THE IMPORTANCE OF CONTRAST FOR THE …shapley/Publications/shapley1986.pdfContrast and activity of...

15
C’iurm Rr.v. Vol. 26. No I, pp. 45-61. 1986 0042-69XY:Xh 53 00 + 0 00 Pnnred ,n Great Bn&in. All rights reserved CopyrIght (‘ 19X6 Pergamon Prw ILrd THE IMPORTANCE OF CONTRAST FOR THE ACTIVITY OF SINGLE NEURONS, THE VEP AND PERCEPTION ROBERT SHAPLEY Rockefeller University, New York, NY 10021. U.S.A. Abstract-The brightness of a visually perceived object is mainly determined by the average local contrast around the border between object and background. This fact is demonstrated here with several examples of equiluminant objects on nonuniformly luminant backgrounds. Even in Mondrian-like patterns resembling those used by Land and McCann (1971), equiluminant objects may appear to be of unequal brightness. This result does not agree with predictions of the Retinex Theory. The importance of contrast in vision is also suggested by neurophysiological findings, both classical and recent, that reveal the dependence of visual responses on contrast over most of the visual operating range of mean illumination. The dependence on contrast appears to be the result of retinal gain control mechanisms and is not due to center-surround interaction in the receptive field. We have discovered parallel neural channels with high and low contrast gain in the monkey’s visual pathway by means of single unit techniques. Visual evoked potential measurements suggest that similar visual pathways. and with high and low contrast-sensitivity. exist in man and monkey. Contrast Brightness Retina Retinex .4ssimilation INTRODUCTION Human observers rapidly and accurately esti- mate the brightnesses of an array of reflecting objects in a complex visual scene. This behav- ioral performance is relatively invariant with changes in illumination on a slow space scale or a slow time scale. It is not known how this visual problem is solved by the human brain. The most well-known computational theory for the brain’s solution to this problem is Land’s el- egant Retinex Theory. My recent experimental work implies that the Retinex Theory needs some revision to account for brightness vision. There are four main properties of visual stimuli that have been found to affect the appar- ent brightness of objects. These are: luminance. contrast, spatial frequency content and assimi- lation. I will discussion these in sequence. Other things being equal, the subjective im- pression of an object’s brightness is mono- tonically related to its luminance, the amount of physiologically effective light per unit area of the object (see Wyszecki and Stiles, 1967). When other things are not equal, especially the luminance distribution in the surroundings of the object, an object’s brightness is not simply related to its luminance. The apparent bright- ness depends to a large extent on the p/zy.sic~/ cotlttmt between the object and its background. Contrast may be defined as (LOB, - LB(;)/& where Los, is the luminance of the object and LBG is the luminance of the background. Con- trast is a measure of the relatice r,arintion of luminance. Probably the reason why brightness depends on contrast is that the visual system associates brightness with reflectance not lumi- nance (Land and McCann, 1971; Stockham, 1972). While an object’s luminance may change with lighting conditions, its reflectance is an invariant property of the surface of the object. The physical contrast as defined here only de- pends on the reflectance of object and back- ground and not on the level of illumination (the argument is given in Shapley and Enroth- Cugell, 1984). Thus, it is plausible that contrast should be the major cue for brightness. Powerful psychophysical evidence that con- trast was most significant for brightness vision was obtained by Heinemann (1955. 1972). Neu- rophysiological evidence indicates that the re- sponse amplitude of retinal ganglion ceils is proportional to contrast up to some saturating value (Shapley and Enroth-Cugell, 1984). It has been known for a long time that neurons in the visual cortex respond to contrast rather than simply to luminance (Hubel and Wiesel, 1962). It is reasonable to presume that the cortical response-dependence on contrast is caused by the retinal response-dependence. The mech- anisms that produce the contrast dependence

Transcript of THE IMPORTANCE OF CONTRAST FOR THE …shapley/Publications/shapley1986.pdfContrast and activity of...

Page 1: THE IMPORTANCE OF CONTRAST FOR THE …shapley/Publications/shapley1986.pdfContrast and activity of single neurons, VEP and perception 47 patterns it produces, it has the great advantages

C’iurm Rr.v. Vol. 26. No I, pp. 45-61. 1986 0042-69XY:Xh 53 00 + 0 00 Pnnred ,n Great Bn&in. All rights reserved CopyrIght (‘ 19X6 Pergamon Prw ILrd

THE IMPORTANCE OF CONTRAST FOR THE ACTIVITY OF SINGLE NEURONS, THE

VEP AND PERCEPTION

ROBERT SHAPLEY

Rockefeller University, New York, NY 10021. U.S.A.

Abstract-The brightness of a visually perceived object is mainly determined by the average local contrast

around the border between object and background. This fact is demonstrated here with several examples

of equiluminant objects on nonuniformly luminant backgrounds. Even in Mondrian-like patterns

resembling those used by Land and McCann (1971), equiluminant objects may appear to be of unequal

brightness. This result does not agree with predictions of the Retinex Theory. The importance of contrast

in vision is also suggested by neurophysiological findings, both classical and recent, that reveal the

dependence of visual responses on contrast over most of the visual operating range of mean illumination.

The dependence on contrast appears to be the result of retinal gain control mechanisms and is not due

to center-surround interaction in the receptive field. We have discovered parallel neural channels with high

and low contrast gain in the monkey’s visual pathway by means of single unit techniques. Visual evoked

potential measurements suggest that similar visual pathways. and with high and low contrast-sensitivity.

exist in man and monkey.

Contrast Brightness Retina Retinex .4ssimilation

INTRODUCTION

Human observers rapidly and accurately esti-

mate the brightnesses of an array of reflecting

objects in a complex visual scene. This behav- ioral performance is relatively invariant with

changes in illumination on a slow space scale or

a slow time scale. It is not known how this visual

problem is solved by the human brain. The most

well-known computational theory for the brain’s solution to this problem is Land’s el-

egant Retinex Theory. My recent experimental

work implies that the Retinex Theory needs some revision to account for brightness vision.

