The effect of constrained processing on auditors’ judgments

download The effect of constrained processing on auditors’ judgments

of 16

Transcript of The effect of constrained processing on auditors’ judgments

  • 7/29/2019 The effect of constrained processing on auditors judgments

    1/16

    The effect of constrained processing on auditors judgments

    Vicky B. Hoffmana, Jennifer R. Joeb, Donald V. Mosera,*aKatz Graduate School of Business, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15260, USA

    bGeorgia State University, School of Accounting, 35 Broad Street, Atlanta, GA 30303, USA

    AbstractThis study examines whether constraining experienced auditors processing by having them process evidence in a

    pre-established sequence (an experimental control technique used in previous studies) prevents them from using their

    usual processing strategies and thereby affects their judgments. We compare the relative attention to evidence and

    judgments of experienced and inexperienced auditors in a constrained versus an unconstrained processing condition.

    Consistent with expectations, experienced auditors going-concern judgments differed from inexperienced auditors

    judgments only when processing was unconstrained. This difference in judgments was the result of differential attention

    to evidence. These results demonstrate that the failure to consider how experienced auditors process evidence can result

    in inadvertently adopting control techniques that limit the generalizability of experimental findings. Although our

    study used a going-concern task, our conclusions are likely to apply to a variety of ill-structured audit tasks that

    require a goal-oriented, directed evaluation of evidence.

    # 2002 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

    Introduction

    Many auditing experiments constrain the way

    experienced auditors are permitted to process

    information in order to achieve experimental con-

    trol. Such processing constraints are often neces-

    sary to isolate the effect of interest. To the extent

    that the processing constraints used in experiments

    are consistent with constraints found in the nat-

    ural audit environment, constraining auditors

    processing in the lab is not a problem. For exam-

    ple, when examining recency effects, experimenters

    present information cues to auditor-subjects in a

    pre-established order to determine whether the

    most recent cues received have the greatest influ-

    ence on auditors judgments (see Trotman &

    Wright, 2000 for a review of such studies). While

    the techniques used in these recency-effect experi-

    ments are simplifications of the natural environ-

    ment, auditors on actual audits are sometimes

    similarly constrained because they receive different

    information items at different times. Thus, in these

    types of studies, constraining processing does not

    prevent the researcher from providing insights into

    auditors judgments in natural settings.

    However, experimental researchers need to be

    careful not to use processing constraints that pre-

    vent experienced auditors from evaluating evi-

    dence the way they normally do in actual audit

    settings. While such processing constraints might

    achieve experimental control, they could also force

    auditors to evaluate information in an unnatural

    way, and thereby limit the ability to generalize the

    experimental results to real-world audit settings.

    Thus, when employing a processing constraint in

    the lab, researchers need to consider how experi-

    enced auditors would normally perform the task

    0361-3682/02/$ - see front matter # 2002 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.doi:10.1016/S0361-3682(02)00068-5

    Accounting, Organizations and Society 28 (2003) 699714

    www.elsevier.com/locate/aos

    * Corresponding author. Fax: +1-412-648-1693.

    E-mail address: [email protected] (D. V. Moser).

    http://www.elsevier.com/locate/aos/a4.3dmailto:[email protected]:[email protected]://www.elsevier.com/locate/aos/a4.3dhttp://www.sciencedirect.com/http://www.sciencedirect.com/http://www.sciencedirect.com/
  • 7/29/2019 The effect of constrained processing on auditors judgments

    2/16

    in actual audit settings. In this study we examine

    how imposing processing constraints on experi-

    enced auditors in the lab can influence their judg-

    ments and decisions.Previous studies in psychology and accounting

    demonstrate that experienced auditors tend to

    perform a goal-oriented, directed evaluation of

    audit evidence (e.g. Bedard & Chi, 1993). That is,

    experienced auditors typically do not process evi-

    dence sequentially in the manner in which it is

    presented to them, but instead use processing

    goals that guide their evaluation of information.

    Therefore, if one is studying how experienced

    auditors make ill-structured audit judgments or

    decisions (e.g. going-concern judgments, fraud

    risk judgments, client acceptance decisions), pre-senting information in a pre-established sequential

    order could prevent experienced auditors from

    making the judgment or decision the way they

    normally would. Indeed, Krull, Reckers, and

    Wong-on-Wing (1993) call for research on this

    issue, speculating that asking more experienced

    auditors to process information sequentially when

    they are unaccustomed to that mode of processing

    could have a detrimental effect on their judgments.

    Trotman and Wright (2000) concur with Krull et

    al. (1993), and suggest that it is important tounderstand the effects of forcing experienced

    auditors to use an information search process in

    an experiment that does not match the process

    they normally use.1

    The purpose of this study is to examine whether

    constraining experienced auditors processing in a

    going-concern task by making them process

    information items in a pre-established sequence

    prevents them from using their usual processing

    strategies and alters their judgments. We test this

    by comparing the relative attention to information

    and the judgments of experienced auditors in a

    constrained processing condition versus an

    unconstrained processing condition. In the con-

    strained processing condition, we use an experi-mental control technique used in previous

    experiments (e.g. Phillips, 1999; Ricchiute, 1992).

    That is, subjects are provided with information

    sequentially, and are not allowed to refer back to

    and reorganize the information. In the uncon-

    strained processing condition, subjects are pro-

    vided with the same information, but the

    information is presented simultaneously and sub-

    jects are allowed to process the information in any

    manner they wish.

    In this study, we also examine the judgments of

    inexperienced subjects (i.e. auditing students). Weuse this group for comparison purposes because

    previous literature (e.g. Larkin, McDermott,

    Simon, & Simon, 1980; Patel, Evans, & Groen,

    1989) suggests that inexperienced individuals nor-

    mally do process information sequentially in the

    order it is presented to them. Thus, the con-

    strained processing condition would not be unna-

    tural for inexperienced auditors. Our

    inexperienced subjects serve as a benchmark in

    two ways. First, in contrast to our expectations for

    experienced auditors, we expect inexperiencedauditors to process information similarly in the

    constrained and unconstrained processing condi-

    tions, and, therefore, we expect their going-con-

    cern judgments to be similar in the two

    processing conditions. Second, the inexperienced

    auditors judgments provide a benchmark against

    which to compare experienced auditors judg-

    ments in the constrained processing condition.

    We expect that constraining experienced auditors

    from using their usual search and evaluation

    strategies will make them perform more like

    inexperienced auditors.

