The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and...

65
The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attention An Empirical Study Bachelor Degree Project in Cognitive Neuroscience Basic level 22.5 ECTS Spring term 2019 Jonathan Bryde Supervisor: Kristoffer Ekman Examiner: Antti Revonsuo

Transcript of The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and...

Page 1: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation

on Affect and Attention

An Empirical Study

Bachelor Degree Project in Cognitive Neuroscience Basic level 22.5 ECTS Spring term 2019

Jonathan Bryde

Supervisor: Kristoffer EkmanExaminer: Antti Revonsuo

Page 2: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

Running Head: MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 2

Abstract

In daily life there are numerous experiences and events that divert people's attention and cause

stress, which may be linked with aspects of ill-being and lowered well-being. Mindfulness

meditation may alleviate such issues. Mindfulness can be summarized as a form of awareness and

attention in the present that is characterized by an open-minded and non-judgemental perspective,

and meditation as a group of practices that engage many of the same processes and may involve

mindfulness. There is evidence that both mindfulness and mindfulness meditation are associated

with activity in brain regions relating to, for example, attention, emotion-regulation, and bodily

awareness. Consequently, mindfulness meditation was hypothesized in the present study to improve

attention as measured by the Attention Network Test, and decrease negative affect as measured by

the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule when compared to a control condition. The mindfulness

meditation instructions employed were largely based on the work of Kabat-Zinn (1982). 14

participants were recruited to the study, and 7 of them completed the experiment. 3 participants

were randomized to the experimental group, and 4 to the control group. Results were largely

contrary to the hypotheses, with only executive attention having statistical significance (p < .05)

and supporting one hypothesis. Although effect sizes were on average large for the variables of the

study, the small sample size may have limited the power and increased the risk for type-II errors.

Keywords: attention, emotion, meditation, mindfulness, neural correlates

Page 3: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 3

Table of Contents

The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attention: An Empirical Study...............5

Mindfulness................................................................................................................................6

Perspectives on the Nature of Mindfulness..........................................................................7

Meditation: A Mindfulness Practice...................................................................................11

Criticism on Mindfulness-Related Research......................................................................13

Beneficial Effects of Mindfulness and Meditation.............................................................15

The Neural Correlates of Mindfulness.....................................................................................16

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI)..............................................................17

Other Methods of Measuring Brain Activity......................................................................23

Indirect Studies on Neural Networks.................................................................................24

Neural Correlates From a Broader Perspective..................................................................25

Review articles..............................................................................................................25

Meta-Analyses...............................................................................................................30

Summary, Purpose, and Hypotheses........................................................................................33

Method...............................................................................................................................33

Participants....................................................................................................................33

Procedure.......................................................................................................................35

Assessment measures....................................................................................................37

Positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS)......................................................37

Attention network test (ANT)...................................................................................38

Results......................................................................................................................................39

Discussion................................................................................................................................42

Limitations and Future Directions......................................................................................45

Conclusion..........................................................................................................................48

Page 4: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 4

References................................................................................................................................49

Appendix..................................................................................................................................64

Acknowledgements..................................................................................................................65

Page 5: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 5

The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attention: An Empirical Study

Each life has stressful and mindless moments that disperse attention in every which way.

Research has indicated that mind-wandering is commonly associated with lowered well-being

(Franklin et al., 2013; Killingsworth & Gilbert, 2010) and that psychological distress is strongly

connected to chronic pain and mental disorders (Blyth et al., 2001; Magni, Caldieron, Rigatti-

Luchini, & Merskey, 1990). Stress may be caused by mundane events but produce similar effects as

in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace sick leave (Lomas, Medina, Ivtzan,

Rupprecht, & Eiroa-Orosa, 2017), and can cause neurotoxic damage due to cortisol release (Khalsa,

2015). Additionally, mindlessness can reflect deficiencies in attention and a defensive stance where

percepts are ignored (Brown & Ryan, 2003).

Meditation is a process that generally involves a mental development or refinement and may

facilitate concentration, joy, or a calm state of mind (Walsh & Shapiro, 2006). Both mindfulness and

meditation can be said to concern present-moment attention and awareness (e.g., Baer, 2003), and

improve health in several ways (e.g., Eberth & Sedlmeier, 2012; Greeson, 2009). As such,

mindfulness meditation (e.g., Kabat-Zinn, 1982) may be one method to ameliorate elements of ill-

being and deficiencies in attention.

Further, Buddhist practice, which meditation is derived from, has as a central goal the

elimination of suffering and the premise that this must concern altering one's emotional and

cognitive states (Lutz, Dunne, & Davidson, 2007). Therefore, it appears germane to apply

mindfulness meditation to the alleviation of facets of ill-being and improvement of attention.

A purpose of this paper is to provide an overview of the extant literature on the topics of

mindfulness and meditation and present prominent and relevant research to clarify these

phenomena, including their associated neural correlates. This literature outline is provided to serve

as bedrock for the main purpose of this paper: an empirical study that will be conducted to

investigate the short-term effects of mindfulness meditation (For more details, see the Method

section).

Page 6: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 6

Article searches were conducted in the databases PubMed, MEDLINE, Scopus, Web of

Science, and Google Scholar using key words such as mindfulness, meditation, neural correlates,

and combinations thereof. References of select articles were additionally examined to follow up

both older and newer research in the respective fields and to identify recurring authors.

The paper will begin with an overview of the concept of mindfulness, after which different

views and theories on mindfulness will be presented. Afterward, meditation will be discussed in a

limited context and related to mindfulness and methods such as open monitoring. Moreover,

criticism on mindfulness and meditation research is considered. And meta-analyses on the impact of

meditation and mindfulness on ill- and well-being facets will be briefly reviewed. The neuroscience

of mindfulness will be examined and divided into sub-categories. Functional magnetic resonance

imaging (fMRI) studies will be introduced, and their results displayed alongside some

considerations for their design and limitations. A cursory overview will be provided for other tools

measuring brain activity, and a few studies on indirect measurements of attention networks are

presented. In addition, review articles and meta-analyses on mindfulness and meditation will be

described to broaden the perspective and limit bias. Following this, the main findings will be

summarized as a basis for the empirical study, and the purpose and hypotheses of the study

presented. The study proper is then described, including participant and procedure-related

information. Next, the measurement scales used in the study will be reviewed. Subsequently, the

obtained data of the experiment will be analyzed, and this is succeeded by a discussion concerning

the results. The discussion additionally contains a scrutiny of the limitations of the experiment,

future directions, and a conclusion regarding the overall study.

Mindfulness

Ahir (as cited in Wheeler, Arnkoff, & Glass, 2017) states that Mindfulness arose over 2.000

years ago from Buddhist meditation techniques. Fundamental elements of mindfulness are usually

described as receptive or conscious awareness and attention to experiences in the present (e.g.,

Brown & Ryan, 2003; Kabat-Zinn, 2003; Niemiec et al., 2010; Van Dam et al., 2018a). Further,

Page 7: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 7

mindfulness is characterized by a non-judgemental and open-minded attitude, among other

attributes such as intentionality (Brown, Ryan, & Creswell, 2007b; Chiesa, Calati, & Serretti, 2011;

Kabat-Zinn, 2003). Mindfulness may therefore select attention to extero- and interoceptive

information (McConnel & Froeliger, 2015), and function to increase behavioral regulation and

disengage individuals from automatic thoughts (Brown & Ryan, 2003; Niemiec et al., 2010).

According to Hölzel et al. (2011), a pillar of mindfulness-awareness of the body-was taught as the

first frame of reference within Theravada Buddhism.

Moreover, mindfulness can be said to contain a non-elaborative element (Chambers,

Gullone, & Allen, 2009), and this view of mindfulness can be explicated as relating to the

intentional use of sustained attention on occurring cognitive, emotional, and sensory experience

(Epstein, 1995) that does not pertain to the judging of said experience (Kabat-Zinn, 2003).

Perspectives on the Nature of Mindfulness

As relayed by Cullen (2011), mindfulness may be viewed as referring to more than the

meaning derived from its Buddhist origin of sati, which concerns a kind of attentiveness (For an

overview of these definitional terms, see Vago & Silbersweig, 2012). Rather, mindfulness may as a

central component relate to the ability to perceive events in the world in a manner not distorted by

mental states (e.g., emotions and moods; Cullen, 2011).

Comparably, the concept of reperceiving relates to the ability to disidentify with contents of

experience (e.g., emotions and thoughts) and shift perspectives of 'subject' to 'object' to achieve

greater coherence and objectivity (Shapiro, Carlson, Astin, & Freedman, 2006). Such a process is

said to be attainable through mindfulness, as reperceiving might be described as a mechanism

intrinsic to mindfulness, and is thought to lead to other mechanisms. One such additional

mechanism might be self-regulation (SR). By cultivating non-judgemental attention or awareness

automatic habits, such as behaviors or thoughts, may decrease as the individual has a greater degree

of freedom with which to understand and interrupt their own patterns (e.g., to avoid using alcohol to

cope with anxiety and instead perceive anxiety as an impermanent emotional state; Shapiro et al.,

Page 8: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 8

2006).

Baer, Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, and Toney (2006) analysed mindfulness questionnaires

to investigate the relationship between mindfulness and related constructs. Based on confirmatory

factor analysis, the questionnaire facets nonreact, nonjudge, act with awareness, and describe

appeared to constitute a larger mindfulness construct. Thus, it might be concluded that to desist

from automatic reactions to experience (non-reactivity) and from judgement about one's

experiences (non-judgement) are central parts of how to be mindful. However, questions concerning

differences on what mindfulness is and what it might lead to (i.e., its outcomes) are important in not

confounding the concept of mindfulness with its practice (Baer et al., 2006).

Chambers, Gullone, and Allen (2009) state that scientific definitions of mindfulness can be

juxtaposed based on if they identify awareness (e.g., Brown & Ryan, 2003), acceptance (i.e., non-

reactivity or non-judgement) and awareness (e.g., Bishop et al., 2004; Kabat-Zinn, 1982), or similar

behavioural factors such as observing and acting with awareness (e.g., Baer et al., 2006) as the

centrally defining features of mindfulness. Acceptance, in the context of mindfulness, may be

delineated as a form of non-reactive awareness where a person's experience is examined in the

absence of bias and habitual responding based on motivation to approach or avoid (Chambers,

Gullone, & Allen, 2009).

Lutz et al. (2015) attempt to avoid many of the issues concerning the term itself (e.g., the

differentiation between religious or spiritual perspectives and goals of mindfulness and

psychological and neuroscientific counterparts), and instead advocate identifying mindfulness as a

family of similar phenomenological states that emanate from the interaction of three main processes

(i.e., meta-awareness, object orientation, and dereification) and four secondary characteristics (i.e.,

effort, clarity, stability, and aperture). Some researchers (e.g., Bernstein et al., 2015) use similar

phenomenological mindfulness explanations as Lutz et al. (2015), but many other researchers (e.g.,

Brown & Ryan, 2003; Kabat-Zinn, 2003; Lindsay & Creswell, 2017) embrace the term in a manner

more in line with its historical foundation (i.e., as related to sati, among other concepts).

Page 9: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 9

Brown, Ryan, and Creswell (2007a) contrast mindfulness with self-focused attention and

self-awareness, stating that mindfulness relates to an unbiased and open-minded “awareness of and

attention to inner experience” (p. 273) that concerns occurring events rather than mental accounts of

the self. However, other researchers (e.g., Baer et al, 2006; Masicampo & Baumeister, 2007) have

proposed that there exist close connections between self-focused attention, self-control-a type of

SR-and mindfulness. But Brown, Ryan, and Creswell (2007a) reject these ideas on the basis that (a)

the evidence displays divergent results for self-focused attention and mindfulness, (b) there exist

differences in operational definitions of self-control and mindfulness, and (c) mindfulness tends to

facilitate SR instead of being a result of it.

Monitor and Acceptance Theory (MAT) proposes two chief components to explain

mindfulness effects (Lindsay & Creswell, 2017). MAT states that attention monitoring (e.g.,

present-moment awareness) is sufficient for the improvement of cognitive outcomes (e.g., working

memory, selective attention). But for improvement in stress, affective, and physical-health

outcomes, training is required in both acceptance (e.g., a non-judgemental and open attitude) and

attention monitoring according to MAT (Lindsay & Creswell, 2017).

Bishop et al. (2004) proposed an operational definition of mindfulness that relates to two

separate components: the regulation of attention as a focus on present-moment experience, and an

approach to all experience that is based in openness, curiosity, and acceptance. Bearing in mind

these ideas of Bishop et al. (2004) and Lindsay and Creswell (2017), it thus appears that central

concerns of mindfulness are the modification of one's attitude toward experiences (e.g., one's

inclination to judge and value events) and attention to current experiences.

On a similar note, Lomas et al. (2017) describe the associated decentered perspective

achieved while in mindful states of mind: a view which refers to the capacity to observe internal or

external events in a detached and impersonal manner. Shapiro et al. (2006) argue that such a

perspective is a meta-mechanism of action that is related to other aspects such as SR, and that SR

and other aspects (e.g., behavioral, emotional, and cognitive flexibility) are influenced by the

Page 10: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 10

decentered perspective to improve well-being and cause other positive outcomes.

In trying to clarify decentering and related phenomena, Bernstein et al. (2015) have

proposed a model that transcends the associated constructs (e.g., cognitive distancing, reperceiving,

self-as-context) and consists of three interrelated processes: (a) meta-awareness (i.e., awareness of

one's own subjective experience or the contents of consciousness), (b) disidentification from

internal experience (i.e., a detached experience of one's internal states), and (c) reduced reactivity to

thought content (e.g., avoiding social situations less due to negative thoughts, or not elaborating as

much on negative emotions). Lutz et al. (2015) have clarified that the term reactive (or non-

reactive) in this context is associated with the mindfulness aspect of being non-judgemental.

Reactive responses display affectively-valenced patterns and tend to relate to narrative or self-

related schemas (Lutz et al., 2015).

Accordingly, Bernstein et al. (2015) group the various constructs under their decentering

model based on the extent to which they engage the three processes of the model. For instance,

although mindfulness is described as only relating to the meta-awareness process in itself it may

generate disidentification as described by the decentering model (Bernstein et al., 2015). Meta-

awareness has been described as a way for an individual to adjust their behavior, and may therefore

be seen as a form of SR (Vago & Silbersweig, 2012).