There are four main properties of visual stimuli that have been found to affect the appar-

ent brightness of objects. These are: luminance. contrast, spatial frequency content and assimi- lation. I will discussion these in sequence.

Other things being equal, the subjective im- pression of an object’s brightness is mono- tonically related to its luminance, the amount of physiologically effective light per unit area of

the object (see Wyszecki and Stiles, 1967).

When other things are not equal, especially the luminance distribution in the surroundings of the object, an object’s brightness is not simply related to its luminance. The apparent bright- ness depends to a large extent on the p/zy.sic~/ cotlttmt between the object and its background. Contrast may be defined as (LOB, - LB(;)/&

where Los, is the luminance of the object and

LBG is the luminance of the background. Con- trast is a measure of the relatice r,arintion of

luminance. Probably the reason why brightness depends on contrast is that the visual system

associates brightness with reflectance not lumi-

nance (Land and McCann, 1971; Stockham,

1972). While an object’s luminance may change with lighting conditions, its reflectance is an invariant property of the surface of the object.

The physical contrast as defined here only de-

pends on the reflectance of object and back- ground and not on the level of illumination (the argument is given in Shapley and Enroth- Cugell, 1984). Thus, it is plausible that contrast

should be the major cue for brightness.

Powerful psychophysical evidence that con- trast was most significant for brightness vision

was obtained by Heinemann (1955. 1972). Neu- rophysiological evidence indicates that the re- sponse amplitude of retinal ganglion ceils is proportional to contrast up to some saturating value (Shapley and Enroth-Cugell, 1984). It has

been known for a long time that neurons in the visual cortex respond to contrast rather than

simply to luminance (Hubel and Wiesel, 1962). It is reasonable to presume that the cortical response-dependence on contrast is caused by the retinal response-dependence. The mech- anisms that produce the contrast dependence

Page 2: THE IMPORTANCE OF CONTRAST FOR THE …shapley/Publications/shapley1986.pdfContrast and activity of single neurons, VEP and perception 47 patterns it produces, it has the great advantages

46 ROBERT SHAPLEY

have been identified as the retinal automatic gain controls for light adaptation (Shapley and Enroth-Cugell, 1984).

Contrast is not the sole factor that influences brightness. Another crucial process is spatial filtering which removes low spatial frequencies from the “neural image” of the visual scene (Robson, 1975). This has the effect of sup- pressing neural responses to spatially gradual changes in illumination which are, under usual natural circumstances, due to variations in the illuminant and not due to gradual changes in reflectance. This low spatial-frequency sup- pression has been observed at all levels of the visual pathway (Robson, 1975; DeValois and DeValois, 1980). It is understood to be due in part to lateral inhibition: neural inhibition of a neuron from other neurons which have spatially displaced or larger visual receptive fields (Ratliff, 1965).

One of the consequences of spatial filtering is the enhancement of the visual salience of refkcting objects at the expense of (the usually slow) spatial variations in illumination (cf. Ratliff, 1965). The spatial frequency content of the sharp-edged borders of natural objects is broad-band. Thus, neural filtering-out of low spatial frequency information does not degrade the response to objects. Such filtering does prevent slow variations in the illuminant from being perceived (Stockham, 1972). Thus, spatial frequency filtering, at the retinal and cortical level, working together with the retinal auto- matic gain controls that produce contrast- dependence, allow the visual system to produce an illumination-invariant response to objects illuminated by light sources that vary slowly in space and time.

Another factor comes into play when the visual scene is not simply an object. on back- ground but becomes only as complicated as object (I) on object (2) on background. Then there is the possibility that the visual system might compute the brightness of object (1) by adding the brightness of object (2) to that of object (I). This possibility has been observed and has been called assimilation (Helson, 1963; Hurvich, 1981). Assimilation is a perceptual phenomenon that tends to antagonize the dom- inant dependence of perception on contrast; assimilation makes the effect of a background additive while contrast makes it subtractive. The concept of assimilation arose in studies of color vision where this phenomenon is observed as the tendency for an object on a colored background

to take on the color of the background (Hur- vich, 1981). Elegant experiments by Helson (1963) and Arend EI al. (197 I) demonstrated that assimilation also played a role in the per- ception of brightness. There was also evidence for an assimilation effect in Heinemann’s experi- ments (Heinemann, 1955). Implicit in the Reti- nex Theory is the assumption of completely global assimilation. Assimilation is not local, but is not completely global according to our recent measurements, discussed below.

The emphasis in this paper is on the im- portance of contrast, which I believe to be the most significant stimulus property in the visual estimation of brightness, I will offer the reader several perceptual demonstrations of the effect of contrast on brightness. Then electro- physiological results will be presented concern- ing the neural machinery for contrast percep- tion.

METHODS

Visual stimulator

The basic tool for many of our experiments is an electronic visual display, designed in our laboratory by Norman Milkman, Michelangelo Rossetto and Gary Schick (cf. Milkman e? al., 1980). The instrument used in the present ex- periments produces two-dimensional patterns on a Tektronix608 cathode ray tube (CRT) monitor. The patterns are homochromatic; in most of our experiments they were white (P4 phosphor). The instrument produces a raster display of 256 picture elements (pixels} per line, 256 lines per frame and 270 frameslsec. Thus there are 65,536 pixels per frame. The instru- ment contains control registers, four RAM memory modules (256 bytes each) which con- tain the spatial profiles of four independent spatiotempora! functions and one RAM memory (65,536 by 2 bits) that contains the bit map which controls which one of the spatio- temporal functions is displayed at each pixel location. The two bits for each pixel are led to a fast analog switch which chooses between the four spatiotemporal waveforms. The spatio- temporal waveforms are created from the spa- tial profiles in the four RAMS by digital/analog multiplication in a multiplying D/A converter. The spatial profile values are multiplied by temporal modulation and depth of modulation signals which are fed to the instrument by a PDP 1 l/23 computer. Besides the great flexibility the instrument has in the kinds of

Page 3: THE IMPORTANCE OF CONTRAST FOR THE …shapley/Publications/shapley1986.pdfContrast and activity of single neurons, VEP and perception 47 patterns it produces, it has the great advantages

Contrast and activity of single neurons, VEP and perception 47

patterns it produces, it has the great advantages of digital accuracy of positioning (to within

0.04 deg of visual angle at our usual viewing

distance) and high precision of intensity control

(nominally more than 20 bits of precision, though this is probably an overestimate because

of noise in the instrument and noise picked up on the cables). A linearization circuit added to

the Tektronix 608 by Mr Rossetto allows us to

modulate contrast in a linear manner up to 60% at a mean luminance of 100 cd/m’. We measure

the light output from the CRT displays before and during each experiment, to make sure the nominal contrasts and mean luminance are cor-

rect. For this purpose we employ a Pritchard Model 1980b Photometer (Photo Research,

Inc.).