    Our results show that when experienced audi-

    tors processing is constrained they perform like

    inexperienced auditors, and it is only when

    experienced auditors can apply their usual proces-

    sing strategies that their judgments reflect their

    expert processing. Specifically, we find that

    experienced auditors in the unconstrained proces-

    sing condition made more positive going-concern

    judgments than auditors in any other condition,

    1 In this study we use experience as a surrogate for expertise

    because, as Marchant (1990, p. 23) argues in his discussion of

    Bonner and Lewis (1990), using years of experience as a

    measure of expertise is appropriate when examining general

    auditing expertise. He notes that auditors with more years of

    general auditing experience will have well-developed strategies

    for organizing and representing knowledge. Because the task in

    this study requires general audit knowledge, we expect that

    experienced auditors processing will be more expert than that

    of inexperienced auditors.

    700 V.B. Hoffman et al. / Accounting, Organizations and Society 28 (2003) 699714

    http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/29/2019 The effect of constrained processing on auditors judgments

    3/16

    where judgments were all very similar to each

    other. Further analysis shows that the reason that

    experienced auditors going-concern judgments

    were more positive in the unconstrained versus theconstrained processing condition was that they

    were able to attend to more mitigating informa-

    tion in the unconstrained condition. Finally, we

    find that the reason that inexperienced auditors

    going-concern judgments did not differ across the

    constrained and unconstrained processing condi-

    tions was that there was no difference in how they

    attended to mitigating information across these

    conditions.

    These results suggest that failure to consider

    how experienced auditors process evidence can

    result in inadvertently adopting experimental con-trol techniques that constrain their processing in

    ways that limit the generalizability of the experi-

    mental results to actual audit environments. This

    concern is an example of the broader concern

    regarding experimental auditing research raised

    by Gibbins and Swieringa (1995). They point out

    that auditors in experiments are expected to act

    as they normally would in practice, but under

    closer scrutiny and with more structure imposed

    on the task. In order to maintain experimental

    control it is often necessary to develop anexperimental task that is a simplified representa-

    tion of an actual audit task. Although main-

    taining experimental control is essential to any

    good experiment, care must be taken to ensure

    that in attempting to achieve control, the

    experimental techniques employed do not pre-

    vent auditors from successfully performing the

    task in the manner they would in the natural

    environment.2

    The next section describes our hypotheses. The

    two subsequent sections describe the experimental

    method and the results of our experiment. The

    paper concludes with a discussion of our results

    and their implications.

    Theory and hypothesis development

    On every audit, auditors are required to evaluate

    the companys ability to continue as a going con-cern. We use a going-concern task in our experiment

    for several reasons. First, making a going-concern

    judgment is an example of an ill-structured task that

    requires experience (Kennedy, 1993; Trotman &

    Wright, 1996). Second, professional guidance

    (SAS No. 59, AICPA, 1988) instructs auditors

    how to perform the going-concern task, which is

    useful in understanding how experienced auditors

    make such judgments. Third, the going-concern

    task requires auditors to integrate information

    from a number of separate audit areas. As such, it

    provides the auditor with a processing objectivethat leads to the purposeful evaluation of relevant

    items of evidence, rather than a passive evaluation

    of evidence in the order that it is received (e.g.

    Shelton, 1999). In this way, the going-concern

    judgment task is a good example of a situation in

    which requiring auditors to process evidence in a

    pre-established order is inconsistent with their nor-

    mal processing tendencies. Thus, the going-concern

    task provides a useful context in which to examine

    the effects of a potential mismatch between experi-

    mental and real-world processing conditions.Previous studies suggest that when auditors

    make going-concern judgments, negative informa-

    tion looms larger than positive information (e.g.

    Abdolmohammadi & Wright, 1987; Anderson &

    Maletta, 1994; Asare, 1992; Kaplan & Reckers,

    1989; Kida, 1984; Libby & Trotman, 1993; Smith

    & Kida, 1991; Trotman & Sng, 1989). Although

    the current study takes into account this general

    tendency toward conservative information proces-

    sing, it is not the primary focus of this study.

    Rather, the current study focuses on the effect of

    constrained versus unconstrained processing on

    auditors attention to information and the impact

    of the resulting differential attention on their

    going-concern judgments.

    A considerable amount of prior research sug-

    gests that experienced individuals perform goal-

    oriented, directed evaluations of evidence (e.g.

    Anderson, 1988; Be dard & Chi, 1993; Biggs, Self-

    ridge, & Krupka, 1993; Bouwman, Frishkoff, &

    Frishkoff, 1987; Cuccia & McGill, 2000; Hodge,

    2 As Swieringa and Weick (1982) note, it is not necessary to

    model every aspect of the natural environment in the labora-

    tory (i.e. the experimental task need not possess mundane

    realism). However, care should be taken not to omit an ele-

    ment essential to understanding how the task is performed (i.e.

    the experimental task should possess experimental realism).

    V.B. Hoffman et al. / Accounting, Organizations and Society 28 (2003) 699714 701

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/29/2019 The effect of constrained processing on auditors judgments

    4/16

    Kennedy, & Maines, 2002; Larkin et al., 1980;

    Patel et al., 1989; Thibodeau, 2001).3 In addition,

    experienced individuals have well-developed

    knowledge structures (e.g. Frederick, Heiman-Hoffman, & Libby, 1994; Tubbs, 1992) that help

    them consider not only consistent evidence, but

    also inconsistent evidence (e.g. Bouwman, 1982,

    1984; Choo & Trotman, 1991; Fiske, Kinder, &

    Larter, 1983; Mautz & Sharaf, 1961; Myles-Wors-

    ley, Johnston, & Simons, 1988; Taylor & Crocker,

    1981; Waller & Felix, 1984). Experienced indivi-

    duals can more effectively consider inconsistent

    evidence because repeated performance of a task

    leads to the routinzation of basic aspects of the

    task (Bargh, 1984; Fazio, 1986, 1990; Langer,

    1978; Rau & Moser, 1999; Schneider & Shiffrin,1977). This routinization of the basic aspects of

    the task (i.e. attending to consistent evidence)

    frees up processing capacity for performing the

    more cognitively demanding parts of the task

    (i.e. attending to and integrating inconsistent

    evidence).