Hölzel et al. (2011) argue that mindfulness may be described as functioning, by and large,

through four components: (1) attention regulation, (2) body awareness, (3) emotion-regulation (ER),

and (4) change in perspective on the self. While engaging in mindfulness practice (e.g., mindfulness

meditation) attention may be regulated by the focusing of attention on an object such as the breath,

and this practice can result in less distraction both during mindfulness practice and in life in general

(Hölzel et al, 2011). Additionally, while focusing on the breath or other objects of internal

experience a person may increase their body awareness as a result of such mindfulness application.

Mindfulness is generally linked with ER improvements and strategies such as reappraisal (e.g.,

reconstruing a past negative event as meaningful or beneficial). However, the precise nature of this

Page 11: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 11

connection is doubtful, as various studies have shown both greater and lesser cognitive control and

prefrontal activation in participants while utilizing mindfulness, with differences depending on

factors such as meditation expertise. Lastly, changes in perspective on the self may relate to insights

gained from using mindfulness, including enhanced awareness and clarity of thought and the

perspective of the self as part of ongoing mental activity that differs from moment to moment

(Hölzel et al., 2011).

Meditation: A Mindfulness Practice

As described by Baer (2003), mindfulness and meditation relate to each other in numerous

ways. For example, mindfulness not only originated from meditative practice but is developed

through various meditation trainings. Akin to mindfulness, meditation is concerned with the

voluntary and intentional regulation of one's attention in current experience. Furthermore, it is

consistent among divers meditation practices, like with many mindfulness practices, to promote the

selection of attention on internal bodily and mental events such as breathing, emotions, and

thoughts, and to external events such as sounds and sights (Baer, 2003). In fact, one of the most

highly cited methods of mindfulness training, mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR), employs

body-sweeping meditation, mindfulness meditation, and hatha yoga (Kabat-Zinn, 1982).

Van Dam et al. (2018a) state that much confusion regarding mindfulness and meditation

may emanate from undifferentiated usages of the words meditation and mindfulness, and this is an

observation that could be understood in that these concepts share many similarities. Vago and

Silbersweig (2012) concur with this view, stating that mindfulness can be viewed as a process, trait,

state, and intervention, and has been obfuscated with various interpretations including that of

meditation practice.

As the experiment described in this paper will not implement more than one form of

meditation, this paper will not in-depth examine a plethora of meditation types. Thus, although

researchers (e.g. Ospina et al., 2007) have identified types of meditation such as Qi Gong, Tai Chi,

Vipassana, and Zen Buddhist meditation, these will not be examined comprehensively in this paper.

Page 12: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 12

Jon Kabat-Zinn is one of the most frequently cited figures on what mindfulness is (Van Dam

et al., 2018a), and has been referred to in the context of how mindfulness can be used in meditative

practice (e.g., Baer, 2003). Kabat-Zinn (1982) defines two types of meditation: mindfulness

meditation and concentration meditation. Attention is restricted to a single point or object in

concentration meditation, whereas mindfulness meditation pertains to attention being maintained

steadily without a specific target and expands from a focus on breath to encompassing all physical

and mental events (Kabat-Zinn, 1982). Mindfulness meditation has also been defined as a non-

judgemental attention to current experiences (Tang, Hölzel, & Posner, 2015) and as a regulatory

attention training (Allen et al., 2012). Broadly, Kabat-Zinn (1982) refers to meditation as “the

intentional self-regulation of attention from moment to moment […] It is neither contemplation nor

rumination” (p. 34).

However, other researchers discriminate between focused attention (FA) meditation and

open monitoring (OM) meditation, with similar distinctions as between concentration and

mindfulness meditation respectively (e.g., Lutz, Jha, Dunne, & Saron, 2015; Lutz, Slagter, Dunne,

& Davidson, 2008; Raffone & Srinivasan, 2010; Travis & Shear, 2010). Some researchers (e.g.,

Nash & Newberg, 2013) use terms such as OM meditation and mindfulness meditation

interchangeably, while others (e.g., Fujino, Ueda, Mizuhara, Saiki, & Nomura, 2018; Tang et al,

2015) state that mindfulness meditation can be divided into methods relating to FA and OM.

Sperduti, Martinelli, and Piolino (2012) describe that schools of meditation can be categorized on a

continuum between FA and OM techniques, and that many use both of these practices

harmoniously.

Meditation encompasses core aspects such as emotional responsiveness, embodiment, and

silence (Kabat-Zinn, 2003). Moreover, mindfulness meditation may promote further valuing of low-

arousal positive states such as calmness or peacefulness (Galante, Galante, Bekkers, & Gallacher,

2014). Thus, when taking into account the characteristics of both mindfulness and meditation, it

appears these practices relate to attention and can be used separately or combined to create SR

Page 13: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 13

processes. As related by Tang et al. (2015), mindfulness meditation consists minimally of three

components that may be said to form an associated process of SR: (a) changed self-awareness (e.g.,

increased body-awareness, decreased self-referential processing), (b) improved ER, and (c)

facilitated attentional control.

Criticism on Mindfulness-Related Research

Some researchers (e.g., Chiesa & Malinowski, 2011; Van Dam et al., 2018a) have criticized

mindfulness research in general. Van Dam et al. (2018a) state that the term mindfulness carries

differing meanings depending on the researcher and context: a reflection of the popularity of the

concept. As a result, theoretical views on mindfulness vary and the concept may refer to attention,

memory, acceptance, Buddhist or meditation practice, or even intervention packages such as MBSR

(Lutz et al., 2015; Vago & Silbersweig, 2012; Van Dam et al., 2018a).

Criticism has also been levied against scientific research on meditation, stating there exists

no shared theoretical perspective and that the methodological quality is low (e.g., Nash & Newberg,

2013; Ospina et al., 2007; Van Dam et al., 2018a).

In addition, individuals may exhibit different effects from the same meditation practices,

including adverse effects such as increases in depression, anxiety, awareness of negative emotions,

and psychosis (Farias & Wikholm, 2016). The literature on harm in these contexts is limited, and

negative meditation effects occur more commonly in intensive retreat settings (Baer, Crane, Miller,

& Kuyken, in press). Adverse meditation experiences may occur at rates between 5 and 30% of

meditation sessions depending on time frame and sample (Van Dam et al., 2018b). One study

discovered that, while psychological stress decreased, salivary cortisol increased after mindfulness

meditation (Creswell, Pacilio, Lindsay, & Brown, 2014). This could be explained by greater coping

efforts among these participants, as other studies have displayed analogous results (Creswell et al.,

2014).

Davidson and Dahl (2018) have responded to some meditation-related criticisms. For

example, similar methodological issues as described by Van Dam et al. (2018a) have been a topic of

Page 14: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 14

discussion for psychologists on issues concerning emotion constructs such as anxiety. That is,

similar terms may be used by researchers for different states or contents of mind, and it is thus

recommended that one specifies operational definitions when measuring psychological constructs

(Davidson & Dahl, 2018). Furthermore, meditation practices have as a prominent focus the well-

being of humans, and their use in clinical or disease-treatment settings is a relatively recent

development (Davidson & Dahl, 2018).

Imprecise or varying usages of the word meditation is discussed by Lutz, Dunne, and

Davidson (2007). Intellectual movements, such as perennialism, may pose a problem for

neuroscience because ideas that surpass reason, perception, or language could be unverifiable (Lutz

et al., 2007). Moreover, unspecific uses of terms such as meditation trivializes the practice itself and

can lead to a failure to detect underlying processes engaging different neural circuitry (Lutz et al.,

2007).

Nash and Newberg (2013) state that the lack of unified meditation definitions can be seen in

that there are two opposing viewpoints on what meditation is within the scientific literature. From

one perspective meditation may be categorized as a group of mental training techniques, and from

another regarded as experiential or altered states of consciousness derived from the same methods

(Nash & Newberg, 2013). In this paper, the perspective concerning mental training will be used as a

foundation.

Based on the dichotomy of the mental training and altered state definitions of meditation,

Nash and Newberg (2013) propound a definitional model that regards meditation as a dynamic

process of six stages. The first stage, normal, occurs before and after meditation and is the waking

or resting state associated with the default mode network (DMN). The second stage, intention to

begin, is described as crucial in the sense that volition is required for a person to engage in the

meditative process. The third stage, preliminaries, relates to preparation where a person might

assume a position for meditation. The fourth stage, method, consists in the practices or procedures

of meditation (e.g., FA, OM). The fifth stage, enhanced mental state, is the causal result of the

Page 15: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 15

method, and might relate to increases in concentration, calm, well-being, and so forth. Lastly, the

sixth stage, intention to finish, comprises the individual's choice to stop meditating and return to

their baseline state of consciousness (Nash & Newberg, 2013).

Beneficial Effects of Mindfulness and Meditation

As potential support of the regulatory aspect or outcome of mindfulness, individuals who

score highly in the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale are less likely to be socially anxious or

ruminate, and more likely to fulfil basic psychological needs (Brown & Ryan, 2003). Accordingly,

this may indicate that mindfulness is linked in some sense to peaceful or low-affect states of mind.

In truth, mindfulness training has been demonstrated to enhance inner peace without being mediated

by changes in self-rated mindfulness (Liu et al., 2015). Other research has displayed positive

correlations between mindfulness and peace of mind (Xu, Rodriguez, Zhang, & Liu, 2015).

Feldman, Greeson, and Senville (2010) compared the effect of loving-kindness meditation,

progressive muscle relaxation, and mindful breathing on decentering. In their research, the mindful

breathing condition increased decentering more than the other conditions both when measured as a

correlation between the frequency of and emotional reactions to repetitive thought and through a

self-report scale. These results may indicate that decentered perspectives can increase awareness of

repetitive thought and decrease the prevalence of repetitive thought via mechanisms such as

emotional processing and habituation (Feldman, Greeson, & Senville, 2010).

Furthermore, mindfulness interventions have been associated with reductions in anxiety,

depression, burnout, anger, stress, and distress, and with increases in satisfaction, compassion, ER,

and health, among other factors (Greeson, 2009; Lomas et al., 2017). Mindfulness practice can

reduce bottom-up processing of cues (e.g., craving and stress; McConnel & Froeliger, 2015), and

may be able to increase inhibitory control of negative emotions (Garland et al., in press).

Mindfulness is associated with improved executive cognitive functions such as working memory,

attentional control, and sustained attention (Chambers, Gullone, & Allen, 2009). One study also

reported that mindfulness may improve the SR skills of youths, improving their ability to meet

Page 16: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 16

school demands, decreasing anxiety, and so forth (Zelazo & Lyons, 2012).

Meta-analyses and reviews on mindfulness meditation procedures have displayed reductions

in depression, stress, pain, anxiety, substance-abuse, and fibromyalgia, and benefits in well-being,

executive functioning, and ER (Eberth & Sedlmeier, 2012; Gallant, 2016; Goyal et al., 2014; Tang

et al., 2015; Toneatto & Nguyen, 2007). One meta-analysis calculated a total effect size of

mindfulness meditation in 39 studies at 0.56, which indicates that meditation benefits are not due to

chance (Eberth & Sedlmeier, 2012 ). A clinical trial utilizing various meditation procedures,

including Hatha yoga and body scan meditation, found improvements in stress, trait and state

anxiety, indicators of psychopathology, and sleep quality (Biegel, Brown, Shapiro, & Schubert,

2009). The benefits of meditation are on average comparable in impact to those of psychotherapy or

behavioral treatments (Sedlmeier et al., 2012).

Breathing is a prominent element in the majority of meditative practices and is closely

connected to mental processes (Zaccaro et al., 2018). A review by Zaccaro et al. (2018) indicates

that slow breathing techniques recruit the parasympathetic nervous system, as suggested by

increases in respiratory sinus arrhythmia and heart-rate variability. Other outcomes of slow

breathing includes increased blood-oxygen-level dependent activity in areas of the brain such as the

thalamus and hypothalamus, the pons, and the parietal, motor, and prefrontal cortices (Critchley et

al., 2015). Slow breathing appears to promote the parasympathetic nervous system while being

mediated by vagal activity, enhance psychological, cerebral, and autonomic interactions, and can

increase well-being and emotional control (Zaccaro et al., 2018). Ditto, Edache, and Goldman

(2006) found similarly that meditation compared to relaxation increased parasympathetic activity.

As such, one may infer that the breathing component of meditative practices accounts for some of

their benefits.

The Neural Correlates of Mindfulness

The neuroscience of mindfulness can be considered an interdisciplinary field combining

neuroscientific research with mindfulness practice (Tang & Posner, 2013). In this field there is an

Page 17: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 17

overlap between various measures and techniques such as neuroimaging, behavioral tests,

mindfulness-based meditation, and mindfulness-based intervention (Tang & Posner, 2013).

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI)

There are numerous studies that have employed fMRI to examine neural correlates related to

mindfulness and types of meditation.

In one study, Westbrook et al. (2011) trained participants to view and rate smoking and

neutral images using mindful attention or by being passive. The mindful attention group had

decreased neural activity in the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (ACC); a region associated with

craving. The mindful attention group also displayed reduced craving via self-report. Thus, by

reducing craving to smoking cues mindfulness may promote smoking cessation (Westbrook et al.,

2011). Although self-reporting of craving may have limited predictive power, there is much support

for the connection between drug usage and craving of drugs (Westbrook et al., 2011).

The study by Westbrook et al. (2011) could be confounded, anyhow, due to the strong desire

to stop smoking expressed by the participants. That is, even if the desire to quit smoking was

controlled by allocating participants to the different groups, since all participants wanted strongly to

quit smoking they might not represent the general population of smokers. In contrast, Westbrook et

al. (2011) controlled for many variables (e.g., medication, brain injury, mental disorder, smoking

before brain scanning), and this increases confidence in the results.

In a study implementing mindful breathing to achieve a non-judgmental concentration,

Hölzel et al. (2007) report that participants had increased rostral ACC and dorsal medial prefrontal

cortex (MPFC) activation. This could be explained by increased processing of emotional conflict

during mindfulness and improved ER as reflected by increased emotion processing, respectively

(Hölzel et al., 2007).

Mindfulness was compared with a mental arithmetic task, but the mindfulness condition

lasted twice as long (1 min) as the arithmetic task (Hölzel et al., 2007). Additionally, the increased

cerebral hemodynamic response observed for meditators over controls is not necessarily explained

Page 18: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 18

by greater mindfulness, since groups were not randomized but self-selected (directionality

problem).

