1344

1345

1346

1347

1348

1349

1350

1351

1352

1353

1354

1355

1356

1357

1358

1359

1360

1361

1362

1363

1364

1365

1366

1367

1368

1369

1370

1371

1372

1373

1374

1375

1376

1377

1378

1379

1380

1381

1382

1383

1384

1385

1386

1387

1388

1389

1390

1391

1392

1393

1394

1395

1396

1397

ARTICLEBR 8596 1–13

J. Markovic et al. / Behavioura

110] Rasmussen K, Jacobs BL. Single unit activity of locus coeruleus neurons inthe freely moving cat. II. Conditioning and pharmacologic studies. Brain Res1986;371:335–44.

111] Manunta Y, Edeline JM. Noradrenergic induction of selective plasticity in thefrequency tuning of auditory cortex neurons. J Neurophys 2004;92:1445–63.

112] Waterhouse BD, Woodward DJ. Interaction of norepinephrine with cerebro-cortical activity evoked by stimulation of somatosensory afferent pathwaysin the rat. Exp Neurol 1980;67:11–34.

113] Woodward DJ, Moises HC, Waterhouse BD, Yeh HH, Cheun JE. Modu-latory actions of norepinephrine on neural circuits. Adv Exp Med Biol1991;287:193–208.

114] Segal M, Bloom FE. The action of norepinephrine in the rat hippocampus.IV. The effects of locus coeruleus stimulation on evoked hippocampal unitactivity. Brain Res 1976;107:513–25.

115] Foote SL, Freedman R, Oliver AP. Effects of putative neurotransmitters onneuronal activity in monkey auditory cortex. Brain Res 1975;86:229–42.

116] Lecas JC. Locus coeruleus activation shortens synaptic drive while decreas-ing spike latency and jitter in sensorimotor cortex. Implications for neuronalintegration. Eur J Neurosci 2004;19:2519–30.

117] Bouret S, Sara SJ. Locus coeruleus activation modulates firing rate and tempo-ral organization of odour-induced single-cell responses in rat piriform cortex.Eur J Neurosci 2002;16:2371–82.

118] Tiesinga P, Fellous JM, Sejnowski TJ. Regulation of spike timing in visual cor-tical circuits. Nat Rev Neurosci 2008;9:97–107.

119] Lestienne R. Spike timing, synchronization and information processing on thesensory side of the central nervous system. Prog Neurobiol 2001;65:545–91.

120] Dan Y, Poo MM. Spike timing-dependent plasticity: from synapse to percep-tion. Physiol Rev 2006;86:1033–48.

121] Cahill L, Alkire MT. Epinephrine enhancement of human memory con-solidation: interaction with arousal at encoding. Neurobiol Learn Mem2003;79:194–8.

122] Roozendaal B, Castello NA, Vedana G, Barsegyan A, McGaugh JL. Noradrener-gic activation of the basolateral amygdala modulates consolidation of objectrecognition memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2008;90:576–9.

123] McGaugh JL, Introini-Collison IB, Nagahara AH. Memory-enhancing effects ofposttraining naloxone: involvement of beta-noradrenergic influences in theamygdaloid complex. Brain Res 1988;446:37–49.

124] McGaugh JL, Cahill L, Roozendaal B. Involvement of the amygdala in mem-ory storage: interaction with other brain systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA1996;93:13508–14.

125] Roozendaal B, McGaugh JL. Amygdaloid nuclei lesions differentially affectglucocorticoid-induced memory enhancement in an inhibitory avoidancetask. Neurobiol Learn Mem 1996;65:1–8.

126] Parent MB, McGaugh JL. Posttraining infusion of lidocaine into the amygdalabasolateral complex impairs retention of inhibitory avoidance training. BrainRes 1994;661:97–103.

127] Ferry B, Roozendaal B, McGaugh JL. Basolateral amygdala noradrenergic influ-ences on memory storage are mediated by an interaction between beta- andalpha1-adrenoceptors. J Neurosci 1999;19:5119–23.