    Thus, in the current study, we expect experi-

    enced auditors well-developed knowledge struc-

    tures to guide their processing of information

    (Ricchiute, 1992; Shelton, 1999), allowing them to

    not only consider evidence consistent with a going-concern problem (i.e. negative evidence), but also

    evidence inconsistent with a going-concern prob-

    lem (i.e. positive or mitigating evidence). Interest-

    ingly, the going-concern judgment guidelines

    specified in SAS No. 59 (AICPA, 1988) are con-

    sistent with this expectation regarding experienced

    auditors processing. The standard guides auditors

    to first determine whether substantial doubt

    exists regarding the firms ability to continue as a

    going concern, and then if substantial doubt

    exists, to look for mitigating evidence that might

    alleviate this doubt. Consistent with SAS 59, Biggs

    et al. (1993) found that experienced auditors first

    recognize that there is a going-concern problem

    and then evaluate mitigating factors.4

    There are additional reasons that experienced

    auditors may attempt to attend to evidenceinconsistent with a going-concern problem. For

    example, experienced auditors are likely to have a

    sense of the infrequency of actual failures (i.e.

    base-rate knowledge) and the importance of client

    retention (i.e. knowledge of the firms loss func-

    tion), both of which would lead them to attend

    to information that could mitigate going-concern

    problems. Therefore, when cues are presented in a

    manner that allows experienced auditors to pro-

    cess evidence any way they wish (i.e. the uncon-

    strained processing condition), they are expected

    to attend to and integrate both consistent (i.e.negative) and inconsistent (i.e. positive or mitigat-

    ing) information the way they normally would

    when making going-concern judgments.

    In contrast to the unconstrained processing

    condition, in the constrained processing condition

    (in which the cues are presented in a one-time-only

    sequential manner) experienced auditors are

    expected to have difficulty processing evidence in

    their usual manner. Specifically, they will be

    unable to perform a directed search for evidence

    that might suggest specific going-concern pro-blems. Instead, the processing constraint forces

    them to evaluate the evidence in the sequence it is

    presented, interfering with their search strategies

    and making it more difficult to identify potential

    going-concern problems.5 As a result, much of the

    auditors cognitive capacity will be used up trying to

    perform this basic aspect of the task (i.e. identifying

    going-concern problems), leaving less capacity to

    focus on inconsistent evidence (i.e. positive evidence

    that could mitigate such problems). Furthermore,

    3 Cuccia and McGill (2000) suggest that experienced tax

    practitioners conduct a goal-oriented evaluation of evidence,

    and that this can mitigate recency bias in their judgments. In

    going-concern studies, Biggs et al. (1993) and Thibodeau (2001)

    found that experienced auditors know where problems are

    likely to arise (e.g. liquidity and operating performance) and

    use this knowledge to guide their evaluation of the evidence.

    4 In a process-tracing study, Biggs et al. (1993) identified

    four reasoning processes that auditors use when making going-

    concern judgments. The first two processes relate to determin-

    ing that there is a going-concern problem, while the third and

    fourth processes relate to evaluating the effect of mitigating

    evidence.5 Cuccia and McGill (2000) make a similar argument

    regarding tax professionals, suggesting that forcing them to

    process evidence in a predetermined order can produce a

    recency bias in their judgments, whereas giving them a choice

    over the order in which they examine evidence can mitigate this

    bias.

    702 V.B. Hoffman et al. / Accounting, Organizations and Society 28 (2003) 699714

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/29/2019 The effect of constrained processing on auditors judgments

    5/16

    until potential going-concern problems are identi-

    fied, experienced auditors will not focus much

    attention on the positive evidence that could miti-

    gate such problems. When the auditors finally areprovided with the evidence necessary to identify

    potential going-concern problems, the processing

    constraint prevents them from revisiting and

    attending to any positive evidence that was pre-

    sented earlier, or from looking ahead for specific

    types of positive evidence in places they might

    expect to find it.

    Consequently, experienced auditors in the con-

    strained processing condition are expected to

    attend to relatively less positive or mitigating evi-

    dence than experienced auditors in the uncon-

    strained processing condition. That is, even if theprocessing constraint results in experienced audi-

    tors attending to fewer total cues than when their

    processing is unconstrained, our prediction is that

    it will be particularly difficult for them to attend to

    the positive cues when their processing is con-

    strained. Thus, when their processing is con-

    strained, we expect experienced auditors to attend

    to fewer positive cues as a proportion of the total

    cues to which they attend. This difference in relative

    attention to positive information is expected to lead

    to differences in going-concern judgments. Specifi-cally, because experienced auditors are expected to

    be able to attend to relatively more positive infor-

    mation in the unconstrained processing condition

    than in the constrained processing condition, their

    going-concern judgments are expected to be more

    positive in the unconstrained condition than in the

    constrained condition. Thus our first hypothesis is:

    H1. Experienced auditors going-concern judg-

    ments will be more positive in the unconstrained

    processing condition than in the constrained pro-

    cessing condition because they will be able to

    attend to relatively more positive cues in the

    unconstrained condition.

    In order to provide a benchmark against which

    to compare the performance of our experienced

    auditor subjects, we also had inexperienced sub-

    jects (auditing students) perform the going-con-

    cern task under the same two processing

    conditions. In contrast to experienced auditors,

    inexperienced auditors typically lack a well-

    developed knowledge structure to guide their

    evaluation of evidence, and thus are more data

    driven in their processing (e.g. Taylor &Crocker, 1981; Waller & Felix, 1984). That is,

    individuals who lack experience with a task tend

    to process information sequentially in the order

    it becomes available rather than performing a

    goal-oriented integrative evaluation of the evi-

    dence (e.g. Anderson, 1988; Be dard & Chi, 1993;

    Larkin et al., 1980; Patel et al., 1989). Therefore,

    irrespective of the processing condition, inexper-

    ienced auditors are expected to start at the

    beginning of the list of presented information

    and proceed sequentially through the list (e.g.

    Yates, 1990).When evaluating evidence sequentially, inexper-

    ienced auditors are expected to attend more to

    evidence consistent with a going-concern problem

    (i.e. negative evidence) than inconsistent with a

    going-concern problem (i.e. positive or mitigating

    evidence) for several reasons. First, inexperienced

    auditors have been shown to process information

    conservatively and attend more to negative infor-

    mation than positive information (e.g. Anderson

    & Maletta, 1994). Second, inexperienced auditors

    are less likely to be influenced by issues learnedthrough experience such as base rates of company

    failure, the importance of client retention, or the

    specific guidelines of SAS No. 59. Finally, because

    inexperienced auditors will not have regularly

    performed the going-concern task, the task will

    not be routine for them, and thus most of their

    processing capacity will be consumed attending to

    the basic aspects of the task (i.e. attending to evi-

    dence consistent with a going-concern problem).

