Time Society 2007 Lindquist 253 85

34
http://tas.sagepub.com/ Time & Society http://tas.sagepub.com/content/16/2-3/253 The online version of this article can be found at: DOI: 10.1177/0961463X07080270 2007 16: 253 Time Society Jay D. Lindquist and Carol Kaufman-Scarborough development and validation Monochronic Tendency Model: PMTS scale -- The Polychronic Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com can be found at: Time & Society Additional services and information for http://tas.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Email Alerts: http://tas.sagepub.com/subscriptions Subscriptions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav Permissions: http://tas.sagepub.com/content/16/2-3/253.refs.html Citations: What is This? - Aug 16, 2007 Version of Record >> by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013 tas.sagepub.com Downloaded from

description

society time2007 lindquist

Transcript of Time Society 2007 Lindquist 253 85

  • http://tas.sagepub.com/Time & Society

    http://tas.sagepub.com/content/16/2-3/253The online version of this article can be found at:

    DOI: 10.1177/0961463X07080270 2007 16: 253Time Society

    Jay D. Lindquist and Carol Kaufman-Scarboroughdevelopment and validation

    Monochronic Tendency Model: PMTS scaleThe Polychronic

    Published by:

    http://www.sagepublications.com

    can be found at:Time & SocietyAdditional services and information for

    http://tas.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

    http://tas.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:

    http://tas.sagepub.com/content/16/2-3/253.refs.htmlCitations:

    What is This?

    - Aug 16, 2007Version of Record >>

    by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from by Mara Stan on November 1, 2013tas.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • The PolychronicMonochronicTendency Model

    PMTS scale development and validation

    Jay D. Lindquist and Carol Kaufman-Scarborough

    ABSTRACT. Polychronicity has traditionally been defined as a formof behavior wherein a person engages in two or more activities duringthe same block of time, while monochronicity occurs when a personengages in one activity at a time. These concepts have become increas-ingly relevant in discussions of time personality, worktime in thehome, and technological impacts on time. Other underlying dimen-sions of polychronicity also exist, such as preferences and feelingstowards whether to combine activities or not. In order to reflect thismore complex perspective, a validated, updated measure of a personspolychronicmonochronic overall tendency is needed. The presentstudy is a re-inquiry and extension of the Polychronic Attitude Index(PAI), introduced in 1991. In the current research plan we propose,develop, and validate a general five-item comprehensive reflectivesingle factor extended model. The general PolychronicMonochronicTendency Model is constructed using confirmatory factor analysis.The five-item summated scale from this model is named thePolychronicMonochronic Tendency Scale (PMTS). A series of fiveseparate studies were used to variously test for social desirabilityresponse bias, internal consistency, discriminant validity, and nomo-logical validity. The PAI is also compared statistically to PMTS withthe latter being clearly stronger. A discussion of the results andresearch implications are presented. KEY WORDS measurement monochronicity multiple simultaneous activities polychronicity polychronicmonochronic tendency

    Time & Society copyright 2007 SAGE (Los Angeles, London, New Delhi and Singapore)VOL. 16 No. 2/3 (2007), pp. 253285 0961-463X DOI: 10.1177/0961463X07080270

    www.sagepublications.com

  • Introduction

    In todays technology-driven society, individuals have many opportunities tocombine their marketplace, home, and work activities. Internet shopping, forinstance, can take place virtually anywhere while doing something else at thesame time. Personal computers allow individuals to bring their workplaces intotheir homes (Brocklehurst, 2001; Tietze and Musson, 2002), telecommutingwhile the clothes dryer is going and dinner is in the oven. Cell phones and per-sonal digital assistants (PDAs) enable us to access the Internet while shopping ata mall, taking a break at work, waiting in line at the supermarket, eating lunch ata restaurant, or having a suit marked for alteration by a tailor. Finally, informa-tion technologies have increased in the home (Silverstone 1993; Horning et al.,1999), allowing for many new activity combinations, such as recording a moviewhile watching another television show and spending time with ones family.As a result, the distinctions between time and space (location) are becomingmore blurred as individuals demand more products and services that allow themto be at work while at home, and vice versa.

    Despite the increased opportunities for polychronic behavior, prior studieshave found that not all persons are comfortable with simultaneous activities, andinstead prefer to do things one at a time (Kaufman et al., 1991a; Bluedorn et al.,1999; Cotte and Ratneshwar, 1999). Such persons are thought to be mono-chronic in their preferences for everyday activities, products, and workplaceculture. Recognizing this alternative tendency, there are many products and services that are aimed at those who typically prefer to do one activity at a time,such as reading a good book, using a table saw to cut wood for a fence, or play-ing a computer game that takes much focus and coordination. Some individualsshop with focus on finding one item at a time, or will not turn their cell phone onwhile grocery-shopping because they want no interruptions, or when they turnthe television on they watch the same network all evening or a program frombeginning to end without switching to another.

    In recent studies, polychronicity has become quite relevant in discussions oftime personality (Francis-Smythe and Robertson, 1999), worktime in the home(Tietze and Musson, 2002), and technological impacts on time (Silverstone,1993). It is frequently mentioned as an underlying construct in discussing anindividuals tendencies to combine activities, but its measurement has sufferedfrom incomplete development, situation-specific measures, and a lack of rigor-ous testing. We propose that other dimensions of polychronicity/monochronici-ty exist that are not consistently accounted for in models and scales. Rather thanbeing independent of a particular context, existing measurement scales havetended to remain discipline-specific and are often linked to specific situationssuch as the marketplace (PAI Kaufman et al., 1991a), the workplace (IPV Bluedorn et al., 1992; Bluedorn et al., 1999), and more recently, to personality

    254 TIME & SOCIETY 16(2/3)

  • in an integrative study of multiple time concepts (TPI Francis-Smythe andRobertson, 1999).

    In the present study, we propose, develop, and validate a five-item compre-hensive reflective single factor model. A reflective model is one where suchthings as the inherent (latent) tendency, position, or value structure of a personresults in or drives certain behaviors, positions or feelings. These results or outcomes should statistically co-vary. Hence measures of internal consistencyare appropriate. A reflective construct is alternatively called a latent construct.The alternative to a reflective model is an emergent or formative construct.Here certain behaviors or feelings are observed and are drivers of the con-struct. These formative indicators should not co-vary and measures of internalconsistency do not apply. We believe that a person has a general polychron-icmonochronic tendency. This results in the individual exhibiting behaviorsand feelings toward such behaviors in line with her or his tendency position.This is a reflective model or construct. The current study is an extension and re-inquiry of the Polychronic Attitude Index (PAI), the first published metricestablished to measure polychronicity. The construct that we name thePolychronicMonochronic Tendency Model is based on theory flowing out ofan extensive literature review and the use of exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis. A series of five separate studies were variously used to test forsocial desirability response bias, internal consistency reliability, and discrimi-nant validity with potentially similar scales from other disciplines, discriminantvalidity with potentially similar scales within individual marketplace behavior,and nomological validity. We draw on the rich heritage of multidisciplinary timestudies in order to build our approach.

    Polychronic and Monochronic Time Use Background: Theory, KeyFindings and Measurement Methods

    Polychronic and monochronic behavior were first conceptualized and named byanthropologist Edward T. Hall (1959). He studied a number of cultures invarious parts of the world and identified patterns of behavior that were commonamong them. He conceptualized time as a silent language that communicatedmeaning and ordered activities. He also observed that societies organized theirtime use monochronically or polychronically. This distinction is well known andis frequently used in comparative cultural analysis.

    According to Hall, Northern European and western cultures, which he calledlow-context, are thought to have a tendency to generally concentrate on oneactivity at a time. This he defined as monochronic time use. Among such cultures time is seen as comparable to money, in that it may be saved, bud-geted, and spent. In contrast, Hall postulated that polychronic time use is

    LINDQUIST & KAUFMAN-SCARBOROUGH: PMTS 255

  • found in high-context cultures such as exist in the Middle East, SouthernEurope, and Central and South America. In these countries, people (individuals)are more likely to engage in two or more activities during the same time block.Flexibility in time use and changes of activity are common and expected. Ablock of time can have many activities within it and people are expected to flowcomfortably from one activity to another. Hall also asserted that people within aculture preferred to behave more monochronically or more polychronically, astheir society dictated (Hall and Hall, 1987). More recently it was found thatpolychronic or monochronic tendency is independent of context (see Palmerand Schoorman, 1999).

    Economic models and time use accounting

    In attempting to understand the growth of industrialization in western nations,studies in home economics and sociology approached time as one of severaleconomic resources (Becker, 1973), implicitly emphasizing the monochronicperspective through their computations and analysis. Daily time use by indi-viduals was expected to sum to 24 hours (Robinson, 1977). Researchers werealso interested in determining how the most efficient output could be achievedas people used their time (for thorough reviews, see Juster and Stafford, 1991).Respondents reported hours and minutes that were subsequently analyzed interms of specific occupations or household demands.

    The data gathering approach in this period was primarily the time diarymethod. It allowed reports of individuals daily time use to be organized and tabulated (Robinson, 1985). It was described as a complete accounting of time.Study participants were instructed to log the time spent in all daily activitiesover a fixed period in days. Though not a focus of these studies, evidence ofpolychronic time use was present. Respondents were asked what they weredoing during certain bounded time periods. This was followed by questions tiedto the situation in which the respondent found himself or herself. People wereasked where they were, with whom they were, and whether they were doinganything else at the same time. These doing anything else activities providedevidence of polychronic behavior, but were arbitrarily called secondary activi-ties, since such activities were viewed as not requiring the primary attentionof the respondent (Walker and Woods, 1976). In the 19751976 Time AllocationStudy, Hill (1985) reports that the dominant forms of secondary activitiesincluded such things as work, housework, and shopping.

