The Positive Side of Emotional Labor: Its Effect on Work ...burnout literature have started to shift...

48
The Positive Side of Emotional Labor: Its Effect on Work Engagement and Job Performance CHAN, How Weng Wynne A Thesis Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Philosophy in Industrial-Organizational Psychology The Chinese University of Hong Kong August 2009

Transcript of The Positive Side of Emotional Labor: Its Effect on Work ...burnout literature have started to shift...

  • The Positive Side of Emotional Labor: Its Effect on Work Engagement and Job

    Performance

    CHAN, How Weng Wynne

    A Thesis Submitted in Partial Fulfillment

    of the Requirements for the Degree of

    Master of Philosophy in

    Industrial-Organizational Psychology

    The Chinese University of Hong Kong

    August 2009

  • Thesis/ Assessment Committee

    Professor Darius K wan-shing Chan (Chair)

    Professor Winton Wing-tung Au (Thesis Supervisor)

    Professor Wai Chan (Committee Member)

    Professor Tahira M Probst (External Examiner)

  • Abstract of thesis entitled:

    The Positive Side of Emotional Labor: Its effect on Work Engagement and Job

    Performance

    Submitted by Chan How Weng Wynne

    for the degree of Master of Philosophy in Industrial-Organizational Psychology

    at The Chinese University of Hong Kong in June 2009

    Abstract

    Emotionallabor, the management of emotions in the workplace, has captured huge

    attention from researchers as well as organizations. The study in emotional labor

    addresses the effect from display rule on emotional regulation strategies and its

    outcomes. Surface acting and deep acting were the two widely researched emotional

    labor strategies used by employees to manipulate their emotions in order to show the

    prescribed emotions by organizations. Besides surface acting and deep acting, this

    current study included the expression of naturally felt emotions, which is an

    automatic process, as a third strategy. Following the upcoming trend of positive

    psychology, this study extended the knowledge of emotionallabor to examining

    work engagement and job performance as the consequences. Another feature of this

    study is the inclusion of third person ratings on the outcome variables which helped

    to reduce common method variances. Customer service representatives in a hotel in

    China participated in this study. Results showed that surface acting, deep acting, and

    the expression of naturally felt emotions were three distinct emotionallabor

    strategies that are available for employees. Moreover, deep acting and the expression

    of naturally felt emotions, which were predicted by empathy, were positively related

  • to work engagement which in turn led to better performance. Implications on

    employee selection and training, and future directions were also discussed.

    11

  • | | |

    摘要

    一直以來學術研究人員以及機構組織都看力研究情緒勞動(ernotional

    labor) 。 情緒勞動是在工作中需要到的情緒調節 。 情緒勞動研究著重於研究情

    緒表現規例 (display rules) 對情緒勞動策略 (ernotionallabor strategies) 及其後果

    的影響 。 過往研究著重於探討員工怎樣利用表面演繹 (surface acting) 和深層演

    繹 (deep acting) 來幫助他們造出公司所要求的情緒 。 本研究除了表面演繹和深

    層演繹之外還有調查員工自然感受的情緒表達 (the expression of naturally felt

    ernotions) 。 跟據近年來正向心理學(positive psychology) 的熱潮, 本研究把情

    緒勞動跟工作投入 (work engagernent) 和工作表現聯繫起來 。 另一特點是包括

    其他人的評分來量度後果的變數以減低共同方法偏差的影響 。 本研究的參與者

    由中國一所酒店的前線員工所組成 。 結果顯示表面演繹、深層演繹和自然感受

    的情緒表達是三個獨立的情緒勞動策略 。 同理心 (ernpathy) 是可以帶動員工表

    達自然感受的情緒而深層演繹及自然感受的情緒表達是可以提升員工的工作投

    入,從而提升工作表現 。 最後,本文亦提及本研究對機構組織和員工培訓及選

    拔的意義和未來研究方向 。

  • iv

    Table of Content

    Abstract .................................................................................. .

    ~~ ...................................................................................... 111

    Table of Content .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. IV

    List of Tables .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . v1

    List of Figures .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . VII

    CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 1

    Background. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1

    Display rule . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4

    Emotional labor strategies .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5

    Empathy......................................................................... 7

    Work engagement............................................................. 8

    CHAPTER 2. METHOD.............................................................. 13

    Participants and procedures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 3

    Measures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14

    Data analysis . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 16

    CHAPTER 3. RESULTS............................................................. . 18

  • V

    Descriptive Analyses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 18

    Structural Equation Modeling . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ... 21

    CHAPTER 4. DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ... . . . . . . . . . 23

    Imp] ications . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 25

    Limitations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . 28

    CHAPTER 5. CONCLUSION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29

    References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 30

    Appendix . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 8

  • vi

    List of Tables

    Table 1. Departments of participants .. ...... .......... ...... .. .......... ...... .. .. .. ...... ...... .... 14

    Table 2. The means, SDs, and bivariate correlations among the indicators .... 19

    Table 3. The correlations among measured variables....................................... 21

  • vii

    List of Figures

    Figure 1. Conceptual model summarizing the relations among constructs.......... 12

    Figure 2. Proposed model .... ... ... ... ....... ..... ... ... ... ... . ..... ...... ...... ... ... ... . .. ... ... ... ... ...... .. 22

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 1

    CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION

    Background

    Researchers and practitioners in organizational behavior and human resources

    management has become interested in the management of emotions in the workplace

    (Ashforth & Humphrey, 1993; Grandey, 2000; Wilk & Moynihan, 2005). Emotional

    labor is becoming one of the most important topics in the literature as people nowadays

    start to pay more attention to employees ' physical and psychological well-being which

    in turn affects organizational efficacy and effectiveness (Spector, 2006). Emotional labor

    is the process in which employees display certain emotions, that might be different from

    what they are actually experiencing, in response to their job expectations of showing the

    appropriate emotions at work especially at customer encounters in order to provide

    better services (Hochschi Id, 1983). The quality of service delivery leads to customer

    loyalty and retention as well as positive impact on profit and organizational

    effectiveness (Rust & Zahorik, 1993; Storbacka, Strandvik, & Gronroos, 1994 ). These

    are the reasons organizations are concerned with their employees ' emotion displays

    when interacting with customers.

    To have employees to show the appropriate emotions, organizations usually have

    norms for the appropriate emotional displays; these are called display rules (Ekman &

    Friesen, 1975). Most organizations have their employees display positive emotions

    especially for customer service industries. Employees ' reactions to display rules are

    different from each other. Display rules were said to cause inauthenticity and burnout to

    some employees but at the same time it is also related to self efficacy and job

    satisfaction in other employees (Brotheridge & Grandey, 2002; Brotheridge & Lee,

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 2

    2002; Diefendorff & Gosserand, 2003). These mix findings were due to the employment

    of different emotional regulation strategies (Grandey, 2000; Johnson & Spector, 2007).