Eiecrrophysiological single-unit experiments

Our techniques for single unit recording are described in detail in previous publications

(Hochstein and Shapley, 1976; Kaplan and Shapley, 1982). Briefly, we use glass micro-

pipette electrodes to record extracellularly in the optic tract or lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN). The animal, cat or monkey, is anesthetized lightly with urethane, or is decerebrated by

midcollicular transection. Eye movements are minimized by means of intravenous infusion of

B muscle relaxant, gallamine triethiodide. The physiological condition of the animal is moni-

tored through measurement of blood pressure,

expired carbon dioxide. core body temperature, electrocardiogram and electroencephalogram

and appropriate steps are taken to keep these in

the physiological range. The eyes of the animal are protected with plastic contact lenses. In the

cat experiments, image quality is improved by

using contact lenses with a small (3 mm dia.)

artificial pupil. Image quality is checked with an

ophthalmoscope throughout each experiment, and data are taken only when the retinal image

is sharp. Neural responses are analyzed using the same

PDP 111’23 computer that controls the visual stimulator. Stimuli are presented repetitively

and periodically in time. The neural impulse

rate is averaged and responses are quantified as the amplitude of modulation of the impulse rate in synchrony with the modulated stimulus.

RESULTS

Bri,~htnes.s perception and contrast

A standard demonstration of the effect of

contrast on brightness is Fig. 1. Here two

equiluminant circles are placed on a bipartite

field. The luminance of the circles is equal to the average luminance of the two halves of the

bipartite field. The two circles are not equally bright to most observers; the circle on the

darker background appears brighter than the circle on the lighter background.

Figure 2 is a picture of another two equi-

luminant circles but this time they have been placed on a linear gradient of luminance. That

is, the luminance across the background is

L(X) = Lo (1 + mx). The slope m is the rate of change of background luminance with position

X, where x = 0 is the middle of the picture, and x varies from - 5 to + 5 cm on the cathode ray tube (CRT) screen which was the original image

photographed to make Fig. 2. L,, is the mean

luminance of the background. The luminance of

the circles is also L,. I believe that there is a simple explanation for the brightness differences of the circles in Fig. I and 2 in terms of the sign of the contrast around their borders. In each of

these pictures, the average local contrast around the circle on the left is positive and so it appears

bright. The average contrast around the circle

on the right is negative and it appears dark. While the appearance of Figs I and 2 can be

accounted for by the local border contrast, it cannot be explained adequately by the Retincx Theory for brightness (Land and McCann, 1971; Land, 1983). Without going into all the

details, the hetinex Theory is constructed to give a constant visual response to a constant

reflectance. Therefore, in a picture like Fig. I in which the two circles are equiluminant and there is no gradient of illumination, the Retinex The-

ory must predict that the two circles will bc

perceived as equally bright. Figure 2 can bc

explained at least qualitatively by Rctinex Thc-

ory because of the gradient of illumination in

the background. However, the theory would

predict that the circles in Fig. I would look quite different in brightness from those in Fig. 7.

Land and McCann (1971) acknowledged im-

plicitly the difficulty that pictures like Fig. I posed for Retinex Theory in their Footnote I:

“Although sensations of lightness show a strong correlation with reflectancos in most real-life situations, there are many important departures from this strong cor-

relation. . . And, of course, any general

theory must, as well, explain the simple situations in which the surround com- prises the entire environment .‘.

Page 4: THE IMPORTANCE OF CONTRAST FOR THE …shapley/Publications/shapley1986.pdfContrast and activity of single neurons, VEP and perception 47 patterns it produces, it has the great advantages

4x ROBERT

An implication of the above quotations is that departures from the Retinex Theory occur mainly in nonreal-life situations, especially in “simple situations in which the surround com- prises the entire environment.” Figure 1 is such a “simple situation.” While this argument may appear plausible, in fact the departures from Retinex Theory are widespread, even in complex pictures. In all the cases I will present, Retinex Theory makes qualitatively incorrect predictions while prediction of apparent bright- ness based on the average local contrast is correct, at least qualitatively. I will concentrate on generalizations of Fig. 1, that is pictures in which objects of equal luminance are perceived to be of unequal brightness, in contradiction to Retinex Theory.

The first illustration of the genera1 im- portance of local contrast is Fig. 3. This is a complex picture which shows twelve circles of equal luminance on a complex background that is composed of a linear gradient of luminance in the lower half of the picture, and a staircase of luminance in the upper half. To make the picture more comprehensible, refer to the lumi- nance profiles of the lower and upper halves of the figure as diagrammed in Figs 4 and 5. In Fig. 4 the locations of the circles along the horizontal axis of the picture are marked by arrows, and their luminance, which is the mean luminance of the gradient, is indicated by the dashed horizon- tal line. The position of the circles in the upper

SHAPLEY

half of Fig. 3 and the mean luminance of the staircase, are indicated in a similar way in Fig. 5. Inspection of Fig. 3 reveals that the circles of positive local contrast always appear light and the circles with negative local contrast always appear dark, regardless of whether they lie on gradient or staircase backgrounds. The bright- ness of the circles is unaffected by covering or uncovering the upper half or lower half or any other fraction of the picture, as long as the border of the circle and its immediate back- ground are visible. These observations are in contradiction to the Retinex Theory, which predicts: (1) that the circles on the staircase should look approximately the same brightness as each other, and (2) only the circles on the gradient should appear to be of different bright- ness, and (3) the circles on staircase or gradient should change in apparent brightness as parts of the picture are covered. In the visual system’s response to Fig. 3, local contrast seems to be much more significant than the long-distance interactions postulated in the Retinex.