    Polychronicity as a hidden construct

    As noted earlier, the common practice was to code one activity as primary andanother as secondary, based on the level of attention given by the individual.

    256 TIME & SOCIETY 16(2/3)

  • The secondary or polychronic activities were regarded as a methodologicalproblem since time was bounded and could only be used for a single purpose(Juster and Stafford, 1991). However, researchers found that the total hours of aweeks activities reported by respondents in several studies frequently summedto more than 168 (7 days x 24 hours) (Robinson, 1985). Such discrepancies were often resolved by recoding the data into sequential monochronic (non-overlapping) activities of shorter duration.

    Some analysts cited concerns regarding this measurement problem in the timediary databases. For instance, Peskin (1982) noted that there was considerableuncertainty associated with the measurement and valuation of household work,since there were instances of joint production in household work. In anothercase, Hefferan (1982: 32) reported that the greater the demands on theemployed womans time, the more likely she is to dovetail household produc-tive activities, by sequencing parts of one activity with parts of another. A focuson primary activities alone masked the intensity of various activities, minimiz-ing the actual impact of polychronic time in peoples lives. Polychronicitybecame somewhat of a hidden construct, being found in the data, but elimi-nated through recoding or time budget accounting that forced the sum of dailyactivities to 24 hours a day and 168 hours a week. The next wave of time studies, however, began to bring such concepts into the forefront of discussion.

    Dissatisfied with the limitations of the fixed resource approach, consumerresearchers attempted to unify existing multidisciplinary time theories intoframeworks for research (for reviews, see Jacoby et al., 1976; Feldman andHornik, 1981; Holbrook and Lehmann, 1981; Hirschman, 1987). While theinfluence of time on behavior was well accepted, the subjective perceptions ofthe general use of time had not been sufficiently explored (Feldman and Hornik,1981; Hirschman, 1987; Bergadaa, 1990; Lehmann, 1999). Researchers beganto question whether how people feel about time affected the products and services that they used. Also, the subjective aspects of polychronic and mono-chronic time use were revealing men and womens perceptions, beliefs, feelings,and preferences as these behaviors were acted out (Kaufman et al., 1991b).

    Blurring the home and the workplace

    At the same time, interest grew in the interactions between peoples time athome and at work as several significant changes took place. Women entered theworkplace in greater numbers, workers began to have many more options fortheir workdays through flextime and weekend work schedules, and increasedtechnologies made it possible for more employees to work at home.

    LINDQUIST & KAUFMAN-SCARBOROUGH: PMTS 257

  • The impact of womens employmentFor instance, studies examining working women identified numerous strate-gies that were followed in their attempts to manage the demands of employment,family, marriage, marketplace behavior and their own personal interests.Specific strategies such as using convenience foods (Reilly, 1982) and time-saving durables such as household appliances were studied (Strober andWeinberg, 1980; Nickols and Fox, 1983). Freezers were sold and used as a wayto balance the pressures of work and home life (Shove and Southerton, 2000).The resulting evidence indicated that many working women were engaging inmultiple simultaneous activities throughout the day, including socializing withfamilies while shopping (Walker and Woods, 1976; Geerken and Gove, 1983;Nickols and Fox, 1983). Products and services were being sought that enabledwives to group activities together in ways that were comfortable to them. Whilenot formally described as polychronic time use, such strategies demonstratedthat many activities were being carried out within the same fixed clock blocks oftime. However, little information was gathered in the consumer studies areaindicating how the individuals felt about combining individual activities inthese ways or doing one activity at a time. For this perspective, we turn to studies in sociology, anthropology, and health (Adam, 1995).

    Flextime and weekend work schedulesOutside of the household, employment opportunities also changed to allow indi-viduals to work different shifts, potentially at times round the clock and onweekends. Such a broadening of worktime schedules caused some researchers tocarefully examine whether the elimination of collective times for rest andsocialization might instead create the need for a more pressured balancing acthaving troublesome social consequences (Garhammer, 1995). Researchersbegan to investigate how working at home affected peoples abilities to handleinterruptions throughout their worktime (Tietze and Musson, 2002), whenchores are intermingled with work tasks. Such a question provides an ideal context to examine individuals attitudes toward polychronic behavior.

    Although trades have been conducted at home for centuries, such as dress-making and farming, new opportunities have grown as more workplaces haveadopted the flexibility to allow employees to take some or all of their workresponsibilities into their homes. In fact, a substantial number of persons all over the world have responded by creating workspaces within their homes(Perin, 1998; Tietze and Musson, 2002). Some carry out manual jobs such asassembling piecework for an industry, while others may stuff envelopes, per-form child care, conduct computer analysis, and conduct home office work (fora detailed overview of homeworking activities around the globe, see Felsteadand Jewson, 2000).

    258 TIME & SOCIETY 16(2/3)

  • The time use of husbands and wives is significantly affected when either orboth spouses are in the workplace. They must juggle jobs, perhaps children, anda multitude of other demands that may present conflicting schedules. And, as theopportunities to carry ones work into ones home have increased, the bound-aries of time and space between the home and the outside world become blurredby the advances of modernity, as new times emerge through the forces ofcapitalism, the growth of industrialism, non-conventional organizational structures, and technological innovations (Friedland and Boden, 1994; Daly,1996).

    Technology and workspaces in the homeFinally, the introduction of information technologies both at work and at homehave blurred the distinctions between the two, as persons at home can work ontheir employment tasks via their home computers and Internet connections.Similarly, individuals at work can start and monitor various tasks at homethrough remote controls and stored memory functions. Thus, a meal might cookat home in a preset oven, while an individual at work emails themselves attach-ments to work on later in the day from their home office. These new tech-nologies increase the opportunities for individuals who so desire to actpolychronically and people in general may be nurtured by technology tobecome more polychronic in behavior. Also the culture itself, as it accepts tech-nological innovations, may become more predisposed to polychronic behavior.

    A research team from Ohio State University in the USA developed a series ofresearch projects aimed at examining the specific occasions of simultaneousmedia usage by individuals (Pilotta et al., 2004). That is, their research askedwhether persons who were using more than one medium in their homes at thesame time would be able to pay attention to several messages or actions at thesame time. Terming this occurrence media multitasking, they gathered datafrom a very large sample of US consumers to determine how widespread suchbehavior is. Their premise was that, given that people are limited to 24 hourseach day, and given that people have a number of new media available in theirwork and home environments, individuals would increase their simultaneoususe of media such as watching television while using ones laptop computer tocheck email and to surf the Web. While such a project would have been anotherideal laboratory for critical study of polychronicity, these researchers neitherdiscussed polychronicity nor did they attempt to see if there was a relationshipbetween a persons polychronicmonochronic tendency position and multiplesimultaneous media use. These findings open the door to a discussion of the possibility of situation-specific polychronicmonochronic tendency and thePMTS scale proposed in this article could be used as a measure in these contexts.

    LINDQUIST & KAUFMAN-SCARBOROUGH: PMTS 259

  • The development and testing of time use instruments

    In the late 1970s through the early 1990s, researchers in several disciplines pursued the development of scales to attempt to measure different aspects oftime perception and use (for a thorough review, see Francis-Smythe andRobertson, 1999). In psychology, the F-A-S-T Scale was constructed as a multi-item measure of four theoretical dimensions of what the authors named timeorientation. These were focus, activity, structure, and tenacity (Settle et al.,1972). Schriber and Gutek (1987) used principal components factor analysis toextract 13 usable scales known as the Time Dimensions Scales. Bond andFeather (1988) proposed and tested the Time Structure Questionnaire (TSQ) inorder to examine the structure and purpose in individuals use of time. TheTime Management Behavior Scale (TMBS), developed by Macan et al.(1990), was used to investigate the complex dynamics of time managementamong college students and in organizational settings (see also Macan, 1994,1996). While some statements in each of these scales dealt with activity sched-uling, interruptions, changes, planning, completion, and preferences, noneaddressed monochronic or polychronic behavior as specific topics.

    The emergence of the polychronicity construct and measurement

    Researchers in management (Bluedorn and Denhardt, 1988) and marketing(Gentry et al., 1990; Lane et al., 1989) argued the need for a set of measureswhose goal was the deliberate examination of polychronic and monochronictime use. With an emphasis on polychronic behavior, Kaufman et al. (1991a)developed and tested the PAI. This four-item one-dimensional scale was amongthe first published measures of polychronic time use. The original PAI scale isshown in Appendix 1.

    The value of the Cronbachs Alpha indicator of internal-consistency reli-ability for this measure was .68, a lower-than-desired value, yet acceptable inearly stages of scale development (Cronbach, 1951). A discriminant validitycheck with Reillys Role Overload scale yielded a correlation value of .15,demonstrating that the two scales were measuring different constructs (Reilly,1982). The negative sign of the correlation was as expected since high polychronic attitude position should result in a lesser feeling of being in roleoverload. Further, the chi-square analysis showed no significant differences ongender, age, income or marital status. The gender finding was especially inter-esting because the widely held belief that women in the USA were in generalmore polychronic than men was not supported by the data. Francis-Smythe andRobertson (1999) also found no statistically significant difference in their poly-chronicity scores for men and women. All things considered, the PAI was aninitial step in the quest for polychronicmonochronic tendency measurement.