    In order to conform to display rules, employees will involve in regulating their emotions

    by employing various strategies. Surface acting, the action that one simply put a smile

    on the face without changing the emotion inside, and deep acting, the action that one

    tries to change the deeply felt emotion, are the two emotional regulation strategies that

    had captured most of the research attention in the past while the expression of naturally

    felt emotions as a third regulation strategy has been neglected (Ashforth & Humphrey,

    1993; Diefendorff & Gosserand, 2003).

    Surface acting is always found to be positively related to stress (Grandey, 2000)

    and negatively associated with well-being while deep acting is found to be positively

    related to well-being (Gross & John, 2003). Other research had found that surface acting

    was positively related to burnout while deep acting was negatively related to burnout

    and positively related to service performance (Brotheridge & Grandey, 2002; Grandey,

    2003; Tottedell & Holman, 2003). It is obvious that the different choices of emotional

    regulation strategies lead to different outcomes. However, these previous studies focused

    only on surface acting and deep acting. The first purpose of this study is to confirm the

    existence of a third strategy and to examine the relationship between display rule

    perceptions and the emotional regulation strategies after including the expression of

    naturally felt emotions as this third neglected strategy.

    If this third strategy really exists, what is the precursor of the employment of this

    strategy? Individual who are capable of expressing their naturally felt emotions are

    expected to be able to understand and be sensitive to others ' emotions. Empathy is

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 3

    defined by Salovey and Mayer (1990) as the "ability to comprehend another' s feelings

    and to re-experience them oneself' . Do empathetic individuals have a higher ability and

    inclination to express their naturally felt emotions when they are asked to express the

    appropriate emotions in front of their customers? Another purpose of this study is to

    understand the reasons of making a choice to expressing naturally felt emotions over

    surface acting and deep acting.

    Previous research in the emotionallabor literature has focused more on negative

    outcomes such as emotional dissonance, emotional exhaustion and burnout (e.g.

    Grandey, 2003; Wilk & Moynihan, 2005 ; Cheung & Tang, 2007). Following the recent

    trend of positive psychology which focuses on human strength and well-being, the

    burnout literature have started to shift its attention to its opposite side to examining work

    engagement (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi , 2000; Schaufeli , Martinez-Pinto, Salanova,

    & Bakker, 2002). Despite a large number of studies addressing the relationship between

    emotionallabor and job satisfaction (e.g. Morris & Feldman, 1997; Grandey, Fisk, &

    Steiner, 2005), how emotional labor is related to other positive outcomes such as work

    engagement and employee job performance is still largely unclear. This study extended

    the knowledge of emotionallabor literature to work engagement and job performance as

    the consequences. Consistent with recent research on emotional labor (e.g. Brotheridge

    & Grandy, 2002; Diefendorff & Richard, 2003; Grandey, 2003), the present study also

    had individuals in the "people work" industry as the participants (Hochschild , 1983). In

    addition, while most of the previous emotional labor research was done in the United

    States, this present study extended our knowledge in understanding emotionallabor in

    the Chinese context by hiring a Chinese sample. Emotional labor could be manifested

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 4

    differently in the Chinese society due to cultural differences in the western and eastern

    countries (Bond, 1996)

    Display Rule

    Display rules are the rules set by the organization that employees have to comply

    with showing the kind of emotion expected and required to display when interacting

    with customers and at business encounters (Ekman, 1992; Rafaeli & Sutton, 1987). For

    most occupations, display rules involve expressing positive and suppressing negative

    emotions (Brotheridge & Grandey, 2002). It is conceptualized as role requirements as

    well as interpersonal job demands which help to attain other work outcomes (Rafaeli &

    Sutton, 1987; Ashforth & Humphrey, 1993; Diefendorff & Richard, 2003). Positive

    displays are usually considered as a part of the job duty especially in the service

    occupations (Brotheridge & Grandey, 2002, Diefendorff, Richard, & Croyle, 2006). For

    a car salesperson, in order to sell a certain number of cars in a month , being friendly and

    cheerful might be considered part of the job. Important customer outcomes such as

    intention to return, intention to advertise the company or a store to others and the

    impression of overall service quality are positively correlated with positive affective

    displays in service interactions (Parasuraman, Zeithaml, & Berry, 1985 ; Pugh, 2001).

    In order to comply to display rules, employees are often engaged in emotional

    regulation strategies to produce the affective displays needed for the job (Hochschild,

    1983). According to Diefendorff and Gosserand (2003), employees are involved in

    comparing one's emotional display with display rules constantly throughout the work

    day. When there is a discrepancy between the two, the employees engage in emotional

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 5

    regulation strategies to bring present or future emotional displays in line with the display

    rules set by the organizations. The more important the employees perceive the display

    rules to be, the higher level of acting and emotional regulation will take place (Grandey,

    2003).

    Emotional Labor Strategies

    It is often the case that employees are not feeling as positive as that is required

    by the organizations (Parkinson, 1991 ). They would then engage in acting to produce the

    needed affective display (Hochschild , 1983). There are different ways that individuals

    could manage or act their emotions to match with the ones required by the organizations.

    Surface acting and deep acting are the two most commonly discussed strategies for

    regulating emotions at work (e.g. Totterdell & Holman, 2003 ; Johnson & Spector,

    2007). Both surface acting and deep acting attempt to manage the displayed emotions,

    but they have different motives and intentions (Grandey, 2003). In surface acting,

    individuals try to hide or fake their felt emotions in response to display rules to fulfill

    job requirement, whereas in deep acting, individuals cognitively change their feeling in

    order to seem authentic. Surface acting usually involve faking positive emotions and

    suppressing negative emotions without changing their inner feelings while deep acting

    involves modifying feelings to match the required displays (Gosserand & Diefendorff,

    2005). Surface acting is called "faking in bad faith" and deep acting is called "faking in

    good faith" as the former involves pretending the fake emotion and the latter involves

    trying to experience the emotion authentically (Rafaeli & Sutton, 1987).

  • Emotional tabor and work engagement 6

    Ash forth and Humphrey ( 1993) and Diefendorff and Gosserand (2003) suggested

    that there is yet another emotional regulation strategy that is overlooked in the emotional

    labor literature. There are individuals who can naturally feel and experience the emotion

    required at work thus positive displays follow automatically. At first it seems it is similar

    to deep acting while both involve genuinely experiencing the required emotions.

    However, deep acting can be considered as minimizing negative emotions to bring one' s

    feeling in line with the display rules, whereas the expression of naturally felt emotions is

    to maximize positive emotions by automatically feeling the positive emotions at

    customer encounters. Employee who expresses their felt emotions naturally experiences

    the positive emotion that is required by the organizations without any conscious efforts.