For those readers who are not familiar with the implications of the Retinex Theory, it is worth pointing out why the circles on the gra- dient in Fig. 3 should look different in bright- ness but the circles on the staircase should look the same in brightness, according to that theory. In the most modern version of the Retinex (Land, 1983), the brightness at a discrete area is determined from the average of the relative

Fig. 4. This is the luminance profile of the lower half of Fig. 3. the equiluminant circles on a gradient.

Zero luminance is indicated by the horizontal axis. The positions of the equiluminant circles are marked

by arrows. The mean luminance is drawn as a dashed line.

Page 5: THE IMPORTANCE OF CONTRAST FOR THE …shapley/Publications/shapley1986.pdfContrast and activity of single neurons, VEP and perception 47 patterns it produces, it has the great advantages

Fig. 1. Two equiluminant circles on a bipartite &Ad of luminance. This is a photograph of a picture created on 8 Tektronix 508 CRT mmitar with ea akctmak visual display (Milkman et 4, IPsa). Uader our standard viewing conditions, the display on the CRT screen wns 10 by IOcm in area and had a mean

luminance of 100 cd/m*.

49

Page 6: THE IMPORTANCE OF CONTRAST FOR THE …shapley/Publications/shapley1986.pdfContrast and activity of single neurons, VEP and perception 47 patterns it produces, it has the great advantages

Fig. 2. Two equiluminant circles on a linear ramp of luminance. In this picture, the background luminance

is described by the equation L(s) = &,*(I + mx), where nr is the slope of the luminance gradient, I.,, is

the mean luminance of the gradient and s is the spatial position with respect to the midline of the picture:

minus to the left and plus to the right. The luminance of the two circles is the mean luminance. L,,.

50

Page 7: THE IMPORTANCE OF CONTRAST FOR THE …shapley/Publications/shapley1986.pdfContrast and activity of single neurons, VEP and perception 47 patterns it produces, it has the great advantages

Fig. 3. Many equiluminant circles on a staircase of luminance, in the upper half of the picture, and on

a linear gradient of luminance, in the lower half. The gradient and the staircase were produced so that

the luminance along the left and right borders of the picture would be uniform along the vertical axis.

The mean luminance of the staircase and the gradient were also set to be identical and also equal to the

luminance of the circles. The average local contrast around the circles on the gradient is lower than for

the circles on the staircase, as can be seen by comparing Fig. 4 and 5, which show, respectively, the

luminance profiles of the lower and upper halves of this figure.

Page 8: THE IMPORTANCE OF CONTRAST FOR THE …shapley/Publications/shapley1986.pdfContrast and activity of single neurons, VEP and perception 47 patterns it produces, it has the great advantages

Fig. 9. A photograph of two equihnninant and equal contrast, circles that look unequally bright. This figure, from unpublished work by Reid and Shapley, illustrates .the phenomenon of assimilation. The two small circles are equiluminant. The annuli that surround them are equal in luminance to each other. Therefore, the border contrast around the two circles is equal. But because the annuli are unequally bright, because of induction by a luminance gradient in the outer surround, the two inner circles appear of unequal brightness to most observers. However, it is a small effect and the picture requires scrutiny. The

luminance profile of this picture is given in Fig. 10.

54

Page 9: THE IMPORTANCE OF CONTRAST FOR THE …shapley/Publications/shapley1986.pdfContrast and activity of single neurons, VEP and perception 47 patterns it produces, it has the great advantages

Contrast and activity of single neurons. VEP and perception

Fig. 5. Luminance profile of the circles on the staircase: the upper part of Fig. 3. Conventions are as in Fig 4

reflectances of that area compared to every

other areal element in the scene. The relative reflectance of areas within one object compared

to those areas in another object, is the sum, along the path between the two objects, of the

logarithms of the ratios of the luminances fall-

ing on adjacent retinal elements. This sum-of-

the-logs-of-the-ratios formula is only true, how- ever, if the luminance ratios exceed a criterion

threshold amount. When the logarithm of the luminance ratio between two adjacent areas is below the threshold amount, the Retinex sets

the logarithm to zero. Therefore, it sets the reflectance ratio between those two elements to one.

Now, let us consider Retinex Theory predic-

tions for Fig. 3. In the case of the gradient

background, the luminance ratio of adjacent

retinal elements could be assumed to be below threshold. Then contributions to the average relative reflectance of one of the circles would

only come from areal elements near its border and the borders of the other circles, and the

estimated relative reflectances of different circles

would be unequal. However, in the case of the staircase, because luminance is constant except at the abrupt steps which must be assumed to be above the Retinex’s threshold, the estimated reflectances of all the circles are equal, meaning that they all must be equally bright were Retinex

Theory right. For example, the Retinex com- putation of the relative reflectance between

circle(l) of luminance L, on background LB, and circle(2) of luminance L,, on background L,, is:

0~&3,/~,1 + ~og[L,~lL,,1 + log[L,,lL,21;

= log[l] = 0. (1)

This is the logarithm of the apparent brightness ratios between any two equiluminant circles on

the staircase: zero. Therefore, the apparent brightness ratios predicted by Retinex Theory are all exp(0) = I. That is, all the circles should

look equally bright. As you can see, it does not predict the appearance of Fig. 3.

It is possible that Fig. 3 is not complicated

enough for the Retinex effects to become pre- dominant. Therefore, I manufactured Fig. 6 which is composed of two staircases of lumi-

nance: an ascending staircase in the lower half of the picture, and a descending staircase in the

upper half. In this picture, as in Figs I- 3. the circular objects are all equiluminant. at the mean luminance of the staircases. By equation

(I), Retinex Theory predicts that all the circles

should look equally bright. In this picture. the circles do not all appear to be the same bright- ness and therefore it is another qualitative coun- terexample to the original Retinex Theory.