Dispositional mindfulness was associated with increased PFC and decreased amygdala

activity in an affect labelling task (Creswell, Way, Eisenberger, & Lieberman, 2007). While

assigning emotions (e.g., scared, angry) to facial expressions, it is thus possible that greater

mindfulness augments the neural regulation of affect-related areas such as limbic regions (Creswell

et al., 2007).

Jang et al. (2011) conducted an fMRI study comparing controls to meditators who focused

their attention on their emotions and bodily sensations. Meditators had increased MPFC activity,

and this is an area with connections to exteroceptive and interoceptive modalities (Jang et al., 2011).

Garrison, Zeffiro, Scheinost, Constable, and Brewer (2015) instructed participants in an

fMRI study in one of three different meditation methods: (1) to concentrate on their breath, (2) to

adopt a loving-kindness perspective and repeat positive mantras, and (3) to monitor their

experiences without any explicit focus. The results for these different conditions appeared to have

been amalgamated by Garrison et al. (2015) and were compared to a cognitive task where

participants judged adjectives.

In the study, mindfulness practice was linked to decreased activity in the ventral precuneus,

ACC, posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), and middle temporal gyrus, and with increased activity in

the bilateral rectal gyrus and orbitofrontal cortex when compared to the cognitive task (Garrison et

al., 2015). The aforementioned areas associated with the DMN also displayed lower activity in

meditators during rest. Finally, functional connectivity in parts of the DMN appears to generally

have differed between meditators and controls. However, none of these results were differentiated

for the three meditation methods implemented. Garrison et al. (2015) state that the results could

imply differences in both DMN resting-state and active-state processing, and in the behavioral state

of individuals in the resting state. For example, analogous decreases in activation of the PCC were

connected to reports of decreased mind-wandering and increased focused attention in meditators in

Page 19: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 19

an fMRI study by Garrison et al. (2013).

When compared to individuals without prior mindfulness experience, mindfulness

practitioners have displayed significant activity reductions in the MPFC as well as increased

activity in the insular cortex (IC), second somatosensory cortex, and right lateral PFC (Farb et al.,

2007). These findings may support the idea that, with mindfulness training, narrative self-focus is

less likely to occur in the presence of momentary self-focus, whereas people who do not engage in

intentional mindfulness practice are more likely to engage narrative self-focus and momentary self-

focus concurrently. In other words, the two forms of self-awareness may be dissociated through

attention training (Farb et al., 2007). The specific training used by Farb et al. (2007) for their fMRI

study was packaged in a MBSR program where participants trained daily in focusing attention on

the present moment.

Brewer et al. (2011) investigated neural patterns via fMRI across meditation practices

concerning concentration, loving-kindness, and choiceless awareness. It was discovered that these

different ways of being mindful each, when contrasted with controls, displayed decreased activation

in the MPFC and the PCC; two predominant areas of the DMN associated with mind-wandering,

self-related thought, and evaluating one's emotional states (Brewer et al., 2011). These results may

be explained by the roles of mindfulness-based practices in orienting the mind away from

wandering, self-reference, and identifying with experiences (Brewer et al., 2011).

However, the study sample was somewhat small and consisted in only highly experienced

meditators. Thus, such results are not necessarily generalizable to larger and less experienced

populations. But there was convergent validity in that all three meditation methods had similar

results (Brewer et al., 2011), and that increases confidence in the results.

Another fMRI study found that experienced compared to novice meditators had decreased

activation in brain regions associated with sustained attention at 44.000 hours of experience but not

at 19.000 hours (Brefczynski-Lewis, Lutz, Schaefer, Levinson, & Davidson, 2007). This may imply

that brain plasticity is involved (Brefczynski-Lewis et al., 2007): an interpretation that Hasenkamp

Page 20: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 20

and Barsalou (2012) arrived at likewise when studying meditation effects on attention.

Furthermore, Hasenkamp and Barsalou (2012) distinguished neural patterns during FA

meditation depending on different cognitive activation. For example, sustained attention was

associated with executive brain networks, and awareness of mind wandering with the salience

network (Hasenkamp & Barsalou, 2012) In their study, Hasenkamp and Barsalou instructed

participants to focus on the sensations of the breath on the nostrils and upper lip. This is different

from many other general mindful breathing instructions (e.g., Feldman et al., 2010; Garrison et al.,

2015; Hölzel et al., 2007), but not necessarily confounding because it might still achieve the same

or a similar present-moment attention.

Allen et al. (2012) produced data indicating that mindfulness meditation improved executive

neural processing and cognitive control as measured by a Stroop task. In the same study it was

further observed that blood oxygen level-dependent activity in areas such as the dorsal lateral PFC

(DLPFC) and dorsal ACC increased. Allen et al. (2012) forward two explanations for this data: (a)

that it underlies increased FA in novices, and (b) that it reflects an accentuated open-mindedness

and sensitivity to affect in participants with higher practice adherence.

The study by Allen et al. (2012) had a sample size of 60, which is larger than many studies

on meditation. Through exclusion criteria, possible confounding variables such as prior meditation

experience, mental disorders, drugs and recreational substances, and age were controlled for. The

mindfulness meditation practice included a focus on breath, body-scanning, a compassionate

perspective, and an open-monitoring method designed to unify all of these elements (Allen et al.,

2012).

The instructions were inspired by MBSR programs and insight meditation traditions such as

Vipassana (Allen et al., 2012). These varying aspects of the meditation potentially confound its

effect. It is not clear if compassion or Vipassana practice during a monitoring of the present moment

facilitates attention, if they synergize, or if other effects emerge from these various elements. And

since Allen et al. (2012) do not clarify how the differing elements together form the mindfulness

Page 21: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 21

meditation method, one can not know how the described methods were delivered to participants

other than by the guidance of experienced instructors.

Manna et al. (2010) explored differences between six minutes of Samatha (FA) and

Vipassana (OM) meditation and rest. Groups were comprised of eight experienced (15.000 hours)

and eight novice meditators, and meditation instructions were written by a meditation expert from a

monastery. In the experiment, all participants meditated based on FA and then OM instructions, with

breaks (3 min) before and after. This was repeated three times (Manna et al., 2010).

Overall, FA and OM meditation were associated with dissociable brain systems concerning

selective attention, conflict monitoring, and sustained attention (Manna et al., 2010). For instance,

FA was related to increased ACC and decreased PCC activity. And OM was associated with lateral

PFC activity compared to the FA condition, and with precuneus activation compared to both rest

and FA. For many of the contrasts, activation was discovered in the left but not right hemisphere

(Manna et al., 2010).

The religious elements in the study by Manna et al. (2010), such as the reliance on aspects of

the Buddhist school of Theravada, could be criticized as imparting a kind of bias onto the study. At

the same time, it appears the FA and OM meditation methods were not out of the ordinary and that

the pauses used could have decreased sequence effects. The sample was very small, however.

A recent study indicated that FA and OM meditation may decrease functional connectivity in

regions linked to mind-wandering (e.g., areas of the DMN such as the PCC; Fujino et al., 2018).

But whereas FA meditation increased connectivity in attention areas, OM meditation decreased

connectivity in these same regions (e.g., the visual cortex). As such, there may be attention-

regulation differences between OM and FA meditation (Fujino et al., 2018). The sample size was

low (n=17), but the within-subject design decreases this issue to some extent.

Jang et al. (2011) coordinated a study comparing 35 experienced meditators with 33

participants that had no experience with meditation or similar practices. An increased connectivity

in the anterior MPFC was discovered for meditators. This connectivity may derive from the brain's

Page 22: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 22

neuronal plasticity, as a result of a meditation practice that asks participants to concentrate on their

emotions and bodily experiences as they move with natural, rhythmic motions. Nevertheless, the

study is limited in its conclusions since the design was cross-sectional. Thus, it is uncertain whether

the meditation increased MPFC connectivity, or if the groups differed on this variable for other

reasons. For example, the part of the population that chooses to meditate may already differ from

the general population (Jang et al., 2011).

Jang et al. (2011) discuss changes in the MPFC after meditation, stating that it is an area

linked with interoceptive and exteroceptive modalities. According to Jang et al. (2011), the MPFC is

activated by mental simulations regarding the self. Moreover, several findings from the wider

literature seem to reinforce the idea that the reduced mental activity observed in connection to

meditation is mediated by activation in brain networks supporting internal attention (Jang et al.,

2011).

Mindfulness may incorporate methods of sustaining attention to interoceptive information

(e.g., the breath; Farb, Segal, & Anderson, 2013). For this reason, Farb et al. (2013) contrasted an

eight-week MBSR group to a wait-list control group to study the effects of mindfulness training on

interoceptive attention. In the experiment, the two groups were compared while engaging in

interoceptive (monitoring of the breath) and exteroceptive (reading words or pressing keys based on

sights) attention tasks. In comparison to controls, MBSR participants had increased recruitment of

the posterior IC and decreased MPFC activity corresponding to the mindfulness goal (Farb et al.,

2013).

There are several possible extraneous variables in the study. Some parts of the MBSR course

were not connected to mindfulness or meditation (i.e., stress management education and diary

writing). And a large portion of the participants reporting being highly stressed and experiencing

negative emotions (Farb et al., 2013).

As many meditation studies compare experienced meditators to novices, Dickenson,

Berkman, Arch, and Lieberman (2012) wished to investigate which neural systems support

Page 23: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 23

mindfulness meditation in those without notable amounts of meditation practice. Comparing

focused to unfocused attention groups, Dickenson et al. (2012) found significant activation in

regions involved with attentional control such as the temporal-parietal junction and superior parietal

lobule. Areas believed to mediate attention to response selection and sensory information, such as

the dorsal ACC, were also recruited. Additionally, regions such as the insula and hippocampus

displayed increased activation. Finally, unfocused attention was associated with activation in

regions related to the DMN while focused attention was conversely related to the same areas.

Dickenson et al. (2012) suggest that, since trait mindfulness measures were correlated with activity

in attention areas, this may imply that brain systems associated with mindfulness are meaningfully

related to each other outside of experimental settings.

Other Methods of Measuring Brain Activity

Using electroencephalography, a study found that dispositional mindfulness might decrease

the amplitude of the late positive potential: a neural marker of ER (Brown, Goodman, & Inzlicht,

2013). Based on highlighted research on the late positive potential by Brown et al. (2013), it

appears it is an indicator of attention to emotional stimuli and emotional arousal. Thus, the late

positive potential seems an appropriate tool for measuring how mindfulness can affect ER.

The effect of a meditation practice centred on mindfulness was tested with thermal pain

training (Grant, Courtemanche, Duerden, Duncan, & Rainville, 2010). Grant et al. (2010)

discovered that meditators had decreased pain sensitivity when compared to controls, and this was

linked with thicker cortex in pain and affect-related areas such as the anterior IC and ACC seen via

MRI.

From a study employing diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) alongisde MRI, it has been

uncovered that experienced mindfulness practitioners using FA methods (e.g., mantras, mindful-

breathing, monitoring of the body) have greater fractional anisotropy in the anterior corpus

callosum, which might be linked to the ability to regulate the fluctuations of the mind (Luders et al.,

2012).

Page 24: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 24

The study design of Luders et al. (2012) could be criticized since participants of the

experimental group had variance in their meditative traditions. Can you treat participants influenced

by Kriya, Vipassana, and Chenrezig practices all as one group, or would that perhaps be an

oversimplification? The relatively moderate sample size (n=30) and high amount of average

meditative experience (20 years) further decreases the likelihood that the results generalize outside

of the study sample.

Indirect Studies on Neural Networks

Although studies with attention tasks by themselves do not examine neural changes, they

could still be related to activation in brain areas. That is, if said attention tasks, for instance the

ANT, are theorized in relation to brain networks in their foundation.

In a study using meditation retreats and methods such as FA, loving-kindness, and mindful

breathing, executive attention but not orienting attention was improved (Elliott, Wallace, &

Giesbrecht, 2014).

Another study implementing 10 minutes of mindfulness meditation training concluded that

participants benefited from the meditation in terms of improved attentional resource allocation,

based on ANT accuracy and reaction time results (Norris, Creem, Hendler, & Kober, 2018).

However, in this study participants were rewarded by a chance to earn one of two 25-dollar prizes

in a lottery (Norris et al., 2018). Moreover, the meditation was guided as participants listened to

audio-tapes, but whether participants followed the instructions delivered through the tape was not

verified (although it may be inferred from the attention results).

Jha et al. (2007) contrasted meditation retreat and MBSR groups with controls. Meditation

involving concentration was connected to improvements in executive and orienting attention, which

Jha et al. (2007) state may reflect changes in the dorsal attention system. More experienced

concentration meditators also displayed greater alerting attention than the other groups (Jha et al.,

2007). Both meditation groups employed sitting and walking meditation, although receptive

attention practice similar to OM meditation was included for the MBSR group during the fifth

Page 25: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 25

week. The amount of meditation differed radically between the groups, with the retreat group

meditating up to 12 hours per day.

Srinivasan and Singh (2017) report that FA meditation can increase the perceived duration

and sharpness of afterimages. This could suggest that improved attentional focus can affect

phenomenal experiences in visual awareness (Srinivasan & Singh, 2017). But the quality of this

study is questionable, as the sample size was low (n=25) and the meditators were teachers of Sahaj

Samadhi meditation. It could be argued that this meditation tradition contains many typical

mindfulness and meditation elements (e.g., present-moment awareness, mantra recitation), but one

could equally state that the sample may not be generalizable to general mindfulness or meditation

populations due to aspects such as the heavy spiritual connotation of Sahaj Samadhi.

Moore and Malinowski (2009) conducted a study comparing Buddhist meditators to a

matched control group. Mindfulness meditation improved attention, such as by decreasing Stroop

interference. Some of the attention improvements might reflect the ability of the meditators to focus

attention and sustain said focus over time (Moore & Malinowski, 2009). The meditators were

described as experienced in mindfulness meditation, but which instructions or method they

followed was not detailed and decreases replicability. Moore and Malinowski (2009) controlled for

sleep prior to participation, and noise during participation.

Neural Correlates From a Broader Perspective

Singular articles may point in one or another direction and contradict each other, but review

articles can illuminate commonalities across a broader range of research to limit such issues.