    Diefendorff, Croyle, and Gosserand (2005) provided the first published research

    studying this third strategy. They confirmed a three-factor model structure indicating

    deep acting, surface acting and the expression of naturally felt emotions were three

    distinct constructs. The expression of naturally felt emotions was uniquely predicted by

    extraversion and agreeableness. They further suggested that expressing felt emotions

    was actually the most often used strategy by individuals in displaying emotions at work,

    thus this third strategy is worth paying attention to and it should not be overlooked.

    However, Austin, Dore, and O'Donovan (2008) suggested that surface acting and the

    expression of naturally felt emotions are only on the opposite side of the same

    continuum rather than two different independent emotional labor strategies available to

    employees. This study is going to clarify these contradictory findings by further testing

    the existence of this third emotional tabor strategy and the relationships between display

    rule perception and the three strategies.

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 7

    HI: Surface acting, deep acting and the expression of naturally felt emotions are

    three distinct emotional regulation strategies.

    H2: Display rule perception is positively related to (a) surface acting, (b) deep

    acting as well as (c) the expression of naturally felt emotions.

    Empathy

    Empathy is broadly defined as the ability to understand and relate to the

    cognitive as well as the affective experiences of another person (Worthington & Wade,

    1999). Berger and Thompson (2000) conceptualized it as an ability to examine another

    person ' s perception, feelings, and experience, and to communicate one' s understanding

    precisely to the other person. Empathy is considered as one of the important factors

    contributing to the success of emotional management, interpersonal popularity, as well

    as prosocial behavior (Eisenberg, Miller, Shell, McNalley, & Shea, 1991). Goldman,

    Boyatzis, and McKee (2002) further argued that empathy is "the sine qua non of all

    social effectiveness in working life" . Empathy helps to create the "effective resonance"

    and the "empathetic bond" between the customer service representatives and the

    customers (Kohut, 1987). When a customer service representative is high in the empathy

    domain, he or she is expected to be able to identify with the customers ' emotions and be

    able to feel and express the needed emotional display. For example, when a customer is

    enquiring sightseeing information, a customer service representative with high empathy

    is expected to be able to step in the shoe of the customer and understand what the

    customer is going through cognitively and affectively. As a result, the service

    representative should know what the customer needs and better services could then be

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 8

    provided. During the service encounter, the service agent's felt emotion is expected to be

    similar to the emotion that is needed to be displayed since both parties are expected to be

    experiencing the same emotions under the same context of enquiring sightseeing

    information. The ability to feel the customer's mood and express true emotions naturally

    may make it easier for the service agent to perform emotional labor.

    Humphrey (2006) suggested that empathy may be a personal characteristic that

    influences the way individuals react to the performance of emotional labor. His review

    paper called for the need to research on the influences empathy has on performing

    emotional labor. He proposed that individual who are high on empathy may be less

    likely to need to employ surface acting, as oppose to deep acting, in response to their

    work duty. Moreover, because these individuals are good at expressing empathy and

    recognizing customers' moods, they are more likely to be able to create a positive

    service atmospheres and interactions with customers. As a result, these individuals are

    more likely to simply express their naturally felt emotions.

    H3a: Empathy is negatively related to surface acting.

    H3b: Empathy is positively related to deep acting.

    H3c: Empathy is positively related to the expression of naturally felt emotions

    and the relationship is expected to be stronger than that of deep acting.

    Work engagement

    Beginning the year of 2000, the concept of positive psychology; the study of

    positive emotion, positive character, and positive institutions, has captured a lot of

    research attention (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). Work engagement is

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 9

    considered a positive experience in itself. It facilitates employee benefits from stressful

    work and more importantly, it is found to be positively related to employee physical

    well-being and positive work affect (Britt, Adler & Bartone, 2001; Rothbard, 2001;

    Schaufeli , Salanova, Gonzales-Roma & Bakker, 2002). It is expected that when

    employees are engaged, they are more likely to experience a positive perception about

    their work (Salanova, Agut, & Peir6, 2005). Work engagement, defined as a "persistent,

    positive affective motivational state of fulfillment", is considered as a relatively new

    area of research (Maslach, Schaufeli , & Leiter, 2001 ). Although this concept emerged

    from the burnout 1 iterature, and given the strong I ink between emotional labor and

    burnout, no one has examined the relationship between emotional tabor and work

    engagement. Work engagement is said to be characterized by three dimensions: vigor,

    dedication and absorption (Schaufeli , Salanova et al. , 2002). Vigor is described as the

    high level of energy and mental resilience at work, the individual with high vigor will be

    willing to invest effort in work even when facing difficult situations. Dedication refers

    to a sense of significance, enthusiasm, inspiration, pride and challenge. Absorption is

    characterized by being fully concentrated, happy and deeply engrossed in one ' s work,

    time passes quickly and there are difficulties when trying to detach from work.

    According to the conservation of resources model , people strive to maintain and

    build resources while stress is experienced when there is potential threat to their current

    resources (Hobfoll & Freedy, 1993). The energy and effort used to regulate one ' s

    emotions, either by surface acting or deep acting, to achieve work goal is considered as

    valuable resources to employees (Hobfoll , 1989; Wilk & Moynihan, 2005). Both

    organizational resources and personal resources were found to be the predictors of work

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 10

    engagement (Demerouti , Bakker, Nachreiner, & Schaufeli , 2001 ; Llorens, Schaufeli ,

    Bakker, & Salanova, 2007). In performing surface acting, high level of effort is needed

    to suppress the negative emotions, fake an unfelt emotion, and compare the expressed

    emotions with the true feelings constantly (Brotheridge & Lee, 2003). It is also effortful

    when one has to cope with the dissonance from the true emotions and the displayed

    emotions. The job demands-resources model also predicts that when there is a lack of

    resources, employees are less likely to meet job demands and as a result this leads to

    disengagement from work (Demerouti et al. , 2001 ). Although deep acting itself is also

    an effortful process, because a huge amount of energy is needed to involve in deeply

    changing one' s felt emotions, it is expected to be less detrimental than surface acting.

    Once the emotion is in line with the required emotion, no more energy or resources are

    needed in monitoring emotion or to cope with the emotional dissonance. Moreover,

    employees who engage in deep acting have the advantage of frequently experiencing

    good mood at work which in turn could enhance well-being, feelings of accomplishment

    and identification with work role (Rafaeli & Sutton, 1987; Brotheridge & Grandey,

    2002). It was found that deep acting is related to positive experiences and feelings which

    then lead to high level of job satisfaction (Kruml & Geddes, 2000). Employees who

    perform deep acting are expected to have a higher level of work engagement then those

    who choose to use surface acting.

    Employees who can naturally express their felt emotions have both the

    advantages. They automatically regulate their emotions in alignment with the prescribed

    emotions. This is considered an effortless process and at the same time they have the

    benefit of truly experiencing the positive emotions at work. It is predicted that the

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 11

    expression of naturally felt emotions would lead to work engagement. The impact would

    be higher and stronger than that from deep acting.