    Consequently, irrespective of the processing con-

    dition, inexperienced auditors are likely to have

    difficulty attending to and integrating positive or

    mitigating evidence (i.e. evidence inconsistent with

    a going-concern problem). Thus, our second

    hypothesis is:

    H2. Inexperienced auditors going-concern judg-

    ments will not differ in the unconstrained versus

    the constrained processing condition because their

    relative attention to positive cues will not differ in

    the two processing conditions.

    V.B. Hoffman et al. / Accounting, Organizations and Society 28 (2003) 699714 703

  • 7/29/2019 The effect of constrained processing on auditors judgments

    6/16

    As mentioned earlier, the results for our inex-

    perienced subjects provide a benchmark against

    which to compare the results for our experienced

    subjects. The judgments of the two groups can becompared to determine whether constraining

    experienced auditors processing results in them

    performing more like inexperienced auditors.

    Because the constrained processing condition pre-

    vents experienced auditors from using the very

    essence of their expert processing strategies (i.e.

    their tendency to perform a directed, goal-oriented

    evaluation of evidence), we expect experienced

    auditors in the constrained processing condition to

    perform like inexperienced auditors, whose per-

    formance is not expected to differ in the con-

    strained and unconstrained processing condition(see H2). However, in the unconstrained proces-

    sing condition, where experienced auditors are

    able to process evidence in their usual manner, we

    expect that they will attend to and integrate rela-

    tively more mitigating evidence, resulting in rela-

    tively more positive judgments (see H1). Thus, our

    third hypothesis predicts that experienced audi-

    tors judgments in the unconstrained processing

    condition will be more positive than the judgments

    in all other conditions.

    H3. Experienced auditors in the unconstrained

    processing condition will make more positive

    going-concern judgments than auditors in the

    other three conditions, which will not differ from

    each other.

    Method

    Subjects

    Two groups of subjects participated in the

    experiment: experienced and inexperienced. The

    experienced subjects were 64 in-charge auditors

    from an international public accounting firm who

    had an average of 3.5 years of experience. In-

    charge auditors typically perform a going-concern

    evaluation and their assessments are generally

    documented in the workpapers (Choo & Trotman,

    1991; Libby & Trotman, 1993; Ricchiute, 1999).

    The inexperienced subjects were 35 undergraduate

    accounting majors in their last week of an auditing

    course.6

    Design

    Our study is designed to examine the effect of

    manipulating the processing condition on both

    experienced and inexperienced auditors going-

    concern judgments. There are two independent

    variables: processing condition (constrained versus

    unconstrained) and experience level (experienced

    and inexperienced). The two dependent variables

    collected were: (1) subjects going-concern judg-

    ments, and (2) the cues subjects recalled. We col-

    lected cues recalled as a measure of subjects

    attention to evidence (e.g. Choo & Trotman, 1991;Libby & Trotman, 1993; Phillips, 1999; Rau &

    Moser, 1999; Tan, 1995). We use the proportion

    of positive cues recalled (i.e. the number of posi-

    tive cues recalled divided by the sum of positive

    and negative cues recalled) as a proxy for relative

    attention because our predictions regarding differ-

    ences in going-concern judgments are based on

    auditors relative attention to positive informa-

    tion. Using the proportion versus the raw number

    of cues recalled also has the advantage of adjust-

    ing for any differences in the total number ofpositive and negative cues recalled across experi-

    mental conditions.

    Materials and procedures

    The experimental session consisted of four

    parts: (1) processing of information cues, (2)

    making a going-concern judgment, (3) performing

    a cued-recall task, and (4) making a second going-

    concern judgment. Subjects could not look ahead

    to subsequent parts of the experiment or return to

    parts completed previously.

    In the first part of the experiment, subjects

    viewed 48 workpaper cues that were adapted from

    Ricchiute (1992) to create an at margin case

    regarding the companys ability to continue as a

    6 The undergraduate students were enrolled in an auditing

    class at one of three universities. Students from each of the

    three universities were included in each of the experimental

    conditions. There were no statistical differences in subjects

    responses across the three universities.

    704 V.B. Hoffman et al. / Accounting, Organizations and Society 28 (2003) 699714

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/29/2019 The effect of constrained processing on auditors judgments

    7/16

    going concern.7 The cues were categorized as they

    would appear in audit workpapers under the fol-

    lowing headings: Audit Planning, Cash, Accounts

    Receivable, Long-Term Assets, Long-Term Debt,Sales, Expenditures, Review of the Minutes, and

    Contracts, Commitments, and Contingencies (see

    Appendix).

    Following Ricchiute (1992), our experimental

    task included four types of cues: positive, negative,

    both, and neither. Using various types of cues

    served several purposes. First, a mix of cues is

    typical of information included in audit work-

    papers, and thus increases the realism of the task.

    Second, including some clearly positive cues (11

    cues) and clearly negative cues (14 cues) provides a

    means of subsequently checking whether subjectsunderstood and attended to the experimental

    materials. Third, cues that have both positive and

    negative aspects (14 cues) are an inherent part

    of the going-concern task. For example, as

    explained by Ricchiute (1992, p. 51), the cue The

    company will try to issue bonds next year has

    both positive and negative aspects; it is positive in

    that the company will have more cash, but it is

    also negative because of the additional burden

    associated with an increase in the companys

    debt. Finally, cues that are neither positive nornegative with respect to the going-concern task (9

    cues) not only add realism to the task, but also

    allow us to control for order effects.8 While using

    this mix of cues served the design purposes

    described earlier, for reasons discussed later, our

    analysis is based on subjects self-reports of how

    they thought the cues they attended to affected

    the firms viability.

    Subjects in each of the two experience-level

    conditions were randomly assigned to either the

    constrained or unconstrained processing condi-

    tions. All subjects were provided with instructions

    that briefly described a medium-sized company

    and the nature of the audit engagement. Theywere also informed that they would subsequently

    make a going-concern judgment based on the

    workpaper information presented.

    The two processing conditions were adminis-

    tered in separate rooms. The only difference

    between the two conditions was the manner in

    which the cues were presented. In the constrained

    processing condition, subjects were informed in

    advance that each heading and cue would be pre-

    sented in sequence and shown only once on a

    monitor in front of the room. Each cue remained

    on the monitor long enough for subjects to readand comprehend each fact (as determined by pilot

    testing).9 Subjects were not permitted to take

    notes or to return to any cue shown previously.

    The total presentation time was approximately

    8 min.

    In the unconstrained processing condition, sub-

    jects were given a page containing all of the head-

    ings and cues, and were allowed to process the

    cues in any manner they wished. Total processing

    time was held constant between this condition and

    the constrained processing condition describedearlier. The page of cues was collected from the

    subjects before they made their going-concern

    judgments.10

    After they viewed the cues, subjects in both the

    constrained and unconstrained processing condi-

    tions assessed the probability that the company

    7 We conducted pilot testing to develop an at margin case

    in order to avoid a ceiling or floor effect on subjects judgments.

