Transportation Research Part A - University College Dublin · 2020. 12. 3. · Transportation...

16
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Transportation Research Part A journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/tra Why is active travel more satisfying than motorized travel? Evidence from Dublin Leonhard K. Lades a,b,c, , Andrew Kelly c , Luke Kelleher a a UCD Environmental Policy, University College Dublin, Beleld, Dublin 4, Ireland b UCD Geary Institute, University College Dublin, Beleld, Dublin 4, Ireland c EnvEcon, 11 Priory Oce Park, Blackrock, Co., Dublin, Ireland ARTICLE INFO Keywords: Travel satisfaction Subjective well-being Environmental transport policy ABSTRACT Some trips are better than others, and more and more studies nd that active travel (walking and cycling) is more satisfying than motorized forms of travel (using the car or public transport). Why is this the case? Using data on travel satisfaction from 4134 commutes to a large University campus in Dublin, Ireland, this paper replicates the dierences in travel satisfaction between active and motorized travel. We attribute these dierences in large part to the duration of the trip. Subjective trip characteristics, such as safety and convenience, also play important roles. The trip duration explains rush-hour eects as well as why people starting from less auent and more dicult-to-reach places are less satised with their trips. Longer-term policy options sug- gested by these results include infrastructure developments and spatial development strategies. A shorter-term initiative would be to delay university schedules in the morning to avoid low travel satisfaction during the slow rush-hour period and simultaneously ease pressure on the transport network at peak times. 1. Introduction People do not always make choices that are in their own best interests. A large and growing literature in behavioral economics has identied and explored this discrepancy between decision utility (which informs choice) and experienced utility (which is the basis for welfare) (Benjamin et al., 2012; Berridge and Aldridge, 2008; Comerford, 2011; De Vos et al., 2016; Kahneman et al., 1997; Read, 2007). For example, some people live far away from their workplaces and yet travel to work by car and public transport despite happiness research indicating that long commutes in motorized modes are one of the least satisfying daily activities (Kahneman et al., 2004; Lancée et al., 2017; Stone and Schneider, 2016). Furthermore, these low levels of travel satisfaction can spill over to low levels of life satisfaction (De Vos, 2019a; Friman et al., 2017; Olsson et al., 2013; Stutzer and Frey, 2008). 1 Such ndings suggest that choices (e.g. about transportation modes) alone oer only a limited perspective to fully understand the welfare consequences of our behavior (Hausman, 2012; Infante et al., 2016; Sugden, 2018). Hence, policy-makers who are interested in increasing peoples welfare when traveling need additional information to better inform transport policy design. 2 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tra.2020.04.007 Received 7 August 2019; Received in revised form 4 February 2020; Accepted 8 April 2020 Corresponding author at: UCD Environmental Policy, University College Dublin, Beleld, Dublin 4, Ireland. E-mail address: [email protected] (L.K. Lades). 1 Of course, in many cases people are not able to decide whether to live close or far away from their workplaces as this is determined by, for example, budget constraints. Travel choices are often inuenced by other, longer-term choices regarding, for example, lifestyle and residential location. 2 The terms welfareand well-being are often used interchangeably in the literature on welfare economics. Transportation Research Part A 136 (2020) 318–333 Available online 06 May 2020 0965-8564/ © 2020 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. T

Transcript of Transportation Research Part A - University College Dublin · 2020. 12. 3. · Transportation...

  • Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

    Transportation Research Part A

    journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/tra

    Why is active travel more satisfying than motorized travel?Evidence from Dublin

    Leonhard K. Ladesa,b,c,⁎, Andrew Kellyc, Luke Kellehera

    aUCD Environmental Policy, University College Dublin, Belfield, Dublin 4, IrelandbUCD Geary Institute, University College Dublin, Belfield, Dublin 4, Irelandc EnvEcon, 11 Priory Office Park, Blackrock, Co., Dublin, Ireland

    A R T I C L E I N F O

    Keywords:Travel satisfactionSubjective well-beingEnvironmental transport policy

    A B S T R A C T

    Some trips are better than others, and more and more studies find that active travel (walking andcycling) is more satisfying than motorized forms of travel (using the car or public transport). Whyis this the case? Using data on travel satisfaction from 4134 commutes to a large Universitycampus in Dublin, Ireland, this paper replicates the differences in travel satisfaction betweenactive and motorized travel. We attribute these differences in large part to the duration of thetrip. Subjective trip characteristics, such as safety and convenience, also play important roles.The trip duration explains rush-hour effects as well as why people starting from less affluent andmore difficult-to-reach places are less satisfied with their trips. Longer-term policy options sug-gested by these results include infrastructure developments and spatial development strategies. Ashorter-term initiative would be to delay university schedules in the morning to avoid low travelsatisfaction during the slow rush-hour period and simultaneously ease pressure on the transportnetwork at peak times.

    1. Introduction

    People do not always make choices that are in their own best interests. A large and growing literature in behavioral economics hasidentified and explored this discrepancy between decision utility (which informs choice) and experienced utility (which is the basisfor welfare) (Benjamin et al., 2012; Berridge and Aldridge, 2008; Comerford, 2011; De Vos et al., 2016; Kahneman et al., 1997; Read,2007). For example, some people live far away from their workplaces and yet travel to work by car and public transport despitehappiness research indicating that long commutes in motorized modes are one of the least satisfying daily activities (Kahneman et al.,2004; Lancée et al., 2017; Stone and Schneider, 2016). Furthermore, these low levels of travel satisfaction can spill over to low levelsof life satisfaction (De Vos, 2019a; Friman et al., 2017; Olsson et al., 2013; Stutzer and Frey, 2008).1 Such findings suggest thatchoices (e.g. about transportation modes) alone offer only a limited perspective to fully understand the welfare consequences of ourbehavior (Hausman, 2012; Infante et al., 2016; Sugden, 2018). Hence, policy-makers who are interested in increasing people’swelfare when traveling need additional information to better inform transport policy design.2

    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tra.2020.04.007Received 7 August 2019; Received in revised form 4 February 2020; Accepted 8 April 2020

    ⁎ Corresponding author at: UCD Environmental Policy, University College Dublin, Belfield, Dublin 4, Ireland.E-mail address: [email protected] (L.K. Lades).

    1 Of course, in many cases people are not able to decide whether to live close or far away from their workplaces as this is determined by, forexample, budget constraints. Travel choices are often influenced by other, longer-term choices regarding, for example, lifestyle and residentiallocation.

    2 The terms “welfare” and well-being are often used interchangeably in the literature on welfare economics.

    Transportation Research Part A 136 (2020) 318–333

    Available online 06 May 20200965-8564/ © 2020 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

    T

    http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/09658564https://www.elsevier.com/locate/trahttps://doi.org/10.1016/j.tra.2020.04.007https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tra.2020.04.007mailto:[email protected]://doi.org/10.1016/j.tra.2020.04.007http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1016/j.tra.2020.04.007&domain=pdf

  • Over the last decade, a growing group of scholars, including transportation researchers, have argued that subjective well-beingcan be used as a welfare measure to inform policy (Dolan and White, 2007; Friman et al., 2018; Layard, 2006). Subjective well-beingis often defined as an experienced momentary feeling, and diary studies or other naturalistic monitoring tools are used to measure it(Kahneman et al., 2004; Shiffman et al., 2008). Arguably the most common finding from the literature on experienced subjectivewell-being in the transport context is that people who travel actively (i.e., who walk or cycle) are more satisfied with their travel thanpeople who drive and/or use public transport (De Vos, 2018, 2019b; De Vos et al., 2013; Ettema et al., 2011; Friman et al., 2017;Lancée et al., 2017; Smith, 2017; Westman et al., 2017). However, it is not yet clear why this is the case. For example, De Vos (2019b)reviews the literature on differences in travel satisfaction across travel modes and notes that “studies on travel satisfaction barely giveexplanations for these differences; and when they do, these explanations are not empirically founded and therefore not entirelyconvincing” (p. 15).

    This paper presents results and analysis from a commuting survey at University College Dublin, Ireland that asked participants toindicate how satisfied they were with their most recent trip to the campus. We initially assess whether active travel (walking andcycling) is ranked as more satisfying than motorized travel (driving and public transport) amongst the 4134 participants whose lastcommute was to the main campus. Thereafter we aim to explain the differences in travel satisfaction across travel modes by em-pirically investigating the effects of starting time of travel, duration of travel, distance of travel, subjectively assessed characteristicsof the trip, social interaction during travel, and demographic variables. We also test whether living close to bus stops, using a real-time information app about bus departures, and parking difficulties on campus are associated with travel satisfaction. Finally, we testfor associations between travel satisfaction and geospatial information on the location from which participants started their trip,focusing on economic deprivation and the accessibility to transport at that location.

    One key finding of the paper is that differences in travel satisfaction across travel modes can be attributed in large part to theduration of the trip (e.g., trips with the bus take longer than when people walk or cycle). This suggests a need to control for tripduration when examining differences in travel satisfaction across travel modes in future studies. We also find that the duration of thetrip depends on the starting time of the trip (a rush-hour effect) and use these insights to formulate an ambitious policy proposal:delaying the starting schedule of universities would deliver higher trip satisfaction ratings, whilst simultaneously easing pressure ontransport infrastructure and services during the morning peak commute.

    The remainder of the paper is structured as follows: Section 2 reviews the literature on the determinants of travel satisfaction;Section 3 presents the data and methodology used for the study; Section 4 presents the results; Section 5 discusses contributions to theliterature, policy implications, and avenues for future research. The last section concludes.