    Previous research had continuously shown that with a high level of work

    engagement, employees are more satisfied with their jobs, less likely to have

    absenteeism and turnover will be low, at the same time they will have higher

    organizational commitment and employee performance (Schaufeli , Martinez, et al. , 2002;

    Schaufeli & Bakker, 2004). However, little is known about the consequences of work

    engagement in the customer service context (Salanova, Agut, & Peir6, 2005). In this

    study, employee job performance was also measured to test the relationship between

    work engagement and job performance in the customer service context. Moreover,

    because coworkers are believed to have high degree of contact with the focal employees

    and they always have the chance to make observations of the employees' typical

    behavior (Murphy & Cleveland, 1995), work engagement and employee performance

    were both rated by the employees themselves as well as by their coworkers in order to

    provide more objective measurements on top of self-report ratings.

    H4a: Surface acting is negatively related to work engagement.

    H4b: Deep acting is positively related to work engagement.

    H4c: The expression of naturally felt emotions is positively related to work

    engagement and the relationship is expected to be stronger than that of deep

    acting and work engagement.

    H5: Work engagement is positively related to employee job performance.

    Figure 1 shows the conceptual model and the hypothesized relationships between

    the constructs.

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 12

    Figure 1. Conceptual model summarizing the relations among constructs.

    In sum, this present study extends the current knowledge in emotionallabor to

    including a third emotional labor strategy apart from the two widely researched

    strategies and examines the antecedent of this third strategy. In addition, this study also

    relates emotional labor, which was always linked with negative outcomes such as

    burnout and emotional exhaustion, to positive outcomes including work engagement and

    employee performance.

  • . Emotional labor and work engagement 13

    CHAPTER 2. METHOD

    Participants and procedures

    The sample consisted of 280 employees from one of the five star hotels located

    in the Guangdong Province in China. They are from 12 departments including restaurant

    waiters and waitresses, front desk service representatives, public relations etc. as

    summarized in Table 1. All of them have direct contact with customers every day when

    they are at work. Among the participants, 4 7 .5o/o were female and 52.5% were male.

    Those who aged below 20 consisted 23.1 o/o of the sample, 64.9% aged between 21 and

    30, 11.2% of them aged between 31 and 40 and 0.8% aged between 41 and 50. Most of

    them, 72.9%, had worked in the hotel for less than five years, 10.1% had worked for 5 to

    10 years, 5 .9o/o of them had worked for 16-20 years, 2. 1% of them had worked over 16

    years. Around 12% of the participants had finished middle school education, 46.3% had

    high school education, while 28.9% had a university degree. All participants participated

    in a half-day workshop on customer service and positive psychology. Participants

    completed the questionnaires before the workshop started. The trainers carefully

    explained the procedures and made sure participants understand and sign the consent

    form prior to filling out the questionnaires. Each employee filled out two sets of

    questionnaires; a 120~item self-report questionnaire and a 33-item questionnaire which

    needed them to rate on a peer colleague from the same department. All questionnaires

    were written in Simplified Chinese. The paring arrangement was randomly drawn and

    then checked by the human resources department to make sure the rater knows the ratee

    good enough to make fair ratings. Anonymity was achieved by using a pre-assigned

    research code instead of the name of the employees. Confidentiality was also ensured by

  • Emotionallabor and work engagement 14

    stressing that the questionnaires would be handled by the research team only and no

    personal identifiable information was collected. It was also emphasized that every

    employee who filled out a questionnaire would be eligible to enter a lucky draw as an

    incentive for filling out the questionnaires seriously. There were six winners receiving a

    cash prize ofRMB300 each. Excluding the 37 incomplete questionnaires there were in

    total 243 pairs of questionnaire in the final sample.

    Table 1. Departments of participants

    Department %age

    Public Relation and Sales 4.60

    Front Desk 14.49

    Room Service 12.73

    Chinese Restaurant 10.95

    Western Restaurant 13.07

    Chef in Western Restaurant 12.02

    Banquet Service 1.06

    Medical Center 1.41

    Entertainment and Spa 11.65

    Security 12.72

    Shuttle and Transportation Service 5.30

    Total 100.00

    Measures

    Display rule perception. Positive display rules and negative display rules were

    measured separately. Three items from Brotheridge and Grandey (2002) and one item

  • Emotional tabor and work engagement 15

    from Schaubroeck and Jones (2000) were used to measure positive display rules. A

    sample item is "Part of my job is to make the customer feel goocf'. One item from

    Brotheridge and Grandey (2002) and two items from Schaubroeck and Jones (2000)

    were used to measure negative display rules. A sample item is "I am expected to

    suppress my bad moods or negative reactions to customers". Participants answered on a

    6-point Likert scale ranging from I (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). The

    internal consistency reliability for the seven items was a= .70.

    Empathy. The 7-item Perspective-Taking (PT) sub-scale of the Interpersonal

    Reactivity Index (IRI) developed by Davis (1980) was used. Participants answered on a

    6-point Likert scale ranging from I (never) to 6 (always). A sample item is "J sometimes

    find it difficult to see things from the "other guy's" point of view". The internal

    consistency reliability for the seven items was a= .70.

    Emotional Labor. The 14-item scale used by Diefendorff, Croyle et al .. (2005)

    was employed. Participants answered on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to

    6 (always). For surface acting, a sample item is "!just pretend to have the emotions I

    need to display for my job ". For deep acting, a sample item is "J try to actually

    experience the emotions that I must show to customers". For the expression of naturally

    felt emotions, a sample item is "The emotions I express to customers are genuine ". The

    internal consistency reliability for the items were a= .82, a= .82, and a= .70 for surface

    acting, deep acting and naturally felt emotions respectively.

    Work engagement. The Utrecht Work Engagement Scale (UWES) developed by

    Schaufeli, Salanova et al. (2002) was used. This scale has three subscales. A sample

    item of the 6-item Vigor subscale is "I feel strong and vigorous in my work". A sample

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 16

    item of the 5-item Dedication subscale is "Jam enthusiastic about my job ". A sample

    item of the 6-item Absorption subscale is "J get carried away by my work ". Participants

    answered on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 7 (always). The internal

    consistency reliability for both self-reported rating and others-reported rating of the 17

    items were a = .92.

    Job performance. The 6-item scale used by Salanova, Agut, and Peir6 (2005)

    was used. This scale has two subscales. A sample item of the 3-item empathy subscale

    based on the SERVQUAL Empathy Scale (Parasuraman, Zeithaml, & Berry, 1998) was

    "Employees understand specific needs of customers". A sample item of the 3-item

    excellent job performance scale based on the Service Provider Performance Scale (Price,

    Arnould, & Tiemey, 1995) was "Employees "surprise " customers with their excellent

    service ". Participants answered on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly

    disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). The internal consistency reliability for the self-reported

    rating of the 17 items was a = .81 and a = .81 for others-reported rating.