    260 TIME & SOCIETY 16(2/3)

  • However, at the time of its construction this was not fully recognized by itsdevelopers.

    The PAI scale was then used in several marketplace, management, and com-puter science applications. Included were the banking industry (Bluedorn et al.,1992), Internet use (Mosley-Matchett, 1996), and technological development(Kaufman and Lane, 1997). Manrai and Manrai (1995) examined its applicationin cross-cultural contexts. In developing the Time Personality Indicator (TPI)one of the surviving polychronicity scale items was also from the PAI (Francis-Smythe and Robertson, 1999). Slocombe and Bluedorn (1997) included the PAIscale in their management context research, modifying it to fit work group situations and adding other items. The resulting scale, the Index of Polychronicvalues (IPV), is primarily focused on tasks, projects, and assignments in theworkplace with 7 of its 10 items containing such words (Bluedorn et al., 1999).It was specifically developed to measure polychronicity as a dimension oforganizational culture (Bluedorn et al., 1999: 207). It is therefore a situation-specific (workplace-group or workplace-individual) scale that appears to measure multitasking in the workplace rather than individual polychronicmonochronic tendency, a much broader construct. Hence the IPV cannot rightlybe classified as a general measure of polychronicmonochronic tendency.

    Finally, Francis-Smythe and Robertson (1999) hypothesized that individualscould be described by a multidimensional construct called a Time Personality,that includes behaviors, cognitions, and affect. After presenting a comprehen-sive review of time measures from multiple disciplines, the authors tested acombined set of existing time measures with newly constructed items, whichwere administered to several samples of participants and refined through factoranalysis. Polychronicity emerged as one of five final dimensions of TimePersonality, and was found to be represented by eight items accounting for fourpercent of the explained variance in their sample. Their findings appear to beconsistent with existing theory, given that high scorers . . . enjoy flippingbetween activities . . . and . . . low scorers prefer to concentrate on one activityat a time (Francis-Smythe and Robertson, 1999: 280). Unfortunately for students of polychronicity, Francis-Smythe and Robertson did not reveal specifically whether any of the surviving scale items were taken from the existing PAI or IPV. In addition, the validity checks were conducted againstother time-related scales, but the latter are less likely to be associated with polychronicity.

    Our work reported in this article focuses on the development and testing of ameasure of polychronicmonochronic tendency that is independent of specificdisciplines and that can be usefully applied in the home, workplace or market-place.

    LINDQUIST & KAUFMAN-SCARBOROUGH: PMTS 261

  • Goals of the present researchThe goals of the present research are to develop and validate a model of poly-chronicmonochronic tendency, extending the work of Kaufman et al. (1991a),Bluedorn et al. (1999), and Francis-Smythe and Robertson (1999). It is to be aconstruct that (1) more thoroughly reflects the multidisciplinary theory underly-ing polychronicmonochronic tendency, (2) is general in nature, removing thesituation specificity tied to the home or workplace found in other measures, (3) is not biased toward polychronic behavior, and (4) is statistically valid andreliable. A multiple study approach will be followed to do this.

    Problem Statement: Proposing a PolychronicMonochronic Tendency Model

    Conceptualizing a tendency towards polychronic/monochronic behaviorsAfter reviewing the time literature beginning with Hall (1959) and consultingrecent discussions on measurement model specification (Jarvis et al., 2003;MacKenzie, 2003), we propose to develop a more exacting reflective model ofpolychronicmonochronic tendency and its resultant measurement scale. Wehave named the model, the PolychronicMonochronic Tendency Model. Themeasurement scale is the PolychronicMonochronic Tendency Scale (PMTS).

    This approach follows Hall who named and defined certain behaviors atopposite ends of a spectrum/continuum as polychronic and monochronic behavior. He also stated that people within a culture would be predominantly atsome position on the spectrum and that this position was culturally learned. Healso asserted that people would have a preference to be at a specifically learnedposition. The idea of preference points to more than simply engaging in mono-or poly-behavior, it is a precursor to our concept of polychronicmonochronictendency. Hall also stated that all members of a society would not be at the sameplace on the polychronicmonochronic continuum, regardless of the averageposition preferred by the culture. For instance, even though the USA is con-sidered by Hall to be monochronic as an overall cultural characteristic, we knowthat some individuals are highly polychronic. Further, he speaks of polychronicand monochronic behaviors as being cultural markers. Most recently, he stated:

    In the strictest sense, a polychronic culture is a culture in which people value, andhence practice, engaging in several activities and events at the same time.Monochronic cultures are more linear in that people prefer to be engaged in onething at a time. (Hall, interviewed by Bluedorn, 1998: 110).

    The focus of our work is on developing a model of individual tendency towardeither monochronic or polychronic behavior and the degree of a persons

    262 TIME & SOCIETY 16(2/3)

  • positive feelings about his or her position on that continuum. No attempt is madeto favor or emphasize one end of the continuum over the other. Is poly-chronicmonochronic tendency an adaptive characteristic of an individual as wemight conclude from Halls cultural observations or is this a dispositional orinherent trait that we see acted out not in line with the norm? This is similar tothe running discussion as to whether a persons personality is based on nurture or nature. Our position is that this tendency inherently exists withineach of us even though the societies in which we live (family, reference groups,social classes, subcultures, culture) may have a preferred tendency. Thereforeour overall tendency will be to varying degrees internally and externally influ-enced.

    Besides behavioral measures, we propose that the tendency model should also include measures of: (1) preference for a type of behavior along the polychronicmonochronic continuum and positive/negative feelings measuresabout the preferred behavior of the individual, (2) liking to engage in multiplesimultaneous activities or doing one activity at a time, and (3) feeling comfort-able with this behavior as the outcomes of ones tendency.

    We propose to characterize polychronicmonochronic tendency as a reflec-tive construct (principal factor model, Jarvis et al., 2003). As discussed earlier,this construct is expected to include measures of an individuals preferredbehavior and feelings about polychronicity/monochronicity and what they perceive is right for them. Since this is a reflective construct, an individualsposition on each one of the measures would be a function of, or driven by, his orher actual polychronicmonochronic tendency position. Prior studies indicatethat some individuals have positive feelings about polychronic behavior andothers have positive feelings about monochronic behavior. That is, some peopleare comfortable doing only one thing at a time, while others enjoy jugglingactivities. Hence a persons position along the continuum from polychronic tomonochronic tendency determines his or her preferences. That is not to say thatthey always are able to follow their tendency preference, especially in employ-ment situations. If a person is required, against his or her desires, to behave polychronically or monochronically, such behavior would not be deemed to beappropriately in line with their tendency. Likewise an individual may have positive feelings about polychronic or monochronic behavior in certain situa-tions because it allows him or her to achieve preferred outcomes, but might notenjoy or feel comfortable with the experience. As in the workplace situation thisperson would not score as highly toward her or his true side of the tendencyscale. We feel that the consideration of the behavioral feelings and preferencesthat flow from a persons polychronicmonochronic tendency should be a vitalpart of the construct.

    An example of monochronic tendency behavior would be the individual whois watching television one evening and selects one network, watches only the

    LINDQUIST & KAUFMAN-SCARBOROUGH: PMTS 263

  • programs on that network including the embedded and between-program com-mercials and programming promos, does not even speak much to others, anduses the remote control only to turn the set on and off and select the channel. Heor she prefers to watch television this way being comfortable with this approach(Lindquist and Kaufman-Scarborough, 2000). Turning to behavior illustratingpolychronic tendency, we see the grocery shopper who is having photosprocessed, sushi made, and is calling mom on the cell phone concerning a greet-ing card purchase, all during the same clock block of time. Again he or sheprefers to grocery-shop this way, is comfortable doing so, and likes the jugglingof activities.

    Hall also postulated that these two ways of using time are culturally-learnedand that each should be recognized as representing actual time use patterns (Lee,1999). Again, he emphasized the cultural influence on time use patterns, stating:

    Monochronic (polychronic) time is arbitrary and imposed, that is, learned.Because it is so thoroughly learned and so thoroughly integrated into our [U.S.]culture, it is treated as though it were the only natural and logical way of organiz-ing life. Yet, it is not inherent in mans biological rhythms or his creative drives,nor is it existential in nature. (Hall, 1983: 45)

    Methodology

    The research program, following a re-inquiry and extension approach, (1) devel-ops and then evaluates a general measure of individual polychronicmono-chronic tendency based on PAI (Kaufman et al., 1991a) and the literature, andthen by way of nomological tests, (2) empirically further demonstrates its valid-ity, and (3) compares the model to the PAI.

    Five quantitative studies are reported here. Since we are proposing a reflec-tive model, tests of internal consistency will be used as one of the procedures toevaluate our model (Jarvis et al., 2003). The overall procedure followed is similar to that of Lastovicka et al. (1999) in their study of frugality. Our firststudy develops a reliable (replicable) measure of polychronicmonochronic tendency, including a check for social desirability response bias confounds. Thesecond is a model test in the general population and discriminant validity tests ofthe PMTS against existing potential parallel scales and the potential confound-ing of social desirability response bias. The third and fourth studies are discrim-inant validity tests against potentially similar individual consumer behaviorconstructs. This applications area was chosen because of the perceived future usefulness of the PMTS in examining the impact of tendency on such areas asconsumer shopping behavior, specific product and service selection, and con-sumer bundling of products and services. This dictates that in fact the PMTS isa true measure of polychronicmonochronic tendency and that other market-

    264 TIME & SOCIETY 16(2/3)

  • place measures do not properly measure this construct. The fifth study consistsof nomological checks of the model against reported behaviors and a compari-son of the PMTS with the PAI.