128] Fox E, Russo R, Dutton K. Attentional bias for threat: evidence for delayeddisengagement from emotional faces. Cogn Emot 2002;16:355–79.

129] Armony JL, Dolan RJ. Modulation of spatial attention by fear-conditionedstimuli: an event-related fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 2002;40:817–26.

130] Bishop SJ, Jenkins R, Lawrence AD. Neural processing of fearful faces:effects of anxiety are gated by perceptual capacity limitations. Cereb Cortex2007;17:1595–603.

131] Holmes A, Nielsen MK, Green S. Effects of anxiety on the processing offearful and happy faces: an event-related potential study. Biol Psychol2008;77:159–73.

132] McClure EB, Monk CS, Nelson EE, Parrish JM, Adler A, Blair RJ, et al. Abnormalattention modulation of fear circuit function in pediatric generalized anxietydisorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2007;64:97–106.

133] Bar-Haim Y, Lamy D, Pergamin L, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IMH.Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: ameta-analytic study. Psychol Bull 2007;133:1–24.

134] Miskovic V, Schmidt LA. Social fearfulness in the human brain. Neurosci Biobe-hav Rev 2012;36:459–78.

135] Calder AJ, Ewbank M, Passamonti L. Personality influences the neuralresponses to viewing facial expressions of emotion. Phil Trans Roy Soc B2011;366:1684–701.

136] Derryberry D, Reed MA. Temperament and attention: orienting toward andaway from positive and negative signals. J Pers Soc Psychol 1994;66:1128–39.

Please cite this article in press as: Markovic J, et al. Tuning to the significanvisual perception and memory. Behav Brain Res (2013), http://dx.doi.org/1

137] Beck JG, Freeman JB, Shipherd JC, Hamblen JL, Lackner JM. Specificity ofstroop interference in patients with pain and PTSD. J Abnorm Psychol2001;110:536–43.

138] Buckley TC, Blanchard EB, Neill WT. Information processing and PTSD: areview of the empirical literature. Clin Psychol Rev 2000;20:1041–65.

PRESS Research xxx (2013) xxx– xxx 13

[139] Vythilingam M, Blair KS, McCaffrey D, Scaramozza M, Jones M, Nakic M, et al.Biased emotional attention in post-traumatic stress disorder: a help as wellas a hindrance? Psychol Med 2007;37:1445–55.

[140] Gilboa A, Shalev AY, Laor L, Lester H, Louzoun Y, Chisin R, et al. Functionalconnectivity of the prefrontal cortex and the amygdala in posttraumatic stressdisorder. Biol Psychiatry 2004;55:263–72.

[141] Hendler T, Rotshtein P, Yeshurun Y, Weizmann T, Kahn I, Ben-Bashat D, et al.Sensing the invisible: differential sensitivity of visual cortex and amygdala totraumatic context. Neuroimage 2003;19:587–600.

[142] Perez-Edgar K, Reeb-Sutherland BC, McDermott JM, White LK, HendersonHA, Degnan KA, et al. Attention biases to threat link behavioral inhibition tosocial withdrawal over time in very young children. J Abnorm Child Psychol2011;39:885–95.

[143] Hariri AR, Weinberger DR. Functional neuroimaging of genetic variation inserotonergic neurotransmission. Genes Brain Behav 2003;2:341–9.

[144] Munafo MR, Brown SM, Hariri AR. Serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR) genotypeand amygdala activation: a meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry 2008;63:852–7.

[145] Fox NA, Hane AA, Pine DS. Plasticity for affective neurocircuitry: how theenvironment affects gene expression. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2007;16:1–5.

[146] Fox NA, Nichols KE, Henderson HA, Rubin K, Schmidt L, Hamer D, et al. Evi-dence for a gene-environment interaction in predicting behavioral inhibitionin middle childhood. Psychol Sci 2005;16:921–6.

[147] Lonigan CJ, Vasey MW. Negative affectivity, effortful control, and attention tothreat-relevant stimuli. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2008.