    All scales, except for the UWES which was already available in Chinese, were

    translated into Chinese. Back translation method was used to ensure the consistency of

    the item meanings and it was done by a researcher who was not involved in this study.

    Data Analyses

    Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted using EQS6.1 to test the

    degree of fit of the measurement model and the existence of the three emotional

    regulation strategies. The CFA consisted of seven latent constructs including display

    rule perception, empathy, surface acting, deep acting, the expression of naturally felt

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 17

    emotions, work engagement, as well as employee performance. Item parceling technique

    was used to improve the sample size to variables ratio (Bandalos & Finney, 2001 ). This

    technique was used to form the latent structure for empathy, surface acting, deep acting,

    work engagement, and performance. For work engagement and performance, the

    indicators were created by averaging the corresponding self-report and other-reported

    scores. Exploratory factor analyses were conducted to assure the unidimensional ity of

    these scales. Item-level indicators were used to form the latent structure for the

    expression of naturally felt emotions because the scale has three items only. For display

    rule perceptions, the first four items were grouped under positive display rules and the

    other three items were grouped under negative display rules. Next, structural equation

    modeling (SEM) was used to examine the proposed model. Finally, Lagrange Multiplier

    test was used to test whether the positive relationship between empathy and the

    expression of naturally felt emotions is stronger than that of deep acting, and whether the

    positive relationship between the expression of naturally felt emotions and work

    engagement is stronger than that of deep acting.

  • Emotionallabor and work engagement 18

    CHAPTER 3. RESULTS

    Descriptive Analyses

    The means, standard deviations, and bivariate correlations of the indicators are

    presented in Table 2 and they ranged from .00 to .84.

  • Em

    otio

    nal

    labo

    r an

    d w

    ork

    enga

    gem

    ent

    19

    Tabl

    e 2.

    The

    mea

    ns,

    SDs,

    and

    biva

    riat

    e co

    rrel

    atio

    ns a

    mon

    g th

    e in

    dica

    tors

    Me

    an

    S

    D

    1 2

    3 4

    5 6

    7 8

    9 10

    11

    12

    13

    14

    15

    16

    17

    18

    1.

    SA

    1

    3.44

    1.

    22

    1.00

    2.

    SA

    2

    2.78

    1.

    36

    .58

    ** 1

    .00

    3.

    SA

    3

    3.09

    1.

    19

    .59

    **

    .60

    **

    1.00

    4.

    DA

    1

    4.01

    1.

    38

    .25

    **

    .16

    * .1

    1 1.

    00

    5.

    DA

    2

    4.35

    1.

    43

    .23

    **

    .14

    * . 2

    1 **

    .5

    9**

    1.

    00

    6.

    DA

    3

    4.24

    1.

    45

    .22

    **

    .21

    **

    .28

    **

    .50

    **

    . 59

    **

    1.00

    7.

    NFE

    1

    5.15

    1.

    06

    -.13

    * -.

    20**

    -.

    32**

    .2

    1 **

    .0

    9 .0

    1 1.

    00

    8.

    NFE

    2

    4.96

    1.

    17

    -.06

    -.

    10

    -.24

    **

    .05

    .06

    -.05

    .4

    8**

    1.

    00

    9.

    NFE

    3

    4.31

    1.

    25

    -.09

    -.

    16*

    -.32

    **

    .13

    * .0

    0 -.

    05

    .39

    **

    .46

    **

    1.00

    10.

    WE

    _AB

    3.

    73

    .62

    .05

    .09

    -.02

    .3

    4**

    .1

    5*

    . 16

    * .2

    5**

    . 1

    5*

    . 15

    * 1.

    00

    11.

    WE

    _DE

    3.

    88

    .73

    -.01

    .02

    -.18

    **

    .39

    **

    .16

    * . 1

    4*

    .36

    **

    .21

    **

    . 18

    **

    .78

    **

    1.00

    12.

    WE

    _VI

    3.68

    . 6

    2 .0

    4 .0

    8 -.

    05

    .36

    **

    . 14

    * . 1

    6*

    .28

    **

    . 15

    * .1

    3*

    .84

    **

    .80

    **

    1.00

    13

    .DR

    _P

    4.

    59

    .75

    .07

    .02

    .02

    .21

    **

    .17

    **

    . 15

    * .2

    4 .1

    3*

    .01

    .19

    **

    .20

    **

    .20

    **

    1.00

    14.

    DR

    _N

    3.8

    7 .9

    4 .2

    7**

    .2

    0**

    .3

    3**

    .0

    9 .1

    4*

    .13

    * -.

    15*

    -.03

    -.

    08

    .05

    -.0

    6

    .05

    .35

    **

    1.0

    0

    15.

    EP 1

    3.

    99

    .54

    .11

    .16

    * .0

    2 .2

    6**

    .2

    4**

    .1

    7**

    . 1

    6*

    . 12

    .04

    .48

    **

    .52

    **

    .53

    **

    .34

    **

    .17

    **

    1.00

    16.

    EP 2

    3.

    80

    .55

    .07

    .13

    * -.

    02

    .26

    **

    . 13

    * .1

    5*

    . 12

    .09

    .06

    .53

    **

    .53

    **

    .52

    **

    .24

    **

    .02

    .54

    **

    1.00

    17.

    EM

    1

    4.69

    .9

    5 .1

    5*

    .14

    * .0

    8 .2

    7**

    .2

    6**

    .2

    2**

    .2

    2**

    .2

    0**

    . 1

    5*

    .26

    **

    .26

    **

    .25

    **

    .22

    **

    .15

    * .2

    7**

    .2

    1 **

    1.

    00

    18.

    EM

    2

    4.33

    .9

    4 . 1

    1 .0

    7 -.

    00

    .18

    **

    .13

    * .0

    7 .1

    5*

    .16

    * .1

    4*

    .25

    **

    .24

    **

    .22

    **

    .19*

    * .1

    4*

    .25

    **

    .20

    **

    .46

    **

    1.00

    No

    te.*

    p<

    .05;

    **p

    <.O

    l. S

    A=

    sur

    face

    act

    ing

    ; DA

    =de

    ep a

    ctin

    g; N

    FE

    =na

    tura

    lly

    felt

    em

    otio

    ns; W

    E_

    AB

    =w

    ork

    enga

    gem

    ent,

    abso

    rpti

    on;

    WE

    _D

    E =

    wo

    rk

    enga

    gem

    ent,

    ded

    icat

    ion

    ; WE

    _ V

    I=

    wor

    k en

    gage

    men

    t, v

    igor

    ; DR

    _P

    =po

    siti

    ve d

    ispl

    ay r

    ule;

    DR

    -N =

    nega

    tive

    dis

    play

    rul

    e; E

    P =

    empl

    oyee

    per

    form

    ance

    ; EM

    =

    empa

    thy

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 20

    CFA results indicated that model fits the data satisfactorily. The chi-square value

    for the model was 210.24 (df=II4 ,p

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 21

    Table 3. The correlations among measured variables

    1 2 3 4 5 6 7

    l. The expression of 1.00 naturally felt emotions

    2. Surface acting -.38* 1.00

    3. Deep acting .10* .35* 1.00

    4. Empathy .39* .15* .39* 1.00

    5. Work engagement .34* -.01 * .31 * .40* 1.00 (averaged self and other)

    6. Display rule perceptions .04* .20* .34* .41* .24* 1.00

    7. Employee performance .22* .13* .36* .48* .77* .49* 1.00

    (averaged self and other)

    Structural Equation Modeling

    The proposed model was then tested by structural equation modeling (Figure 2).