    Study 1: Model and scale development and checks for social desirabilityresponse bias

    The first action to be accomplished in Study 1 was the identification of potentialitems for polychronicmonochronic tendency determination. Fifty questions(statements) were selected based on the literature and tied to preferences, behav-ior, feelings and other statements that had potential to be part of a reflectivepolychronicmonochronic tendency construct (Settle et al., 1972; Bond andFeather, 1988; Kaufman et al., 1991a; Bluedorn et al., 1999; Cotte andRatneshwar, 1999; Kaufman-Scarborough and Lindquist, 1999a). Several itemswere either revised or newly developed by the authors in order to give equalemphasis to monochronically oriented statements. Examples include state-ments such as I get more things done each day by doing one activity at a timeand I almost always complete one activity before beginning another. Care wastaken to ensure that items were not situation-specific and they were phrased tomaximize their general applicability. For example, in most situations in my lifeI perform best under pressure is an item included in this first round of scaledevelopment. The items comprised the input list used for an item reduction procedure using exploratory factor analysis.

    The sample consisted of 265 adults who self-administered the instrument. Thesample was drawn using a convenience quota sampling procedure in the south west region of a Midwestern state in the USA. Data collectors personallydelivered and picked up the questionnaires. The quota conditions were that thesample was to be one-half male and one-half female with each of these groupsof adults 18 years of age and older to be equally divided into 18 to 44 years ofage and 45 years of age and older. The questionnaire contained 65 items. Fiftywere potential polychronicmonochronic tendency items. Since self-reportswere involved, the seven-item Strahan and Gerbasi Social Desirability ResponseBias X1 scale (1972) as revised by Fischer and Fick (1993) was included. Theremaining were demographic items. Seven-point Likert scales were used for allbut the demographic questions, ranging from 7 (strongly agree) to 1 (stronglydisagree). Only non-minority, US citizens were included in the sample to pre-vent potential results confounding based on subcultural or cultural backgroundinfluences.

    The data set was randomly split in half with 133 respondents for the modelgeneration analysis and 132 in the replication analysis sample. When doingstructural equation modeling, using a maximum-likelihood estimation approach,the recommended sample size range is 100 to 200 (Hair et al., 1998).

    LINDQUIST & KAUFMAN-SCARBOROUGH: PMTS 265

  • Model generation analysisData reduction was carried out on the randomly chosen sample of 133 respon-dents using a principal components unrotated factor solution approach. Theextraction was based on Eigenvalues of at least 1. The Scree Plot indicated aone-factor solution, as did the variances explained. The first factor accounted for42.4 percent of the variance. Twenty-seven of the variables loaded at .5 or aboveon the first factor.

    Model generation was based on theory and the data being analyzed (Joreskogand Sorbom, 1993). A confirmatory factor analysis was used to find the single-factor model that would fit the data well, be theoretically sound, and yield a parsimonious solution. A five-item good-fitting model was found using the SASProc Calis approach. Figure 1 is an illustration of the reflective PolychronicMonochronic Tendency Model that was found. Note that five indicators areshown. Hence Table 1 shows five items and, in column one, the standardizedfactor loading estimates and fit indices.

    266 TIME & SOCIETY 16(2/3)

    Poly-monobehavior preference

    Reported poly-monobehavior

    Comfort with poly-mono behavior

    Liking to juggle simultaneous activities

    Poly-mono behavior asmost efficient way touse time

    FIGURE 1The PolychronicMonochronic Tendency Model

    PolychronicMonochronic

    Tendency

    e1

    e2

    e3

    e4

    e5

    Note: There are five underlying indicators that are part of the polychronicmonchronic tendencyconstruct

  • Notice that all of the standardized factor loadings range from .82 to .90. TheGoodness of Fit Index (GFI), the Bentler Comparative Fit Index (CFI), andBentler and Bonnetts Nonnormed Fit Index (NNFI) are all above the .9 valueminimum (Bollen, 1989) and close to. 95 (Hu and Bentler, 1999). Further, thebounds of the root mean square error confidence interval are .00 and .09. Theroot mean square residual (RMR) was less than the .08 reasonable fit valueadvocated by Browne and Cudek (1993) and the root mean square error ofapproximation (RMSEA) less than the .06 suggested by Hu and Bentler (1999).Note that variance extracted by the model is 73.9 percent. This is very good and

    LINDQUIST & KAUFMAN-SCARBOROUGH: PMTS 267

    TABLE 1Study 1 and Study 2 maximum-likelihood estimates of a

    one-common-factor model

    Study 2: Study 1: Study 1: General

    Generation Validation Population Items Sample Sample Sample

    I prefer to do two or more activities at the .90 .88 .81same timeI typically do two or more activities at the .82 .88 .73same timeDoing two or more activities at the .85 .80 .85same time is the most efficient way to use my timeI am comfortable doing more than one activity .88 .82 .75at the same timeI like to juggle two or more activities at the .85 .85 .84same time

    n = 133 n = 132 n = 141Fit indicesGFI adjusted for d.f. .97 .90 .86RMR root mean square residual .04 .07 .10RMSEA .00 .10 .1490% confidence interval of root mean square .00, .09 .01, .17 .07, .21error of approx.Bentlers CFI 1.00 .99 .97Bentler and Bonnetts NNFI .99 .98 .96Chi-square, Pr with d.f. = 5 3.15, .68 11.24, .05 18.08, .003Variance extracted 73.9% 71.8% 63.1%Cronbachs alpha .93 .93 .90

    Note: Factor loading estimates: Lambdax

  • well above the 50 percent minimum recommended by Fornell and Larker(1981). Cronbachs alpha was .93, an excellent internal-consistency value. Thishigh level of intercorrelation is expected for a reflective model where indicatorsshould share a common theme (Jarvis et al., 2003). Chi-square was not statis-tically significant, a positive outcome, and none of the standardized residualsexceeded an absolute value of 2.58. The five items (the resultant PMTS) that aremeasures for the PolychronicMonochronic Tendency Model are also listed inAppendix 2. The model illustrates what we view as a theoretically stronger measure of polychronicmonochronic tendency than the PAI (Kaufman et al.,1991a).

    Since the PMTS is to be used to position individuals along the poly-chronicmonochronic continuum, the scale must be designed for replication inother situations and among other populations. This suggests the use of a summated scale is a better approach than a scale based on factor scores. Theneed for replicability over orthogonality was the key to this decision (Hair et al.,1998). There is a note of caution here in that a possibility exists that using a summated scale score to represent a reflective model may result in measurementerror effects (Jarvis et al., 2003). However, the authors feel that the strong theo-retical foundations of the model and the very consistent results of the statisticalanalyses and validity outcomes to this point strengthen the case for using a summated scale to represent the model.

    A check for social desirability response bias was also carried out. This is recommended when gathering self-reported information from individuals sothat there is assurance that the results are not confounded by such bias (Fischerand Fick, 1993). The correlation between the five-item PMTS (sum of itemscores, with the item relating to doing one activity at a time being reverse-scaled) and the revised Strahan and Gerbasi X1 seven-item scale was .06; a nocorrelation finding. Hence one can conclude that there is no evidence of asocially desirable response set operating.

    Replication analysisThe second half of the randomly split sample (n = 132) was used in an attemptto replicate the model. Hence only confirmatory factor analysis was used(Joreskog and Sorbom, 1993). The second column in Table 1 contains the resultsof the test on the structure found in the model generation analysis phase. Thestructure was replicable and well fitting, with the fit statistics slightly lower thanfor model generation. The social desirability response bias check yielded a correlation value with the PMTS of .12, a very small association. Social desirability response bias is not present in these data.

    268 TIME & SOCIETY 16(2/3)

  • Study 2: Tests of model structure in the general population, potentialresponse bias confounds, and preliminary discriminant validity

    Study 2 had multiple objectives. First, would the five-item PMTS structure fit asecond distinct data set? Next, will the answers provided by respondents beinfluenced by social desirability? Here we used both the Strahan and Gerbasirevised X1 scale (7-items) and the version 6 modified 37-item Paulhus SocialDesirability Response Bias Scale (BIDR) (Robinson et al., 1991). The doublecheck was seen as prudent to show that the results were truly not being con-founded by such bias. Finally, is the PMTS correlated with potentially similarscales, namely, Reillys Role Overload (Reilly, 1982) and Spence et al.s (1987)Type AB achievement striving and impatience/irritability personality scales?These are checks of discriminant validity. In a later study we will look at discriminant validity where the PMTS will be compared to potentially similarmeasures of individual marketplace behavior.

    Concerning role overload, Kaufman et al. (1991a) tested the assertion thatdemands placed by different roles may sometimes be satisfied within the sametime block to see if role overload measurement would suffice as a substitute forPAI. They found a statistically significant .15 correlation value between PAIand Reillys scale. This led to the conclusion that there was little associationbetween the two. It seemed prudent that we should run the same test.