    Pilot subjects were PhD and MBA students who were former

    auditors with at least 3 years of audit experience.8 We controlled for both primacy and recency order effects.

    We controlled for primacy effects by presenting cues that were

    neither positive nor negative with respect to the going-concern

    task at the beginning of the list. We controlled for recency

    effects by presenting positive and negative cues on an alternat-

    ing basis at the end of the list.

    9 Because the cues differed in length, we could not present

    them to subjects for a uniform duration of time. Instead, we

    performed pilot testing to determine how long it took to read

    and comprehend each cue while still attending to the task. We

    determined that to understand the meaning of the cue, it should

    be kept on the monitor long enough for a subject to read it

    aloud twice. In the experiment, we kept each cue on the moni-

    tor for this duration of time.10 It is possible that processing on a monitor could be more

    difficult generally than reading from a page, but any effect due

    to such difficulty should be the same for both experienced and

    inexperienced subjects. In contrast, our prediction is that the

    constrained processing condition causes a differential effect on

    experienced and inexperienced auditors relative attention and

    judgments.

    V.B. Hoffman et al. / Accounting, Organizations and Society 28 (2003) 699714 705

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/29/2019 The effect of constrained processing on auditors judgments

    8/16

    would continue as a going concern.11 Subjects

    placed their going-concern judgments in envelopes

    and set them aside. Subjects then performed a

    cued-recall task. They were provided with cate-gory headings and asked to recall as many cues as

    they could. Subjects listed each cue they recalled

    on a separate line, and raised their hands when

    they had completed the recall task. When subjects

    raised their hands, the experimenter gave them an

    instruction sheet asking them to indicate whether

    they thought each cue they recalled was positive

    (indicating the company was more likely to con-

    tinue), negative (indicating the company was less

    likely to continue) or neutral (does not affect

    whether the firm will continue). We asked sub-

    jects to indicate how they interpreted the cuesthey recalled because previous studies (e.g.

    Greenwald, 1968; Hastie & Pennington, 1989;

    Lingle & Ostrom, 1979; Moser, 1992) have

    shown that it is subjects interpretation of infor-

    mation rather than the experimenters classifica-

    tion that influences subjects judgments.

    Moreover, because, by design, some cues had

    both positive and negative aspects, it was critical

    that our analysis be based on subjects inter-

    pretations of such cues. Subjects placed their

    completed recall instruments in a second envel-ope and set it aside.

    After all subjects completed the recall task, they

    were asked to make a second going-concern judg-

    ment, and to place their responses in a third

    envelope. Subjects were asked to provide a second

    going-concern judgment to determine whether

    the act of recalling cues changed their judg-

    ments.12 All three envelopes were collected from

    the subjects, and this concluded the experiment.

    Results

    We begin by testing H3 because the first two

    hypotheses are embedded in H3, which predicts

    the overall pattern of going-concern judgments.

    We then test H1 and H2 to assess the reasoning

    underlying the overall pattern predicted in H3. H3

    predicted that experienced auditors in the uncon-

    strained processing condition would make more

    positive going-concern judgments than subjects in

    the other three conditions, and that in the other

    three conditions subjects going-concern judgmentswould not differ from each other. The pattern of

    results predicted by H3 is the nonsymmetrical

    ordinal interaction illustrated in Buckless and

    Ravenscroft (1990, Fig. 2, p. 938). We follow

    Buckless and Ravenscrofts (1990) recommenda-

    tion to use contrast analysis to test for this pattern.

    Fig. 1 depicts subjects going-concern judgments.

    Using the contrast weights recommended by Buck-

    less and Ravenscroft (1990, Table 4, p. 939) we find

    that the overall pattern of results is consistent with

    H3.13

    That is, the contrast is statistically significant

    11 Subjects responded on either a continue scale or a fail

    scale. If they thought the company would continue, they indi-

    cated their estimate of the probability that the company would

    continue (from 50 to 100%) on the continue scale. If they

    thought the company would fail, they indicated their estimate

    of the probability that the company would fail (from 50 to

    100%) on the fail scale. We explained to subjects that prob-

    abilities from 0 to 50% were not included on either scale

    because a less than 50% chance of continuing (failing) would

    mean that the subject really thought the company would fail

    (continue). All analysis of going-concern judgments reported in

    the paper are based on the probability that the company would

    continue as a going concern.

    12 The first and second going-concern judgments are highly

    correlated (r=0.87, P

  • 7/29/2019 The effect of constrained processing on auditors judgments

    9/16

    (F=6.42, P

  • 7/29/2019 The effect of constrained processing on auditors judgments

    10/16

    We now test the reasoning underlying H3 by

    testing H1 and H2. H1 predicted that experienced

    auditors going-concern judgments would be more

    positive in the unconstrained processing conditionthan in the constrained processing condition

    because they would be able to attend to relatively

    more positive cues in the unconstrained condition.

    We test this hypothesis by performing a path ana-

    lysis examining whether the processing condition

    influences experienced auditors relative attention

    to positive cues, which in turn influences their

    going-concern judgments. We measure subjects

    relative attention to positive cues as the propor-

    tion of positive cues recalled (i.e. the number of

    positive cues recalled divided by the sum of posi-

    tive and negative cues recalled) and refer to thismeasure as Relative Attention.14 Table 1 presents

    mean Relative Attention measures, the mean num-

    ber of positive and negative cues recalled, and mean

    going-concern judgments for experienced and inex-

    perienced auditors in each processing condition.15

    We test the causal path hypothesized in H1

    using three regressions. The first is a univariate

    regression of subjects going-concern judgments

    on Processing Condition, which shows that, con-

    sistent with H1, experienced auditors made sig-

    nificantly more positive going-concern judgments(t=1.6, P=0.055, one-tailed) in the unconstrained

    processing condition (mean=0.60) than in the

    constrained processing condition (mean=0.51).

    The second is a univariate regression of Relative

    Attention on Processing Condition, which shows

    that, as expected, experienced auditors relative

    attention to positive information was significantly

    greater (t=1.8, P0.75) in the

    unconstrained (Relative Attention=0.41) and

    constrained (Relative Attention=0.40) processing

    conditions. Thus, consistent with H2, processing

    condition did not affect either inexperienced audi-

    tors relative attention to positive information or

    their going-concern judgments. Because there was

    no effect of processing condition on inexperienced

    auditors going-concern judgments, there is no

    reason to test for the mediating effect of relative

    attention on judgments (i.e. no reason to perform

    the third regression performed for the experienced

    auditors earlier).