    2. Literature review

    2.1. Measuring travel satisfaction

    When measuring the subjective quality of travel, researchers do not elicit general travel satisfaction ratings.3 Rather, they askstudy participants about feelings during specific trips. This focus on specific trips reduces biases in the response patterns and allows tocorrelate satisfaction ratings with situational context variables such as travel mode, time of the day, and other situational trip-specificvariables (Bryson and MacKerron, 2017; Dolan et al., 2008; Friman et al., 2018).

    There are a number of scales to measure the quality of specific trips (e.g., see Mokhtarian and Pendyala, 2018). The most commonis the Satisfaction with Travel Scale (STS) introduced by Ettema et al. (2011).4 The STS consists of nine question items that allowanswers on a scale from −3 to 3 with a higher score implying higher satisfaction (De Vos et al., 2015; Friman et al., 2013). The STSmeasures three sub-components of travel satisfaction with three questions each. The first two sub-components deal with affect anddifferentiate between valence and activation as suggested by the Swedish Core Affect Scale (SCAS) (Västfjäll et al., 2002). Theymeasure feelings from negative activation to positive deactivation (“stressed to calm”, “worried to confident”, and “hurried to relaxed”)and from negative deactivation to positive activation (“bored to enthusiastic”, “tired to alert”, and “fed up to engaged”). The third sub-component is cognitive evaluation, which is measured by the following three items: “the trip was the worst/best I can think of”; “the tripwas very low/high standard”; and “the trip did not work out/worked out well”.

    Most studies differentiate between all three sub-components of the STS (e.g., De Vos, 2018; Ettema et al., 2012; Ettema et al.,2011; et al., 2017). However, De Vos et al. (2015) suggest using a two-factor interpretation and differentiate only between affectivefeelings and cognitive evaluation (De Vos, 2019a; De Vos et al., 2019). Friman et al. (2013) show that the three dimensions of the STScan also be used as a global measure of travel satisfaction as they form an overall construct and this approach is adopted by severalstudies (Friman et al., 2017; Taniguchi et al. 2014; Ye and Titheridge, 2017).

    2.2. Determinants of travel satisfaction

    The literature on the determinants of travel satisfaction is growing rapidly. Detailed reviews of this literature are providedelsewhere (De Vos, 2019b; De Vos and Witlox, 2017; Friman et al., 2018; Gao et al., 2018), and we review only selected findings of

    3 Exceptions are studies that ask survey respondents to indicate how satisfied they are with their commutes in general, i.e. not specific to oneparticular commute (e.g., Abou Zeid, 2009; Abou-Zeid and Ben-Akiva, 2011; Bergstad et al., 2011).

    4 Other studies rely on single-item measures of travel satisfaction (e.g., Gao et al., 2017, 2018).

    L.K. Lades, et al. Transportation Research Part A 136 (2020) 318–333

    319

  • relevance to the present study. We differentiate between trip-specific and individual-specific determinants of travel satisfaction.In terms of trip-specific determinants of travel-satisfaction, several studies find that people who travel actively (i.e., who walk or

    cycle) are more satisfied with their travel than people who drive and use public transport (De Vos, 2018; De Vos et al., 2013; Ettemaet al., 2011; Friman et al., 2017; Lancée et al., 2017; Singleton, 2019; Smith, 2017; St-Louis et al., 2014; Westman et al., 2017; Ye andTitheridge, 2017; Zhu and Fan, 2018). It is not entirely clear what explains these mode-effects. Highlighting this knowledge gap, DeVos (2019b) argues that the effect of travel mode on travel satisfaction is in part explained by whether travelers have positive ornegative attitudes towards their chosen travel mode.

    Amongst other determinants of travel satisfaction, trip duration and trip distance are often negatively related to travel satisfaction(De Vos et al., 2016; Handy and Thigpen, 2019; Higgins et al., 2018; Higgins et al., 2018; Morris and Guerra, 2015; St-Louis et al.,2014; Stone and Schneider, 2016; Westman et al., 2017; Zhang et al., 2016). When trips are unusual, longer trips may also representan escape from the daily routine (Mokhtarian et al., 2015) and thus influence travel satisfaction positively. However, in these casespeople may confound their liking for the uncommon activity that starts after the trip with the liking for the trip itself (De Vos et al.,2016).

    Subjective evaluations of trip attributes that are correlated with travel satisfaction include reliability (Mahmoud and Hine, 2016),punctuality, speed, and service frequency (Mouwen, 2015; de Oña et al., 2016), flexibility (Mao et al., 2016), comfort (Mahmoud andHine, 2016; Susilo and Cats, 2014), convenience (Mahmoud and Hine, 2016; de Oña et al., 2016), congestion levels and over-crowding (Higgins et al., 2018; Morris and Hirsch, 2016; Smith, 2017), as well as cleanliness and safety (Susilo and Cats, 2014).Travelling alone (in contrast to travelling in company) has a negative effect on travel satisfaction in some studies (De Vos, 2019a;Lancée et al., 2017; Mokhtarian et al., 2015; Zhu and Fan, 2018).

    There is limited research on the associations between geospatial determinants and travel satisfaction. First findings suggest thatsuburban respondents report higher levels of travel satisfaction than urban respondents, a relationship strongest for car and publictransport users (De Vos et al., 2016). Mokhtarian et al. (2015) find that urban residents consider trips less tiring, but also less pleasantcompared to non-urban residents. Other studies do not find direct associations between the residential location and travel satisfaction(Mokhtarian and Pendyala, 2018; Ye and Titheridge, 2017). In a sample of walkers, Manaugh and El-Geneidy (2013) do not findassociations between travel satisfaction, distance, and elevation change. However, they do find effects in sub-group analyses dividingwalkers by their motivations to walk.

    Research has also shown that people living in lower-income areas tend to have relatively fewer travel options, are more likely toexperience transport poverty which can impact on their ability to undertake activities and consequently their well-being, and facemore dangerous and unhealthy travel conditions (Currie et al., 2009; Lucas et al., 2016). More generally, there is considerableresearch on the connections between transport and social disadvantage (Lucas, 2012; Lucas et al., 2018; Schwanen et al., 2015).However, there are no studies examining the links between the affluence of the area from where the trips start, social disadvantage,and travel satisfaction.

    In terms of individual-specific factors, several studies show that older people report higher travel satisfaction (Cao and Ettema,2014; De Vos et al., 2016; Ettema et al., 2013; Friman et al., 2017; St-Louis et al., 2014; Susilo and Cats, 2014; Ye and Titheridge,2017). Some studies find that males are more satisfied than females (Handy and Thigpen, 2019; Higgins et al., 2018), and in somestudies this effect is present only for specific modes of transport (mainly public transport) (St-Louis et al., 2014). There are alsostudies that do not find significant associations between gender and travel satisfaction (Carrel et al., 2016). A number of studies showthat positive attitudes towards travelling predict higher satisfaction with travel, especially when the attitudes are mode-specific (Caoand Ettema, 2014; De Vos, 2018; Ye and Titheridge, 2017). Also moods such as feeling happy, relaxed and angry predict travelsatisfaction (Gao et al., 2017). Some studies find that people with pro-environmental attitudes are more satisfied with active modes oftravel (Manaugh and El-Geneidy, 2013) and that personality traits, such as being critical and being easy-going, predict travel sa-tisfaction (Gao et al., 2017).

    The literature reviewed above shows that there are various potential pathways that may explain differences in travel satisfactionacross travel modes. Hence, the results of one study might not be replicable in other contexts. It is also clear that the empiricalstrategy, and in particular the choice of control variables, matters when analyzing and quantifying the differences of travel sa-tisfaction across travel modes and different studies and contexts. Finally, the literature suggests that the role of geospatial de-terminants on travel satisfaction is somewhat under-researched thus far.

    3. Data and methodology

    3.1. Setting

    The data used in this research is taken from a large-scale travel behavior survey carried out at University College Dublin (UCD) inIreland by the University’s Estates Service Team who have run a Commuting Survey annually since 2011.5 The main aim of thesecommuter surveys is to inform campus development by identifying commuting requirements. The 2018 survey was the first to includequestions on travel satisfaction and other factors of the most recent commute to campus.

    The university has several campuses in Dublin but over 90% of staff members and students work or study at the largest campusBelfield (according to the 2018 UCD commuting survey). Accordingly, this paper focuses on commutes to this campus. Belfield is

    5 University College Dublin (www.ucd.ie) is Ireland’s largest university with a staff and student population in excess of 30,000.

    L.K. Lades, et al. Transportation Research Part A 136 (2020) 318–333

    320

    http://www.ucd.ie

  • located approximately 4 km south of the city center in one of the city’s more affluent areas. It is a stand-alone campus with 11entrances. Permit parking spaces can be used (if available) for an annual fee and hourly parking spaces are available as well. Publictransport is available for students at a discount. The campus is serviced by a number of bus routes but does not have a high-capacityrail transport link nor dedicated train or tram stops. The closest tram and train stations are roughly a 30-minute walk from thecampus.

    UCD staff members and students often need to commute long distances.6 Reasons for this include a limited supply of living spacenear the campus, high rents in surrounding areas and the city center, and a propensity for lower density development in the past inDublin. Commuters who use the car tend to arrive early on campus in order to find a free parking spot. Moreover, levels of congestionin the city are high, and a report suggests that only in Bogota and Rome do commuters lose more time in congestion.7 Commuting tothe campus by bike is sometimes perceived as unsafe and inconvenient because bike lanes are shared with buses for long stretches ofroad or end somewhat abruptly. Commuting to the campus on foot is possible for those living close to the campus as footpaths areprovided in all built-up areas. In terms of public transport, the bus network is described as discontinuous and the attractiveness of busjourneys is reduced by delays caused by a small number of choke points across the road network (NTA, 2016).