    Conte et al. (1999) offered an analysis of polychronicity and Type AB per-sonality (TABP) behavior, suggesting a relationship. They used the achieve-ment striving (AS) and impatience/irritability (II) scales proposed in Spenceet al. (1987). These two subcomponents of the TABP measure are described asthe tendency to be active and to work hard at achieving ones goals (AS) andreflects intolerance, frustration, and obsession with time (II). The Conte et al.(1999) position was that polychronicity is likely related to AS and II. They suggested that AS people and II people might attempt to multitask in order toaccomplish more goals in the same amount of time. They used the IPV(Bluedorn et al., 1999) to represent polychronicity (recall it is our assertion thatIPV is indeed a multitask measure) and correlated this with the seven-item ASscale and the five-item II scale. The correlations were .17 and .09, respectively,indicating little correlation. The question is whether overall polychronicmono-chronic tendency, our broader construct, may be measuring something that issimilar to either AS or II. Hence a discriminant validity comparison of PMTS toAS and II is in order.

    The data set was gathered using the exact same method as for study 1 and con-sisted of 141 non-student adults. The instrument was self-administered andincluded the scales already mentioned plus demographic items.

    LINDQUIST & KAUFMAN-SCARBOROUGH: PMTS 269

  • PolychronicMonochronic Tendency Model testAttention should again be turned to Table 1. In column three find the summaryof the fit indices of the confirmatory factor analysis. We see that the indices arecomparable though slightly lower than those found in the study 1 generationsample model and the study 1 validation sample model. The general model ofpolychronic tendency is replicable. A z-analysis of the skewness and kurtosisvalues of the distribution of the PMTS scores revealed that the distribution wasnormal for this respondent sample. This finding points to being able to capturepeople all along the continuum, and that individuals may indeed be normallydistributed within a society even though cultures may vary in mean position.The mean score for the sample was 22.7 whereas the average of 5 (lowest possi-ble score by a respondent) and 35 (highest) would be 20. This shows that therespondent group was on average in the neutral position but toward the polychronic tendency side.

    Social desirability response bias confound checksTable 2 is a summary of the correlations among the PMTS and the other fivescales presented to the respondents. The Strahan and Gerbasi X1 revised socialdesirability response bias scale and the Paulhus social desirability response biasscale (BIDR) showed a .6 correlation with each other; this is in the large orstrong range (.5 to 1.0), as one would expect. However, the correlations ofthese two with the PMTS were .07 and .05, respectively, confirming that theresults found were not confounded by the respondents giving socially desirableanswers. This was in line with the study 1 bias check. The Cronbachs alpha values for the two bias scales are given in Table 2.

    Discriminant validity testsIs there evidence that measuring an individuals perception of role overload isanother way of measuring his or her polychronicmonochronic tendency?Recall that Kaufman et al. (1991a) also tested this potential match betweenReillys role overload measure and PAI. They found a correlation of .15(small). Referring to Table 2, we see the correlation between PMTS and theReilly scale is .11. The role overload measure is not the same as PMTS.Discriminant validity is demonstrated.

    Two subscales of a measure of type AB personality were also included amongthe discriminant validity checks. Table 2 shows the correlations between PMTSand AS and II. They were .29 and .12, respectively. These association levels aretoo small to be of consequence. Neither of the two measures would be appropri-ate substitutes for PMTS. Discriminant validity is demonstrated. Also note thatthe Cronbachs alpha for the PMTS was .90 and those of the other five scalesranged from .68 to .87.

    270 TIME & SOCIETY 16(2/3)

  • Studies 3 and 4: Discriminant validity tests against potentially similarindividual consumer behavior constructs

    Further checks of discriminant validity were conducted. Since the PAI was initially developed in a consumer behavior context, we attempted to determineif the PMTS is indeed measuring something different than other similar indi-vidual consumer behavior constructs. For this test, we chose four validatedscales that could potentially have behavior related to polychronicity and mono-chronicity, in their underlying switching and changing behaviors. For example,it is possible that persons with a polychronic tendency are more likely to bebrand switchers since polychronicity suggests greater activity changing andsimultaneous activity. That is, similar to changing between activities, perhapspolychronic persons also change frequently from one brand to another. Anotherrelated follow-up assumption would be that polychronicmonochronic tendencymight be indicated by measuring an individuals level of multiple to single brandloyalty or willingness to try new products, that is, a measure of innovativeness.Also, one would expect that the probability of variety seeking among activitiesis higher in the more polychronic persons life than in the more monochronicindividuals. Monochronic persons, who seem to prefer order and sticking to thesame activity within a given time period, might be more likely to stick with the

    LINDQUIST & KAUFMAN-SCARBOROUGH: PMTS 271

    TABLE 2Study 2 correlations among scales: discriminant validity checks and potential

    response bias confounds tests

    Strahanand Gerbasi Paulhus

    Polychronic Social SocialMonochronic Desirability Desirability Type AB Type AB

    Tendency Response Response Reilly Achievement Impatience Scale Bias Bias Role Striving Irritability

    (PMTS) (S & G) (BIDR) Overload (AS) (II)PMTS (.90)S & G .07 (.68)BIDR .05 .60 (.79)Reilly .11 .02 .14 (.87)AS .29 .05 .11 .21 (.74)II .12 .25 .17 .42 .29 (.68)

    Notes: Correlation of .21 is statistically significant at the .01-level (2-tailed). Correlations at .25and above are statistically significant at the .001-level (2-tailed). Sample size = 141. The numberson the diagonal in parentheses are the Cronbachs alphas for each scale.

  • same products and not be attracted by variety. Polychronic persons, on the otherhand, might also seek product or service variety in the marketplace or even buyon impulse, changing from their planned brand to something spontaneously.

    Discriminant validity testsA high level of correlation between one or more of the scales used to measurethe four constructs (brand switching, innovativeness, variety seeking, impulsebuying) and the PMTS would indicate that there is a good chance that theseexisting marketplace behavior scale(s) would suffice and that a scale to measurepolychronicmonochronic tendency does not add to our understanding. The fourconstructs and the measurement scales used in this study are: Rajus (1980)Innovativeness Scale (Spearman-Brown reliabilities .80, .85, two studies),Rajus (1980) Brand Switching Scale (Spearman-Brown reliabilities .78, .83,two studies), Donthu and Gillilands (1996) Variety Seeking Scale(Cronbachs alpha .87), and Rook and Fishers (1995) Impulse Buying Scale(Cronbachs alphas .88, .82, two studies). Though in some cases other scalesdesigned to measure the same marketplace behavior construct were found in theliterature, the four chosen are seen as representative and with good internal consistency reliability.

    Study 3 examines a known-groups populationIn conducting validity tests, researchers have frequently recruited persons whowere thought to possess the trait under investigation in order to examine howsuch persons score on the measurement scale being developed (Lastovicka et al.,1999). Ideally, the authors could have recruited polychronic or monochronicpersons via a subscription to a specific publication, participation in a specialinterest group, or membership in a chat room. However, we were unable to identify a readily available method. Instead we chose to distribute the self-administered questionnaires to persons who were in two situations in which thelikelihood for polychronic behavior, namely, combining activities, would bequite high. Families at a neighborhood swim club and families at a little leaguebaseball field during games were selected. Half of the questionnaires were completed at each location. The location of these two was a large metropolitanarea in the eastern part of the USA. The sample size was 201. A conveniencesampling method was used without gender and age quotas. Again a participanthad to be an adult US citizen. One hundred and one of the respondents were at aswim club and 100 were at a little league baseball field. The ratio of women tomen was 69 percent to 31 percent. Though not tested in the current study, thetwo to one female to male ratio results may be different than the one to oneresults found in all other studies for this project. The reason there were overtwice as many women as men is that more men declined to participate thanwomen and more women were available in the stands and on the grounds at the

    272 TIME & SOCIETY 16(2/3)

  • little league while more men were involved in the games. The age split also wasskewed in favor of 18- to 44-year-olds. Seventy percent of the sample was in thisage group, the remainder were 45 years of age and older. The age split in thissample may also affect the ability to compare the results with the other studies.This younger age sample dominance was not unexpected when one considerswho would more likely be involved in a swim club and in little league, that is,families with young to teenage children.

    The correlations between PMTS and each of the other four scales are found inTable 3. The correlations run from .04 to .14; these are small correlations. Theconclusion reached for the study 3 respondents is that PMTS is not perceived tobe the same as any one of the four individual marketplace behaviors based on thescales used. Hence discriminant validity is confirmed in all cases when PMTS iscompared to measures of innovativeness, brand switching, variety seeking, andimpulse buying. The Cronbachs alphas are shown on the diagonal of the table.The PMTS internal consistency coefficient is .93 and the other four coefficientsrange from .73 to .89. The four scales tested against the PMTS used items thatwere Likert-type, seven-position agreement scales identical to those for PTMSitems.

    Study 4 replicationThe data for study 4 were gathered using a convenience quota sampling methodidentical to that carried out for studies 1 and 2, but a year later. The sample was322 non-student adults. The instrument was self-administered and included thescales already mentioned for study 3 plus demographic items.

    LINDQUIST & KAUFMAN-SCARBOROUGH: PMTS 273

    TABLE 3Study 3 known-groups sample: PMTS compared to four

    consumer behaviour marketplace scales

    Brand Variety Impulse PMTS Innovativeness Switching Seeking Buying

    PMTS (.93)Innovativeness .14 (.73)Brand Switching .04 .59 (.73)Variety Seeking .04 .37 .36 (.83)Impulse Buying .12 .20 .18 .33 (.89)

    Notes: Correlation value of .17 is statistically significant at the .01-level (2-tailed) and correlationsat .18 and above are statistically significant at the .001-level (2-tailed). Sample size = 201. Thenumbers in parentheses on the diagonal are the Cronbachs alphas for each scale.