It might be argued that all the pictures shown

up to now are not good tests of the Retinex Theory because they are not Mondrian-like

Page 10: THE IMPORTANCE OF CONTRAST FOR THE …shapley/Publications/shapley1986.pdfContrast and activity of single neurons, VEP and perception 47 patterns it produces, it has the great advantages

56 ROBERT SHAPLEY

patterns, like those favored by Land and Mc- Cann (1971). The crucial aspect of the Mondrian-like patterns is that the border of one object in the Mondrian is not completely sur- rounded by another object of uniform lumi- nance. Therefore, Fig. 7 was produced. This is a Mondrian-like pattern with two test areas of equal luminance: the circle in the lower right quadrant of the picture, and the square in the upper left (the brightnesses of these two test objects were not dependent on their shapes), The measured relative luminances of the areas in the picture, when it was illuminated diffusely and evenly, are given in Fig. 8. Note that the luminance of each patch in this picture is con- stant across the patch; there are no gradients of illumination which might allow the Retinex Theory to predict that equiluminant areas could look different. The pattern was constructed to provide another strong test of the Retinex The- ory, which must predict in such a pattern that equiluminant areas look equally bright, by the argument given above in equation (1). It is immediately evident that even in a uniformly illuminated Mondrian pattern, equiluminant objects may appear to be of unequal brightness, presumably because of unequal average local contrast. If one calculates the average physical contrast around the borders of the circle and the square equiluminant objects, one obtains - 0.11 for the circle and 0.38 for the square. The bright object is always associated with positive contrast, and the dark with negative contrast. The reader may note that there are other pairs

H’“l r-4 I I 21

Fig. 8. The relative luminances of the Mondrian-like pattern in Fig. 7. The luminances or areas in Fig. 7 were measured from a photographic print illuminated uniformly and diffusely. The luminance was measured with a Pritchard l98Ub telephotometer. The numbers in the figure were the actual luminance readings in cd/m?, but these could be taken

to be relative luminances or relative reflectances.

of equiluminant patches in Fig. 7 which do not appear equally bright, also contrary to Retinex Theory. Equiluminant patches in more complex Mondrian-like patterns exhibit the same phe- nomenon of nonequality of brightness if the average local contrasts around their borders are unequal.

I believe the implication of these perceptual demonstrations is that the computation of brightness is much more localized in fact than in Retinex Theory. It cannot be completely local, because border contrast some distance away determines the brightness at a point. Never- theless, the computation cannot be as global as in the Retinex Theory or else Figs l-3, 6, 7 would look different.

Besides local contrast, another component of the brightness computation is assimilation, the effect on the brightness of an object of the brightness of its background. In general it has been hard to separate assimilation from contrast because the way backgrounds have been made brighter or darker is by varying their luminances and this affects border contrast of an object on the background. R. Clay Reid and I have done some experiments recently on assimilation where we avoided this difficulty by varying the brightness of a background by brightness in- duction from an outer background (Shapley and Reid, 1985). In this way we could change brightness while keeping contrast fixed. An ex- ample of the kind of pattern we used is shown in Fig. 9. The luminance distribution in Fig. 9 is diagrammed in Fig. 10. The outer background is a gradient of luminance. The small inner backgrounds are equiluminant annuli. The test spots are equiluminant circles. These circles not only are equally luminous, they also have equal local contrast around their borders. Neverthe- less, they appear to be of unequal brightness; the circle on the brighter inner background appears to be a little brighter. This must be due to assimilation. Reid and I measured the magni- tude of this assimilation effect and compared it to the strength of the effect of local contrast on brightness. Assimilation is about half as strong as contrast, and it wanes with distance between the borders of the test spot and inner back- ground. These results to some extent explain why the Retinex Theory cannot account for the earlier figures. In the Retinex Theory, a sum- mation of brightness from one point to another

in the visual field is postulated to occur without any decrement with distance (see Land and

McCann, 1971 and Land, 1983). This process

Page 11: THE IMPORTANCE OF CONTRAST FOR THE …shapley/Publications/shapley1986.pdfContrast and activity of single neurons, VEP and perception 47 patterns it produces, it has the great advantages

Contrast and activity of single neurons, VEP and perception 57

Fig. 10. Luminance profile of the assimilation demonstration. The small circles are the two inner

increments on top of the annular backgrounds which were at the mean luminance indicated by the dashed

horizontal line.

would be equivalent to total assimilation with no spatial decrement. The fact that assimilation

does get weaker with distance implies that the calculation of brightness is more localized than

is proposed in the Retinex Theory. The com- putation of brightness contains more compli-

cations: Reid and I have observed nonlinear effects of brightness induction and assimilation.

However, this topic is beyond the scope of the present paper.

El~~ctrnphysiological results

The importance of contrast is also revealed by

electrophysiological results that suggest that

contrast and color are signaled to the brain by

different parallel pathways. This is the conclu-

sion of work done by Ehud Kaplan and I on

single cells in the LGN of macaque monkeys.

The LGN in monkeys as in man is a highly

layered neural organ. There are six main cell

layers: four dorsal layers composed of small

cells (parvocellular layers) and two ventral lay-

ers composed of larger neurons (magnocellular

layers). We recorded the response amplitudes of

cells in the LGN to drifting gratings with a spatial frequency high enough that they would

stimulate the center of the receptive field only (cf. Shapley and Lennie, 1985). We were sur- prised to find that the cells in the parvocellular layers had a poor contrast sensitivity by com- parison with cat geniculate and retinal ganglion

cells. The magnocellular cells were about IO times more sensitive than the parvocellular neu- rons and were comparable in sensitivity to cat

visual neurons (Shapley et al., 198 I ; Kaplan and Shapley, 1982). The situation in the monkey’s

LGN is demonstrated in Fig. I I which is a sketch of one histological section through a monkey’s LGN, and shows the course of an electrode track through it. The first unit with

high contrast sensitivity recorded on this track

was the one marked with a dot at the top of the magnocellular layers. We repeated this kind of

experiment many times, always marking with

dye spots the places where highly sensitive cells were recorded and they were always located in

the magnocellular layers.