    3.2. Sample

    On November 5th and November 14th, 2018, the University’s Estates Service distributed an invitation to participate in the AnnualCommuting Survey via email to all students, university staff members, and faculty. As an incentive, participants were offered theopportunity to enter a lottery for a €200 voucher. During the time that the survey was live, 6253 responses were recorded (4608students and 1645 staff members).

    For this study, we focus only on those participants who worked or studied in the biggest campus (Belfield), who were aged 18 orolder, for whom we have full information on travel mode, travel duration, companion, subjective experience, and satisfaction of themost recent commute to campus as well as key demographics (student/staff status, age, gender, the number of days the participantusually commutes to the campus). We retained only participants who made their most recent commute by bus, train, car, bicycle oron foot (thus dropping 189 observations of passengers in the car and 72 observations who indicate having used “Other” travelmodes). This left us with a final sample size of 4134. Table 1 presents the details of this sample.

    3.3. Descriptive statistics

    We focus on the survey questions that dealt with the participants’most recent commute to the campus. The survey measured theirtravel satisfaction using the Satisfaction with Travel Scale as described in Section 2.1 and presented in full in the SupplementaryInformation. The Satisfaction with Travel Scale was included in the survey as it is the most elaborated measure for travel satisfactionand has become the standard domain-specific measure in the domain of travel (De Vos, 2019b). For this paper’s analyses, we use theglobal outcome measure for travel satisfaction in order to keep the already detailed analyses as straightforward as possible. Using thetwo or three sub-components of the Satisfaction with Travel Scale would render the presentation of our findings very complicated. Inour data, travel satisfaction has a mean of 0.165 (SD = 1.284; Skewness = −0.078; Min = −3; Max = 3; alpha = 0.9017).

    The survey asked participants to indicate the type of travel mode they used for their trip. The primary travel mode was the bus in38.83% of the trips, followed by the car (24.43%), the bicycle (18.36%), walking (12.58%), and the train (6.10%). Fig. 1, Panel Ashows a density plot of the distributions of travel satisfaction for all modes combined as well as separated by travel mode. Already thisfigure indicates that people are more satisfied when they walk or cycle than when they take the car, and in particular, than when theyuse public transport.

    Participants indicated the time of the day when they started their most recent commute to the campus. Most trips started between7 am and 9 am. Fig. 1, Panel B presents density plots based on the start of the commute in 15-minute intervals by travel mode. Itshows that active commutes began later than motorized commutes. In terms of the duration of the trip, 662 (16.01%) trips took lessthan 15 min, 945 (22.86%) took between 15 min and 30 min, 692 (16.74%) took between 30 min and 45 min, 634 (15.34%) tookbetween 45 min and an hour, and the remaining 1201 (29.05%) trips took more than an hour. Trip duration is measured from door todoor, i.e. it includes, for example, the ca. 30-min walk when people take the train. The duration of the trip differed by travel mode asillustrated in Fig. 2, which shows, for example, that active trips were shorter than other trips. Trips for which participants used thetrain as the primary mode took the longest time.

    Participants were also asked to describe their commute on a scale from “0 (Not at all)” over 1–5 to “6 (Very much)” for thefollowing five attributes - “Safe”, “Clean”, “Congested/Overcrowded”, “Strenuous”, and “Convenient”. Table 3 presents the mean valuesof these attributes for all trips and also separated by travel mode. It shows, for example, that biking was perceived as the least safe andmost convenient travel mode.

    In terms of the company during the trip, 2222 (53.75%) of the trips were alone, on 1476 (35.70%) trips strangers were present,

    6 For example, only about 24% of our sample indicate having a typical commute of less than 4 km and more than 18% have a typical commute ofover 25 km.

    7 This is based on an Inrix Report (2018) that compares congestion in over 200 cities across the world. The same report also shows that Dublin hasthe slowest downtown peak speed across the 200 cities analyzed with drivers travelling at an average of less than 10 km/ph at peak times in the citycenter.

    L.K. Lades, et al. Transportation Research Part A 136 (2020) 318–333

    321

  • and on 436 (10.55%) trips people known to the participant were around. Almost half (48.69%) of the participants indicated that theyregularly use a mobile phone app that provides real-time information about bus departures, and 41.94% of the participants indicatedthat they lived less than 500 m away from a bus stop. Of the 1381 participants who indicated that they drove regularly to the campus,

    Table 1Individual-specific variables of the 4134 participants.

    Variables Observations (percent)

    GenderMale 1619 (39.16%)Female 2515 (60.84%)Age categories18–24 2400 (58.06%)25–34 683 (16.52%)35–44 490 (11.85%)45–54 359 (8.68%)Over 55 186 (4.5%)Rather not to say 16 (0.39%)

    StatusStudent 3046 (73.68%)Staff 1088 (26.32%)

    Fig. 1. Panel A: Distribution of travel satisfaction based on the average of all nine items by travel mode. Panel B: Start of commute in 15 min-intervals by travel mode.

    Fig. 2. Distribution of trip duration for each transport mode.

    Table 2Travel satisfaction and subjective trip characteristics by travel mode.

    Total Bus Car Bike Feet Train

    Travel Satisfaction 0.16 −0.28 0.11 0.83 0.83 −0.19Safe 4.25 4.48 4.34 3.21 4.87 4.33Clean 3.93 3.68 4.33 3.66 4.57 3.41Congested 3.29 3.82 3.22 2.69 2.21 4.26Strenuous 2.66 2.82 2.42 2.79 2.15 3.31Convenient 3.70 3.14 3.89 4.53 4.33 2.68

    N 4134 1593 1010 759 520 252

    L.K. Lades, et al. Transportation Research Part A 136 (2020) 318–333

    322

  • 27.15% indicated rarely or never having problems finding a parking place, 30.70% answered with 1–2 times per week, 19.12% with3–4 times per week, and 23.03% with 5 or more times per week.

    Participants also indicated the origin point of their trip using a list of regions that could be accessed from a drop-down menu.Respondents started their trips from 259 different regions with 221 (5.35%) starting from the Campus itself. We mapped all of theseregions to the nearest Local Election Area (LEA) in order to produce Fig. 3.8 The numbers in each LEA in this figure indicate thenumber of participants who started their trip from the respective area along with the travel mode share in percent for publictransport, car use, and active transport. The figure shows, for example, that active commutes are much more prevalent from LEAs

    Fig. 3. Travel Satisfaction by Local Election Areas. The numbers indicate the number of respondents who start their trip in the respective area alongwith the travel mode share in percent for (Public Transport, Car, Active Transport).

    8 Each county and city in Ireland are divided into Local Electoral Areas (LEAs) for the purposes of local elections. In 2019 there were 166 LEAs inIreland.

    L.K. Lades, et al. Transportation Research Part A 136 (2020) 318–333

    323

  • close to the campus than from LEAs far away from the campus. The color-coding in the figure indicates the average travel satisfactionrating for each LEA, using a 5-point color-coded classification from red, indicating lower levels of travel satisfaction, to green,indicating higher levels of travel satisfaction. The map suggests that the distance to UCD is an important geographical determinant oftravel satisfaction. However, it is possible that distance only predicts lower travel satisfaction because distance and duration arepositively correlated. Moreover, there seems to be a North/South divide with commutes that start in LEAs north of the river Liffeybeing less satisfying than commutes from south of the river.

    There are recognized differences in affluence between the south and the north of the city (e.g., Doherty et al., 2015) and priorresearch has investigated the link between economic deprivation and travel satisfaction (e.g., Currie et al., 2009). The accessibility topublic transport may be an important reason for this link between economic deprivation and travel satisfaction. In order to in-vestigate the impact of deprivation and accessibility on travel satisfaction in Dublin, we referred to two complementary geospatialdatasets: (i) the 2016 Haase-Pratschke (HP) Index of Relative Deprivation described in Haase et al. (2012) and available online,9 and(ii) the 2008 Accessibility Index (Gamma and Haase, 2008). The former measures the relative deprivation of Ireland’s regions basedon economic, demographic, and social Census-based indicators and the latter focuses purely on transport-based accessibility in-dicators. Both datasets are available at Electoral Division Level, which is more detailed than the level of LEAs.10 Hence, we linked thesurvey participants’ points of journey origin also to the Electoral Division Level.

    Finally, using the information on the Electoral Divisions, we created a measure for the distance of each journey. We used thenetwork analyst extension of ArcGIS and the Open Street Map road network to generate an Origin-Destination matrix with the originof each trip defined as the centroid of each Electoral Division and the destination point defined as the UCD Belfield Campus. Thisprovided us with a distance measure in kilometers indicating the shortest distance that people would need to travel on the existingroad network between the trip origin and the campus. This procedure allowed us to calculate the distance for 3970 trips. The meandistance of these trips is 13.5 km (SD: 19.03 km; Min: 0.15 km; Max: 130.44 km). There are substantial differences in distance acrossthe main travel mode people used. The averages for the respective travel modes are as follows: Walking: 2.21 km; Cycling: 6.19 km;Bus: 16.11 km; Car: 19.06 km; Train: 26.72 km).

    3.4. Analysis strategy

    In order to analyze the differences in travel satisfaction across travel modes, we run a number of linear regressions with the globalSatisfaction with Travel Scale (STSi) for individual i as the dependent variable.11 The full regression model of the main analysis isspecified as

    = + + + + + + + ∊Char XSTS β β Mode β Dur β β ToD β Comp ,ii i i i i i i0 1 2 3 4 5 (1)

    where Mode indicates a categorical variable for the primary travel mode, Dur is the duration of the commute, Char is a vectorindicating five subjective trip characteristics (safe, clean, congested/overcrowded, strenuous, and convenient) interpreted as con-tinuous variables, ToD is a continuous variable indicating the time of the day when the commute started, Comp is a dummy variableindicating whether there was a company or not, X is a vector indicating the individual-specific control variables (age, gender, role atuniversity, and days per week on the campus), and ∊ is the robust error term. In the main analysis, we first quantify the effect of thetravel mode on travel satisfaction controlling for the time of the day, company, and the variables described in X . We then add furthervariables to the mode. We are particularly interested in the effect of travel mode on travel satisfaction and hence observe how thetravel mode coefficients change when adding further variables (travel duration and subjective trip characteristics). This stepwiseaddition of variables helps us to explain differences in travel satisfaction across travel modes as identified in the previous literatureand also helps us to control for sample heterogeneity (e.g., travel duration differs systematically across travel modes).