Review articles. Cahn and Polich (2006) reviewed meditation practices involving aspects such as

mindfulness or FA (e.g., with the breath as focus). A variety of meditation traditions and research

designs were included in the review, and Cahn and Polich (2006) attribute part of the divergence in

results to these aspects. Meditation practice generally increases attentional resources and ACC and

prefrontal brain areas appear to be recruited (Cahn & Polich, 2006).

According to Tang and Posner (2013), several studies have used cortical thickness mapping

Page 26: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 26

and diffusion tensor imaging to explore the long-term effects of meditation on brain structure. In

these studies, meditators have displayed significantly greater cortical thickness compared to

controls in the anterior regions of the brain including the MPFC, superior frontal cortex, temporal

pole, and the middle and interior temporal cortices. Moreover, reductions in cortical thickness were

found in the posterior regions of the brain including the postcentral cortex, inferior parietal cortex,

middle occipital cortex, and PCC. Lastly, in the region adjacent to the MPFC, both higher fractional

anisotropy values and greater cortical thickness have been observed (Tang & Posner, 2013).

One interpretation of this data is that thinner cortical thickness of brain regions in

meditators, including the lateral and medial parietal areas, can be explained as influenced by the

enhancement of cognitive functions such as improved attention and self-perception (Kang et al.,

2012). The increase in cortical thickness in frontal and temporal areas may be elucidated by

reference to emotion processing. For instance, previous research has reported beneficial effects of

meditation on ER, and the MPFC is involved in ER (Kang et al., 2012). Note that the article by

Kang et al. (2012) is not a review article, but was referenced here to explain results not elucidated

but referred to by Tang and Posner (2013).

McConnell and Froeliger (2015) compared mindfulness mechanisms to addiction research.

There is evidence that mindfulness practice is associated with activation in the striatum, amygdala,

and frontolimbic and frontostriatal MPFC: areas that have been described as related to affect and

ER (McConnell & Froeliger, 2015).

One common thesis to describe how mindfulness meditation can influence ER is the idea

that prefrontal cognitive control mechanisms are facilitated to downregulate affect-processing in

areas such as the amygdala (Tang et al., 2015). Results regarding regions such as the anterior IC,

thalamus, and orbitofrontal cortex, and mechanisms such as nociceptive and pain processing, appear

to support the thesis that mindfulness meditation can improve ER (Tang et al., 2015).

Meditation practices are regularly accompanied by an emphasis on attention-regulation as a

necessary component, and on attentional control as a core aspect (Tang et al., 2015). The ACC and

Page 27: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 27

DLPFC have continually been linked with mindfulness meditation effects on attention (Tang et al.,

2015).

In an overview by Chiesa et al. (2011), meditative practices containing mindfulness

compared to controls were associated with executive attention improvements in nine studies. For

example, van den Hurk, Giommi, Gielen, Speckens, and Barendregt (2010) discovered

improvements in orienting and executive attention for mindfulness meditators. And for participants

on a MBSR course implementing meditation practices such as body scan meditation and mindful

breathing, mindfulness was associated with non-directed attention but not attentional control

(Anderson, Lau, Segal, & Bishop, 2007). There was high variation in study design and positive

results were commonly displayed in case-control studies (Chiesa et al., 2011). Therefore, the

conclusions of the overview should be considered with caution to an extent.

Gallant (2016) conducted a systematic review of the connection between mindfulness

practice (primarily mindfulness meditation) and aspects of executive functioning. Mindfulness

practices may improve executive functioning components such as inhibition. Gallant (2016)

underlines that several mindfulness studies have documented alterations in areas important for

executive functions (e.g., the ACC).

Lutz et al. (2008) state that effects of OM meditation can be understood from a global

perspective, where moment-to-moment processing is affected by a changed default mode

functioning. However, these mechanisms are predictions that currently lack much empirical

support. Further, there is speculation that, since OM meditation has no direct attentional focus, it

should not have to depend on brain areas connected to sustaining attention on specific objects but

on areas involved in monitoring and disengaging attention from distractions (Lutz et al., 2008).

In their review, Wheeler et al. (2017) state that research indicates that individuals high in

dispositional mindfulness have increased grey matter volume in areas of the amygdala and IC,

which is associated with greater emotional control. Furthermore, greater dispositional mindfulness

is connected to activation in regions of the PFC such as the DLPFC (Wheeler et al., 2017).

Page 28: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 28

Wheeler et al. (2017) additionally review the relation of mindfulness to the DMN: the brain

at rest. The DMN is related to present-moment self-related processing (e.g., interoception) and

narrative self-related processing (e.g., episodic memory). Whereas rumination and mind-wandering

have been connected to narrative self-related processing, mindfulness has been related to present-

moment self-related processing. Brain areas such as the right somatosensory cortex, IC, and MPFC

have been associated with present-moment self-related processing (Wheeler at al., 2017).

This perspective is supported by other researchers. For example, Jang et al. (2011) state that

brain regions associated with mindfulness practice (e.g., meditation) are notably alike those that

have been connected to the DMN. Areas continually activated during stimulus-independent

thoughts (e.g., reflective and self-referential thoughts) believed to be part of the DMN include the

lateral temporal cortex, inferior parietal cortex, PCC, ACC, and MPFC (Jang et al., 2011).

Vago and Silbersweig (2012) have connected the mechanisms of attention, SR, and ER in

the context of mindfulness practice, stating that there is overlap between self- and emotion-

regulation and that they, in general, refer to the ability to modulate emotional activity and

attentional focus. Thus, brain areas (e.g., the DLPFC and ACC) that have by other researchers (e.g.,

Tang et al., 2015) been related to attention, are also linked to ER by Vago and Silbersweig (2012).

Furthermore, Vago and Silbersweig (2012) describe that mindfulness practices (e.g., types of

meditation) can affect other regulatory functions such as stress and homeostasis by, for instance,

reducing hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity. For example, various meditation studies have

affected aspects such as cortisol, oxygen consumption, and blood pressure, and displayed reduced

sympathetic and increased parasympathetic nervous system activity (Vago & Silbersweig, 2012).

Moreover, Vago and Silbersweig (2012) proposed a framework focusing on self-awareness,

-regulation, and -transcendence (S-ART) to better understand mindfulness and its neural correlates.

The elements of S-ART are based on brain networks relating to self-processing. Self-processing is

further divided by Vago and Silbersweig, as inspired by other research, into three terms based on

characteristics of self-processing concerning agency and consciousness (e.g., self-reflection and

Page 29: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 29

sensory-affective-motor processing): (a) the enactive experiential self (EES), (b) the experiential

phenomenological self (EPS), and (c) the evaluative narrative self (NS; Vago & Silbersweig, 2012).

The EES concerns non-conscious neural activity and has been associated with areas of the

brain such as the posterior IC, thalamus and hypothalamus, and midbrain colliculi, which are

regions involved in interoceptive feedback (Vago & Silbersweig, 2012). The EES is also connected

to networks regarding preparatory behavior and action selection, such as the pre-motor area,

cerebellum, and ventral lateral PFC (Vago & Silbersweig, 2012).

The EPS relates to self-specifying experience in which there exists awareness that can be

affected by cognition (Vago & Silbersweig, 2012). The EPS is linked with attentional networks and

functions such as top-down processing, and is thought to construct higher order experience such as

affective, motivational, and social feelings. Areas that are believed to be important for the EPS

include the dorsal ACC, temporo-parietal junction, and the right DLPFC. Studies have indicated

that the anterior IC has a role in states associated with the EPS, and that its activity is inversely

correlated to areas connected with the NS such as the PCC and posteromedial cortex (Vago &

Silbersweig, 2012).

Lastly, the NS concerns a form of metacognitive knowledge and reflective identification of

the self via narratives. The NS is linked with the hippocampal-cortical memory system and,

consequently, brain structures such as the amygdala, medial parietal cortex, and areas of the

cingulate cortex. The NS has been related to task-negative networks (Vago & Silbersweig, 2012).

The DNM, a task-negative network, is associated with mind-wandering and thought processes

(Hasenkamp & Barsalou, 2012), which is similar to what the NS relates to.

Conversely, present-moment and attention processes are, in parallel with the EES and EPS,

related to task-positive networks (Hasenkamp & Barsalou, 2012). Present-moment processing has,

moreover, been linked with the salience network: a network that involves the anterior IC and dorsal

ACC (Seeley et al., 2007). Another system, the frontoparietal control system, appears to integrate

information from the networks associated with the NS, EES, and EPS, and its substrates (e.g., the

Page 30: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 30

frontopolar cortex) facilitate meta-monitoring and might be imperative to SR and self-awareness

(Vago & Silbersweig, 2012). Mindfulness intervention is reported to decrease NS activity and

increase EPS and frontoparietal control system activity (Vago & Silbersweig, 2012).

Kerr, Sacchet, Lazar, Moore, and Jones (2013) proposed a framework of neural mechanisms

to illuminate the effects of standardized mindfulness training (e.g., MBSR). Kerr et al. (2013)

describe that, based on conventional mindfulness components (e.g., present-moment attention,

awareness of mind-wandering), mindfulness training might be reflected at the neural level in brain

alterations concerning signal-to-noise ratio in sensory-attentional processing.

More precisely, the body-focused attention of mindfulness might enhance the 7-14 Hz alpha

rhythm believed to be implicated in regulating sensory input (Kerr et al., 2013). There are some

preliminary studies that appear to support the framework of Kerr et al. (2013), but few of the neural

functions connected to mindfulness in their overview are thoroughly studied.

Meta-Analyses. Reviews have their place in synthesizing data and inferring conclusions from a

broader perspective. However, it requires a meta-analysis to statistically analyse such

commonalities and display their significance.

One meta-analysis attempted to locate similarities in brain activation across dissimilar

meditation practices (Sperduti et al., 2012). Based on 10 neuroimaging studies involving both OM

and FA meditation practices, there was prominent activation in the MPFC, caudate nucleus, and

enthorinal cortex. Sperduti et al. (2012) discuss these results by, for instance, mentioning the high

involvement of the enthorinal cortex in ER and the potential role of the striatum in response

inhibition, ER, and attention processes.

The 10 analyzed studies had a total of 91 participants, which means the average sample size

was very small. Sperduti et al. (2012) did not appear to account for any differences in the meditative

traditions they collected under the labels FA and OM aside from noting that practices such as Yoga

(e.g., Kundalini) and Tantra meditation were included. This may confound the results.

In a meta-analysis by Fox et al. (2014), eight brain regions were routinely associated with

Page 31: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 31

change in meditators over 21 morphometric studies: (a) the frontopolar cortex, (b) the sensory

cortices and IC, (c) the hippocampus, (d) the ACC, (e) the mid-cingulate cortex, (f) the orbitofrontal

cortex, (g) the superior longitudinal fasciculus, and (h) the corpus callosum. Meditation types from

the analyzed studies included concentration, OM, and mindfulness methods.

The results can be interpreted as revealing that mindfulness meditation possibly activates

brain areas that, for example, relate to memory, hemispherical communication, ER, cognitive

awareness, and body awareness. A medium (d=.46) effect size was calculated for 16 of the studies

(Fox et al., 2014), and may indicate that the observed brain changes are non-trivial.

Nonetheless, it should be noted that the meta-analysis by Fox et al. (2014) encompassed a

multitude of meditation practices (e.g., Zen, Soham, and insight meditation) and that data were

collapsed across these practices. Another possible issue with the meta-analysis is that data from

different morphometric neuroimaging techniques were compared. Fox et al. (2014) state,

nevertheless, that there is preliminary evidence for the validity of such comparisons.

Structural changes were discovered in several brain regions in the meta-analysis, with

significance depending on the selected thresholds (Fox et al., 2014). As with many previous studies,

meditation affected the IC. The structural changes of the IC may be due to its implication in

interoception and, tentatively, its function in metacognitive awareness and emotional self-

awareness. Further, the primary and secondary somatomotor cortices were affected, which may be

from the body-awareness focus of some the types of meditation analyzed. Other areas, such as ones

relating to instrospection (the rostrolateral PFC), SR and ER (e.g., the ACC and orbitofrontal

cortex), and spatial processing (the superior longitudinal fasciculus), also exhibited structural gray

or white-matter changes (Fox et al., 2014).

Fox et al. (2016) presented a meta-analysis that differentiated between meditation types such

as FA and OM meditation. Studies included utilized fMRI and positron emission tomography (Fox

et al., 2016). Meditation traditions were various (Vipassana, Tibetan Buddhist, Theravada) but

grouped together into categories based on their instructions. Loving-kindness and compassion

Page 32: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 32

meditation studies were analyzed by Fox et al. (2016) but are omitted from this paper in favour of

focusing on FA and OM methods. A total of 25 studies were used in the analysis, and these were

compared across different baseline and comparison groups which could be a confound (Fox et al.,

2016).

FA meditation was associated with activation in areas of the PFC such as the dorsal ACC

and premotor cortex (Fox et al., 2016). According to Fox et al. (2016) these brain areas are

commonly connected to regulation in action, cognitive control, and self-reflection. Deactivation

during FA meditation was observed in the left inferior parietal lobule and ventral PCC. These

regions have been related to conceptual processing and episodic memory. Moreover, Fox et al.

(2016) conclude that this is consistent with goals of FA meditation regarding decreases in mind-

wandering and regulation demands of sustained attention. Fox et al. (2016) state that several areas

(e.g., the dorsal ACC and areas of the PFC) were previously hypothesized by Lutz et al. (2008) to

be involved in FA meditation and that the current analysis supported such hypotheses.

Mantra recitation meditation, which could be considered a kind of FA meditation, was

associated with activation in parts of the motor control network (e.g., Broca's area, premotor cortex)

and in the basal ganglia (i.e., the putamen and globus pallidus; Fox et al., 2016). The results can

partly be explained in that Broca's area is linked with speech production and the parts of the basal

ganglia with inhibiting undesired and facilitating desired movements. There was deactivation in

regions relating to interoceptive (e.g., the IC) and auditory (e.g., the primary auditory cortex)

information, which could be understood since particular kinds of FA might decrease awareness of

some stimuli (Fox et al., 2016).

OM meditation was connected to activation in several areas, such as the premotor cortex,

supplementary motor area, and the IC (Fox et al., 2016). As stated by Fox et al. (2016), these areas

relate to interoceptive processing and voluntary control of action. Further, OM meditation was

associated with significant deactivation in the right thalamus, which might reflect the OM

meditation goal of openness (Fox et al., 2016). There were only small differences between

Page 33: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 33

meditation practitioners with short- to long-term training, and the IC was active during all forms of

meditation (Fox et al., 2016).