  • The results of the correlation and internal consistency analyses are found inTable 4. The correlations between PMTS and each of the four individual con-sumer behavior scales were higher for this sample than that used for study 3.They ranged from .12 to .30. The levels of correlation with the PTMS are stillsmall in all cases. Recall this was a balanced male/female and balanced agesample, whereas the study 3 sample was two to one female to male and 70 percent were 18- to 44-year-olds. This might have accounted for the differencesin correlation values along with sampling effects. The conclusion reached is thatthe respondents in this study perceived some scale associations, but very little.Hence discriminant validity in this second study is confirmed in each case whenPMTS is compared to measures of innovativeness, brand switching, varietyseeking, and impulse buying. The diagonal of the table lists the Cronbachsalpha for PMTS and the other four measures. The coefficient for PMTS is .93,as it was in study 3. The internal consistency coefficients for the four individualbehavior measures ranged from .78 to .93.

    Study 5: Nomological validity tests and concurrent model comparisonThe data were collected for study 5 using the same methodology and location asfor studies 1, 2 and 4. The sample size was 375, and the profile was balancedwith an equal split on gender and each of these two groups had half of therespondents 18 to 44 years of age and half 45 and older. Included in the instru-ment for this data collection were the four items that make up the PAI scale.

    274 TIME & SOCIETY 16(2/3)TABLE 4

    Study 4 Midwest sample: PMTS compared to four consumer behaviour marketplace scales

    Brand Variety Impulse PMTS Innovativeness Switching Seeking Buying

    PMTS (.93)Innovativeness .29 (.78)Brand Switching .22 .69 (.79)Variety Seeking .30 .47 .44 (.83)Impulse Buying .12 .34 .30 .34 (.93)

    Notes: Correlation value of .13 is statistically significant at the .01-level (2-tailed) and correlationsat .21 and above are statistically significant at the .001-level (2-tailed). Sample size = 322. Thenumbers in parentheses on the diagonal are the Cronbachs Alphas for each scale. All scales wereLikert-type seven-position agreement with 1 being strongly disagree and 7 being stronglyagree.

  • A preliminary check for social responsibility response bias using the Strahanand Gerbasi Scale showed a correlation between this scale and PMTS of .04(no correlation). Hence no answer bias was present.

    Nomological validity testsRespondents were asked to report the greatest number and the average numberof simultaneous activities they engaged in on a typical day. The overall meanvalues were 3.7 (greatest number) and 2.67 (average number). The correlationsbetween these quantities and the PMTS were .51 and .46, respectively, showingnomological validity tied to reported behavior. A third check of nomologicalvalidity was to ask people to report on the combining of activities and choresaround the house. The correlation with PMTS was .55, a positive nomologicaloutcome. Fourth, people were asked about whether they gave their undividedattention to the television when watching, a media-use check. Here the correla-tion was .33, as expected, since polychronic people have a tendency to switchchannels more, watch more programs simultaneously, and divide attentionbetween television viewing and other activities (Lindquist and Kaufman-Scarborough, 2000). All of the scales used in the nomological tests consisted ofLikert-type, seven-position agreement scales where 1 was strongly disagreeand 7 was strongly agree.

    Finally, respondents were asked about brands selection in general over thepast 12 months. Did they always buy the same brand, split purchases betweentwo brands, or three brands or more. The correlation coefficient was .11 in this

    LINDQUIST & KAUFMAN-SCARBOROUGH: PMTS 275

    TABLE 5Study 5 nomological validity checks correlations of

    PMTS with behavioral variables

    Scale/Item PMTS

    Greatest number of simultaneous daily activities .51(mean value 3.7 activities)Average number of simultaneous daily activities .46(mean value 2.67 activities)Combine activities and chores around the house .55Give television undivided attention .33Brand purchase habits .11

    Notes: All correlations at absolute value of .23 and above statistically significant at the .01-level(2-tailed). The .11 is statistically significant at the .05-level (2-tailed). Scales for all items wereseven-position Likert-type agreement with 1 being strongly disagree and 7 being stronglyagree.

  • case, showing small association. We concluded that there was evidence ofnomological validity among the findings related to reported behavior concern-ing number of simultaneous activities carried out on an average day, combiningof chores and other activities, and television viewing, but the brand purchasepattern data gathered do not work as a test of such validity.

    Concurrent model comparisonThe study 5 data for the two constructs, the PolychronicMonochronicTendency Model and the Polychronic Attitude Index were input to the ProcCalis CFA program for analysis. The results are summarized in Table 6. ThePolychronicMonochronic Tendency Model is the better of the two models.This conclusion is primarily based on a comparison of the standardized factorloadings of the models and the resultant variance extracted for this respondentpool. Degrees of freedom of the two model scales and internal-consistency reliability (Cronbachs alpha) also support this conclusion. All the factor load-ings were .80 and above and the average loading was .84 for the current model.In the case of the PAI only one factor loading exceeded .8 and the average acrossthe four items was .69. The PolychronicMonochronic Tendency Model yielded a value of 71.4 percent of variance extracted. The variance extracted byPAI was 49.3 percent, which is slightly less than the 50 percent recommendedminimum (Fornell and Larker, 1981). In general, the fit indices of both modelswere good. However, the Cronbachs alpha for the PAI was .76 versus .93 forthe PolychronicMonochronic Tendency Model. There is some concern aboutthe fit indices found for the PAI since the four-item scale had only two degreesof freedom (see chi-square statistics). Likely the fit numbers for PAI are toohigh. The standardized factor loadings and the resultant lower value of varianceextracted also confirm this possibility.

    An additional measure relating to the position along the polychronicmono-chronic tendency continuum being seen as the most efficient way to use onestime was found as part of the PolychronicMonochronic Tendency ReflectiveModel.

    Correlations were also run between the scales. These are reported in Table 7along with mean scale scores, standard deviations, and alpha values. The corre-lation of .75 is at the large level. This would be expected since two of the fourPAI items (liking or disliking to juggle several activities at the same time andcomfortable doing several things at the same time) in another form appear in thePMTS. Also, the two scales are designed to attempt to measure or positionpeople along the polychronicmonochronic tendency continuum. We are seeingconcurrent validity. However, the PMTS, based on its theoretical foundationsand CFA results is the strongest.

    Calculating the mean position on the polychronicmonochronic tendencycontinuum, with a lower score (from 1 to 7 for each item) being more mono-

    276 TIME & SOCIETY 16(2/3)

  • chronic and a higher score being more polychronic, we see: PMTS mean (23.6/5= 4.72), which is very near slightly agree more polychronic and the PAImean (4.33) about a third of the way from neutral to slightly agree moreneutral. These results also point to their measuring a similar construct.

    LINDQUIST & KAUFMAN-SCARBOROUGH: PMTS 277

    TABLE 6Study 5 Midwest sample: comparison of the PMT and PAI models

    Items Polychronic Polychronic The numbers given match the listings Monochronic Attitude Indexof the statements in the appendix Tendency Model (1991)1 .91 .882 .85 .563 .80 .584 .80 .745 .86

    n = 373 n = 372Fit indicesGFI adjusted for d.f. .91 .98RMR root mean square residual .06 .05RMSEA .11 .0590% confidence interval of root mean square .07, .15 .00, .12error of approx.Bentlers CFI .98 1.00Bentler and Bonnetts NNFI .98 .99Chi-square, Pr, d.f. 27.4, .0001, 5 3.53, .17, 2Variance extracted 71.4% 49.3%Cronbachs alpha .93 .76

    Note: Factor loading estimates: Lambdax

    TABLE 7Midwest sample: means, standard deviations, intercorrelations, and coefficients

    alpha reliabilities for PMTS and PAI

    No. of items Mean SD PMTS PAI

    PMTS 5 23.6 7.77 (.93)PAI 4 17.3 5.38 .75 (.76)

    Notes: All correlation values are statistically significant at the .001-level (2-tailed). Sample size =373. The numbers in parentheses on the diagonal are the Cronbachs alphas for each scale.

  • Discussion

    This reinquiry and extension of the foundational PAI model of polychronicmonochronic tendency was carried out using a five-study approach. The valida-tion process for the extended reflective model is comprehensive and rigorous.Further, the introduction of checks for social desirability response bias for eachstudy gives us greater faith in the self-reported data analyzed. The samples infour out of five studies were made up of adult, non-students who were screenedto include only non-minority US citizens. Further, half of the participants ofstudies 1, 2, 4, and 5 were male and half female and each of these gender poolswas split 5050 into age groups of 1844 years and 45 years and older. Study 3also consisted of qualified adults though the convenience sample was youngerand more female dominated than the other four. General discriminant validitychecks with role overload and Type A/B personality were carried out along withdiscriminant validity checks against marketplace behaviors (innovativeness,brand switching, variety seeking, and impulse buying). The results confirmedthe difference of PMTS from any of them.

    Comparisons were then made across the two models. We found the scaleswere related; yet the PMTS model proposed in this project was a significantimprovement over the foundational PAI model and can replace the PAI as thestandard for measuring general polychronicmonochronic tendency among non-minority adults in the USA. The research confirms that polychronicmono-chronic tendency is multi-faceted. One must measure: (1) preference to behavemore monochronically or more polychronically, (2) to what extent a personreports typically behaving as preferred, (3) whether they are comfortable behaving this way, (4) whether they like to juggle two or more activities at atime, and (5) whether they see behaving in their preferred way as the most efficient way to use their time. In sum, a reflective model of polychronicmono-chronic tendency is composed of five main indicators shown in Figure 1.Therefore a five-item scale (PTMS) is to be used to measure the construct (seeAppendix 2). These indicators are: (1) polychronicmonochronic behavior preference, (2) reported polychronicmonochronic behavior, (3) comfort withpolychronicmonochronic behavior, (4) liking to juggle simultaneous activities,and (5) polychronicmonochronic behavior as the most efficient way to use oftime.