    In additional regression analyses, we omit the travel mode variable and run separate regressions for each travel mode with theremaining variables. This provides mode-specific associations between the independent variables and travel satisfaction. We alsomake use of some additional questions asked in the travel survey and test whether living close to bus stops, using a real-timeinformation app, and parking difficulties are associated with travel satisfaction for certain modes. Finally, we test for associationsbetween travel satisfaction and external information on the location from which participants started their trip, focusing on distance,economic deprivation, and the accessibility to transport.

    4. Regression results

    Table 3 presents the study’s main results on the determinants of travel satisfaction. Models (1)–(3) present the results using thefull sample adding variables gradually. Specifically, model (2) adds the trip duration and model (3) the subjective trip characteristicsto the regression. Models (4)–(8) repeat model (3) but for each of the travel modes separately in order to identify mode-specific

    9 The 2016 Pobal HP Deprivation Index for Small Areas (SA) is available from http://trutzhaase.eu/deprivation-index/the-2016-pobal-hp-deprivation-index-for-small-areas/ (retrieved on June, 04, 2019).

    10 Electoral Divisions (EDs) are the smallest legally defined administrative areas in Ireland. Generally, LEAs are created by the aggregation of EDs.In 2019 Ireland was divided into 3440 EDs.

    11 While we interpret the global travel satisfaction variable as continuous and run OLS regressions in the main analyses, we present the results ofan ordered logit model in the Supplementary Information. The results are qualitatively unchanged.

    L.K. Lades, et al. Transportation Research Part A 136 (2020) 318–333

    324

    http://trutzhaase.eu/deprivation-index/the-2016-pobal-hp-deprivation-index-for-small-areas/http://trutzhaase.eu/deprivation-index/the-2016-pobal-hp-deprivation-index-for-small-areas/

  • determinants of travel satisfaction.

    4.1. Travel mode effects

    Model (1) shows that active modes of transport (walking and biking) are associated with higher travel satisfaction compared tonon-active modes. Calculating the predicted travel satisfaction ratings for each travel mode holding all other variables constant at

    Table 3Results of linear regression models predicting global travel satisfaction.

    (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)

    VARIABLES All All All Bus Car Bike Feet Train

    Travel mode. Base = BusCar 0.166*** −0.0874 −0.154***

    (0.0607) (0.0598) (0.0531)Bike 0.984*** 0.386*** 0.597***

    (0.0530) (0.0626) (0.0593)Walking 1.067*** 0.522*** 0.414***

    (0.0606) (0.0682) (0.0608)Train 0.0349 0.305*** 0.379***

    (0.0773) (0.0753) (0.0631)Length of the trip. Base = Less than 15 min15–30 min −0.175*** −0.00501 −0.227 −0.202* 0.0777 −0.0628 −0.609

    (0.0562) (0.0506) (0.176) (0.121) (0.0739) (0.111) (0.537)30–45 min −0.347*** 0.00978 −0.247 −0.293** 0.125 0.135 0.243

    (0.0658) (0.0595) (0.172) (0.131) (0.103) (0.145) (0.384)45–60 min −0.671*** −0.155** −0.370** −0.525*** 0.294* 0.232 −0.204

    (0.0726) (0.0665) (0.173) (0.139) (0.159) (0.198) (0.219)60+ min −1.202*** −0.517*** −0.711*** −0.867*** −0.293 0.0140 −0.479**

    (0.0728) (0.0681) (0.169) (0.146) (0.637) (0.389) (0.194)Safe 0.185*** 0.106*** 0.292*** 0.216*** 0.250*** 0.185**

    (0.0211) (0.0325) (0.0505) (0.0413) (0.0737) (0.0849)Clean 0.0846*** 0.147*** −0.0158 0.0959** 0.0832 0.104

    (0.0196) (0.0305) (0.0427) (0.0411) (0.0602) (0.0820)Congested −0.0674*** −0.0975*** −0.0989** −0.0489 0.0403 −0.0968

    (0.0194) (0.0326) (0.0403) (0.0409) (0.0530) (0.0833)Strenuous −0.157*** −0.192*** −0.105** −0.0598 −0.196*** −0.235***

    (0.0202) (0.0321) (0.0440) (0.0408) (0.0583) (0.0795)Convenient 0.376*** 0.412*** 0.284*** 0.388*** 0.375*** 0.448***

    (0.0197) (0.0301) (0.0431) (0.0483) (0.0623) (0.0634)Time of day 0.0226*** 0.00554 0.00332 7.72e-05 0.00624 0.00327 0.00192 −0.00117

    (0.00411) (0.00405) (0.00341) (0.00492) (0.00745) (0.00709) (0.0108) (0.0163)Company. Base = Strangers were aroundI was alone 0.0304 0.0317 0.0539 0.0520 0.130 0.113 −0.000807 0.0134

    (0.0472) (0.0458) (0.0396) (0.0567) (0.403) (0.0862) (0.104) (0.142)People I knew 0.134** 0.138** 0.108** 0.0421 0.150 0.122 0.201 0.219

    (0.0670) (0.0637) (0.0545) (0.0801) (0.409) (0.173) (0.150) (0.208)Female −0.135*** −0.139*** −0.109*** −0.189*** −0.0793 0.0182 −0.0594 −0.0836

    (0.0374) (0.0357) (0.0312) (0.0486) (0.0707) (0.0630) (0.0959) (0.127)Age Category. Base = 18–2425–34 0.0827 0.106* 0.129*** 0.0514 0.101 0.278*** 0.130 −0.0129

    (0.0597) (0.0565) (0.0496) (0.0806) (0.119) (0.0928) (0.142) (0.223)35–44 0.297*** 0.379*** 0.328*** 0.109 0.439*** 0.435*** 0.231 0.446

    (0.0839) (0.0796) (0.0690) (0.120) (0.134) (0.140) (0.222) (0.272)45–54 0.659*** 0.697*** 0.596*** 0.642*** 0.599*** 0.655*** 0.691*** 0.473*

    (0.0923) (0.0881) (0.0769) (0.156) (0.146) (0.140) (0.221) (0.265)55+ 0.770*** 0.719*** 0.564*** 0.556*** 0.642*** 0.700*** 0.115 0.295

    (0.124) (0.115) (0.100) (0.195) (0.165) (0.186) (0.364) (0.375)Rather not say 0.381 0.271 0.361 1.205*** 0.137 −1.214*** 1.075** 0.577***

    (0.316) (0.269) (0.251) (0.326) (0.327) (0.156) (0.524) (0.218)Staff (vs Student) 0.0914 0.0347 −0.00111 −0.0807 −0.117 0.133 0.299* 0.0370

    (0.0696) (0.0652) (0.0565) (0.0990) (0.112) (0.110) (0.162) (0.227)Days in per week 0.0271 −0.0211 −0.0247 −0.0336 −0.00310 −0.00980 −0.00777 −0.0242

    (0.0204) (0.0197) (0.0171) (0.0259) (0.0381) (0.0356) (0.0488) (0.0673)Constant −0.854*** 0.461*** 0.129 0.577** 0.0690 0.401 0.412 0.655

    (0.136) (0.157) (0.136) (0.241) (0.481) (0.283) (0.402) (0.453)

    Observations 4134 4134 4134 1593 1010 759 520 252R-squared 0.181 0.255 0.430 0.384 0.359 0.314 0.303 0.453

    Robust standard errors in parentheses. ***p < 0.01, **p < 0.05, * p < 0.1.

    L.K. Lades, et al. Transportation Research Part A 136 (2020) 318–333

    325

  • their mean shows that walking and biking obtain predicted travel satisfaction ratings of 0.894 (95% Confidence Intervals: 0.795,0.994) and 0.810 (95% CIs: 0.739, 0.882), respectively. Car trips obtain predicted travel satisfaction ratings of −0.021 (95% CIs:−0.707, 0.065), train trips of −0.194 (95% CIs: −0.333, 0.05), and bus trips of −0.207 (95% CIs: −0.265, −0.149). These valuescorrespond to the black diamonds in Fig. 4.12 While public transport receives the lowest travel satisfaction ratings in model (1), inmodels (2) and (3) the car is the mode that people are least satisfied with. This suggests that some of the additionally added variablesexplain why people consider car trips to be more satisfying than, for example, bus trips when not controlling for anything and that weneed to conduct more detailed analyses to unpack this effect. However, Fig. 4 also shows that active travel stays more satisfying thanmotorized travel even when adding several control variables to the models.

    4.2. Influence of trip duration and trip distance

    The effects of travel mode on travel satisfaction change substantially when we control for the duration of the trip in model (2). Forwalking and biking, adding trip duration reduces predicted travel satisfaction ratings to 0.553 (95% CIs: 0.446, 0.661) and 0.416(95% CIs: 0.330, 0.503), respectively. For the train and the bus, travel satisfaction increases to 0.336 (95% CIs: 0.190, 0.482) and0.031(95% CIs: −0.035, 0.097), respectively. The reason for these changes is that active travel tends to be relatively short and publictransport trips tend to be long (see Fig. 3) and that travel duration itself is a strong and significant predictor of trip satisfaction overall(see Table 3, model 2). For example, based on model 2, the predicted travel satisfaction for trips lasting less than 15 min is 0.715(95% CIs: 0.616, 0.814) and the predicted travel satisfaction for trips lasting more than an hour is −0.488 (95% CIs: −0.568,−0.408).13 Once the differences in travel duration are controlled for, active travel is less good and public transport is less bad,suggesting that one main advantage of active travel is its short duration and that one main disadvantage of public transport is its longduration.