Summary, Purpose, and Hypotheses

To summarize, mindfulness and meditation generally concern the deployment of attention

and awareness to experiences or events in the present moment in a non-judgemental and open-

minded manner. Such a usage of the mind appears to be associated with neural alterations, including

grey- and white-matter changes and increases and decreases in activation of brain areas such as the

ACC, parts of the PFC (e.g., DLPFC, MPFC), the IC, and regions related to functions such as

emotion, attention, and self-processing.

Based on such neural findings of mindfulness and associated practices such as mindfulness

meditation, the aim of the present study was to employ an empirical investigation into the short-

term effect of mindfulness meditation on negative affect and three forms of attention (alerting,

orienting, and executive; e.g., Petersen & Posner, 2012). For this reason, an experiment with one

experimental group and one control group was conducted. Attention was measured using the

Attention Network Test (ANT) and negative affect with the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule

(PANAS). PANAS was used since negative affect (e.g., distress, shame, fear) is a measure of

aversive mood states that can be said to represent central features of ill-being such as anxiety and

depression (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). Consequently, this study did not aim to provide an

investigation into the concept of ill-being but of the related component of negative affect.

In accordance with Monitor and Acceptance Theory (Lindsay & Creswell, 2017) and based

on findings from the neuroscience of mindfulness and mindfulness meditation, it was hypothesized

that mindfulness meditation would on average (a) decrease negative affect and (b) improve alerting,

(c) orienting, and (d) executive attention when compared to a control condition.

Method

Participants. In the present study, convenience sampling was employed when selecting

where to recruit. Recruiting occurred by inviting people to report their interest via e-mail. Contact

Page 34: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 34

information was provided on a pamphlet that was hung on pinboards on the grounds of the

University of Skövde. The paper briefly described background information of the study, how to

become a participant, and basic information such as who the author and supervisor of the study are

as well as the ethical guidelines the study followed. Programme coordinators responsible for

programs at the University of Skövde were contacted and requested to share the same paper with

students of the associated programs. A total of 17 programme coordinators were contacted, with

four responding and sharing the recruitment paper to students of the first through third year of the

Cognitive neuroscience – applied positive psychology programme and Public health programme, as

well as to students of the Midwife and Social Psychology programmes of the University of Skövde.

When participants sent an e-mail signalling their willingness to participate they were

randomized to either the experimental or control conditions and provided with the associated files

required for participating in the study, as well as informed about the expected length of the

procedure, in what way to report their results, and other details such as the ethical guidelines the

study followed. The digital files provided to participants included: (1) an instruction document

containing all the steps of the experiment adapted based on group with the difference that the

control version did not mention mindfulness meditation, (2) a mindfulness meditation instruction

document sent only to experiment-condition participants, and (3) the ANT file.

Data were collected from the 13th of March to the 7th of April. Participants were excluded

from the study if they could not respond with their study results before the end of the experiment.

However, there was an exception for one participant who requested to send in their results on the 8th

of April. One further exclusion criterion was not being able to provide data corresponding to all

parts of PANAS and ANT in this study: There was no exclusion for reporting of all data for one of

either.

14 participants were recruited to the study. The retention rate of the study was 50%, as seven

participants dropped out before reporting any results. Out of the remaining seven participants (1

without university education, 2 undergraduates, 4 graduates; 4 males, 3 females), one participant in

Page 35: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 35

the experimental group did not report ANT results due to technical errors. Age of participants

ranged from 24 to 52, with a mean age of 35. Three participants (Mage = 42.00, SDage = 15.62) were

assigned to the experimental group via randomization. Four participants (Mage= 30.00, SDage = 3.74)

were assigned to the control group by randomization.

Procedure. In the present study, I chose the experimental design of a randomized, posttest-

only, control-group design (See Figure 1).. Pre-tests were excluded to avoid sequence effects. All

instructions were delivered in written form in English through Microsoft Word documents to

minimize biases like different treatment by experimenters. A random number generator based on

atmospheric noise was used to allocate participants to either the experimental or control-group

conditions.

Figure 1. Illustration of the randomized, posttest-only, control-group design used in this study.

The settings of the experiment varied, with the participants using digital devices to access

the instructions. However, the participants were instructed to implement the following conditions to

their setting at the time that they partook in the study: (a) bright lighting; (b) performed in the

afternoon (13-18:00 pm); (c) limitation of noise and distractions by being in a quiet room.

The ANT was administered through a digital tool provided by Fan (n.d.). Participants were

instructed to download ANT (ver. 1.3.0). The ANT computer file was delivered via email and

contained a Java version of the test (See the Assessment measures section for more details).

At the beginning of the procedure, each participant of the experimental condition read the

instructions and engaged in mindfulness meditation alone for 15 minutes in a secluded room.

Page 36: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 36

Participants were instructed to set a timer, such as one found in a mobile phone, to 15 minutes

during meditation. Directly after meditation experimental-group participants filled in PANAS and

performed the ANT. Participants of the control group performed the ANT and answered PANAS but

did not participate in a meditation exercise.

After answering PANAS all participants were also instructed to answer questions regarding

age, sex, education level (graduate, undergraduate, or no university education), and English

proficiency (low, medium, or high) on a separate page. The levels of English proficiency were

defined in the following manner, from low to high: (a) has trouble with most English reading or

comprehension, (b) comprehends English in everyday speech and writing, and (c) rarely encounters

trouble comprehending English. All participants were asked to estimate their total previous

experience of meditation using the following five levels: (a) less than 10 hours, (b) less than 100

hours, (c) between 100 and 1.000 hours, (d) more than 1.000 hours, and (e) more than 10.000 hours.

Those in the experimental condition were additionally asked to answer the question “After

meditating I felt calmer” with a yes or a no.

Four control questions were used to gauge the extent to which participants followed the

instructions and if there were any distractions during their participation in the study. The questions

were: 'Did you follow the instructions?', 'Did you partake in the study in the afternoon?', 'Was there

any part you did not understand?', and 'Were you at any time distracted during the study, and if so

by what?'.

Meditation instructions were primarily based on the characteristics of mindfulness

meditation described by Kabat-Zinn (1982), and by general meditation descriptions by Frisch

(2006) regarding aspects such as the placement of the hands. Furthermore, the instructions were

informed by relevant meditation descriptions from other researchers (e.g., Brewer et al., 2011; Nash

& Newberg, 2013; Ospina et al., 2007; Phongsuphap, Pongsupap, Chandanamattha, & Lursinsap,

2009; Tang et al., 2015) to limit bias.

Summarized, the instructions of mindfulness meditation were as follows: (a) sit erect in a

Page 37: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 37

chair with your shoulders back and your back straight; (b) look down or forward with your eyes

open or closed; (c) try to sit as perfectly still as possible; (d) place your hands on your thighs; (e)

concentrate on your breath, belly, bodily sensations, sounds or sights around you, or a mantra: (f)

maintain steady attention and transition from a concentration on one object to a moment-to-moment

attention to the changing field of events and experiences around you; (g) if your attention drifts

repeat the two previous steps.

Assessment measures. This study measured alerting, orienting, and executive attention,

negative affect, and positive affect. The measurement materials ANT and PANAS were used to

measure these dependent variables. PANAS instructions were adjusted to ask participants to report

their affect for the last hour if they were in the control condition, and for the duration of the

meditation procedure if they were in the experimental condition. This PANAS change was

conducted since the procedure was designed to investigate how mindfulness meditation affects

mood shortly after practice, as asking about a person's experience of negative affect over longer

time periods would then be less relevant.

Positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS). PANAS was created by Watson, Clark, and

Tellegen (1988) for the sake of filling the gap of reliable and valid measures of affect. It is stated by

Watson et al. (1988) that PANAS consists of two 10-item mood scales, one concerning positive

affect and one negative affect (See Appendix for a full list of the items). Summated, positive affect

regards the degree a person feels alert, active, and enthusiastic (Watson et al., 1988). Negative affect

relates to subjective distress and an assortment of aversive mood states (Watson et al., 1988).

PANAS is rated on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (very slightly or not at all) to 5 (very much;

Watson et al., 1988). The test-retest reliability of PANAS extends from .39 to .71 depending on time

frame, and is generally above .50 (Watson et al., 1988). PANAS is internally consistent, and has

good convergent and discriminant correlations (Watson et al., 1988). Other research has displayed

Cronbach alphas of PANAS at or above .90 (Serafini, Malin-Mayor, Nich, Hunkele, & Carroll,

2016).

Page 38: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 38

Attention network test (ANT). ANT was developed to provide a behavioral measure of the

efficiency of the three attentional networks within a single task (Fan, McCandliss, Fossella,

Flombaum, & Posner, 2005). Alerting attention is defined as attaining and maintaining a state of

high sensitivity to incoming stimuli, orienting attention is the selection of information from sensory

input, and executive control is defined as relating to mechanisms that resolve conflict among

feelings, thoughts, and responses (Fan et al., 2002; Fossella et al., 2002). The ANT is a combination

of the Eriksen flanker task and a cued reaction time (RT) task (Fan et al., 2002). The Eriksen flanker

task is represented in ANT by the six stimuli used.

In the ANT, participants are asked to indicate if a central arrow points left or right (Fan et

al., 2002). Participants are instructed to fixate on a central cross that appears on a computer screen,

and from this baseline other conditions are introduced. For instance, the arrow may appear above or

below the fixation point. There are four cue conditions, where cues in the form of asterisks may

appear before the arrow (1) in the centre, (2) above and below the fixation point at the same time,

(3) above or below separately, or (4) not at all. The way the arrow is accompanied by flankers or not

creates six different stimuli: (1) a lone left-pointing arrow, (2) a lone right-pointing arrow, (3) a left-

pointing arrow with two left-pointing arrows on each side, (4) a right-pointing arrow with two right-

pointing arrows on each side, (5), a left-pointing arrow with two right-pointing arrows on each side,

and (6) a right-pointing arrow with two left-pointing arrows on each side (Fan et al., 2002).

A session of ANT consists in three experimental blocks (Fan et al., 2002). Each block

contains 96 trials (4 cue conditions * 2 target locations * 2 target directions * 3 flanker conditions *

2 repetitions) and every trial has five events. The events are the following: (a) a fixation period

lasting 400-1600 ms at random, (b) a cue condition for 100 ms, (c) another fixation point for 400

ms, (d) simultaneous presentation of flanker conditions and the target until participant response or

up to 1700 ms if no response, and (e) a post-target fixation period with a duration calculated based

on RT (3500 ms – duration of first fixation period and participant RT). Each trial lasts for 4000 ms

and an experimental block lasts for roughly five minutes (Fan et al., 2002). Participants can take a

Page 39: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 39

pause between blocks.

The results of the three networks are calculated by subtracting the RT measured for

competing cues (Fan et al., 2005). For the alerting network, the RT of the center-cue condition is

subtracted from the no-cue condition RT. The orienting network score is calculated by subtracting

the spatial RT score from the center-cue RT score. Lastly, the executive network is measured by

subtracting the congruent RT score from the incongruent RT score (Fan et al., 2005). Fan et al.

(2002) have shown that RT and error rate are increased in incongruent compared to neutral and

congruent conditions.

The alerting network of the ANT has a test-retest correlation of .52, whereas the orienting

and executive control networks have correlations of .61 and .77 respectively (Fan et al., 2002).

Employing the Spearman-Brown prophesy formula, MacLeod et al. (2010) calculated test-retest RT

correlations of the executive network at .81, the orienting network at .55, and the alerting network at

.38. In one study measuring brain network efficiency, the inferior parietal gyrus and thalamus were

strongly linked with the alerting network of ANT, the inferior occipital gyrus and paracentral lobule

with the orienting network, and the thalamus, parahippocampal gyrus, and dorsolateral superior

frontal gyrus with the executive network (Xiao et al., 2016). Van Veen and Carter (2002) proposed

that the ACC is involved with conflict monitoring and has connections to systems related to top-

down control, which may be interpreted as stating that the ACC enables executive attention (Hölzel

et al., 2011).

Results

The hypotheses of this study was that mindfulness meditation would on average decrease

negative affect and improve attention when compared to the control condition. Data were

distributed normally (p>.05) for all variables except ANT accuracy, as assumed by Shapiro-Wilk

tests and seen through Q-Q plots. Therefore, to test the hypotheses negative affect and attention

means were compared between the two groups via t-tests (excluding ANT accuracy). Aside from

attention data of accuracy, only correct responses were included in the calculation of attention

Page 40: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 40

means.

Four independent samples t-tests (one for negative affect and three for the three types of

attention measured) were used to analyze the four main dependent variables. Levene's test for

equality of variances was conducted for each t-test to assess whether variance was about equal

between the groups or not. SPSS (ver. 25) was employed for the t-tests and Levene's tests. See Table

1 for an overview of results between experimental and control groups.

Table 1.PANAS and ANT Means (SD) Compared Between Meditators and Non-Meditators

Dependent variable Groupn Experimental Control p

Negative affect

Positive affect

Alerting attention

Orienting attention

Executive attention

Reaction time

Accuracy

7

7

6

6

6

6

6

16.33 (3.79)

37.00 (2.00)

72.50 (3.54)

51.00 (1.41)

49.00 (2.83)

558.00 (42.43)

97.50 (.71)

16.50 (7.05)

29.50 (6.56)

49.50 (16.92)

49.25 (8.10)

100.75 (20.32)

600.75 (59.20)

98.00 (2.00)

.97

.12

.07

.70

.03

.42

.33

Note. SD = Standard deviation. PANAS = Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). ANT = Attention Network Test (Fan, McCandliss, Sommer, Raz, & Posner, 2002). All results were analyzed via SPSS (ver. 25).

There was no significant statistical difference between participants who meditated and those

who did not in terms of their reported experience of negative affect; t(5) = -.04, p = .97, 95% CI [-

11.02, 10.70]. Levene's test indicated that an assumption of homogeneity of variance was violated.

Cohen's d and Hedge's g were .03.

There was no significant statistical difference between participants who meditated and those

who did not in terms of their alerting attention; t(4) = 2.61, p = .07, 95% CI [-3.05, 49.05]. Variance

was unequal between groups. Cohen's d was 1.88 and Hedge's g was 1.56.

There was no significant statistical difference between participants who meditated and

participants who did not concerning orienting attention; t(4) = .42, p = .70, 95% CI [-10.79, 14.29].