    Overall, the results above indicate that experi-

    enced auditors going-concern judgments were

    more positive in the unconstrained processing

    condition because they were able to attend torelatively more positive information. Moreover,

    inexperienced auditors judgments did not differ

    across processing conditions because their relative

    attention to positive information did not differ

    across processing conditions. In combination with

    the results of H3, these results show that when

    experienced auditors are constrained from using

    their usual processing strategies, they do not per-

    form the way they normally would when uncon-

    strained, but instead perform more like

    inexperienced auditors.

    Discussion

    This study examined whether constraining the

    processing of experienced auditors by forcing

    them to process information items in a pre-estab-

    lished sequence prevents them from processing

    information in their usual manner, and thereby

    alters their judgments. We examined this issue

    Table 1

    Mean (standard deviation) of positive and negative cues recalled, relative attention measure, and going-concern judgments of experi-

    enced and inexperienced auditors in the constained and unconstrained processing conditions

    Positive

    a

    Negative Relative

    b

    Going-concern

    c

    Cues Cues Attention Judgment

    Experience auditors

    Unconstrained processing 9.2 11.8 0.45 0.60

    Condition (3.8) (5.4) (0.16) (0.23)

    Constrained processing 6.6 10.6 0.38 0.51

    Conditons (3.6) (4.8) (0.15) (0.22)

    Inexperienced auditors

    Unconstrained processing 5.7 8.2 0.41 0.46

    Condition (2.1) (2.7) (0.13) (0.21)

    Constrained processing 5.4 8.7 0.40 0.47Conditons (3.6) (5.0) (0.18) (0.23)

    a Cue type is based on subjects classifications of the cues they recalled.b The measure of Relative Attention for each subject is the proportion of positive cues divided by the sum of positive and negative

    cues recalled. The mean of individual subjects proportions is reported here (not the proportion of the mean positive divided by the

    mean negative cues).c Going-concern judgment is the probability that the company will continue.

    V.B. Hoffman et al. / Accounting, Organizations and Society 28 (2003) 699714 709

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/29/2019 The effect of constrained processing on auditors judgments

    12/16

    using a going-concern task, but expect our con-

    clusions to apply to a variety of ill-structured

    decision tasks that require experienced auditors to

    perform a goal-oriented, directed evaluation ofevidence. We found that constraining experienced

    auditors processing prevented them from attend-

    ing to as much positive evidence as they did when

    their processing was unconstrained, and this

    resulted in lower going-concern judgments in the

    constrained processing condition. For comparison

    purposes, we also examined the relative attention

    to positive evidence and the going-concern judg-

    ments of inexperienced auditors. As expected, the

    processing condition manipulation did not affect

    either inexperienced auditors relative attention

    to evidence or their going-concern judgments.Finally, because constraining experienced audi-

    tors processing prevented them from using the

    very essence of their expert processing strategy,

    their going-concern judgments were statistically

    indistinguishable from those of inexperienced

    auditors.

    This study provides evidence regarding Krull et

    al.s (1993) and Trotman and Wrights (2000)

    speculations that forcing experienced auditors to

    process information sequentially when they are

    more accustomed to processing it simultaneouslycan have a detrimental effect on their judgments

    (i.e. it can cause them to perform like inexper-

    ienced subjects). As such, our results have poten-

    tial implications for previous auditing studies that

    constrained auditors information processing in a

    way that is inconsistent with their normal proces-

    sing strategies. Phillips (1999) and Ricchiute

    (1992) are two studies that used the same experi-

    mental control technique used in the constrained

    processing condition of the current study (i.e.

    requiring subjects to view the evidence items in a

    pre-established sequential order without being

    able to reorganize or revisit the cues). Our study

    extends their studies by manipulating the proces-

    sing condition to be either constrained or uncon-

    strained to demonstrate the importance of this

    factor.

    Like the current study, Ricchiute (1992) exam-

    ined auditors going-concern judgments. Because

    his research question concerned the effect on judg-

    ment of two different ways of organizing audit

    workpapers (i.e. causal order versus workpaper

    order), the processing constraint was necessary to

    attain experimental control and to answer the

    research question. However, to the extent thatexperienced auditors would naturally reorganize

    evidence and perform a goal-oriented evaluation

    of that evidence, it might not be necessary to

    change how workpapers are ordered on actual

    audits.

    Phillips (1999) examined auditors judgments in

    a workpaper review task. His research question

    was whether viewing riskier items earlier in the

    review process would influence the interpretation

    of ambiguous cues viewed later. Again, to achieve

    experimental control to answer his research ques-

    tion, Phillips needed to restrict the order in whichhis experienced auditors evaluated the evidence

    items. However, to the extent that auditors in

    actual audit settings normally review high-risk

    accounts earlier in the review process, it would not

    be necessary to change the order in which work-

    papers are reviewed.

    Our study also has potential implications for

    studies of expertise. Libby (1995) and Libby and

    Luft (1993) propose guidelines to help predict and

    explain how experienced auditors superior

    knowledge affects their performance. They pointout that it is critical to consider the knowledge

    necessary to perform the experimental task and

    when and how that knowledge is acquired. They

    also note the importance of considering the pro-

    cess by which auditors knowledge is brought to

    bear on their judgments. Although many previous

    auditing studies (e.g. Abdolmohammadi &

    Wright, 1987; Bonner, 1990; Frederick & Libby,

    1986) have adopted the first suggestion (i.e. selec-

    ted an experimental task that required superior

    knowledge), little attention has been paid to how

    experienced auditors processing strategies influ-

    ence their judgments.

    The current study demonstrates that in addition

    to selecting an expert task that requires superior

    knowledge, it is critical to consider whether the

    experimental procedures allow experienced audi-

    tors to process evidence in their usual manner. In

    our study, experienced auditors knowledge and

    the task they were asked to perform were both

    held constant across the two processing condi-

    710 V.B. Hoffman et al. / Accounting, Organizations and Society 28 (2003) 699714

  • 7/29/2019 The effect of constrained processing on auditors judgments

    13/16

    tions. The only factor that differed was the man-

    ner in which they were permitted to process the

    evidence. While experimentally manipulating pro-

    cessing condition (constrained versus uncon-strained) influenced experienced auditors

    attention to information and their going-concern

    judgments, it had no effect on inexperienced audi-

    tors attention or judgments. These results reflect

    the fact that the constrained processing condition

    is incompatible with experienced auditors natural

    processing, but is more compatible with inexper-

    ienced auditors processing. Thus, consistent with

    the guidelines proposed by Libby (1995) and

    Libby and Luft (1993), our results demonstrate

    the importance of considering how experienced

    auditors normally process evidence to ensure thatthe experimental procedures adopted do not inad-

    vertently prevent such processing, resulting in

    experienced auditors performing like inexper-

    ienced ones.