    In additional regressions (see Appendix Table SI 4), we tested for the effect of trip distance on travel satisfaction. Adding tripdistance (in kilometers) to the specification of model (1) in Table 3 shows a significantly negative association (b = −0.00731;p < 0.001). However, when additionally controlling for trip duration, the association between distance and travel satisfactionbecomes insignificant and the size reverses (b = 0.00227; p = 0.086). This suggest that long distance trips are less satisfying becausethey take a lot of time. Due to the close correlation between trip duration and trip distance (the mean trip distance in kilometers forthe duration categories “15 min or less”, “15–30 min”, “35–45 min”, “45–60 min”, and “60 min or more” are 2.42 km, 5.73 km,10.08 km, 14.10 km, and 29.71 km, respectively), in the following regressions we will only include trip duration.

    4.3. Time of the trip

    In model 1 of Table 3, the time of the day when the trip begins has a positive effect on travel satisfaction (b = 0.226, p < 0.001).In model (2), this effect is insignificant (b = 0.006, p = 0.171) which indicates that the time of the day effect is entirely explained bytrip duration. This suggests that trips become longer when people travel later, most likely due to rush-hour effects. Fig. 5 summarizesa more detailed analysis of these time of the day effects showing the effect of time of the day (in 15-minute intervals) on travelsatisfaction for active modes (walking and biking), driving the car, and public transport (bus and train). Panel A shows that time ofthe day is positively associated with travel satisfaction for inactive travel-modes (the car and public transport) when not controlling

    Fig. 4. Travel satisfaction by mode corresponding to models (1)–(3) from Table 3 with 95% Confidence Intervals.

    12 The predicted travel satisfaction ratings from model (1), i.e. the black diamonds in Fig. 4, are very similar to the average travel satisfactionratings as presented in Table 2. This indicates that the control variables included in model (1) are not strong determinants of the association betweentravel mode and travel satisfaction.

    13 We can also predict travel satisfaction by trip duration in a model without controlling for travel modes. When dropping the travel mode frommodel 2, travel duration becomes a slightly stronger predictor of travel satisfaction: For example, the predicted travel satisfaction for trips lastingless than 15 min is then 0.887 (95% CIs: 0.801, 0.973) and the predicted travel satisfaction for trips lasting more than an hour is −0.581 (95% CIs:−0.652, −0.509).

    L.K. Lades, et al. Transportation Research Part A 136 (2020) 318–333

    326

  • for anything. This effect is significant (see Table SI 5 in the Supplementary Information). However, when controlling for trip duration,these associations become insignificant and the lines for the car and public transport in Panel B become approximately horizontal (seealso Table SI 5). As such, the association between leaving time and travel satisfaction is fully explained by the time it takes to reachthe campus.

    4.4. Influence of subjective trip characteristics

    Model (3) in Table 3 adds the subjective trip-characteristics to the regression. The main effects of these subjective trip-char-acteristics on travel satisfaction are summarized in Panel A of Fig. 6. This figure shows that trips that are experienced as safe, clean,and convenient are associated with higher travel satisfaction and trips that are experienced as congested/overcrowded and strenuousare associated with lower travel satisfaction. Convenience is the most important characteristic. A one-standard-deviation increase ofconvenience is associated with an increase of 0.376 (p < 0.001) in travel satisfaction, controlling for all other subjective char-acteristics. Congestion is the least important trip characteristic (b = −0.067; p = 0.001).

    Adding the subjective trip characteristics to the regression in model (3) also changes the effects of travel mode on travel sa-tisfaction. In particular, and as illustrated in Fig. 3, the predicted travel satisfaction for biking increases, which is an effect in theopposite direction of the effect that adding duration in model (2) has. Moreover, once we control for the trip characteristics, the effectof duration on travel satisfaction is attenuated. For example, in model (3) trips that last for less than 15 min are not significantlydifferent in terms of their travel satisfaction compared to trips that last 30–45 min.

    4.5. Effects of demographics

    In terms of demographics, females are less satisfied with their trip than males (b = −0.135, p < 0.001; based on model 1) andthis effect attenuates only slightly when controlling for more variables in models (2) and (3). Age is positively associated with tripsatisfaction. For example, based on model (1), travelers aged 18–24 have a mean travel satisfaction score of 0.022 (95% CIs: −0.038,0.082) and travelers aged over 55 have a mean travel satisfaction score of 0.793 (95% CIs: 0.571, 1.014). There are no differences intravel satisfaction between students and staff members and it does not matter for travel satisfaction how often participants commuteto the campus per week on average.

    Fig. 5. Travel satisfaction by time the trip started with and without controlling for the length of the trip. The shaded areas indicate 95% ConfidenceIntervals.

    Fig. 6. Effects of the five subjective trip-characteristics on travel satisfaction for all modes combined (Panel A) and separated by mode (Panel B).

    L.K. Lades, et al. Transportation Research Part A 136 (2020) 318–333

    327

  • 4.6. Travel mode-specific effects

    Models (4)–(8) repeat the analysis from model (3) for each of the five travel modes separately. This analysis reveals that tripduration matters more for bus and car-trips than when people bike, walk, or take the train. The effects of trip duration on travelsatisfaction in each of the travel modes is attenuated, however, by the inclusion of the five trip characteristics. In (not reported)regressions that do not control for subjective trip characteristics, trip duration is a significant and negative predictor for all travelmodes. Models (4)–(8) also show that females are less satisfied with their travel than males only during bus trips. There are no genderdifferences in other modes. Fig. 6, Panel B shows the associations between the subjective trip-characteristics and travel satisfactionseparated by travel mode, suggesting that, for example, cleanliness matters for bike and bus trips, but not when walking, for traintrips, or car trips.

    4.7. Distance from a bus stop, use of the real-time app, and parking difficulties

    The survey contained a number of additional variables and we tested for the associations between these and travel satisfaction.We found that travel satisfaction is higher when people live less than 500 m away from the nearest bus stop (b = 0.146, p < 0.001),and that this effect is driven by bus users, walkers, and train users. We also found that bus travelers were not more or less satisfiedwith their trip depending on whether they used a real-time information app (an app that gives real-time information about when thenext bus arrives) (b = −0.087, p = 0.137). Parking difficulties, which were assessed only for participants who indicated that theydrive to the campus regularly, had a strong effect on travel satisfaction for those whose last commute was by car. In a model similar tomodel (2) from Table 3, the predicted travel satisfaction ratings for commuters who have parking difficulties 5 or more times a weekis−0.236 (95% CIs:−0.390,−0.082). The predicted travel satisfaction for those who have parking difficulties 3 or 4 times a week is−0.134 (95% CIs: 0.257, −0.010), for those who have parking difficulties 1–2 times per week it is 0.252 (95% CIs: 0.146, 0.359),and for those who rarely or never have parking difficulties it is 0.547 (95% CIs: 0.420, 0.675).

    4.8. Deprivation and accessibility effects

    Finally, we tested for associations between travel satisfaction and the geospatial variables economic deprivation and accessibilityat the location where the trip started. As illustrated in Fig. 7, Panel A when only controlling for demographics the economic de-privation index is positively associated with travel satisfaction (b = 0.0241, p < 0.001). People are more satisfied with their tripswhen they come from a more affluent area. This association is in the expected direction as the campus is in an affluent area of the cityand people are more satisfied with shorter trips. When additionally controlling for the travel mode, the effect is attenuated(b = 0.0104, p < 0.001), indicating that travelers in affluent areas close to the campus more frequently use active travel modes.Finally, when also controlling for the duration of the trip (and some other variables as in model 3), the association between thedeprivation index and travel satisfaction becomes negative and significant (b = −0.0144, p < 0.001). This suggests that if the triplasted for the same amount of time, travelers would be more satisfied with the trip when starting from a deprived area rather than aless deprived area. However, the data show that deprivation alone is not a reason for lower levels of travel satisfaction. We repeat thesame analysis with the accessibility index and the results are shown in Fig. 7, Panel B. Again, better accessibility to public transport ispositively associated with travel satisfaction. This effect is entirely explained by other control variables and in particular by travelmode and the duration of the trip.

    5. Discussion

    5.1. Contributions to the literature

    The paper contributes to the literature on travel satisfaction in a range of ways. First, it replicates several associations with travelsatisfaction in a different travel environment with a comparatively large sample of over 4000 participants in a city with a less

    Fig. 7. Travel satisfaction by deprivation index (Panel A) and Accessibility index (Panel B) measured at the Electoral Division level at the placewhere the trip began.