Variance was not equal between groups. Cohen's d was .30 and Hedge's g was .25.

Page 41: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 41

There was a significant statistical difference between meditators and non-meditators

regarding their executive attention; t(4) = -3.39, p = .03, 95% CI [-94.20, -9.30]. Variance was equal

between groups. Cohen's d was 3.57 and Hedge's g was 2.93.

For exploratory purposes, I compared the results of the control questions used in this study

and the answers to the two questions about meditation experience. All participants answered that

they followed the instructions and performed the study in the afternoon (yes = 7, no = 0). No

participant answered that they did not understand a part of the study or that they were distracted

during the study (yes = 0, no = 7). All participants that meditated answered that they felt calmer

afterwards (yes = 3, no = 0). One participant estimated that they had less than 10 hours of total

meditation experience, three less than 100 hours, two between 100 and 1.000 hours, one more than

1.000 hours, and no more than 10.000 hours.

Further, additional independent samples t-tests were conducted post-hoc to compare positive

affect and RT scores between experimental and control groups (See Table 1), PANAS and ANT

scores between gender groups, and age between experimental and control groups and gender

groups. Since ANT accuracy was not normally distributed, a two-independent-samples Mann-

Whitney U test was conducted to investigate the distribution of accuracy across experimental and

control groups.

There was no significant statistical difference between participants who meditated and those

who did not in terms of their experience of positive affect; t(5) = 1.88, p = .12, 95% CI [-2.78,

17.78]. Variance was equal between groups. Cohen's d was 1.55 and Hedge's g was 1.43.

There was no significant statistical difference between participants who meditated and those

who did not with respect to their mean RT; t(4) = -.89, p = .42, 95% CI [-176.16, 90.66].Variance

was equal between groups. Cohen's d was .83 and Hedge's g was .77.

Comparing ANT accuracy between the experiment (n = 2) and control (n = 4) groups, no

significant difference was uncovered; U = 2.00, p = .33.

Comparing mean age between the experiment (M = 42.00, SD = 15.62) and control (M =

Page 42: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 42

30.00, SD = 3.74) groups, no significant difference was uncovered; t(5) = 1.30, p = .31, 95% CI [-

24.75, 48.75]. Homogeneity of variance did not exist between groups. Cohen's d was 1.06 and

Hedge's g was 1.17.

Lastly, there was no significant difference between men and women on any variable except

negative affect, where men (M = 13.00, SD = 4.08) had a lower mean than women (M = 21.00, SD

= 3.00) which was statistically significant; t(5) = -2.84, p = .04, 95% CI [-15.24, -.76]. Variance was

equal between groups. Cohen's d was 2.23 and Hedge's g was 2.30.

Discussion

A purpose of this paper was to overview the mindfulness and meditation literature and to

clarify the effects of these practices on the brain. However, the scope of this overview was limited

to a few specific (e.g., mindfulness meditation, OM meditation) rather than numerous meditation

practices (though other practices were referred to in a few of the studies). Lastly, the main purpose

was to conduct an empirical study rooted in this overview.

From the literature search, it was discovered that much research has displayed alterations in

similar brain areas related to mindfulness and meditation. ER may be affected by both mindfulness

and meditation, as evidenced by alterations in PFC areas, such as the DLPFC and MPFC, and

subcortical regions such as the ACC and amygdala (e.g., Fox et al., 2014; Hölzel et al., 2007; Kang

et al., 2012; McConnell & Froeliger, 2015; Tang et al., 2015; Wheeler et al., 2017 ).

Meditation and mindfulness appear to affect attention, as interpreted based on associations

with attentional networks, the DMN, and areas such as the orbitofrontal cortex, IC, and ACC (e.g.,

Allen et al., 2012; Brewer et al., 2011; Fox et al., 2016; Garrison et al., 2015; Jha et al., 2007; Vago

& Silbersweig, 2012).

Various other regulatory, inhibitory, or executive functions are related to mindfulness and

meditation and connected brain changes in the corpus callosum, IC, ACC, and frontoparietal control

system, among other regions (e.g., Fox et al., 2016; Luders et al., 2012; Sperduti et al., 2012; Vago

& Silbersweig, 2012).

Page 43: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 43

The hypotheses of the experiment, that mindfulness meditation would on average decrease

negative affect and improve alerting, orienting, and executive attention, were largely not supported.

Four t-tests were conducted to analyse the data with respect to the original hypotheses, and

additional t-tests were conducted post-hoc to investigate other variables of interest.

Concerning the four initial t-tests, only the test for executive attention displayed significant

results (although alerting attention neared significance). Meditators were on average twice as fast as

non-meditators in the congruent to non-congruent attention comparison. Studies have demonstrated

support for the improvement of executive attention through mindfulness meditation (e.g., van den

Hurk et al., 2010), and since the executive network is usually displayed as having the largest test-

retest correlations out of the three networks (e.g., Fan et al., 2002) this can help explain why the

executive but not alerting or orienting networks had significant results.

When comparing mean values between the experimental and control groups the results

appeared to support the hypotheses about as much as they did not. For example, participants who

meditated reported experiencing less negative affect and more positive affect than participants of

the control group. But at the same time, participants of the control group sometimes performed

better on aspects of the ANT, as evidenced by lower scores (i.e., for alerting and orienting

attention). Nonetheless, most t-test results were not statistically significant, likely influenced by the

small sample size.

Out of the 14 participants that applied to the study, only six completed all parts of the

experiment and one all parts except the attention measurement. Such a low sample size may

influence the likelihood to discover significant results despite mostly high effect sizes. For instance,

Cohen (1994) gives the example that to detect a medium difference (d = .5) between two

independent samples 64 participants are required in each group. Thus, it is to no surprise that few

significant results were discovered in this study even if prior research has indicated that mindfulness

meditation supports mechanisms such as emotion and attention regulation which in turn may affect

the prevalence of negative feelings and the performance on attention tasks.

Page 44: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 44

Since there were only two participants in the experimental group when comparing the

attention measurements (as one experimental-group participant did not leave attention data), it is

dubious whether such a small sample size can be said to constitute a group at all. For instance,

perhaps it would be more accurate to consider the experiment a case study due to the low amount of

participants? These considerations emphasize the importance of sample size and the limited

conclusions that can be drawn from the current study.

As for the meditation experience and control questions, it appears participants understood

the instructions and did not experience difficulties with the procedure itself (aside from the

previously mentioned technical issue for one participant). Further, although the sample size is small,

the three meditators all answered that the mindfulness meditation made them feel calmer. Thus,

while there were no pre-tests, the manipulation check might indicate that negative affect did not

increase because of the experiment. Lastly, the distribution of estimated previous meditation

experience was spread out similarly between the two groups and appears to not have confounded

the results.

The independent variable of this study was mindfulness meditation. The meditation

procedure included (a) assuming a comfortable position, (b) focusing attention on an object (e.g.,

the breath, a mantra, a sound, a sight), (c) maintaining said attention steadily without distraction,

and (d) transitioning from concentration to an open monitoring of the whole of moment-to-moment

experience. This variable was designed to test the short-term induction effects of mindfulness

meditation, and thus this study is limited to conclusions regarding such effects. For instance,

although all meditators reported feeling calmer after the meditation one can not assume that this

effect lasted long after the meditation.

Several studies (e.g., Leite et al., 2010; Young et al., 2009) have conducted meditation or

mindfulness experiments favorably while relying on participants understanding and using written

instructions to engage in the described practices, as well as in locations other than a laboratory or

similarly controlled setting. Sliwinski, Katsikitis, and Jones (2015) have illustrated the potential role

Page 45: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 45

of digital tools in promoting mindfulness-related training. Lastly, Querstret, Cropley, and Fife-

Schaw (2017) used an online course to administer mindfulness. Thus, there is some evidence that

studies can use written instructions and digital tools to perform meditation or mindfulness

experiments and successfully induce the desired meditation or mindfulness effects.

Periods of meditation or mindfulness interventions of less than half an hour at a time have

been linked to improved attention, SR, mood, and cough reflex sensitivity (Arch & Craske, 2006;

Broderick, 2005; Gorman & Green, 2016; Tang et al., 2007; Young et al., 2009). One study

compared the effects of listening to audio-books and engaging in mindfulness meditation for 20

minutes once a day over four days (Zeidan, Johnson, Diamond, David, & Goolkasian, 2010).

Mindfulness meditation, but not audio-books, reduced anxiety and fatigue, and improved working

memory and executive functioning (Zeidan et al., 2010). With that in mind, it appears there is some

support for the use of shorter periods of meditation or mindfulness-induction techniques.

Finally, among the post-hoc tests investigating positive affect, accuracy, and mean RT

between experimental and control groups, and all PANAS and ANT variables between men and

women, only the t-test comparing men and women on negative affect displayed significant results.

Several studies (e.g., Fujita, Diener, & Sandvik, 1991; Verhofstadt, Buysse, De Clercq, & Goodwin,

2005) support a notion of gender differences in the prevalence of negative affect (i.e., that women

score higher on negative affect on average), but other studies (e.g., McRae, Ochsner, Mauss,

Gabrieli, & Gross, 2008) appear to disconfirm such findings.

From another viewpoint, since most of the t-tests did not discover any significant differences

between men and women this might imply these groups have more similarities than differences.

Furthermore, there were non-significant (p = .31) mean age differences between the experimental

and control groups, although effect sizes (Cohen's d = 1.06, Hedge's g = 1.17) were large. Similarly,

significant age differences were not found (p = .98) between males and females (Cohen's d = .02,

Hedge's g = .02). Thus, age does not appear to have been an impactful factor in this study.

Limitations and Future Directions

Page 46: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 46

The paper was narrowed in its breadth to mainly deliberate on definitions and theories of

mindfulness, certain meditation practices (e.g., FA meditation, mindfulness meditation), and their

connections to the brain and aspects such as affect, executive attention, SR, and ER. Ergo, the

preponderance of research in these topics was excluded through the aim of the paper. In addition,

many articles (e.g., Chiesa & Serretti, 2010; Leyland, Rowse, & Emerson, 2019; Opialla et al.,

2016) could not be detailed in this paper due to the total amount of allowed words.

As the experiment did not include a pre-test to inspect if groups were roughly equal at the

beginning, and between-subjects designs rely on chance by randomization to equate groups, it is

possible that the compared groups were not equal pre-intervention. This could increase the error

variance and skew the results, and is a limitation in this study. With the small sample size this issue

could be exacerbated as outliers would more greatly affect the mean. For instance, Borg and

Westerlund (2012) describe that with smaller sample sizes the risk of type-II errors increases

alongside the standard error of the mean.

Due to convenience sampling, the sample mainly consisted in undergraduate and graduate

students from the University of Skövde. As such, results collected in this study may be biased by

the nature of the sample. For example, Lauricella (2014) writes that undergraduate students

comprise a group of which it is of particular importance to manage stress and where meditation may

play a distinctive role. Thus, it may be premature to assume that the results of this study generalize

to other samples.

In this study, all instructions regarding how to participate and perform the tasks of the study

were delivered via electronic devices and computer files. Although not all individuals are equally

proficient in using such tools, this variable was expected to be controlled by the use of

randomization. However, by choosing this less controlled method of delivering information to

participants a systematic effect may yet have biased the results. For instance, Lauricella (2014)

discovered that, when comparing in-person to digital meditation instructions, 59% of participants

preferred the former over the latter. Reasons for this preference included less distraction and greater

Page 47: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 47

personal connection (Lauricella, 2014). Therefore, the choice of instruction delivery in the present

study may have tilted the results for both groups in an unintended manner. This was not expected to

affect statistical significance as both groups were affected similarly due to randomization, but may

reduce the generalizability of the findings and decrease the appeal of the study to potential

participants.

One participant reported that ANT (ver. 1.3.0) would not function when inputting the word

guest as the group name. This was solved by using the alternate testant as group name along with its

password, as enabled by the work of Fan (n.d.). Previous participants had no issue using the guest

group name, but when testing I encountered this issue myself at the time that the one participant

reported it. When I opened the program by using testant and then re-opened it by inputting guest,

the issue did not re-appear. If other participants who exited the study encountered this issue without

reporting it, which may have been the cause of their exiting the study, this may be a limiting factor

of this study in that it may have decreased the sample size and thus lowered power.

The study was administered to participants via e-mail using digital tools such as Word

document files. On the one hand, the experiment could not be tightly controlled in terms of the

behaviors of the participants and settings of the experiment. On the other hand, participants were

requested to follow specific instructions and implement their participation within certain

parameters, such as the time of day being between 13-18:00 pm. Regulating the time during which

participants are allowed to partake in the experiment may be important as time of day has been

linked with aspects such as hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity (e.g., Kudielka, Schommer,

Hellhammer, & Kirschbaum, 2004).

The experiment was controlled by the instructions used, but how participants followed these

instructions was not verified objectively. However, four control questions were used to assess the

extent to which participants followed the instructions. Answers to these control questions indicate

that participants did not experience confusion or difficulty during the experiment.

In future studies probabilistic sampling, larger sample sizes, and experimental designs less

Page 48: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 48

prone to technical malfunctions may increase power and decrease the risk of type-II errors, and

subsequently improve chances of discovering fruitful results of mindfulness meditation.

Furthermore, there remain many topics, theoretical connections, and experimental designs to

be explored in the mindfulness and meditation literature not covered by this paper (e.g., exploring

decentering, reperceiving, and related constructs and their neural correlates to clarify possible

differences, ego-depletion effects or possible counteraction of mindfulness, or further differentiating

between mindfulness meditation and methods such as OM).

Conclusion

The aims of this paper were to present an overview of mindfulness and of specific

meditation practices, provide links to their effects on the brain, and to conduct an experiment

grounded in the overview.

In the study, there were no significant statistical differences between the mindfulness

meditation group and control group for any variable except executive attention. The result for

executive attention is in line with previous studies on mindfulness meditation and attention.

There are several limiting factors to this study, including a small sample size, a restricted

control of the experimental setting and participant behavior, and possible technical difficulties with

the ANT. Therefore, what conclusions may be drawn from the data of this study is limited.

Nonetheless, participants reported feeling calmer after meditating, understanding the instructions,

and not being distracted while participating. Effect sizes were generally large, implying there were

differences between the groups on some of the variables that were not trivial.