    The chi-square value for the model was 237.06 (d.f=l2l , p

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 22

    also found that empathy undermined surface acting (/J=-.54, p

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 23

    CHAPTER 4: DISCUSSION

    This present study showed that the relationship between display rule perceptions

    and the three emotionallabor strategies could also be applied in the Chinese context.

    Gosserand and Diefendorff (2005) showed that the more committed employees are to

    display rules and the higher the level of perceived display rules are, the more the

    employees are to use more of surface and deep acting. The present study confirmed this

    relationship between display rule perception and the two emotional regulation strategies.

    In addition, this study linked display rule to a third , usually neglected strategy- the

    expression of naturally felt emotions. Specifically, the stronger display rule perceptions

    the employees have, the less they are able to express their naturally felt emotions. These

    findings further supported that surface acting, deep acting, and the expression of

    naturally felt emotions are three distinct independent emotional regulation strategies

    available to employees. This three factor structure of emotional labor strategies is

    consistent with what was found in Diefendorff, Croyle, and Gosserand ' s (2005) study in

    which they showed that dispositional variables such as extraversion and agreeableness

    could uniquely and strongly predict employees ' display of naturally felt emotions. This

    current study added to the findings that empathy is yet another predictor of the

    employees displaying naturally felt emotions at work. The higher level of empathy the

    employees have, the more they are to express their naturally felt emotions at work and

    more importantly, they are less 1 ikely to employ surface acting.

    Emotional labor had always been 1 inked up with negative outcomes such as work

    stress and emotional exhaustion in previous studies (e.g. Hochschild , 1983, Grandy,

    2003; Cheung & Tang, 2007). The present study extended our knowledge in

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 24

    understanding the positive side of emotional labor and showed us that emotional labor

    could also relate to positive outcomes such as work engagement and higher employee

    performance. Employees performing deep acting and expressing their naturally felt

    emotions at work indeed were more engaged in work which in turn could lead to better

    performance. In line with previous finding that surface acting was always found to be

    related to emotional exhaustion (e.g. Grandey, 2003), this study found that, among the

    three strategies, only surface acting was not related to work engagement. Although the

    expression of naturally felt emotions was not proven to be a stronger predictor of work

    engagement when compared to deep acting, both strategies correlated significantly with

    work engagement. According to Totterdell and Holman (2003), deep acting was more

    positively related to service performance than surface acting. This present study was not

    able to replicate the result and indeed found that the three emotional strategies were not

    directly related to performance. Work engagement mediated the effect of deep acting

    and the expression of naturally felt emotions to employee performance. The paths

    between the three emotional strategies and employee performance were also tested but

    none of them were found to predict performance directly. This further confirmed that the

    positive effect of deep acting and the expression of naturally felt emotions on

    performance were fully mediated by work engagement. More research effort is needed

    to better understand this inconsistency on the relationship between emotional labor

    strategies and service performance.

    Another feature ofthis study is the inclusion of third person ratings on the

    outcome variables (i.e. , work engagement and performance) thus minimizing the

    problem brought about by common method variance. Cheung and Tang (2007) found

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 25

    that after controlling for common method variance on examining the effect of emotional

    dissonance on burnout, the relationship became insignificant. Thus, this study had taken

    one step further to also measure ratings on the outcome variables from the focal

    employees ' coworkers. By including a secondary source of information on the outcome

    variables, we are more confident in interpreting the results found in this study. That is

    the positive effect of deep acting and the expression of naturally felt emotions on

    employee work engagement which in turn led to better performance.

    Given the closely link between emotional intelligence and emotionallabor, this

    study had originally included emotional intelligence as one of the variables in the model.

    Austin et al. (2008) have shown that personalities and the level of emotional intelligence

    of an individual would affect his or her choice of emotional regulation strategies.

    Emotional intelligence is the ability to manage or control emotions thus it is obvious that

    emotional labor and emotional intelligence is highly relevant. Despite the importance of

    such construct .in emotional labor, little research has examined its effect in the emotional

    labor context (Grandey, 2000). Emotionallabor was included in the model as a

    moderator moderating the relationships among the antecedents, the consequences, as

    well as the three emotional strategies. However, no moderation effect was found and this

    is why emotional intelligence has been taken out from the above analysis and discussion.

    Future research should find out where emotional labor stands or how it could be fitted

    into the emotional labor context.

    Implications

    Organizations should be careful on implementing display rules on their

    employees. Display rule perceptions were much closely related to surface acting than to

    deep acting, and it was negatively related to the expression of naturally felt emotions.

  • Emotional tabor and work engagement 26

    Due to the positive relationship of deep acting and the expression of naturally felt

    emotions on work engagement, when display rules are needed, organizations should

    encourage their employees to perform deep acting. Training workshops on how to

    manipulate one's inner feelings more effectively could be used to help employees to

    better manage their emotions from the inside. Other techniques such as reframing and

    re-appraisal could also be introduced to employees. Through these various workshops,

    employees will be more able to experience the positive emotions needed for their job

    and as a result employees will be more able to employ deep acting or the expression of

    naturally felt emotions. In addition, organizations could also consider building a happy,

    cheerful and enjoyable environment for their employees so the employees are able to

    show their genuine emotions while working comfortably and happily. In this case,

    employees will be able to display the appropriate emotions at work even without the

    implementation of display rules. The advantage of employees expressing their naturally

    felt emotions calls for further research on finding out other antecedents of this third

    strategy so as to maximize the chance that employees could show true emotions at work

    instead of acting or faking an emotion.