[148] Hakamata Y, Lissek S, Bar-Haim Y, Britton JC, Fox NA, Leibenluft E, et al. Atten-tion bias modification treatment: a meta-analysis toward the establishmentof novel treatment for anxiety. Biol Psychiatry 2010;68:982–90.

[149] Eldar S, Apter A, Lotan D, Edgar KP, Naim R, Fox NA, et al. Attention bias mod-ification treatment for pediatric anxiety disorders: a randomized controlledtrial. Am J Psychiatry 2012;169:213–20.

[150] Hallion LS, Ruscio AM. A meta-analysis of the effect of cognitive bias modifi-cation on anxiety and depression. Psychol Bull 2011;137:940–58.

[151] Small KM, Brown KM, Forbes SL, Liggett SB. Polymorphic deletion of threeintracellular acidic residues of the alpha 2B-adrenergic receptor decreases Gprotein-coupled receptor kinase-mediated phosphorylation and desensitiza-tion. J Biol Chem 2001;276:4917–22.

[152] de Quervain DJ, Kolassa IT, Ertl V, Onyut PL, Neuner F, Elbert T, et al. A dele-tion variant of the alpha2b-adrenoceptor is related to emotional memory inEuropeans and Africans. Nat Neurosci 2007;10:1137–9.

[153] Rasch B, Spalek K, Buholzer S, Luechinger R, Boesiger P, Papassotiropoulos A,et al. A genetic variation of the noradernergic system is related to differentialamygdala activation during encoding of emotional memories. Proc Natl AcadSci USA 2009.

[154] De Martino B, Strange BA, Dolan RJ. Noradrenergic neuromodulation ofhuman attention for emotional and neutral stimuli. Psychopharmacology2008;197:127–36.

[155] Todd RM, Muller DJ, Lee DH, Robertson A, Eaton T, Freeman N, et al. Genes foremotion-enhanced remembering are linked to enhanced perceiving. PsycholSci 2013;24:2244–53.

[156] Devilbiss DM, Waterhouse BD. Phasic and tonic patterns of locus coeruleusoutput differentially modulate sensory network function in the awake rat. JNeurophysiol 2011;105:69–87.

[157] Nieuwenhuis S, Gilzenrat MS, Holmes BD, Cohen JD. The role of the locuscoeruleus in mediating the attentional blink: a neurocomputational theory. JExp Psychol Gen 2005;134:291–307.

[158] Cousijn H, Rijpkema M, Qin S, van Marle HJ, Franke B, Hermans EJ, et al. Acutestress modulates genotype effects on amygdala processing in humans. ProcNatl Acad Sci USA 2010;107:9867–72.

[159] Cunningham WA, Van Bavel JJ, Johnsen IR. Affective flexibility: evaluativeprocessing goals shape amygdala activity. Psychol Sci 2008;19:152–60.

[160] Todd RM, Muller DJ, Palombo DJ, Robertson A, Eaton T, Freeman N, et al.Deletion variant in the ADRA2B gene increases coupling between emotionalresponses at encoding and later retrieval of emotional memories. NeurobiolLearn Mem 2013.

[161] Vuilleumier P, Pourtois G. Distributed and interactive brain mechanismsduring emotion face perception: evidence from functional neuroimaging.Neuropsychologia 2007;45:174–94.

[162] Vuilleumier P, Schwartz S, Beware. be aware: capture of spatial attention byfear-related stimuli in neglect. Neuroreport 2001;12:1119–22.

[163] Anderson AK, Christoff K, Panitz D, De Rosa E, Gabrieli JD. Neural correlates ofthe automatic processing of threat facial signals. J Neurosci 2003;23:5627–33.

[164] Mather M, Sutherland MR. Arousal-biased competition in perception and

t: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of0.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.018

memory. Perspect Psychol Sci 2011;6:114–33.[165] Grossberg S, Seidman D. Neural dynamics of autistic behaviors: cognitive,

emotional, and timing substrates. Psychol Rev 2006;113:483–525.[166] Summerfield C, Egner T. Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition.

Trends Cogn Sci 2009;13:403–9.

1398

1399

1400

1401

1402