    L.K. Lades, et al. Transportation Research Part A 136 (2020) 318–333

    328

  • developed transport infrastructure. In our data, travel satisfaction has a mean of 0.165 (on the Satisfaction with Travel Scale thatranges from −3 to +3), which is comparatively low compared to other studies using the same scale in different geographical areas(e.g., compared to De Vos et al., 2015; Friman et al., 2013, 2017).14

    The study replicates recent findings indicating that travel satisfaction is highest for active transport modes (e.g., De Vos, 2018,2019b; De Vos et al., 2013; Ettema et al., 2011; Friman et al., 2017; Lancée et al., 2017; Smith, 2017; Ye and Titheridge, 2017; Zhuand Fan, 2018). Going beyond showing these associations, our study empirically investigates why travel satisfaction differs acrosstravel modes. As highlighted by De Vos (2019b), “no study up till now has been able to indicate why travel satisfaction levels differaccording to the chosen travel mode” (p. 15). He argues that the strong effects of travel mode on travel satisfaction typically found inthe literature are likely to be overestimated. We show, for example, that the duration of the trip explains why people prefer car tripsover public transport trips: If both trip types had the same duration, people would prefer using public transport in our sample. Ourfindings thus suggest that it is essential for future studies to control for trip duration when examining differences in travel satisfactionacross travel modes. While De Vos (2019b) suggests that attitudes towards travel modes mediate the mode effect on travel sa-tisfaction, we show empirically that trip duration is also an important mediator. However, travel mode remains a significant predictorof travel satisfaction in our data even after controlling for many variables (such as the duration of the trip). But if we had alsocontrolled for (unavailable) data on people’s attitudes towards travel modes, the travel mode effects on travel satisfaction might beeven more attenuated.

    The study also contributes to the literature on the links between subjective trip attributes and travel satisfaction (Higgins et al.,2018; Mahmoud and Hine, 2016; Mao et al., 2016; Morris and Hirsch, 2016; Mouwen, 2015; de Oña et al., 2016; Smith, 2017; Susiloand Cats, 2014). We test the correlations of 5 subjective trip characteristics (safe, clean, congested/overcrowded, strenuous, andconvenient) with travel satisfaction and find that, for example, safety and in particular convenience predict travel satisfaction po-sitively. It is noteworthy that in the current study, biking is perceived as more satisfying than the non-active travel modes althoughpeople perceive it to be relatively dangerous to bike in Dublin (the average safety rating for all modes on a scale from 0 to 6 in ourdata is 4.25; the average safety rating for biking is only 3.21).

    We also advance the nascent literature on geospatial determinants of travel satisfaction. For example, some studies find thatsuburban respondents report higher levels of travel satisfaction than urban respondents (e.g., De Vos et al., 2016) and other studies donot find direct associations between the residential location and travel satisfaction (e.g., Ye and Titheridge, 2017). As can be seen inFig. 3, in our data commutes from suburban areas are less satisfying than commutes from urban areas. However, once we controlledfor various variables, including duration of the commute, this effect reversed. This suggests that the inclusion of duration-relatedvariables in the regression models might explain the differences found across studies in the previous literature. We also incorporatemore specific geospatial determinants of the commute – specifically the economic deprivation and accessibility of the location wherethe trip started and present some associations of these variables with travel satisfaction. Hence, we contribute to the literature on thelinks between transportation and social disadvantage (e.g., Lucas, 2012) by directly investigating the relationship between the spatialdistribution of deprivation and travel satisfaction.

    The paper also provides new evidence on the link between trip duration, distance, and travel satisfaction. In line with a number ofstudies (De Vos et al., 2016; Handy and Thigpen, 2019; Higgins et al., 2018, 2018; Morris and Guerra, 2015; St-Louis et al., 2014;Stone and Schneider, 2016; Westman et al., 2017; Zhang et al., 2016), we find that longer trips are less satisfying. Other researchfinds that longer trips can be positively correlated with travel satisfaction (De Vos et al., 2016; Mokhtarian et al., 2015), but this istypically the case for unusual trips and not for commutes and routine trips as we analyze in this paper.

    5.2. Implications for transport policy-making

    The findings presented in this paper offer insights that can inform the design of interventions aimed at encouraging shifts to non-motorized or mass transit modes. Furthermore, the evidence leads us to consider an ambitious policy proposal that could encouragemodal shifts, whilst also substantially easing transport infrastructure pressures at peak travel times.

    First, we find that active travel commutes are the most satisfying form of travel. This suggests that investments in infrastructure(e.g. safe and well-lit cycling and walking paths) on the extended approach routes to the campus can help to sustain and encourageactive travel. Moreover, active travel could boost the travel satisfaction of those commuting to the campus. However, to validate thatthis association is causal, future research should implement causal designs to test whether it is active travel that causally increasestravel satisfaction.

    Second, the duration of the trip is a strong predictor of travel satisfaction. Thus, we surmise that interventions that reducecommute time can increase travel satisfaction. An important characteristic of most active commutes is their short duration, which isoften linked to the proximity between the origin and the destination. Whilst long-term strategies can seek to reduce commutedistances through higher density planning and so forth,15 in the short to medium term, this is not an option. However, reducing

    14 For example, using the same scale Friman et al. (2013) report a mean travel satisfaction rating of 0.75 in a sample from Stockholm, Goteborg,and Malmö, De Vos et al. (2015) report mean travel satisfaction ratings in Ghent by travel mode that can be averaged to a travel satisfaction rating of1.23, and Friman et al. (2017) report the three sub-components of the satisfaction with travel scale that average to 0.87 in a sample in Stockholm,Goteborg, and Karlstad. However, our sample contains more bus users than most of the other studies which reduces the average trip satisfaction. Anoptimal study design would compare the travel satisfaction to very similar destinations (e.g., in terms of location) across different cities.

    15 See for example the compact development concept in the National Planning Framework, as well as details of linked national infrastructure

    L.K. Lades, et al. Transportation Research Part A 136 (2020) 318–333

    329

  • distance is not the only way to reduce trip durations. For example, the National Transport Authority of Ireland is proposing to re-design the Dublin bus network over the coming years to make bus journeys faster, more predictable, and more reliable.16 Where theseinvestments and actions can shorten trip durations, there should be a further stimulus for modal shift to active and mass transitmodes. However, the findings of this paper also suggest that it may be useful to establish a general ambition for the reduction of tripjourney times within a reasonable radius to less than 45 min, on the basis of this representing a clear threshold point for tripsatisfaction ratings.17

    Third, the analysis indicates convenience as a key characteristic of travel satisfaction, suggesting that transport policies shouldaim to increase convenience for travelers. This can include efforts to reduce waiting times for mass transit modes, facilitating easierand more direct access to the campus via walking and cycling routes and so forth.

    Fourth, the data show that difficulties with finding a parking space on campus have a very strong negative effect on the travelsatisfaction of drivers. This reinforces the importance of parking policy and strategy with regard to encouraging modal shift andinfluencing private car journeys to campus. Whilst the immediate response may be to consider the provision of additional parkingspaces, the lower relative travel satisfaction of car journeys where parking is either scarce or strategically priced also represents anopportunity to stimulate additional transfers to non-motorized or mass transit modes of travel to campus.

    Finally, we find that travelling at rush-hour, which impacts negatively on travel satisfaction, is explained by the longer time ittakes to get to the campus during these peak travel times. This insight and the overall findings in this paper lead us to consider anambitious short-term initiative that could reduce journey times for commuters to the campus, ease pressure on the morning peaktransport and travel infrastructure, and potentially encourage more non-motorized and mass transit mode choices. Specifically, wewould recommend that the universities consider the introduction of a newly regulated schedule that ensures classes, coursework, andmost meetings do not commence before 10 am. The anticipated impact of shifting more than 30,000 staff and students to a deferredstarting hour would be to reduce associated commutes during the peak travel times. Related initiatives, compatible with the systemsand skillsets developed during the COVID-19 pandemic, could also see increased options into the future for remote participation inmeetings and classes. These measures could contribute to reduced road congestion and potentially increased safety for cyclists andpedestrians. The reduced commuting on public transport in the busiest period of the morning, would ease pressure on publictransport service capacity and open up opportunities for other commuters (e.g. 9–5 city center workers) to consider a shift to publictransport. There are of course recognizable challenges that range from motorists continuing to travel early to secure parking spaces,to challenges for the campus population relating to their own personal scheduling of commitments and activities such as picking upchildren, doing groceries, going to leisure activities, etc. However, an initial response may suggest complementary parking strategies,and a recognition that young university students are likely amongst the most flexible of all morning commuters. Furthermore, theCOVID-19 pandemic has accelerated a global understanding of what is possible in regard to remote working and learning. While amore detailed analysis of these policy proposals would need to be conducted, our results provide an important contribution to suchanalyses.

    5.3. Further research

    Our study suggests several avenues for future research. We use cross-sectional data in this study and therefore cannot interpret theassociations we find in a causal way. For example, the finding that people are more satisfied with active than passive travel couldreflect that people who are generally more satisfied with things are particularly likely to use active travel modes. Moreover, other,unobserved variables could explain both travel mode choice and travel satisfaction. Further research is needed in order to identify thedirection of causality by either including travel satisfaction questionnaires in experimental designs, evaluating natural experiments,or making use of econometric techniques to allow us to infer causality from observational data. As a first step, it would be helpful tohave the same study respondent rate a higher number of trips made with different travel modes in order to control for individual-specific fixed effects. Such studies are more burdensome for participants to complete, and as a result, they will likely have smallersample sizes. Nonetheless, we believe that it is an important step for research in this area that could also identify further associationswith travel satisfaction, such as weather and the time of the year.

    An alternative avenue for future research is to identify more variables that could explain differences in travel satisfaction acrosstravel modes. For example, active travel has health and well-being benefits (Humphreys et al., 2013; Martin et al., 2014) and activecommutes might lead to happier individuals over time. To get first insights into this link, future research could test for differences intravel satisfaction between active commuters who just started commuting actively and those who already commute actively for anextended period of time. More psychological variables, such as (perceived) lack of control, might also explain differences on travelsatisfaction across travel modes where active travelers perceive a higher sense of control (Beirão and Sarsfield Cabral, 2007). Finally,variables related to the environment (such as air quality, noise, and green spaces) might explain differences in travel satisfactionacross travel modes.

    Another avenue for further research concerns the items used in this study. For example, it would be important to test whether

    (footnote continued)investment plans - http://npf.ie/project-ireland-2040-national-planning-framework/ and http://npf.ie/wp-content/uploads/NSO-Feb18.pdf.

