Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

25
Learning to escape robotic spiders: predator avoidance learning in bumblebees Tom Ings & Lars Chittka

Transcript of Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

Page 1: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

Learning to escape robotic spiders: predator

avoidance learning in bumblebees

Tom Ings & Lars Chittka

Page 2: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

Outline

• Challenges facing bees

– Finding food BUT avoiding predators

• Colour change in spiders

• Impact of crab spiders on bees

• Dynamics of predator avoidance

– Avoidance learning paradigm

– Predator crypsis, learning & memory

Page 3: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

Finding food

• Complex landscapes – too much choice!

Page 4: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

Avoiding predators - invertebratesSit-and-wait ambush predators

Page 5: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

Misumena vatia – colour change

Yellow White

Page 6: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

Aims

• Does predator crypsis disrupt avoidance learning?

• Quantify dynamics of predator avoidance learning – bumblebees

• Does the foraging behaviour of bees alter in response to predation risk?

Time

Avoid

ance Attack

Page 7: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

Effects of crab spiders on bees & plants

• Consumptive – reduce density & visitation

– Most (>90%) attacks unsuccessful (ample opportunity to learn)

• Non-consumptive – FEAR

– Avoid flowers with dead bees & spiders

– Avoid flower patches with high densities of spiders (sometimes)

• Lower seed set when spiders present

e.g. Dukas (2001), Gonçalves-Souza et al (2008)

Page 8: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

Avoidance learning paradigm

Meadow of artificial flowers

10 cm

100 cm

72 cm

72 cm

Page 9: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

Avoidance learning paradigm

“Safe” flowers

(trap inactive)

Trapping mechanism

“Dangerous”

flowers(active trap)

+

Life-sized 3D

spider model

“Robotic” spiders

Feeding

mechanism

Remotely

controlled

solenoid

Page 10: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

Simulated predation attempts

Page 11: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

Simulated predation attempts

2) Trap closed by

remote control

1) Bee lands to feed

3) Bee held for 2s

Page 12: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

1) Learning to avoid predators in a single flower species meadow

vs

Questions

• Does spider crypsis affect avoidance learning & memory?

Hypotheses

• Bees are less accurate when spiders are cryptic

• Avoidance of spiders is well maintained

Page 13: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

Design: avoidance learning

2 treatment groups

conspicuous

(16 bees)

cryptic

(16 bees)

Ings & Chittka (2008) Current Biology, 18

Page 14: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

Colour vision in bees

• 3 colour receptors

Adapted from: Chittka & Brockmann (2005) PLoS Biology, 3

Wavelength (nm)

UV Blue Green

Rela

tive

sensitiv

ity

Page 15: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

Colour differences: bee colour space

0.03 units

Blue

GreenUV

0.26 units

Bees can discriminate

0.1 units

Spider

Flower

Page 16: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

Design: avoidance learning

• 25% chance of attack

• Learning curves

• Flight behaviour (3D video tracking)

2 treatment groups

conspicuous

(16 bees)

cryptic

(16 bees)

Ings & Chittka (2008) Current Biology, 18

Page 17: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

Procedure

• pre-training (no spiders)

• learning (4 spiders & attacks)

• mid-term memory (spiders, no attacks)

• reinforcement (as training)

• overnight memory (spiders, no attacks)

Page 18: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

0.00

0.05

0.10

0.15

0.20

0.25

25 50 75 100 125 150 175 200

Flower visit

Pro

po

rtio

n d

an

ge

rous f

low

ers

Accuracy: probability of attack

RandomLearning

Page 19: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

0.00

0.05

0.10

0.15

0.20

0.25

25 50 75 100 125 150 175 200

Flower visit

Pro

po

rtio

n d

an

ge

rous f

low

ers

MM OR

Memory

Accuracy: probability of attack

Learning

Page 20: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

Flight data: rejection of flowers

• Bee enters flower

zone

• Inspects flower

• Does not land &

feed

• Time in flower zone

Flower zone

Feeding zone

Flower zone

Feeding zone

Page 21: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

0.4

0.5

0.6

0.7

0.8

0.9

1.0

1.1

1.2

25 50 75 100 125 150 175 200

Visit number

Du

ratio

n (

s)

Speed: rejection of dangerous flowers

Learning

Page 22: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

MM OR

0.4

0.5

0.6

0.7

0.8

0.9

1.0

1.1

1.2

25 50 75 100 125 150 175 200

Visit number

Du

ratio

n (

s)

Speed: rejection of dangerous flowers

Learning Memory

Page 23: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

‘False alarms’

P = 0.04

LearningN

um

be

r of

safe

flo

we

rs r

eje

cte

d MM(~15mins)

OM(~24hrs)

Page 24: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

Summary: speed-accuracy trade-off

• Bees rapidly learn to avoid conspicuous & cryptic spiders

• Bees do not make more mistakes when spiders are cryptic

• Rejecting dangerous flowers takes longer if spiders are cryptic

• Bees reject more safe flowers when spiders are cryptic

Page 25: Tom Ings & Lars Chittka - Queen Mary University of London

• Reduced foraging efficiency

• Reduced fitness

• Cryptic spiders have no advantage - why use camouflage?

• How does flight behaviour change in response to spiders?

Conclusions