The function of the organization diagrammed

in Fig. I I has been interpreted in terms of

separate signal processing for contrast and

color. The parvocellular neurons are narrowly tuned in the dimension of wavelength, while the

magnocellular neurons are more broadly tuned.

Therefore, the function of the parvocellular neurons may be more connected with wave-

length discrimination and colour vision, while

the magnocellular neurons may be the thalamic

relay stage for a separate contrast-sensing path-

way. In recent work we have found that the retinal input to the LGN cell determines its contrast sensitivity (Kaplan and Shapley,

1984a). We have done this by recording pairs of LGN cell impulses and the unitary S potentials which are the locally. extracellularly recorded EPSP’s from the retinal input (Levick 01 NI., 1972; Kaplan and Shapley. 1984b). Thus, we believe that parallel processing begins in the retina.

Page 12: THE IMPORTANCE OF CONTRAST FOR THE …shapley/Publications/shapley1986.pdfContrast and activity of single neurons, VEP and perception 47 patterns it produces, it has the great advantages

5x ROBERT SHAPLEY

Visual Etloked Potentials (VEPs). The VEP grating was modulated in contrast above and

can be used to investigate the importance of below a mean level of contrast by 100% of the contrast in human vision with an electro- mean contrast, so that at the trough of the physiological as opposed to a psychophysical modulation waveform, the sine grating disap- technique. While this has been the focus of peared and the screen became blank at the mean much study in other laboratories, as discussed luminance, while at the peak of the modulation below, I would like to illustrate a basic point waveform, the contrast of the grating was twice with experimental results obtained by colleagues the mean contrast. The stimulus is referred to as at Rockefeller University: Conte et al. (1983). “Appearance-Disappearance” for obvious rea- The experiment was to measure the occipital sons. The “Appearance-Disappearance” VEP VEP to the following stimulus: a 7.3 c/deg sine was averaged and analyzed into two Fourier

I High

Fig. I I. Drawing of a histological section of the monkey’s LGN and an electrode truck through it. This

is a tracing of an approximately coronal section through the LGN. It shows the four dorsal prvocellular layers and the two more ventral magnocellular layers. An electrode track is indicated by the dashed line.

The filled circular spots were locations where current was passed through the dye-filled micropipette to

mark electrode location during P cell recording. The uppr dot was at a ptaze where a impulses from a

typical color-opponent parvocellular neuron of low contrast sensitivity was recorded. The lower spot. at

the top of the magnocellular layers, was the locus of recording of the first cell on the track that had high

contrast sensitivity. From Shapley CI (11. (1981).

Page 13: THE IMPORTANCE OF CONTRAST FOR THE …shapley/Publications/shapley1986.pdfContrast and activity of single neurons, VEP and perception 47 patterns it produces, it has the great advantages

Contrast and activity of single neurons. VEP und perceptIon

Appearance - dmppearance VEP

0 4 8 12 16 20 24 28

Contrast (%)

Fig. 12. Human VEP to contrast modulation of ;I sine

grating. This is the response of a human subject recorded

with gold cup electrodes placed on the occiput and the

forehead. The stimulus was contrast modulation of B

7.3 ckleg sine grating. The contrast was modulated at 6.3 Hz

m the “appearance-disappearance” mode, that is, the

stimulus was .x(-Y. 0 = ~‘*&*[l + sin(27rJ/)]*[sin(2nks)]

where (’ was the average contrast, L, was the average

lumm:mce. Jwas the temporal frequency, and k was the

spatial frequency. The 1st and 2nd harmonic amplitudes of

I min of VEP data are plotted against average contrast.

Conte (v (11. (1983).

components: one at the modulation frequency

(1st Harmonic) and the other at twice the

modulation frequency (2nd Harmonic). It was

found that the 1st Harmonic response was very sensitively dependent on the mean contrast of such a stimulus while the 2nd Harmonic was

much less sensitive, as shown in Fig. 12. A striking fact about the VEP amplitudes in Fig.

I2 is that the 1st Harmonic amplitude saturates

at a very low mean contrast: about 6%. This

corresponds to a peak contrast of twice that value. The high sensitivity for contrast and the very low value for the saturation level of the response is similar but not identical to the behavior of the magnocellular neurons and their

retinal inputs in the human’s close biological

relative, the rhesus monkey. It is quite different

from the contrast dependence of the responses of the insensitive and nonsaturating parvocellular

neurons. Cortical networks must act on the contrast signal that enters the cortex from the

LGN and the exact form of the contrast-response function of the VEP almost

certainly is strongly affected by cortical pro- cessing. Nevertheless, these results are consis- tent with the hypothesis that contrast and color are handled separately, in parallel, by the visual system and that there is a distinct, highly sensi-

tive channel for perceiving contrast.

DISCUSSION

There have been few attempts to provide a

comprehensive theory of brightness perception

in terms of an experimentally testable model. The Retinex Theory of Land and his colleagues

is such a theory and has justifiably attracted the

interest and the attention of vision scientists.

However, models exist to be disproved and

improved. The perceptual results presented in

this paper demand the formulation of a new

theory of brightness vision to improve on the Retinex Theory. The crucial experimental re-

sults are that the retina sends a signal to the

brain coded in terms of local border contrast.

and the brain seems to assign brightnesses to

areas in complex scenes that correlate highly

with their average border contrasts. However,

contrast is not everything; the amount of assimi-

lation must be taken into account. At present, we are just beginning to find out the magnitude

and rules of interaction for assimilation. When

these are known, a comprehensive theory for

brightness may be formulated in terms of the combined effect of contrast and assimilation. However, even in as primitive a visual behavior

as perception of brightness, higher level (for

example, form dependent) information may complicate life (cf. Kanizsa, 1979).

It is worth remembering that the Retinex Theory was initially proposed as an explanation

for color vision. It was originally thought that

the extension of the concepts of the Retinex

from brightness to color only required repeating the Retinex calculations of brightness (or “light-

ness”) for three independent cone photo- receptor systems (Land and McCann, 1971; Land, 1983). The brightness problem was sup- posed to be the easy case where all three cone

systems provided the same answer in the brightness calculation. It is now clear that the Retinex Theory computation does not account

for brightness vision. It is still possible that

Retinex Theory helps to explain color vision better than it can account for the phenomena of

brightness. However, because color constancy with change of illuminant is not as robust as

brightness constancy (Hurvich, 1981), there is some reason to doubt the general validity of the

Retinex Theory in color.