    16 https://busconnects.ie/media/1184/bus-connects-cbc-route-maps-web_14.pdf.17 We acknowledge that future work can set more refined travel time bands to gain additional insight on associated travel satisfaction threshold

    points.

    L.K. Lades, et al. Transportation Research Part A 136 (2020) 318–333

    330

    http://npf.ie/project-ireland-2040-national-planning-framework/http://npf.ie/wp-content/uploads/NSO-Feb18.pdfhttps://busconnects.ie/media/1184/bus-connects-cbc-route-maps-web_14.pdf

  • subjective trip characteristics, such as “safe” and “congested” are interpreted differently by travelers using different modes.Experiencing a busy pathway on the bike might be a different experience compared to commuting on a congested bus. Hence,comparing subjective attribute measures across modes might be an invalid comparison and further research should identify sub-jective attributes that are interpreted similarly across modes to allow for valid comparisons. A related avenue for future research is toinvestigate whether the associations we find when using the general Satisfaction with Travel Scale are also present when we in-vestigate the sub-components of the scale.

    The University campus provides us with an excellent case of substantial scale. However, the methodology could be applied morebroadly, with a view to the development of a city-wide travel satisfaction index. Many national authorities run travel surveys as partof their national census and adding a question on travel satisfaction for each trip would provide valuable information that could beused to develop a dynamic index that could monitor travel satisfaction and thereby inform policy-makers of the relative strengths andweaknesses of the travel infrastructure in their city.

    Finally, we have proposed an ambitious policy intervention about a delayed starting schedule and/or increased remote learningfor the University campus. National ambitions relating to reduced GHG emissions, improved ambient air quality, and better citizenhealth and well-being could benefit from these policies. There are surmountable complexities relating to the design of such schemes,as well as a number of interactions and outcomes that would influence the degree to which the initiative may reduce emissions.Nonetheless, the primary motivation for the measure would be to reduce travel time, ease pressure on peak transport services, andenhance travel satisfaction for all. In order to advance this proposal, we would recommend further research on the policy design, witha potential ex-ante ex-post case study utilizing selected University units as a precursor to the possibility of a campus-wide measure.

    CRediT authorship contribution statement

    Leonhard K. Lades: Conceptualization, Methodology, Software, Formal analysis, Data curation, Writing - original draft, Writing -review & editing, Visualization. Andrew Kelly: Conceptualization, Writing - review & editing. Luke Kelleher: Conceptualization,Software, Data curation, Writing - review & editing, Visualization.

    Acknowledgments

    We like to thank UCD’s Estates Service Team for sharing the data and the UCD Smarter Travel Working Group for valuable inputs.We also appreciate comments from members of the UCD Behavioural Science and Policy Group, from the IAREP/SABE 2019 con-ference in Dublin, and from three anonymous reviewers. Thanks also to Aisling Reidy for excellent research assistance. All errors areours. Leonhard Lades has been supported by a grant from the Irish Environmental Protection Agency (Project name: EnablingTransition; Project number: 2017-CCRP-FS.32). The EnvEcon Decision Support team research was supported by the Irish Departmentof Communications, Climate Action and Environment (DCCAE). The opinions, findings and conclusions or recommendations ex-pressed in this material are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the DCCAE.

    Appendix A. Supplementary material

    Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tra.2020.04.007.

    References

    Abou Zeid, M., 2009. Measuring and modeling activity and travel well-being (PhD Thesis). Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Abou-Zeid, M., Ben-Akiva, M., 2011. The effect of social comparisons on commute well-being. Transp. Res. Part A: Policy Pract. 45, 345–361.Beirão, G., Sarsfield Cabral, J.A., 2007. Understanding attitudes towards public transport and private car: A qualitative study. Transp. Policy 14, 478–489.Benjamin, D.J., Heffetz, O., Kimball, M.S., Rees-Jones, A., 2012. What do you think would make you happier? What do you think you would choose? Am. Econ. Rev.

    102, 2083–2110.Bergstad, C.J., Gamble, A., Gärling, T., Hagman, O., Polk, M., Ettema, D., Olsson, L.E., 2011. Subjective well-being related to satisfaction with daily travel.

    Transportation 38, 1–15.Berridge, K.C., Aldridge, J.W., 2008. Decision utility, the brain, and pursuit of hedonic goals. Social Cogn. 26, 621–646.Bryson, A., MacKerron, G., 2017. Are you happy while you work? Econ. J. 127, 106–125.Cao, J., Ettema, D., 2014. Satisfaction with travel and residential self-selection: How do preferences moderate the impact of the Hiawatha Light Rail Transit line? J.

    Transp. Land Use 7, 93–108.Carrel, A., Mishalani, R.G., Sengupta, R., Walker, J.L., 2016. In pursuit of the happy transit rider: dissecting satisfaction using daily surveys and tracking data. J. Intell.

    Transp. Syst. 20, 345–362.Comerford, D.A., 2011. Attenuating focalism in affective forecasts of the commuting experience: Implications for economic decisions and policy making. J. Econ.

    Psychol. 32, 691–699.Currie, G., Richardson, T., Smyth, P., Vella-Brodrick, D., Hine, J., Lucas, K., Stanley, J., 2009. Investigating links between transport disadvantage, social exclusion and

    well-being in Melbourne—Preliminary results. Transp. Policy 16, 97–105.De Vos, J., 2018. Do people travel with their preferred travel mode? Analysing the extent of travel mode dissonance and its effect on travel satisfaction. Transp. Res.

    Part A: Policy Pract. 117, 261–274.De Vos, J., 2019a. Analysing the effect of trip satisfaction on satisfaction with the leisure activity at the destination of the trip, in relationship with life satisfaction.

    Transportation 46, 623–645.De Vos, J., 2019b. Satisfaction-induced travel behaviour. Transp. Res. Part F: Traff. Psychol. Behav. 63, 12–21.De Vos, J., Mokhtarian, P.L., Schwanen, T., Van Acker, V., Witlox, F., 2016. Travel mode choice and travel satisfaction: Bridging the gap between decision utility and

    experienced utility. Transportation 43, 771–796.De Vos, J., Schwanen, T., Acker, V.V., Witlox, F., 2013. Travel and Subjective Well-Being: A focus on findings, methods and future research needs. Transp. Rev. 33,

    L.K. Lades, et al. Transportation Research Part A 136 (2020) 318–333

    331

    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tra.2020.04.007http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0005http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0010http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0015http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0020http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0020http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0025http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0025http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0030http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0035http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0040http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0040http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0045http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0045http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0050http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0050http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0055http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0055http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0060http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0060http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0065http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0065http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0070http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0075http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0075http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0080

  • 421–442.De Vos, J., Schwanen, T., Acker, V.V., Witlox, F., 2019. Do satisfying walking and cycling trips result in more future trips with active travel modes? An exploratory

    study. Int. J. Sustain. Transp. 13, 180–196.De Vos, J., Schwanen, T., Van Acker, V., Witlox, F., 2015. How satisfying is the Scale for Travel Satisfaction? Transp. Res. Part F: Traff. Psychol. Behav. 29, 121–130.De Vos, J., Witlox, F., 2017. Travel satisfaction revisited. On the pivotal role of travel satisfaction in conceptualising a travel behaviour process. Transp. Res. Part A:

    Policy Pract. 106, 364–373.Doherty, B., O’Leary, H., Hynes, W., 2015. Dublin City. Dublin City Socio-Economic Profile Indicator Catalogue. Dublin.Dolan, P., Peasgood, T., White, M., 2008. Do we really know what makes us happy? A review of the economic literature on the factors associated with subjective well-

    being. J. Econ. Psychol. 29, 94–122.Dolan, P., White, M.P., 2007. How can measures of Subjective Well-Being be used to inform public policy? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2, 71–85.Ettema, D., Friman, M., Gärling, T., Olsson, L.E., Fujii, S., 2012. How in-vehicle activities affect work commuters’ satisfaction with public transport. J. Transp. Geogr.

    24, 215–222.Ettema, D., Gärling, T., Eriksson, L., Friman, M., Olsson, L.E., Fujii, S., 2011. Satisfaction with travel and subjective well-being: Development and test of a measurement

    tool. Transp. Res. Part F: Traff. Psychol. Behav. 14, 167–175.Ettema, D., Gärling, T., Olsson, L.E., Friman, M., Moerdijk, S., 2013. The road to happiness: Measuring Dutch car drivers’ satisfaction with travel. Transp. Policy 27,

    171–178.Friman, M., Ettema, D., Olsson, L.E. (Eds.), 2018. Quality of Life and Daily Travel. Springer International Publishing.Friman, M., Fujii, S., Ettema, D., Gärling, T., Olsson, L.E., 2013. Psychometric analysis of the satisfaction with travel scale. Transp. Res. Part A: Policy Pract. 48,

    132–145.Friman, M., Gärling, T., Ettema, D., Olsson, L.E., 2017. How does travel affect emotional well-being and life satisfaction? Transp. Res. Part A: Policy Pract. 106,

    170–180.Gamma, Haase, T., 2008. Towards the Development of a Transport Accessibility Index.Gao, Y., Rasouli, S., Timmermans, H., Wang, Y., 2017. Effects of traveller’s mood and personality on ratings of satisfaction with daily trip stages. Travel Behav. Soc. 7,

    1–11.Gao, Y., Rasouli, S., Timmermans, H., Wang, Y., 2018. Trip stage satisfaction of public transport users: A reference-based model incorporating trip attributes, perceived

    service quality, psychological disposition and difference tolerance. Transp. Res. Part A: Policy Pract. 118, 759–775.Haase, T., Pratschke, J., Gleeson, J., 2012. All-island deprivation index: Towards the development of consistent deprivation measures for the island of Ireland.