    Apart from display rule, organizations could also pay attention on employees '

    level of empathy. Empathy is not only found to be a strong precursor of expressing

    naturally felt emotion, which could lead to higher level of work engagement, it also

    decreases the performance of surface acting, which is widely accepted to be detrimental

    to the employees ' well-being both emotionally and physically (e.g. Morris & Feldman,

    1997; Abraham, 1998). According to Duan and Hill (1996) and Worthington & Wade

    ( 1999), empathy could be seen as a dispositional factor as well as an ability which could

  • Emotional tabor and work engagement 27

    be learned and trained. It is worth for organizations to invest in training their employees

    on how to be more empathetic. In addition, other personality variables such as

    extraversions were also found to predict the expression of naturally felt emotions

    (Diefendorff, Croyle, and Gosserand, 2005). A personality test measuring these

    personalities or the potential to be empathetic could be added to the recruitment

    screening process in order to identify the suitable employees who are better at

    expressing their natural emotions at work. Given that empathy is strongly related to the

    expression of naturally felt emotions, which is considered to be the best emotional labor

    strategies among the three, future study could also look at what other direct

    consequences empathy could bring in the "people work" industry such as whether

    individuals with high level of empathy are better at handling customers.

    The expression of naturally felt emotions and deep acting could lead to higher

    level of work engagement and work engagement was found to be a mediator between

    the two strategies and employee performance. In order to have employees better perform

    at work, increasing their work engagement level is essential. Work engagement is

    positively related to a wide range of positive consequences at both the individual and

    organizational level. Previous studies have found that work engagement is positively

    related to teachers ' organizational commitment (Schaufeli & Bakker, 2004; Hakanen,

    Bakker, & Schaufeli, 2006), employee performance (Kahn, 1990), and customer

    satisfaction and loyalty, productivities, and profitability etc. (Harter, Schmidt, & Hayes,

    2002). Thus, it is very important to encourage employees to express their naturally felt

    emotions. If not possible, deep acting is a better solution over surface acting.

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 28

    Limitations

    This study, like most of the previous studies, has a number of limitations. First,

    although supervisors also filled out a part of the questionnaires, the emotional regulation

    strategies and some other variables were still measured by self-report. Self report

    measure might be subject to social desirability bias thus exaggerating some of the

    results. Although measures were taken to ensure anonymity, the questionnaires were still

    filled out during a training workshop which was organized by the hotel and with the

    presence of other peer colleagues or supervisors, participants might have answered in a

    way that they did not disappoint their supervisors. Future research could consider also

    collecting other objective data such as absenteeism or sales volume as the measurements

    of work engagement and performance. Second, with cross sectional design causality

    could not be inferred. Interpretations should be made with caution and longitudinal

    studies could be carried out to investigate the relationship between the variables over

    time. Third, this study was done with people from one single organization;

    generalization to other area can be achieved only if this study is replicated in some other

    occupations.

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 29

    CHAPTER 5. CONCLUSION

    The present study examined the existence of the three factor structure of

    emotional labor strategies as well as relating positive outcomes with emotional labor in

    the Chinese service representative context. It was found that the three emotional labor

    strategies were three distinctive strategies that are available for employees to respond to

    display rules and job duties. Deep acting and the expression of naturally felt emotions

    were found to be able to lead to work engagement, while work engagement was not

    predicted by surface acting. Moreover, empathy was found to be a strong predictor of

    expressing naturally felt emotions. Given the possible effect of this third strategy on

    positive work outcomes, more research effort should be paid to examining the

    antecedents as well as other consequences that are related to this third strategy.

  • Emotionallabor and work engagement 30

    References

    Abraham, R. (1998). Emotional dissonance in organizations: Antecedents, consequences,

    and moderators. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 124,

    229-246.

    Austin, E. J. , Dore, T. C. P., & O'Donovan, K. M. (2008). Associations of personality

    and emotional intelligence with display rule perceptions and emotional labour.

    Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 679-688.

    Ashforth, B. E., & Humphrey, R. H. (1993). Emotionallabor in service roles: The

    influence of identity. Academy of Management Review, 18, 88-115.

    Bandalos, D. L. , & Finney, S. J. (200 1). Item parceling issues in structural equation

    modeling. In G. A. Marcoulides & R. E. Schumacker (Eds.) , Advanced structural

    equation modeling: New developments and techniques. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence

    Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Berger, K. S., & Thompson, R. A. (2000). The developing person through childhood and

    adolescence. New York: Worth.

    Bond, M. H., ( 1996). The Handbook of Chinese Psychology. Hong Kong: Oxford

    University Press.

    Britt, T. W. , Adler, A. B. , & Bartone, P. T. (2001). Deriving benefits from stressful

    events: The role of engagement in meaningful work and hardiness. Journal of

    Occupational Health Psychology, 6, 53-63.

    Brotheridge, C. M., & Grandey, A. A. (2002). Emotional labor and burnout: Comparing

    two perspectives of"people work". Journal of Vocational Behavior, 60, 17-39.

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 31

    Brotheridge, C. M., & Lee, R . T. (2002). Testing a conservation of resources model of

    the dynamics of emotional labor. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 7,

    57-67.

    Brotheridge, C. M. , & Lee, R. T. (2003). Development and validation of the Emotional

    Labour Scale. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 76, 365-

    379.

    Cheung, F. , & Tang, C. (2007). The influence of emotional dissonance and resources at

    work on job burnout among Chinese human service employees. International

    Journal of Stress Management, 14, 72-87.

    Davis, M. H. (1980). A multi-dimensional approach to individual differences in empathy.

    JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85.

    Demerouti, E. , Bakker, A. B., Nachreiner, F. , & Schaufeli, W. B . (2001). The job

    demands-resources model of burnout. Journal of Management, 31 , 126-152.

    Diefendorff, J. M., & Gosserand, R. H. (2003). Understanding the emotional tabor

    process: A control theory perspective. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 24,

    945-959.

    Diefendorff, J. M., Croyle, M. H., & Gosserand, R. H. (2005). The dimensionality and

    antecedents of emotional labor strategies. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 66,

    339-357.

    Diefendorff, J. , & Richard, E. (2003). Antecedents and consequences of emotional

    display rule perception. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 284-294.

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 32

    Diefendorff, J. M., Richard, E. M., & Croyle, M. H. (2006). Are emotional display rules

    formal job requirements? Examination of employee and supervisor perceptions.

    Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 79, 273-298.

    Duan, C. , & Hill , C. E. ( 1996). The current state of empathy research. Journal of

    Counseling Psychology, 43, 261-274.

    Eisenberg, N., Miller, P.A., Shell , R. , McNalley, S. , & Shea, C. (1991). Prosocial

    development in adolescence: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology,

    27, 849-857.

    Ekman, R. ( 1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 169-200.

    Ekman, R. , & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the face. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:

    Prentice-Hall.

    Goleman, D. , Boyatzis, R. , & McKee, A. (2002). Prima/leadership, Harvard Business

    School Press, Cambridge M. A.

    Gosserand, R. H., & Diefendorff, J. M. (2005). Emotional display rules and emotional

    labor: The moderating role of commitment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90,

    1256-1264.

    Grandey, A. A. (2000). Emotion regulation in the workplace: A new way to

    conceptualize emotional labor. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5,

    95-110.