Cofltrmt and retinal gain mnfrols

The idea that the visual dependence on con-

trast was a result of light adaptation rather than

Page 14: THE IMPORTANCE OF CONTRAST FOR THE …shapley/Publications/shapley1986.pdfContrast and activity of single neurons, VEP and perception 47 patterns it produces, it has the great advantages

60 ROBEHT SHAPLIIY

lateral inhibition was (as far as 1 know) first explicitly stated by Whittle and Challands (1969). Helmholtz (1909) probably had a some- what similar idea when he suggested that simul- taneous contrast might really be successive con- trast, because of eye movements. That retinal responses depend on the contrast of the stimulus rather than the luminance, and that this is a consequence of the gain control acting on the center of the receptive field, is implicit in the results of many investigators of cat ganglion cell physiology (Enroth-Cugell and Robson, 1966; Sakmann and Creutzfeldt, 1969; Barlow and Levick, 1969; Cleland and Enroth-Cugell, 1970; Enroth-Cugell and Shapley, 1973; among others reviewed in Shapley and Enroth-Cugell, 1984). It is also known that the center’s gain control is not affected by light that falls on the receptive field surround (Enroth-Cugell et al., 1975). Therefore, the neural mechanisms that lead to retinal response dependence on contrast seem to be associated with the neural machinery of the receptive field center of retinal ganglion cells.

The gain controls of light adaptation have sometimes been supposed to be too slow to account for the phenomena of brightness per- ception, some of which can be perceived in brief flashes. However, it is now known that under the conditions of most experiments, light adap- tation is rapid, being 90% complete in under 200 msec for a 1 log unit jump in mean level. There are slow components as well but they only influence precise quantitative measurements. Light adaptation is also very localized in space, a requirement for computing the correct value of contrast on shallow gradients (as in Figs 2 and 3). These properties of retinal gain controls are discussed in detail in our recent review article (Shapley and Enroth-Cugell, 1984) and I will spare the reader the details here.

Parallel pathways for color and contrast in the primate

Our finding of two different parallel systems for color and contrast in the primate visual pathway (Shapley et al., 1981; Kaplan and Shapley, 1982) has been reinforced by the sub- sequent work of Hicks et al. (1983). Derrington and Lennie (1984), Hawken and Parker (1984) and Biasdell and Fitzpatrick (1984). It also is consistent with the earlier findings of Sperling et al. (1978) and Lee, Creutzfeldt and Elepfandt (1979). Schiller and Colby (1983) have recently presented results on responses to luminance and color contrast in the monkey LGN which are

also consistent with the idea that the mag- nocellular pathway is the high gain path and the parvocellular neurons possess low contrast sen- sitivity.

The single unit studies of contrast sensitivity pathways have been complemented by in- vestigations of the VEP. In addition to the work presented above, Spekreijse et ul. (1977) have presented evidence that the “contrast” com- ponent of the human VEP possesses very high contrast sensitivity and saturates at very low contrast. Nakayama and Mackeben (I 982) have shown that there are two components of the VEP to contrast reversal in the macaque mon- key. One of these components has high sensi- tivity and saturates at low contrast. The other is a much lower sensitivity component and does not saturate at high contrast. These authors interpret their results as indicating two cortical response mechanisms which get input from the magnocellular and parvocellular parallel path- ways separately.

In the literature on visual evoked potentials, “contrast ” often is used in a different way from the way I have used it in this paper. For example, see Spekreijse et al. (1977). The Am- sterdam group has consistently meant by con- trast what I would call the magnitude or abso- lute value of (what I call) contrast. My definition of contrast has a signature, plus or minus, while what they call contrast is by definition positive. I hope this difference in nomenclature will not obscure the fundamental similarity of results obtained in Amsterdam and New York.

Acknowledgemen&---I thank my colleagues Ehud Kaplan, R. Clay Reid, James Gordon, and Norman Milkman for their help with work reported here. 0~) research has been supported by grants from the National Eye Institute of the U.S. National Institutes of Health: grants ROI EY 1472 and ROI EY 188. For making the Workshop on Systems Ap- proach to Vision possible, I thank the U.S. Air Force Office of Scientific Research and the Netherlands Royal Academy of Arts and Science.

REFERENCES

Arend L. E., Buehler J. N. and Lockhead G. R. (IY71) Ditl’erence information in brightness perception. Percept. t’sychophys. 9, 367-370.

Barlow H. B. and Levick W. R. (1969) Three factors limiting the reliable detection of light by retinal ganglion cells of the cat. 1. Physiol. 200, t-24.

Blasdell G. G. and Fitzpatrick D. (1984) Rhysiotogicai organization of layer 4 in macaque striate cortex. J. Neurosci. 4. 88&895.

Page 15: THE IMPORTANCE OF CONTRAST FOR THE …shapley/Publications/shapley1986.pdfContrast and activity of single neurons, VEP and perception 47 patterns it produces, it has the great advantages

Contrast and activity of single neurons. VEP and perceptjon

Clcland B. G. and Enroth-Cugell C. (1970) Quantitative

acpccts of gain and latency in the cat retina. J. P/~J,.s;o~.

206. 73 9 I. Contc M.. Zemon V.. Camisa J. and Milkman N. (19X3)

Comparmg psychophysical and VEP contrast sensitivity,

IlU?\I. Ophllrtrl. r.i.vurrl sci.. SuppI. 24, 47.

Derrington A. M. and Lennie P. (1984) Spatial and

temporal contrast sensitivities of neurons in lateral

genlculate nucleus of macaque. J. Physiol. 357, 219-240.

DeValols R. L. (1960) Color vision mechanisms in the

monkey. J. ,qc,, Plr~.vio/. 43, 115-128.

DcValols R. L. and DeValois K. K. (1980) Spatial Vision.