    Borderlands: J. Spatial Plan. Ireland 2, 21–38.Handy, S., Thigpen, C., 2019. Commute quality and its implications for commute satisfaction: Exploring the role of mode, location, and other factors. Travel Behav.

    Soc. 16, 241–248.Hausman, D.M., 2012. Preference, Value, Choice, and Welfare. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.Higgins, C.D., Sweet, M.N., Kanaroglou, P.S., 2018. All minutes are not equal: Travel time and the effects of congestion on commute satisfaction in Canadian cities.

    Transportation 45, 1249–1268.Humphreys, D.K., Goodman, A., Ogilvie, D., 2013. Associations between active commuting and physical and mental wellbeing. Prev. Med. 57, 135–139.Infante, G., Lecouteux, G., Sugden, R., 2016. Preference purification and the inner rational agent: A critique of the conventional wisdom of behavioural welfare

    economics. J. Econ. Methodol. 23, 1–25.Kahneman, D., Krueger, A.B., Schkade, D.A., Schwarz, N., Stone, A.A., 2004. A survey method for characterizing daily life experience: The day reconstruction method.

    Science 306, 1776–1780.Kahneman, D., Wakker, P., Sarin, R., 1997. Back to Bentham? Explorations of experienced utility. Quart. J. Econ. 112 (2), 375–405.Lancée, S., Veenhoven, R., Burger, M., 2017. Mood during commute in the Netherlands: What way of travel feels best for what kind of people? Transp. Res. Part A:

    Policy Pract. 104, 195–208.Layard, R., 2006. Happiness and public policy: A challenge to the profession. Econ. J. 116, C24–C33.Lucas, K., 2012. Transport and social exclusion: Where are we now? Transp. Policy 20, 105–113.Lucas, K., Mattioli, G., Verlinghieri, E., Guzman, A., 2016. Transport poverty and its adverse social consequences. Proc. Instit. Civil Engineers – Transp. 169, 353–365.Lucas, K., Philips, I., Mulley, C., Ma, L., 2018. Is transport poverty socially or environmentally driven? Comparing the travel behaviours of two low-income populations

    living in central and peripheral locations in the same city. Transp. Res. Part A: Policy Pract. 116, 622–634.Mahmoud, M., Hine, J., 2016. Measuring the influence of bus service quality on the perception of users. Transp. Plan. Technol. 39, 284–299.Manaugh, K., El-Geneidy, A.M., 2013. Does distance matter? Exploring the links among values, motivations, home location, and satisfaction in walking trips. Transp.

    Res. Part A: Policy Pract. 50, 198–208.Mao, Z., Ettema, D., Dijst, M., 2016. Commuting trip satisfaction in Beijing: Exploring the influence of multimodal behavior and modal flexibility. Transp. Res. Part A:

    Policy Pract. 94, 592–603.Martin, A., Goryakin, Y., Suhrcke, M., 2014. Does active commuting improve psychological wellbeing? Longitudinal evidence from eighteen waves of the British

    Household Panel Survey. Prev. Med. 69, 296–303.Mokhtarian, P.L., Papon, F., Goulard, M., Diana, M., 2015. What makes travel pleasant and/or tiring? An investigation based on the French National Travel Survey.

    Transportation 42, 1103–1128.Mokhtarian, P.L., Pendyala, R.M., 2018. Travel Satisfaction and Well-Being. In: Friman, M., Ettema, D., Olsson, L.E. (Eds.), Quality of Life and Daily Travel. Springer

    International Publishing, Cham, pp. 17–39.Morris, E.A., Guerra, E., 2015. Are we there yet? Trip duration and mood during travel. Transp. Res. Part F: Traff. Psychol. Behav. 33, 38–47.Morris, E.A., Hirsch, J.A., 2016. Does rush hour see a rush of emotions? Driver mood in conditions likely to exhibit congestion. Travel Behav. Soc. 5, 5–13.Mouwen, A., 2015. Drivers of customer satisfaction with public transport services. Transp. Res. Part A: Policy Pract. 78, 1–20.National Transport Authority (Ireland), 2016. Transport Strategy for the Greater Dublin Area, 2016–2035. Retrieved from https://www.nationaltransport.ie/planning-

    policy/greater-dublin-areatransport-strategy-2016-2035/.Olsson, L.E., Gärling, T., Ettema, D., Friman, M., Fujii, S., 2013. Happiness and satisfaction with work commute. Soc. Indic. Res. 111, 255–263.de Oña, J., de Oña, R., Eboli, L., Forciniti, C., Mazzulla, G., 2016. Transit passengers’ behavioural intentions: The influence of service quality and customer satisfaction.

    Transportmet. A: Transp. Sci. 12, 385–412.Read, D., 2007. Experienced utility: Utility theory from Jeremy Bentham to Daniel Kahneman. Think. Reason. 13, 45–61.Schwanen, T., Lucas, K., Akyelken, N., Cisternas Solsona, D., Carrasco, J.-A., Neutens, T., 2015. Rethinking the links between social exclusion and transport dis-

    advantage through the lens of social capital. Transp. Res. Part A: Policy Pract. 74, 123–135.Shiffman, S., Stone, A.A., Hufford, M.R., 2008. Ecological momentary assessment. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 4, 1–32.Singleton, P.A., 2019. Walking (and cycling) to well-being: Modal and other determinants of subjective well-being during the commute. Travel Behaviour and Society

    16, 249–261.Smith, O., 2017. Commute well-being differences by mode: Evidence from Portland, Oregon, USA. J. Transp. Health 4, 246–254.St-Louis, E., Manaugh, K., van Lierop, D., El-Geneidy, A., 2014. The happy commuter: A comparison of commuter satisfaction across modes. Transp. Res. Part F: Traff.

    Psychol. Behav. 26, 160–170.Stone, A.A., Schneider, S., 2016. Commuting episodes in the United States: Their correlates with experiential wellbeing from the American Time Use Survey. Transp.

    Res. Part F: Traff. Psychol. Behav. 42, 117–124.Stutzer, A., Frey, B.S., 2008. Stress that doesn’t pay: The commuting paradox. Scand. J. Econ. 110, 339–366.Sugden, R., 2018. The Community of Advantage: A Behavioural Economist’s Defence of the Market. Oxford University Press.Susilo, Y.O., Cats, O., 2014. Exploring key determinants of travel satisfaction for multi-modal trips by different traveler groups. Transp. Res. Part A: Policy Pract. 67,

    L.K. Lades, et al. Transportation Research Part A 136 (2020) 318–333

    332

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0080http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0085http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0085http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0090http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0095http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0095http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0105http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0105http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0110http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0115http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0115http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0120http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0120http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0125http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0125http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0130http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0135http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0135http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0140http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0140http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0150http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0150http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0155http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0155http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0160http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0160http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0165http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0165http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0170http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0175http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0175http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0180http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0185http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0185http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0190http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0190http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0195http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0200http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0200http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0205http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0210http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0215http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0220http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0220http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0225http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0230http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0230http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0235http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0235http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0240http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0240http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0245http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0245http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0250http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0250http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0255http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0260http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0265https://www.nationaltransport.ie/planning-policy/greater-dublin-areatransport-strategy-2016-2035/https://www.nationaltransport.ie/planning-policy/greater-dublin-areatransport-strategy-2016-2035/http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0275http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0280http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0280http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0285http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0290http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0290http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0295http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0300http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0300http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0305http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0310http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0310http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0315http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0315http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0320http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0325http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0330

  • 366–380.Taniguchi, A., Grääs, C., Friman, M., 2014. Satisfaction with travel, goal achievement, and voluntary behavioral change. Transp. Res. Part F: Traff. Psychol. Behav. 26,

    10–17.Västfjäll, D., Friman, M., Gärling, T., Kleiner, M., 2002. The measurement of core affect: A Swedish self-report measure derived from the affect circumplex. Scand. J.

    Psychol. 43, 19–31.Westman, J., Olsson, L.E., Gärling, T., Friman, M., 2017. Children’s travel to school: Satisfaction, current mood, and cognitive performance. Transportation 44,

    1365–1382.Ye, R., Titheridge, H., 2017. Satisfaction with the commute: The role of travel mode choice, built environment and attitudes. Transp. Res. Part D: Transp. Environ. 52,

    535–547.Zhang, C., Juan, Z., Lu, W., Xiao, G., 2016. Do the organizational forms affect passenger satisfaction? Evidence from Chinese public transport service. Transp. Res. Part

    A: Policy Pract. 94, 129–148.Zhu, J., Fan, Y., 2018. Daily travel behavior and emotional well-being: Effects of trip mode, duration, purpose, and companionship. Transp. Res. Part A: Policy Pract.

    118, 360–373.

    L.K. Lades, et al. Transportation Research Part A 136 (2020) 318–333

    333

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0330http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0335http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0335http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0340http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0340http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0345http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0345http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0350http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0350http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0355http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0355http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0360http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0965-8564(20)30562-0/h0360

    Why is active travel more satisfying than motorized travel? Evidence from DublinIntroductionLiterature reviewMeasuring travel satisfactionDeterminants of travel satisfaction

    Data and methodologySettingSampleDescriptive statisticsAnalysis strategy

    Regression resultsTravel mode effectsInfluence of trip duration and trip distanceTime of the tripInfluence of subjective trip characteristicsEffects of demographicsTravel mode-specific effectsDistance from a bus stop, use of the real-time app, and parking difficultiesDeprivation and accessibility effects

    DiscussionContributions to the literatureImplications for transport policy-makingFurther research

    CRediT authorship contribution statementAcknowledgmentsSupplementary materialReferences