    Limitations and opportunities within the present study

    While our approach has added to the polychronicitymonochronicity literature,there is considerable work yet to be done in the future. As in many studies, ourdata have limitations that are addressable in future rounds of research. First,while our samples were adequate for model development and validation,

    278 TIME & SOCIETY 16(2/3)

  • samples across groups on such bases as subcultures, gender, age, lifestyle, culture, and reference groups would be very interesting to study. There is also aneed to compare actual behaviors to those reported. Certainly this might beoperationally difficult to carry out, but would provide another sound nomologi-cal test.

    Appropriate regression analysis involving the PMTS as the independent vari-able and various types of general and specific behaviors as dependent variablescould be carried out to see what its predictive power is. Another question ofinterest is that when a person tries to act polychronically (or monochronically)or actually does, will the comfort and enjoyment items potentially revealwhether these are voluntary or forced behaviors? A tendency-position scorecloser to the side of the scale away from normal for that person could give anindication of this. On the other hand, a person may engage in non-preferredbehavior to achieve certain outcomes though she or he is neither comfortablewith nor enjoying the process. Again the tendency-position score would be awayfrom normal. Differentiating between causes for similar deviant scores isanother research challenge.

    Research implications

    Work and homeA validated measure of polychronicmonochronic tendency can provide us witha useful tool in trying to thoroughly examine how individuals balance their timebetween work and home. In addition, it would be useful to determine whether anindividuals own tendencies toward monochronicity or toward polychronicityactually match the time cultures found in their workplace and/or in theirhomes. For instance, suppose that a highly polychronic individual is employedby a company that is rigidly tied to schedules and sequences of activities. Theindividual might experience considerable job stress due to the mismatch. Itwould also be interesting to examine whether individuals develop a time culturein their homes that matches their own position along the polychronicmonochronic tendency continuum.

    Consumer researchBased on the combination of responsibilities and activities within specifichomes, future research can focus on the impact of polychronic and monochronictime styles on shopping for various types of products and services in the contextof the use occasion. Also, how is choice of product or service affected by a persons position the polychronicmonochronic tendency continuum? Potentialimpact on product and service design and positioning also calls for exploration.Are there viable individual market segments whose main distinguishing factor is

    LINDQUIST & KAUFMAN-SCARBOROUGH: PMTS 279

  • level of monochronic or polychronic tendency? Are individuals different or similar across cultures in these tendencies? Findings in these areas should proveuseful in addressing the marketing mix decisions of a firm or organization.

    Internet use and technology linking work and homeThere is no doubt that the Internet has become a widely used part of peopleslives, whether at home, at work, in the marketplace, while commuting, or else-where. How they use the Internet has been the subject of many studies (forinstance, see the special issue of the Journal of the Academy of MarketingScience 2002). One consistent finding is that people are often engaged in otheractivities, whether on or off their computers, when they are using the Internet.The PMTS might be used, for instance, to determine which persons are morelikely to shop on the Internet while they are at work. It would be interesting tolearn how they juggle their work and individual activities, whether their searchand evaluative processes are affected by switching back and forth to work activities, and whether they would make the same purchase in a work setting asthey would while shopping interactively at home.

    Salesperson selection and trainingOne intuitive assumption concerning a salespersons behavior is that a goodsalesperson is able to juggle many clients, incoming phone calls, and accountsall at the same time. Although it has not been formally studied in academicresearch, the relationship between polychronic tendency and successful personalselling can be examined using the PMTS scale. For instance, a firm with a salesforce could administer the PMTS to all of its salespeople. Salespeople could alsobe classified by managers as to their performance on the job. A rating scorecould be assigned based on performance position. Next, the correlation betweenperformance ratings and PMTS scores could be determined. If there is a strongpositive correlation, an examination of the actual PMTS scores could be usedand cut-off points determined based on job performance level. In the future asjob openings occurred, the PMTS would be administered to candidates and theirscores would be considered as part of the hiring decision. While many employ-ment advertisements mention the ability to juggle activities, it is doubtful thatpersonnel screening includes execution of a polychronicmonochronic tendency scale.

    Cross-cultural studiesFinally, an individual-level, situation-independent scale of polychronicmono-chronic tendencies would enable researchers to determine the extent to whichmembers of a specific culture match the overall cultural timestyle that is predominant. As noted earlier, anthropological researchers have identified andclassified numerous cultures, subcultures, and countries according to whether

    280 TIME & SOCIETY 16(2/3)

  • their members are primarily polychronic or monochronic. The PMTS wouldallow researchers to further examine the variation in tendency and the resultingpreferences, behaviors, feelings toward juggling, comfort with behaviors, andsense of efficiency when behaving polychronically or monochronically within aspecific cultural group in order to build in-depth understanding of time from across-cultural perspective.

    References

    Adam, B. (1995) Timewatch: The Social Analysis of Time. Cambridge: Polity Press.Becker, G. (1973) A Theory of Marriage: Part I, Journal of Political Economy

    81(JulyAug): 81346.Bergadaa, M. M. (1990) The Role of Time in the Action of the Consumer, Journal of

    Consumer Research 17(3): 289302.Bluedorn, A. C. (1998) An Interview with Anthropologist Edward T. Hall, Journal of

    Management Inquiry 7(June): 10915.Bluedorn, A. C. and Denhardt, R. B. (1988) Time and Organizations, Journal of

    Management 14(2): 299320.Bluedorn, A. C., Kalliath, T. J., Strube, M. J. and Martin, G. D. (1999) Polychronicity

    and the Inventory of Polychronic Values (IPV): The Development of an Instrument toMeasure a Fundamental Dimension of Organizational Culture, Journal ofManagerial Psychology 14(34): 20530.

    Bluedorn, A.C., Kaufman, C. J. and Lane, P. M. (1992) How Many Things Do You Liketo Do at Once?: An Introduction to Monochronic and Polychronic Time, TheAcademy of Management Executive 14(2): 1726.

    Bollen, K. A. (1989) Structural Equations with Latent Variables. New York: Wiley.Bond, M. J. and Feather, N. T. (1988) Some Correlates of Structure and Purpose in the

    Use of Time, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 55(2): 3219.Brocklehurst, M. (2001) Power, Identity, and New Technology Homework:

    Implications for New Forms of Organizing, Organization Studies 22(3): 44566.Browne, M. W. and Cudek, R. (1993) Alternative Ways of Assessing Model Fit, in

    K.A. Bollen and J. S. Long (eds) Testing Structural Equation Models, pp. 13661.Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Conte, J. M., Rizzuto, T. E. and Steiner, D. D. (1999) A Construct-Oriented Analysis ofIndividual-Level Polychronicity, Journal of Managerial Psychology 14(34):26987.

    Cotte, J. and Ratneshwar, S. (1999) Juggling and Hopping: What Does It Mean to WorkPolychronically?, Journal of Managerial Psychology 14(34): 184204.

    Cronbach, L. J. (1951) Coefficient Alpha and the Internal Structure of Tests,Psychometrica 16: 297334.

    Daly, K. J. (1996) Families & Time: Keeping Pace in a Hurried Culture. Thousand Oaks,CA: Sage.

    Donthu, N. and Gilliland, D. (1996) The Infomercial Shopper, Journal of AdvertisingResearch 36(2): 6976.

    Feldman, L. P. and Hornik, J. (1981) The Use of Time: An Integrated ConceptualModel, Journal of Consumer Research 7(March): 40717.

    LINDQUIST & KAUFMAN-SCARBOROUGH: PMTS 281

  • Felstead, A. and Jewson, N. (2000) In Work, at Home: Towards an Understanding ofHomeworking. London: Routledge.

    Fischer, D. G. and Fick, C. (1993) Measuring Social Desirability: Short Forms of theMarlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale, Educational & Psychological Measure-ment 53(2): 417724.

    Fornell, C. and Larker, D. F. (1981) Evaluating Structural Equation Models withUnobservable Variables and Measurement Error, Journal of Marketing Research18(February): 3950.

    Francis-Smythe, J. and Robertson, I. (1999) Time-related Individual Differences, Time& Society 8(2): 27392.

    Friedland, R. and Boden, D. (eds) (1994) NowHere: Space, Time, and Modernity.Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Garhammer, M. (1995) Changes in Working Hours in Germany: The Resulting Impacton Everyday Life, Time & Society 4(2): 167203.

    Geerken, M. and Gove, W. R. (1983) At Home and At Work: The Familys Allocation ofLabor. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Gentry, J. W., Ko, G. and Stoltman, J. J. (1990) Measures of Personal Time Orientation,in J. C. Chebat and V. Venkatesan (eds) Time and Consumer Behavior. Montreal:UQAM.