Can mindfulness meditation alleviate aspects of ill-being and improve attention? Based on

the literature overview of this paper and functions such as attention-regulation and ER, it appears

there is support for such a notion. However, such ideas received little support in the study of this

paper, possibly influenced by the low sample size.

Page 49: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 49

References

Allen, M., Dietz, M., Blair, K. S., van Beek, M., Rees, G., Vestergaard-Poulsen, P., ... & Roepstorff,

A. (2012). Cognitive-affective neural plasticity following active-controlled mindfulness

intervention. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(44), 15601-15610.

doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2957-12.2012

Anderson, N. D., Lau, M. A., Segal, Z. V., & Bishop, S. R. (2007). Mindfulness‐based stress

reduction and attentional control. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 14(6), 449-463.

doi:10.1002/cpp.544

Arch, J. J., & Craske, M. G. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness: Emotion regulation following a

focused breathing induction. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(12), 1849-1858.

doi:10.1016/j.brat.2005.12.007

Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: A conceptual and empirical

review. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10(2), 125-143.

doi:10.1093/clipsy.bpg015

Baer, R., Crane, C., Miller, E., & Kuyken, W. (in press). Doing no harm in mindfulness-based

programs: Conceptual issues and empirical findings. Clinical Psychology Review.

doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2019.01.001

Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report

assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13(1), 27-45.

doi:10.1177/1073191105283504

Bernstein, A., Hadash, Y., Lichtash, Y., Tanay, G., Shepherd, K., & Fresco, D. M. (2015).

Decentering and related constructs: A critical review and metacognitive processes model.

Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(5), 599-617. doi:10.1177/1745691615594577

Biegel, G. M., Brown, K. W., Shapiro, S. L., & Schubert, C. M. (2009). Mindfulness-based stress

reduction for the treatment of adolescent psychiatric outpatients: A randomized clinical trial.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77(5), 855-866. doi:10.1037/a0016241

Page 50: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 50

Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., ... & Devins, G.

(2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and

Practice, 11(3), 230-241. doi:10.1093/clipsy.bph077

Blyth, F. M., March, L. M., Brnabic, A. J. M., Jorm, L. R., Williamson, M., & Cousins, M. J.

(2001). Chronic pain in Australia: A prevalence study. Pain, 89(2), 127–36.

doi:10.1016/S0304-3959(00)00355-9

Borg, E., & Westerlund, J. (2012). Statistik för beteendevetare: Faktabok. Solna, Sweden: Liber.

Brefczynski-Lewis, J. A., Lutz, A., Schaefer, H. S., Levinson, D. B., & Davidson, R. J. (2007).

Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners. Proceedings

of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(27), 11483-11488.

doi:10.1073/pnas.0606552104

Brewer, J., Worhunsky, P., Gray, J., Tang, Y., Weber, J., & Kober, H. (2011). Meditation experience

is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(50),

20254-20259. doi:10.1073/pnas.1112029108

Broderick, P. C. (2005). Mindfulness and coping with dysphoric mood: Contrasts with rumination

and distraction. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29(5), 501-510. doi:10.1007/s10608-005-

3888-0

Brown, K. W., Goodman, R. J., & Inzlicht, M. (2013). Dispositional mindfulness and the

attenuation of neural responses to emotional stimuli. Social Cognitive and Affective

Neuroscience, 8(1), 93-99. doi:10.1093/scan/nss004

Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in

psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822-848.

doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822

Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007a). Addressing fundamental questions about

mindfulness. Psychological Inquiry, 18(4), 272-281. doi:10.1080/10478400701703344

Page 51: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 51

Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007b). Mindfulness: Theoretical foundations and

evidence for its salutary effects. Psychological Inquiry, 18(4), 211-237.

doi:10.1080/10478400701598298

Cahn, B. R., & Polich, J. (2006). Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies.

Psychological Bulletin, 132(2), 180-211. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.180

Chambers, R., Gullone, E., & Allen, N. B. (2009). Mindful emotion regulation: An integrative

review. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(6), 560-572. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.06.005

Chiesa, A., Calati, R., & Serretti, A. (2011). Does mindfulness training improve cognitive abilities?

A systematic review of neuropsychological findings. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(3),

449-464. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.11.003

Chiesa, A., & Malinowski, P. (2011). Mindfulness‐based approaches: Are they all the same?

Journal of Clinical Psychology, 67(4), 404-424. doi:10.1002/jclp.20776

Chiesa, A., & Serretti, A. (2010). A systematic review of neurobiological and clinical features of

mindfulness meditations. Psychological Medicine, 40(8), 1239-1252.

doi:10.1017/S0033291709991747

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155-159.

Creswell, J. D., Pacilio, L. E., Lindsay, E. K., & Brown, K. W. (2014). Brief mindfulness meditation

training alters psychological and neuroendocrine responses to social evaluative stress.

Psychoneuroendocrinology, 44, 1-12. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.02.007

Creswell, J. D., Way, B. M., Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Neural correlates of

dispositional mindfulness during affect labeling. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69(6), 560-565.

doi:10.1097/PSY.0b013e3180f6171f

Critchley, H. D., Nicotra, A., Chiesa, P. A., Nagai, Y., Gray, M. A., Minati, L., & Bernardi, L.

(2015). Slow breathing and hypoxic challenge: Cardiorespiratory consequences and their

central neural substrates. PloS One, 10(5). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0127082

Cullen, M. (2011). Mindfulness-based interventions: An emerging phenomenon. Mindfulness, 2(3),

Page 52: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 52

186-193. doi:10.1007/s12671-011-0058-1

Davidson, R. J., & Dahl, C. J. (2018). Outstanding challenges in scientific research on mindfulness

and meditation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(1), 62-65.

doi:10.1177/1745691617718358

Dickenson, J., Berkman, E. T., Arch, J., & Lieberman, M. D. (2012). Neural correlates of focused

attention during a brief mindfulness induction. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,

8(1), 40-47. doi:10.1093/scan/nss030

Ditto, B., Eclache, M., & Goldman, N. (2006). Short-term autonomic and cardiovascular effects of

mindfulness body scan meditation. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 32(3), 227-234.

doi:10.1207/s15324796abm3203_9

Eberth, J., & Sedlmeier, P. (2012). The effects of mindfulness meditation: A meta-analysis.

Mindfulness, 3(3), 174-189. doi:10.1007/s12671-012-0101-x

Elliott, J. C., Wallace, B. A., & Giesbrecht, B. (2014). A week-long meditation retreat decouples

behavioral measures of the alerting and executive attention networks. Frontiers in Human

Neuroscience, 8. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00069

Epstein, M. (1995). Thoughts without a thinker: Psychotherapy from a Buddhist perspective. New

York City, NY: Basic Books.

Fan, J. (n.d.). Attention Network Test (ANT). Retrieved 3, January 2019, from

https://www.sacklerinstitute.org/cornell/assays_and_tools/ant/jin.fan/

Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Fossella, J., Flombaum, J. I., & Posner, M. I. (2005). The activation of

attentional networks. Neuroimage, 26(2), 471-479. doi:0.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.004

Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and

independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 340-347.

doi:10.1162/089892902317361886

Farb, N. A., Segal, Z. V., & Anderson, A. K. (2013). Mindfulness meditation training alters cortical

representations of interoceptive attention. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(1),

Page 53: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 53

15-26. doi:10.1093/scan/nss066

Farb, N. A., Segal, Z. V., Mayberg, H., Bean, J., McKeon, D., Fatima, Z., & Anderson, A. K.

(2007). Attending to the present: Mindfulness meditation reveals distinct neural modes of

self-reference. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(4), 313-322.

doi:10.1093/scan/nsm030

Farias, M., & Wikholm, C. (2016). Has the science of mindfulness lost its mind? BJPsych Bulletin,

40(6), 329-332. doi:10.1192/pb.bp.116.053686

Feldman, G., Greeson, J., & Senville, J. (2010). Differential effects of mindful breathing,

progressive muscle relaxation, and loving-kindness meditation on decentering and negative

reactions to repetitive thoughts. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(10), 1002-1011.

doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.06.006

Fossella, J., Sommer, T., Fan, J., Wu, Y., Swanson, J. M., Pfaff, D. W., & Posner, M. I. (2002).

Assessing the molecular genetics of attention networks. BMC Neuroscience, 3(1), 1-11.

doi:10.1186/1471-2202-3-14

Fox, K. C., Dixon, M. L., Nijeboer, S., Girn, M., Floman, J. L., Lifshitz, M., … & Christoff, K.

(2016). Functional neuroanatomy of meditation: A review and meta-analysis of 78

functional neuroimaging investigations. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 65, 208-

228. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.021

Fox, K. C., Nijeboer, S., Dixon, M. L., Floman, J. L., Ellamil, M., Rumak, S. P., ... & Christoff, K.

(2014). Is meditation associated with altered brain structure? A systematic review and meta-

analysis of morphometric neuroimaging in meditation practitioners. Neuroscience &

Biobehavioral Reviews, 43, 48-73. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.016

Franklin, M. S., Mrazek, M. D., Anderson, C. L., Smallwood, J., Kingstone, A., & Schooler, J. W.

(2013). The silver lining of a mind in the clouds: Interesting musings are associated with

positive mood while mind-wandering. Frontiers in Psychology, 4.

doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00583

Page 54: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 54

Frisch, M. B. (2006). Quality of life therapy: Applying a life satisfaction approach to positive

psychology and cognitive therapy. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Fujino, M., Ueda, Y., Mizuhara, H., Saiki, J., & Nomura, M. (2018). Open monitoring meditation

reduces the involvement of brain regions related to memory function. Scientific Reports,

8(1). doi:10.1038/s41598-018-28274-4

Fujita, F., Diener, E., & Sandvik, E. (1991). Gender differences in negative affect and well-being:

The case for emotional intensity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(3), 427-

434.

Galante, J., Galante, I., Bekkers, M.-J., & Gallacher, J. (2014). Effect of kindness-based meditation

on health and well-being: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Consulting and

Clinical Psychology, 82(6), 1101-1114. doi:10.1037/a0037249

Gallant, S. N. (2016). Mindfulness meditation practice and executive functioning: Breaking down

the benefit. Consciousness and Cognition, 40, 116-130. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2016.01.005

Garland, E. L., Bryan, M. A., Priddy, S. E., Riquino, M. R., Froeliger, B., & Howard, M. O. (in

press). Effects of mindfulness-oriented recovery enhancement versus social support on

negative affective interference during inhibitory control among opioid-treated chronic pain

patients: A pilot mechanistic study. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. doi:10.1093/abm/kay096

Garrison, K. A., Santoyo, J. F., Davis, J. H., Thornhill, T. A., Kerr, C. E., & Brewer, J. A. (2013).

Effortless awareness: Using real time neurofeedback to investigate correlates of posterior

cingulate cortex activity in meditators' self-report. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7.

doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00440

Garrison, K. A., Zeffiro, T. A., Scheinost, D., Constable, R. T., & Brewer, J. A. (2015). Meditation

leads to reduced default mode network activity beyond an active task. Cognitive, Affective,

& Behavioral Neuroscience, 15(3), 712-720. doi:10.3758/s13415-015-0358-3

Gorman, T. E., & Green, C. S. (2016). Short-term mindfulness intervention reduces the negative

attentional effects associated with heavy media multitasking. Scientific Reports, 6, 1-7.

Page 55: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 55

doi:10.1038/srep24542

Goyal, M., Singh, S., Sibinga, E. M. S., Gould, N. F., Rowland-Seymour, A., Sharma, R., ... &

Haythornthwaite, J. A. (2014). Meditation programs for psychological stress and well-being:

A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Internal Medicine, 174(3), 357-368.

doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.13018

Grant, J. A., Courtemanche, J., Duerden, E. G., Duncan, G. H., & Rainville, P. (2010). Cortical

thickness and pain sensitivity in zen meditators. Emotion, 10(1), 43-53.

doi:10.1037/a0018334

Greeson, J. M. (2009). Mindfulness research update: 2008. Complementary Health Practice Review,

14(1), 10-18. doi:10.1177/1533210108329862

Hasenkamp, W., & Barsalou, L. W. (2012). Effects of meditation experience on functional

connectivity of distributed brain networks. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6.

doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00038

Hölzel, B. K., Lazar, S. W., Gard, T., Schuman-Olivier, Z., Vago, D. R., & Ott, U. (2011). How does

mindfulness meditation work? Proposing mechanisms of action from a conceptual and

neural perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(6), 537-559.

doi10.1177/1745691611419671

Hölzel, B. K., Ott, U., Hempel, H., Hackl, A., Wolf, K., Stark, R., & Vaitl, D. (2007). Differential

engagement of anterior cingulate and adjacent medial frontal cortex in adept meditators and

non-meditators. Neuroscience Letters, 421(1), 16-21. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2007.04.074

Jang, J. H., Jung, W. H., Kang, D.-H., Byun, M. S., Kwon, S. J., Choi, C.-H., & Kwon, J. S. (2011).

Increased default mode network connectivity associated with meditation. Neuroscience

Letters, 487(3), 358-362. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2010.10.056

Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007). Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of

attention. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(2), 109-119.

doi:10.3758/CABN.7.2.109

Page 56: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 56

Kabat-Zinn, J. (1982). An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based

on the practice of mindfulness meditation: Theoretical considerations and preliminary

results. General Hospital Psychiatry, 4(1), 33-47.

Kabat‐Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness‐based interventions in context: Past, present, and future.

Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10(2), 144-156. doi:10.1093/clipsy/bpg016

Kang, D. H., Jo, H. J., Jung, W. H., Kim, S. H., Jung, Y. H., Choi, C. H., … & Kwon, J. S. (2012).

The effect of meditation on brain structure: Cortical thickness mapping and diffusion tensor

imaging. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(1), 27-33. doi:10.1093/scan/nss056

Kerr, C. E., Sacchet, M. D., Lazar, S. W., Moore, C. I., & Jones, S. R. (2013). Mindfulness starts

with the body: Somatosensory attention and top-down modulation of cortical alpha rhythms

in mindfulness meditation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7.

doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00012

Khalsa, D. S. (2015). Stress, meditation, and Alzheimer’s disease prevention: Where the evidence

stands. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 48(1), 1-12. doi:10.3233/JAD-142766

Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science,

330(6006), 932-932. doi:10.1126/science.1192439

Kudielka, B. M., Schommer, N. C., Hellhammer, D. H., & Kirschbaum, C. (2004). Acute HPA axis

responses, heart rate, and mood changes to psychosocial stress (TSST) in humans at

different times of day. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29(8), 983-992.

doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2003.08.009

Lauricella, S. (2014). Mindfulness meditation with undergraduates in face-to-face and digital

practice: A formative analysis. Mindfulness, 5(6), 682-688. doi:10.1007/s12671-013-0222-x

Lazarus, R. S. (1993). From psychological stress to the emotions: A history of changing outlooks.