:l,r,, Rr1.. /?s,l~c~/ro/. 31, 309-34 I Enroth-Cugell C.. Lennie P. and Shapley R. (1975)

Surround contribution IO light adaptation in cat retinal

ganglion cells, J. Ph~~siol. 247, 579-588.

Enroth-Cugell C. and Robson J. G. (1966) The contrast

\ensltivity of retinal ganglion cells of the cat. J. Ph,vsiol.

187, 517 -552.

Enroth-Cugell C. and Shapley R. M. (1973) Adaptation

and dynamics of cal retinal ganglion cells. J. Physiol.

233, 271 309.

Hawken M. J. and Parker A. J. (1984) Contrast sensitivity

and orientation selectivity in lamina IV of the striate

cortex of Old World monkeys. Expl Bruin Res. 54,

367 372.

Hetnemann E. G. (1955) Simultaneous brightness in-

ductlon as a function of inducing- and test-field lumi-

nances. J. cvp. P.v?;chol. 50, 89-96.

tlelnemann E. G. (1972) Simultaneous brightness in-

ductlon. In Handbook q/Sensory Physiology (Edited by

Jamcson D. and Hurvich L.). Vol. VII/4. Springer,

Herltn.

Helmholtz H. von (1909) Contrast. In Trearise on

Ph~.~io/o~ical Oprics (Edited by Southall J. P. C.),

3rd edn, Chap. 24. Vol. 2, 1924; report 1962. Dover,

New York.

llclson H. (1963) Studies of anomalous contrast and

assimilation. J. op/. Sot. Am. 53, 179-184.

Ilickc T. P., Lee B. B. and Vidyasagar T. R. (1983) The

resp<,nses of cells in macaque lateral geniculate nucleus

to sInusoIdal gratings. J. Physiol. 337, 183-200.

Hochsleln S. and Shapley R. (1976) Quantitative analysis

of retinal ganglion cell classifications. J. Physiol. 262,

‘37 264.

fluhe D. H. and Wiesel T. N. (1962) Receptive fields,

bInocular interaction, and functional architecture in the

cat’s visual cortex. J. fhysiol. 160, 106-154.

Hurvlch L. (1981) Color Vision. Sinauer, Sunderland,

Mass.

Kani~sa G. (1979) Organizurion in Vision. Praeger, New

Yorh.

Kaplan E. and Shapley R. (1982) X and Y cells in the

lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque monkeys. J.

PItmid. 330. 125- 143.

Kaplan E. and Shapley R. (1984a) The primate retina

contains two groups of ganglion cells, with high and

low contrast sensitivity. Inrresr. Ophthal. visual Sci. 25,

170. Kaplan E. and Shapley R. (1984b) The origin of the

6 I

S(slow) potential in the mammalian lateral geniculatc

nucleus. E-t-p/ Bruin Rm. 55. I I I 116.

Land E. H. (1983) Recent advances in retinex theory and

some implications for cortical computations: Color

vision and the natural image. Proc. tz~l)t. ..Iuu/. SC/.

U.S.A. 80, 5163-5169.

Land E. H. and McCann J. J. (1971) The retinex theory

of vision. J. opr. SM. Am. 61. 1~ I I. Levick W. R., Cleland B. G. and Dubin M. W. (1972):

LGN neurons of the cat: retinal inputs and physiology.

Inrc~sr. Opl~thulrnol. risd .%i. I I, 302 3 I I

Milkman N., Schick G., Rossetto M., R~IIIIT F.. Shaplcy

R. and Victor J. (19X0) A two-dimensional computer-

controlled visual stimulator. fleh~c,. Ret. .Mvt/r /mv/r~r.

12, 283-292.

Nakayama K. and Mackeben M. (1982) Steady state visual

evoked potentials in the alert primate. I’ision Rev. 22.

1261~1271.

Ratliff F. (1965) Mac/z Buncfs. Holden-Day. San Francisco.

Calif.

Robson J. G. (1975) Receptive fields: spatial and intensive

representations of the visual image. In Hwdhook o/

Perception (Edited by Carterette and Friedman), Vol. 5.

Academic Press, New York.

Sakmann B. and Creutzfeld 0. D. Scotopic and mesopic

light adaptation in the cat’s retina. pfl~ger.~ urr$. ~c’s,

Physiol. 313, 168-185.

Schiller P. and Colby C. L. (1983) The response of single

cells in the lateral geniculate nucleus of rhesus monkeys

to color and luminance contrast. Vi.vion Rex It,

1631-1642.

Shapley R. M. (1982) Parallel pathways in the mammalian

visual system. Ann. N. Y. Acud. Sci. 388, I I-20.

Shapley R.. Kaplan E. and Soodak R. (1981) Spatial

summation and contrast sensitivity of X and Y cells in

the lateral geniculate nucleus of the macaque. iVrrrrrrr

292, 543-545.

Shapley R. and Enroth-Cugell C. (1984) Visual adaptation

and retinal gain controls. Prog. Rerind Res. 3, 263-346.

Shapley R. and Lennie P. (1985) Spatial frequency analysis

in the visual system. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 547-583.

Shapley R. and Reid R. C. (1985) Contrast and assimi-

lation in the perception of brightness. Proc,. nurn. .4ccrd.

Sri. U.S.A. 82, 5983-5986.

Spekreijse H., Estevez 0. and Reits D. (1977) Visual

evoked potentials and the physiological analysis of visual

processes in man. In Visuul evoked porenrid~ in mrm: nen’

developments (Edited by Desmedt J.), pp. I6 89. Clar-

endon Press, Oxford.

Sperling H. G., Crawford M. L. J. and Espinoza S. (1978)

Threshold spectral sensitivity of single neurons In the

lateral geniculate nucleus of performing monkeys. ~Mod-

em Prob. Ophthal. 19, 2-18.

Stockham T. G. (1972) Image processmg in the context 01

a visual model. Proc. I.E.E.E. 60, 828-842.

Whittle P. and Challands P. D. C. (1969) The effect of

background luminance on the brightness of flashes.

Vi.rion Res. 9, 1095. I I IO.

Wyszecki G. and Stiles W. S. (1967) Co/or Scirwe. Wiley,

New York