    Grandey, A. A. (2003). When "the show must go on": Surface acting and deep acting as

    determinants of emotional exhaustion and peer-rated service delivery. Academy

    of Management Journal, 46, 86-96.

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 33

    Grandey, A. A., Fisk, G. M., & Steiner, D. D. (2005). Must "service with a smile" be

    stressful? The moderating role of personal control for American and French

    employees. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 893-904.

    Gross, J. J., & John, 0. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation

    processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of

    Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348-362.

    Hakanen, J. J., Bakker, A. B., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2006). Burnout and work engagement

    among teachers. Journal of School Psychology, 43, 495-513.

    Harter, J. K., Schmidt, F. L., & Hayes, T. L. (2002). Business-unit-level relationship

    between employee satisfaction, employee engagement, and business outcomes:

    A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 268-279.

    Hobfoll, S. E. ( 1989). Conservation of resources: A new attempt at conceptualizing

    stress. American Psychologist, 44, 513-524.

    Hobfoll, S., & Freedy, J. (1993). Conservation of resources: A general stress theory

    applied to burnout. In W. Schaufeli, C. Maslach, & T. Marek (Eds.),

    Professional burnout: Recent developments in theory and research. Washington,

    DC: Taylor & Francis.

    Hochschild, A. R. ( 1983). The managed heart: Commercialization of human feeling.

    Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

    Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. ( 1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure

    analysis : conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation

    Modeling, 6, 1-55.

  • Emotionallabor and work engagement 34

    Humphrey, R. H. (2006). Promising research opportunities in emotions and coping with

    conflict. Journal of Management and Organization, 12, 179-186

    iJorens, S., Schaufeli, W ., Bakker, A. , & Salanova, M. (2007). Does a positive gain

    spiral of resources, efficacy beliefs and engagement exist? Computers in Human

    Behavior, 23, 825-841.

    Johnson, H. M. , & Spector, P. E. (2007). Service with a smile: Do emotional

    intelligence, gender, and autonomy moderate the emotional labor process?

    Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 12, 319-333.

    Kahn, W. A. (1990). Psychological conditions of personal engagement and

    disengagement at work. Academy of Management Journal, 33, 692-724.

    Kohut, H. (1987). The search for the self: Selected writing of Heinz Kohut: 1950-1978.

    New York: International Universities Press.

    Kruml, S., & Geddes, D. (2000). Catching fire without burning out: Is there an ideal way

    to perform emotionallabor? In N. M . Ashkanasy, C. E. J. Hartel. , & W. J. Zerbe

    (Eds.). Emotions in the workplace: Research, theory and practice (pp. 177 -188).

    W estport, CT: Quorum Books.

    Maslach, C. , Schaufeli , W. B. , & Leiter, M. P. (2001). Job burnout. Annual Review of

    Psychology, 52, 397-422.

    Morris, J. A. , & Feldman, D. C. (1996). The dimension, antecedents, and consequences

    of emotional tabor. Academy of Management Review, 21 , 986-1 01 0.

    Morris, J. A. , & Feldman, D. C. (1997). Managing emotions in the workplace. Journal

    of Managerial Issues, 9, 257-274.

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 35

    Murphy, K. R. , & Cleveland, J. N. (1995). Understanding performance appraisal.

    London: Sage.

    Parkinson, B. (1991). Emotional stylists: Strategies of expressive management among

    trainee hairdressers. Cognition and Emotion, 5, 419-434.

    Parasuraman, A. , Zeithaml, V. A., & Berry, L. L. (1985). A conceptual model of service

    quality and its implications for future research. Journal of Marketing, 49, 41-50.

    Parasuraman, A. , Zeithaml, V. A. , & Berry, L. L. (1988). SERVQUAL: A multiple-item

    scale for measuring consumer perception of service quality. Journal of Retailing,

    64, 12-40.

    Price, L. L. , Amould, E. J. , & Tierney, P. (1995). Going to extremes: Managing service

    encounters and assessing provider performance. Journal of Marketing, 59, 83-97.

    Pugh, S. D. (200 I). Service with a smile: Emotional contagion in the service encounter.

    Academy of Management Journal, 44, 1018-1027.

    Rafaeli , A. , & Sutton, R. I. (1987). Expression of emotion as part of the work role.

    Academy of management Review, 12, 23-37.

    Rothbard, N. P. (200 1 ). Enriching or depleting? The dynamics of engagement in work

    and family roles. Administrative Science Quarterly, 46, 655-684.

    Rust, R. , & Zahorik, A. (1993). Customer satisfaction, customer retention, and market

    share. Journal of Retailing, 69, 193-215.

    Salanova, M., Agut, S., & Peir6, J. M. (2005). Linking organizational resources and

    work engagement to employee performance and customer loyalty: The mediation

    of service climate. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 1217-1227.

  • Emotional labor and work engagement 36

    Salovey, P. , & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition, and

    Personality, 9, 185-211.

    Schaubroeck, J. , & Jones, J. R. (2000). Antecedents of workplace emotional labor

    dimensions and moderators of their effects on physical symptoms. Journal of

    Organizational Behavior, 21 , 163-183.

    Seligman, M. E. P. , & Csikszentmihalyi , M. (2000). Positive psychology: An

    introduction. American Psychologist, 55, 5-14.

    Schaufeli, W. B. , & Bakker, A. B. (2004). Job demands, job resources, and their

    relationship with burnout and engagement: A multi-sample study. Journal of

    Organizational Behavior, 25 , 293-315.

    Schaufeli, W. B. , Martinez, 1. , Marques-Pinto, A. , Sal an ova, M. , & Bakker, A. B.

    (2002). Burnout and engagement in university students: A cross national study.

    Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33, 464-481 .

    Schaufeli, W. B. , Salanova, M. , Gonzales-Roma, V. , & Bakker, A. B. (2002). The

    measurement of engagement and burnout: A two sample confirmatory factor

    analytic approach. Journal of Happiness Studies, 3, 71-92.

    Spector, P. E. (2006). Industrial and organizational psychology, research and practice

    (pp. 295). Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons.

    Storbacka, J., Strandvik, T. , & Gronroos, C . (1994). Managing customer relationship

    quality. International Journal of Service Industry Management, 5, 21-28 .

    Totterdell , P. , & Holman, D. (2003). Emotion regulation in customer service roles:

    Testing a model of emotional labor. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology,

    8, 55-73.

  • Emotional tabor and work engagement 3 7

    Wilk, S. L. , & Moynihan, L. M. (2005). Display rule "regulators" : The relationship

    between supervisors and worker emotional exhaustion. Journal of Applied

    Psychology, 90, 917-927.

    Worthington, E. L. , & Wade, N. G. (1999). The psychology ofunforgiveness and

    forgiveness and implications for clinical practice. Journal of Social and Clinical

    Psychology, 18, 385-418.

  • CUHK Libraries

    004660058