Annual Review of Psychology, 44(1), 1-22. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.44.020193.000245

Leite, J. R., Ornellas, F. L. D. M., Amemiya, T. M., de Almeida, A. A. F., Dias, A. A., Afonso, R., …

& Kozasa, E. H. (2010). Effect of progressive self-focus meditation on attention, anxiety,

Page 57: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 57

and depression scores. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 110(3), 840-848.

doi:10.2466/pms.110.3.840-848

Leyland, A., Rowse, G., & Emerson, L.-M. (2019). Experimental effects of mindfulness inductions

on self-regulation: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Emotion, 19(1), 108-122.

doi:10.1037/emo0000425

Lindsay, E. K., & Creswell, J. D. (2017). Mechanisms of mindfulness training: Monitor and

acceptance theory (MAT). Clinical Psychology Review, 51, 48-59.

doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2016.10.011

Liu, X., Xu, W., Wang, Y., Williams, J. M. G., Geng, Y., Zhang, Q., & Liu, X. (2015). Can inner

peace be improved by mindfulness training: A randomized controlled trial. Stress and

Health, 31(3), 245-254. doi:10.1002/smi.2551

Lomas, T., Medina, J. C., Ivtzan, I., Rupprecht, S., & Eiroa-Orosa, F. J. (2017). The impact of

mindfulness on the wellbeing and performance of educators: A systematic review of the

empirical literature. Teaching and Teacher Education, 61, 132-141.

doi:10.1016/j.tate.2016.10.008

Luders, E., Phillips, O., Clark, K., Kurth, F., Toga, A., & Narr, K. (2012). Bridging the hemispheres

in meditation: Thicker callosal regions and enhanced fractional anisotropy (FA) in long-term

practitioners. Neuroimage, 61(1), 181-187. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.026

Lutz, A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Meditation and the neuroscience of consciousness.

Cambridge Handbook of Consciousness, 499-555. Cambridge University Press.

doi:10.1017/CBO9780511816789

Lutz, A., Jha, A. P., Dunne, J. D., & Saron, C. D. (2015). Investigating the phenomenological matrix

of mindfulness-related practices from a neurocognitive perspective. American Psychologist,

70(7), 632-658. doi:10.1037/a0039585

Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and

monitoring in meditation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(4), 163-169.

Page 58: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 58

doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.005

MacLeod, J. W., Lawrence, M. A., McConnell, M. M., Eskes, G. A., Klein, R. M., & Shore, D. I.

(2010). Appraising the ANT: Psychometric and theoretical considerations of the Attention

Network Test. Neuropsychology, 24(5), 637-651. doi:10.1037/a0019803

Magni, G., Caldieron, C., Rigatti-Luchini, S., & Merskey, H. (1990). Chronic musculoskeletal pain

and depressive symptoms in the general population. An analysis of the 1st national health

and nutrition examination survey data. Pain, 43(3), 299-307. doi:10.1016/0304-

3959(90)90027-B

Manna, A., Raffone, A., Perrucci, M. G., Nardo, D., Ferretti, A., Tartaro, A., … & Romani, G. L.

(2010). Neural correlates of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in meditation. Brain

Research Bulletin, 82(1-2), 46-56. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2010.03.001

Masicampo, E. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). Relating mindfulness and self-regulatory processes.

Psychological Inquiry, 18(4), 255-258. doi:10.1080/10478400701598363

McConnell, P. A., & Froeliger, B. (2015). Mindfulness, mechanisms and meaning: Perspectives

from the cognitive neuroscience of addiction. Psychological Inquiry, 26(4), 349-357.

doi:10.1080/1047840X.2015.1076701

McRae, K., Ochsner, K. N., Mauss, I. B., Gabrieli, J. J. D., & Gross, J. J. (2008). Gender differences

in emotion regulation: An fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal. Group Processes &

Intergroup Relations, 11(2), 143-162. doi:10.1177/1368430207088035

Moore, A., & Malinowski, P. (2009). Meditation, mindfulness and cognitive flexibility.

Consciousness and Cognition, 18(1), 176-186. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2008.12.008

Nash, J. D., & Newberg, A. (2013). Toward a unifying taxonomy and definition for meditation.

Frontiers in Psychology, 4. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00806

Niemiec, C. P., Brown, K. W., Kashdan, T. B., Cozzolino, P. J., Breen, W. E., Levesque-Bristol, C.,

& Ryan, R. M. (2010). Being present in the face of existential threat: The role of trait

mindfulness in reducing defensive responses to mortality salience. Journal of Personality

Page 59: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 59

and Social Psychology, 99(2), 344-365. doi:10.1037/a0019388

Norris, C. J., Creem, D., Hendler, R., & Kober, H. (2018). Brief mindfulness meditation improves

attention in novices: Evidence from ERPs and moderation by neuroticism. Frontiers in

Human Neuroscience, 12. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2018.00315

Opialla, S., Lutz, J., Scherpiet, S., Hittmeyer, A., Jäncke, L., Rufer, M., … & Brühl, A. B. (2015).

Neural circuits of emotion regulation: A comparison of mindfulness-based and cognitive

reappraisal strategies. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 265(1),

45-55. doi:10.1007/s00406-014-0510-z

Ospina, M. B., Bond, T. K., Karkhaneh, M., Tjosvold, L., Vandermeer, B., Liang, Y., … & Klassen,

T. P. (2007). Meditation practices for health: State of the research. Collingdale, PA: Diane

publishing.

Petersen, S. E., & Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after.

Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 73-89. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525

Phongsuphap, S., Pongsupap, Y., Chandanamattha, P., & Lursinsap, C. (2009). Changes in heart rate

variability during concentration meditation. International Journal of Cardiology, 130(3),

481-484. doi:10.1016/j.ijcard.2007.06.103

Querstret, D., Cropley, M., & Fife-Schaw, C. (2017). Internet-based instructor-led mindfulness for

work-related rumination, fatigue, and sleep: Assessing facets of mindfulness as mechanisms

of change. A randomized waitlist control trial. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology,

22(2), 153-169. doi:10.1037/ocp0000028

Raffone, A., & Srinivasan, N. (2010). The exploration of meditation in the neuroscience of attention

and consciousness. Cognitive Processing, 11(1), 1-7. doi:10.1007/s10339-009-0354-z

Sedlmeier, P., Eberth, J., Schwarz, M., Zimmermann, D., Haarig, F., Jaeger, S., & Kunze, S. (2012).

The psychological effects of meditation: A meta analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138(6),

1139-1171. doi:10.1037/a0028168

Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Kenna, H., … & Greicius, M.

Page 60: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 60

D. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive

control. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(9), 2349-2356. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-

06.2007

Serafini, K., Malin-Mayor, B., Nich, C., Hunkele, K., & Carroll, K. M. (2016). Psychometric

properties of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS) in a heterogeneous sample

of substance users. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 42(2), 203-212.

doi:10.3109/00952990.2015.1133632

Shapiro, S. L., Carlson, L. E., Astin, J. A., & Freedman, B. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness.

Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(3), 373-386. doi:10.1002/jclp.20237

Sliwinski, J., Katsikitis, M., & Jones, C. M. (2015). Mindful gaming: How digital games can

improve mindfulness, presented at IFIP Conference on Human-Computer Interaction,

Bamberg, 2015. Cham, Switzerland: Springer.

Sperduti, M., Martinelli, P., & Piolino, P. (2012). A neurocognitive model of meditation based on

activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(1),

269-276. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2011.09.019

Srinivasan, N., & Singh, A. (2017). Concentrative meditation influences visual awareness: A study

with color afterimages. Mindfulness, 8(1), 17-26. doi:10.1007/s12671-015-0428-1

Tang, Y. -Y., Hölzel, B. K., & Posner, M. I. (2015). The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation.

Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(4), 213-225. doi:10.1038/nrn3916

Tang, Y.-Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., Feng, S., Lu, Q., … Posner, M. I. (2007). Short-term

meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proceedings of the National

Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(43), 17152-17156.

doi:10.1073/pnas.0707678104

Tang, Y. -Y., & Posner, M. I. (2013). Special issue on mindfulness neuroscience. Social Cognitive

and Affective Neuroscience, 8(1), 1-3. doi:10.1093/scan/nss104

Toneatto, T., & Nguyen, L. (2007). Does mindfulness meditation improve anxiety and mood

Page 61: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 61

symptoms? A review of the controlled research. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 52(4),

260-266. doi:10.1177/070674370705200409

Travis, F., & Shear, J. (2010). Focused attention, open monitoring and automatic self-transcending:

Categories to organize meditations from Vedic, Buddhist and Chinese traditions.

Consciousness and Cognition, 19(4), 1110-1118. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2010.01.007

Vago, D. R., & Silbersweig, D. A. (2012). Self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-transcendence

(S-ART): A framework for understanding the neurobiological mechanisms of mindfulness.

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00296

Van Dam, N. T., van Vugt, M. K., Vago, D. R., Schmalzl, L., Saron, C. D., Olendzki, A., … &

Meyer, D. E. (2018a). Mind the hype: A critical evaluation and prescriptive agenda for

research on mindfulness and meditation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(1), 36-

61. doi:10.1177/1745691617709589

Van Dam, N. T., van Vugt, M. K., Vago, D. R., Schmalzl, L., Saron, C. D., Olendzki, A., … &

Meyer, D. E. (2018b). Reiterated concerns and further challenges for mindfulness and

meditation research: A reply to Davidson and Dahl. Perspectives on Psychological Science,

13(1), 66-69. doi:10.1177/1745691617727529

van den Hurk, P. A. M., Giommi, F., Gielen, S. C., Speckens, A. E. M., & Barendregt, H. P. (2010).

Greater efficiency in attentional processing related to mindfulness meditation. The Quarterly

Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(6), 1168-1180. doi:10.1080/17470210903249365

Van Veen, V., & Carter, C. S. (2002). The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP

studies. Physiology & Behavior, 77(4-5), 477-482. doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00930-7

Verhofstadt, L. L., Buysse, A., De Clercq, A., & Goodwin, R. (2005). Emotional arousal and

negative affect in marital conflict: The influence of gender, conflict structure, and demand‐

withdrawal. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35(4), 449-467. doi:10.1002/ejsp.262

Walsh, R., & Shapiro, S. L. (2006). The meeting of meditative disciplines and Western psychology:

A mutually enriching dialogue. American Psychologist, 61(3), 227-239. doi:10.1037/0003-

Page 62: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 62

066X.61.3.227

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of

positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070.

Westbrook, C., Creswell, J. D., Tabibnia, G., Julson, E., Kober, H., & Tindle, H. A. (2011). Mindful

attention reduces neural and self-reported cue-induced craving in smokers. Social Cognitive

and Affective Neuroscience, 8(1), 73-84. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr076

Wheeler, M. S., Arnkoff, D. B., & Glass, C. R. (2017). The neuroscience of mindfulness: How

mindfulness alters the brain and facilitates emotion regulation. Mindfulness, 8(6), 1471-

1487. doi:10.1007/s12671-017-0742-x

Xiao, M., Ge, H., Khundrakpam, B. S., Xu, J., Bezgin, G., Leng, Y., ... & Liu, S. (2016). Attention

performance measured by Attention Network Test is correlated with global and regional

efficiency of structural brain networks. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 10.

doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00194

Xu, W., Rodriguez, M. A., Zhang, Q., & Liu, X. (2015). The mediating effect of self-acceptance in

the relationship between mindfulness and peace of mind. Mindfulness, 6(4), 797-802.

doi:10.1007/s12671-014-0319-x

Young, E. C., Brammar, C., Owen, E., Brown, N., Lowe, J., Johnson, C., … & Smith, J. A. (2009).

The effect of mindfulness meditation on cough reflex sensitivity. Thorax, 64(11), 993-998.

doi:10.1136/thx.2009.116723

Zaccaro, A., Piarulli, A., Laurino, M., Garbella, E., Menicucci, D., Neri, B., & Gemignani, A.

(2018). How breath-control can change your life: A systematic review on psycho-

physiological correlates of slow breathing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12.

doi:10.3389/fnhum.2018.00353

Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Diamond, B. J., David, Z., & Goolkasian, P. (2010). Mindfulness

meditation improves cognition: Evidence of brief mental training. Consciousness and

Page 63: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 63

Cognition, 19(2), 597-605. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2010.03.014

Zelazo, P. D., & Lyons, K. E. (2012). The potential benefits of mindfulness training in early

childhood: A developmental social cognitive neuroscience perspective. Child Development

Perspectives, 6(2), 154-160. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00241.x

Page 64: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 64

Appendix

Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS)

This scale consists of a number of words that describe different feelings and emotions. Read each

item and then list the number from the scale below next to each word. Indicate to what extent you

have felt this way over the past week.

1 = Very Slightly or Not At All

2 = A Little

3 = Moderately

4 = Quite a Bit

5 = Extremely

____ 1. Interested ____ 11. Irritable

____ 2. Distressed ____ 12. Alert

____ 3. Excited ____ 13. Ashamed

____ 4. Upset ____ 14. Inspired

____ 5. Strong ____ 15. Nervous

____ 6. Guilty ____ 16. Determined

____ 7. Scared ____ 17. Attentive

____ 8. Hostile ____ 18. Jittery

____ 9. Enthusiastic ____ 19. Active

____ 10. Proud ____ 20. Afraid

Note. Items 1, 3, 5, 9, 10, 12, 14, 16, 17, and 19 comprise the positive-affect score. Scores

can range from 10 – 50, with higher scores representing higher levels of positive affect. Items 2, 4,

6, 7, 8, 11, 13, 15, 18, and 20 represent negative-affect. Scores can range from 10 – 50, with lower

scores representing lower levels of negative affect.

Page 65: The Effect of Mindfulness Meditation on Affect and Attentionhis.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1333055/FULLTEXT01.pdf · in combat situations (Lazarus, 1993), is linked with workplace

MINDFULNESS MEDITATION 65

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank Kristoffer Ekman for his supervision and feedback in this study, and

Antti Revonsuo for his examination.