    Hair, J. F., Jr, Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L. and Black, W. C. (1998) Multivariate DataAnalysis, 5th edn. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Hall, E. T. (1959) The Silent Language. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.Hall, E. T. (1983) The Dance of Life: The Other Dimension of Time. Garden City, NY:

    Doubleday.Hall, E. T. and Hall, M. R. (1987) Hidden Differences: Doing Business with the

    Japanese. Garden City, NY: Anchor Press/Doubleday.Hefferan, C. (1982) New Methods for Studying Household Production, Family

    Economics Review 3: 303.Hill, M. S. (1985) Patterns of Time Use, in T. F. Juster and F. P. Stafford (eds) Time,

    Goods and Well-Being, pp. 13376. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research,University of Michigan.

    Hirschman, E. C. (1987) Theoretical Perspectives of Time Use: Implications forConsumer Behavior Research, Journal of Individual Research 2: 5181.

    Holbrook, M. B. and Lehmann, D. R. (1981) Allocating Discretionary Time: Comple-mentarity Among Activities, Journal of Consumer Research 7(March): 395406.

    Horning, K.-H., Ahrens, D. and Gerhard, A. (1999) Do Technologies Have Time?: NewPractices of Time and the Transformation of Communication Technologies, Time &Society 8: 293308.

    Hu, L. and Bentler, P. M. (1999) Cutoff Criteria for Fit Indexes in CovarianceStructure, Structural Equation Modeling 6(1): 155.

    Jacoby, J., Szybillo, G. J. and Berning, C. K. (1976) Time and Individual Behavior: AnInterdisciplinary Overview, Journal of Consumer Research 2(March): 32039.

    Jarvis, C. B., Mackenzie, S. B. and Podsakoff, P. M. (2003) A Critical Review ofConstruct Indicators and Measurement Model Misspecification in Marketing andIndividual Research, Journal of Consumer Research 30(2): 199218.

    Joreskog, K. G. and Sorbom, D. (1993) LISREL 8: Structural Equation Modeling withthe Simplis Command Language. Chicago, IL: Scientific Software International.

    Juster, F. T. and Stafford, F. P. (1991) The Allocation of Time: Empirical Findings,

    282 TIME & SOCIETY 16(2/3)

  • Behavioral Models, and Problems of Measurement, Journal of Economic Literature29(2): 471522.

    Kaufman, C. F. and Lane, P. M. (1997) Understanding Individual Information Needs:The Impact of Polychronic Time Use, Telematics and Informatics 14(2): 17384.

    Kaufman, C. F., Lane, P. M. and Lindquist, J. D. (1991a) Exploring More Than 24Hours a Day: A Preliminary Investigation of Polychronic Time Use, Journal ofConsumer Research 18(3): 392401.

    Kaufman, C. F., Lane, P. M. and Lindquist, J. D. (1991b) Time Congruity in theOrganization: A Proposed Quality-of-Life Framework, Journal of Business andPsychology 6(1): 79106.

    Kaufman-Scarborough, C. and Lindquist, J. D. (1999a) Time Management and Poly-chronicity: Comparisons, Contrasts, and Insights for the Workplace, Journal ofManagerial Psychology 14 (34): 288312.

    Kaufman-Scarborough, C. and Lindquist, J. D. (1999b) The Polychronic Attitude Index:Refinement and Preliminary Individual Marketplace Behavior Applications, in AnilMenon and Arun Sharma (eds) Marketing Theory and Applications, Vol. 10, pp. 1517.

    Lane, P. M., Kaufman, C. J. and Lindquist, J. D. (1989) More than 24 Hours a Day, inT. Childers (ed.) Marketing Theory and Practice, pp. 12330. Chicago, IL: AmericanMarketing Association.

    Lastovicka, J. L., Bettencourt, L. A., Hughner, R. S. and Kuntze, R. J. (1999) Lifestyleof the Tight and Frugal, Journal of Consumer Research 26(June): 8598.

    Lee, H. (1999) Time and Information Technology: Monochronicity, Polychronicity andTemporal Symmetry, European Journal of Information Systems 8: 1626.

    Lehmann, D. R. (1999) Introduction: Individual Behavior and Y2K, Journal ofMarketing 63(Special Issue): 1418.

    Lindquist, J. D. and Kaufman-Scarborough, C. (2000) Polychrons and Monochrons:How Do They Watch and Use Television, in Harlan E. Spotts and H. Lee Meadow(eds) Developments in Marketing Science, Vol. 23, pp. 2048. Coral Gables, FL:Academy of Marketing Science.

    Macan, T. A. (1994) Time Management: Test of a Process Model, Journal of AppliedPsychology 79(3): 38191.

    Macan, T. A. (1996) Time-Management Training: Effects on Time Behaviors,Attitudes, and Job Performance, The Journal of Psychology 130(3): 22936.

    Macan, T. A., Shahani, C., Dipboye, R. and Phillips, A. P. (1990) College Students TimeManagement: Correlations with Academic Performance and Stress, Journal ofEducational Psychology 82(4): 7608.

    MacKenzie, S. B. (2003) The Dangers of Poor Construct Conceptualization, Journal ofthe Academy of Marketing Science 31(3): 3236.

    Manrai, L. A. and Manrai, A. K. (1995) Effects of Cultural-Context, Gender, andAcculturation on Perceptions of Work versus Social/Leisure Time Usage, Journal ofBusiness Research 32: 11528.

    Mosley-Matchett, J. D. (1996) The Effects of Internet-Based Interactive Marketing onIndividual Impressions of Service Quality, in Proceedings of the AmericanMarketing Association Summer Educator Conference, pp. 5418.

    Nickols, S. Y. and Fox, K. D. (1983) Buying Time and Saving Time: Strategies forManaging Household Production, Journal of Consumer Research 10(September):197208.

    LINDQUIST & KAUFMAN-SCARBOROUGH: PMTS 283

  • Palmer, D. K. and Schoorman, F. D. (1999) Unpackaging the Multiple Aspects of Timein Polychronicity, Journal of Managerial Psychology 14(34): 32344.

    Perin, C. (1998) Work, Space, and Time on the Threshold of a New Century, in P. J.Jackson and J. M. van der Wielen (eds) Teleworking: International Perspectives, pp.4055. New York: Routledge.

    Peskin, J. (1982) Measuring Household Production for the GNP, Family EconomicsReview 3: 1625.

    Pilotta, J. J., Schultz, D. E., Drenik, G. and Rist, P. (2004) Simultaneous Media Usage:A Critical Consumer Orientation to Media Planning, Journal of ConsumerBehaviour 3(3): 28592.

    Raju, P. S. (1980) Optimum Stimulation Level: Its Relationship to Personality,Demographics, and Exploratory Behavior, Journal of Consumer Research7(December): 27282.

    Reilly, M. D. (1982) Working Wives and Convenience Consumption, Journal ofConsumer Research 8(March): 40718.

    Robinson, J. P. (1977) How Americans Use Time: A Social-Psychological Analysis ofEveryday Behavior. New York: Praeger.

    Robinson, J. P. (1985) The Validity and Reliability of Diaries versus Alternative TimeUse Measures, in F. T. Juster and F. P. Stafford (eds) Time, Goods, and Well-Being,pp. 3362. Survey Research Center, Institute for Social Research, University ofMichigan.

    Robinson, J. P., Shaver, P. R. and Wrightsman, L. S. (1991) Measures of Personality andSocial Psychological Attitudes, Vol. 1, pp. 3741. New York: Academic Press.

    Rook, D. W. and Fisher, R. J. (1995) Normative Influences on Impulsive BuyingBehavior, Journal of Consumer Research 22(December): 30513.

    Schriber, J. B. and Gutek, B. A. (1987) Some Time Dimensions of Work: Measurementof an Underlying Aspect of Organization Culture, Journal of Applied Psychology72(4): 64250.

    Settle, R. B., Alreck, P. and Glasheen, J. W. (1972) Individual Time Orientation andIndividual Life Style, in H. K. Hunt (ed.) Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 1,pp. 31519. Ann Arbor, MI: Association for Consumer Research.

    Shove, E. and Southerton, D. (2000) Defrosting the Freezer: From Novelty toConvenience, Journal of Material Culture 5(3): 30119.

    Silverstone, R. (1993) Time, Information, and Communication Technologies and theHousehold, Time & Society 2(3): 283311.

    Slocombe, T. E. and Bluedorn, A. C. (1997) Organizational Behavior Implications of theCongruence Between Preferred Polychronicity and Experienced Work-UnitPolychronicity, Journal of Organizational Behavior 20(1): 7599.

    Southerton, D. (2003) Squeezing Time: Allocating Practices, Coordinating Networks,and Scheduling Society, Time & Society 12(1): 525.

    Spence, J. L., Helmreich, R. L. and Pred, R. S. (1987) Impatience versus AchievementStrivings in the Type A Pattern: Differential Effects on Students Health andAcademic Achievement, Journal of Applied Psychology 72: 52228.

    Strober, M. H. and Weinberg, C. B. (1980) Strategies Used by Working andNonworking Wives to Reduce Time Pressures, Journal of Consumer Research6(March): 33848.

    Tietze, S. and Musson, G. (2002) When Work Meets Home: Temporal Flexibility asLived Experience, Time & Society 11(23): 31534.

    284 TIME & SOCIETY 16(2/3)

  • Walker, K. and Woods, M. E. (1976) Time Use: A Measure of Household Production ofFamily Goods and Services. Washington, DC: American Home EconomicsAssociation.

    APPENDIX 1Measures of PolychronicMonochronic Tendency:

    The PAI Scale (Kaufman et al., 1991)

    1. I do not like to juggle several activities at the same time. (r)2. People should not try to do many things at once. (r)3. When I sit down at my desk, I work on one pr