    Finally, our results show that relative attention

    to mitigating factors mediated the relation

    between the processing condition and experienced

    auditors judgments. However, it is possible that

    in addition to attending to a greater number of

    mitigating factors (as documented in this study),

    attention to mitigating factors reduces subjectsrelative weighting of the negative information to

    which they attend. Although our study was not

    designed to assess the relative weights subjects

    placed on positive and negative cues, such weights

    could potentially add explanatory power beyond

    the number of positive versus negative cues exam-

    ined in this study. This additional aspect of the

    going-concern judgment process could be investi-

    gated in future studies.

    Acknowledgements

    We are grateful to KPMG Peat Marwick for

    financial support to Jennifer Joe through the

    PhD Project. We also appreciate the comments

    of workshop participants at Duke University,

    the University of Connecticut, the University of

    Iowa, New England Behavioral Accounting

    Research Series (NEBARS), and the University

    of Pittsburgh on earlier versions of the paper.

    Appendix. Cues used in experiment

    Note: The design of the experiment included four

    types of cues: 11 cues that were clearly positive (+),14 cues that were clearly negative (), 14 cues that

    have both positive and negative aspects (B), and 9

    cues that are neither positive nor negative with

    respect to the going-concern task (N). These classifi-

    cations are indicated below. However, subjects own

    classifications of the cues they recalled are used in

    data analysis, because it is subjects interpretations of

    the cues that are expected to drive their judgments.

    AUDIT PLANNING

    N Inherent risk is low.

    N Control risk is low.N Allowable detection risk is high.

    N The preliminary estimate of materiality is

    moderate.

    The company will report a net loss for the

    second consecutive year.

    CASH

    N There are various arrangements with financial

    institutions for compensating balances.

    The companys current ratio is the lowest in

    the industry.N A confirmation verified that one of the

    companys bank accounts is overdrawn.

    ACCOUNTS RECEIVABLE

    B All trade receivables more than 30 days old

    have been factored.

    + The number of days sales in receivables is

    lower than in any of the previous five years.

    + There are no trade-receivable accounts more

    than 90 days old.

    LONG-TERM ASSETS

    + Unused fixed assets are readily marketable.

    B The company plans to delay scheduled

    maintenance on some of its productive fixed

    assets.

    B The replacement cost of fixed assets is not

    fully insured.

    B The company is operating at 70% capacity.

    A patent for a unique production process

    expires next year.

    V.B. Hoffman et al. / Accounting, Organizations and Society 28 (2003) 699714 711

  • 7/29/2019 The effect of constrained processing on auditors judgments

    14/16

    LONG-TERM DEBT

    + The debt-to-equity ratio is down slightly this

    year.

    B A director has agreed to accept deferredpayment on a loan he made to the company.

    + The company has no bond indebtedness

    outstanding.

    SALES

    + Total revenues rose 5% over last year.

    The companys market share has declined.

    EXPENDITURES

    N Results of tests of controls indicate that

    projected error did not exceed tolerable error

    for any expenditures.N Substantive tests did not reveal any problems

    with reported expenditures.

    B The company postponed a consulting project

    related to new product development.

    B The company plans a 50% reduction in

    budgeted research and development

    expenditures.

    REVIEW OF THE MINUTES

    B The company will try to issue bonds next year.

    B The company contemplates an initial publicoffering of preferred stock next year.

    Competition from foreign imports has risen

    sharply.

    N Two competitors reported net losses last year.

    + The company is negotiating a long-term

    contract to ship copper products to a Pacific

    rim country.

    The president has accepted a job offer from a

    competitor, and a successor has not been

    found.

    In midyear, there was a two-day work

    stoppage related to employee fringe benefits.

    B A dividend has not been declared in four

    consecutive quarters.

    B A personal injury insurance policy was

    canceled.

    The company was denied credit from a

    copper supplier.

    The vice-president for manufacturing

    announced his resignation effective in six

    months.

    The companys largest customer, representing

    30% of sales, has not placed an expected

    order.

    B The company sought to finance the purchaseof a subsidiary that had significant NOL

    carry-forwards.

    Domestic demand for wire and cable products

    is down.

    B The companys production employees voted

    not to strike.

    CONTRACTS, COMMITMENTS AND

    CONTINGENCIES

    The company defaulted a payment on its only

    bank loan.

    + The company renegotiated its only bank loan. Loan covenants restrict the disposal of some

    fixed assets.

    + The lawyers representation letter indicates no

    current litigation outstanding against the

    company.

    B A newspaper clipping indicates the company

    may be sued for patent infringement.

    + There were no violations of standards

    requiring disclosure of a loss contingency.

    The company entered into an unprofitable

    long-term purchase commitment for copper.+ The company entered into a long-term contract

    to supply copper tubing to a new domestic

    customer.

    References

    Abdolmohammadi, M., & Wright, A. (1987). An examination

    of experience and task complexity on audit judgments. The

    Accounting Review, 62(January), 113.

    American Institute of Certified Public Accountants. (1988).

    Statement on auditing standards No. 59: the auditors con-

    sideration of an entitys ability to continue as a going concern .

    New York: AICPA.

    Anderson, M. (1988). A comparative analysis of information

    search and evaluation behavior of professional and non-

    professional financial analysts. Accounting, Organizations

    and Society, 13, 431446.

    Anderson, B. H., & Malletta, M. (1994). Auditor attendance to

    negative and positive information: the effects of experience-

    related differences. Behavioral Research in Accounting, 6, 120.

    Asare, S. K. (1992). The auditors going concern decision:

    interaction of task variables and the sequential processing of

    evidence. The Accounting Review, 67(April), 379393.

    712 V.B. Hoffman et al. / Accounting, Organizations and Society 28 (2003) 699714

  • 7/29/2019 The effect of constrained processing on auditors judgments

    15/16

    Bargh, J. (1984). Automatic and conscious processing of social

    information. In R. Wyer, & T. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of

    social cognition, Vol. 3. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Be dard, J., & Chi, M. T. H. (1993). Expertise in auditing.

    Auditing: A Journal of Practice and Theory, 12(Suppl.), 120.Biggs, S., Selfridge, M., & Krupka, G. (1993). A computational

    model of auditor knowledge and reasoning process in the

    going concern judgment. Auditing: A Journal of Practice and

    Theory, 12(Suppl.), 8299.

    Bonner, S. E. (1990). Experimental effects in auditing: the role of task-

    specific knowledge. The Accounting Review, 65(January), 7292.

    Bonner, S. E., & Lewis, B. (1990). Determinants of auditor

    expertise. Journal of Accounting Research, 26(Suppl.), 120.

    Bouwman, M. J. (1982). The use of accounting information:

    Expert versus novice behavior. In G. R. Ungson, &

    D. N. Braunstein (Eds.), Decision making: an inter-

    disciplinary inquiry. Boston, MA: Kent.

    Bouwman, M. J. (1984). Expert versus novice decision making

    in accounting: a summary. Accounting, Organizations and

    Society, 9(June), 325327.

    Bouwman, M. J., Frishkoff, P. A., & Frishkoff, P. (1987). How

    do financial analysts make decisions? A process model of the

    investment screening decision. Accounting, Organizations and

    Society, 12(January), 129.

    Buckless, F. R., & Ravenscroft, S. P. (1990). Contrast coding: a

    refinement of ANOVA in behavioral analysis. The Account-

    ing Review, 65(October), 933945.

    Choo, F., & Trotman, K. T. (1991). The relationship between

    knowledge structure and judgments for experienced and inex-

    perienced auditors. The Accounting Review, 66(July), 464485.

    Cuccia, A. D., & McGill, G. A. (2000). The role of decision

    strategies in understanding professionals susceptibility tojudgment biases. Journal of Accounting Research,

    38(Autumn), 419435.

    Fazio, R. (1986). How do attitudes guide behavior? In

    R. Sorrentino, & E. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation

    and cognition: foundations of social behavior. New York, NY:

    The Guilford Press.

    Fazio, R. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide

    behavior: the mode model as an integrative framework. In

    M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology

    (Vol. 23). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Fiske, S. T., Kinder, D. R., & Larter, W. M. (1983). The novice

    and the expert: knowledge-based strategies in political cognition.

    Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19(July), 381400.

    Frederick, D. M., & Libby, R. (1986). Expertise and auditorsjudgments of conjunctive events. Journal of Accounting

    Research, 24(Autumn), 270290.

    Frederick, D. M., Heiman-Hoffman, V. B., & Libby, R. (1994).

    The structure of auditors knowledge of financial statement

    errors. Auditing: A Journal of Practice and Theory,

    13(Spring), 121.

    Gibbins, M., & Swieringa, R. J. (1995). Twenty years of judg-

    ment research in accounting and auditing. In R. H. Ashton,

    & A. H. Ashton (Eds.), Judgment and decision-making

    research in accounting and auditing. Cambridge, NY: Cam-

    bridge University Press.

    Greenwald, A. (1968). Cognitive learning, cognitive response to

    persuasion, and attitude change. In A. Greenwald, T. Brock,

    & T. Ostrom (Eds.), Psychological foundations of attitudes.

    New York: Academic Press.

    Hastie, R., & Pennington, N. (1989). Notes on the distinctionbetween memory-based vs. on-line judgments. In J. N. Bassili

    (Ed.), On-line cognition in person perception. Hillsdale, NJ:

    Erlbaum.

    Hodge, F., Kennedy, J., & Maines, L. (2002). Recognition ver-

    sus disclosure in financial statements: does XBRL improve

    transparency? Working paper, University of Washington.

    Kaplan, S. E., & Reckers, P. M. J. (1989). An examination of

    information search during initial audit planning. Accounting,

    Organizations, and Society, 14, 539550.

    Kennedy, J. (1993). Debiasing audit judgment with account-

    ability: a framework and experimental results. Journal of

    Accounting Research, 31(Autumn), 231245.

    Kida, T. (1984). The impact of hypothesis-testing strategies on

    auditors use of judgmental data. Journal of Accounting

    Research, 22(Spring), 332340.

    Krull, G., Reckers, P. M. J., & Wong-on-Wing, B. (1993). The

    effect of experience, fraudulent signals and information pre-

    sentation order on auditors beliefs. Auditing: A Journal of

    Practice and Theory, 12(Fall), 143154.

    Langer, E. (1978). Rethinking the role of thought in social

    interaction. In J. Harvey, W. Ickes, & R. Kidd (Eds.), New

    directions in attribution research (Vol. 2). Hillsdale, NJ:

    Erlbaum.

    Larkin, J. H., McDermott, J., Simon, D. P., & Simon, H. A.

    (1980). Expert and novice performance in solving physics

    problems. Science, 13351342.

    Libby, R. (1995). The role of knowledge and memory in auditjudgment. In R. H. Ashton, & A. H. Ashton (Eds.), Judg-

    ment and decision-making research in accounting and auditing.

    Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Libby, R., & Luft, J. (1993). Determinants of judgment per-

    formance in accounting settings: ability, knowledge, motiva-

    tion, and environment. Accounting, Organizations and

    Society, 18(July), 425450.

    Libby, R., & Trotman, K. T. (1993). The review process as a

    control for differential recall of evidence in auditor judgments.

    Accounting, Organizations and Society, 9(June), 559574.

    Lingle, J., & Ostrom, T. (1979). Retrieval selectivity in mem-

    ory-based impression judgments. Journal of Personality and

    Social Psychology, 37, 180194.

    Marchant, G. (1990). Discussion of determinants of auditorexpertise. Journal of Accounting Research, 26(Suppl.), 2128.

    Mautz, R. K., & Sharaf, H. A. (1961). The philosophy of audit-

    ing. Sarasota, FL: AAA.

    Moser, D. (1992). Does memory affect judgment? Self-gener-

    ated versus recall memory measures. Journal of Personality

    and Social Psychology, 62, 555563.

    Myles-Worsley, M., Johnston, W. A., & Simmons, M. A.

    (1988). The influence of expertise on X-ray image processing.

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and

    Cognition, 553557.

    Patel, V. L., Evans, D. A., & Groen, G. L. (1989). Biomedical

    V.B. Hoffman et al. / Accounting, Organizations and Society 28 (2003) 699714 713

  • 7/29/2019 The effect of constrained processing on auditors judgments

    16/16

    knowledge and clinical reasoning. In D. Evans, & V. Patel

    (Eds.), Cognitive science in medicine: biomedical modelling.

    Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Phillips, F. (1999). Auditor attention to and judgments of

    aggressive financial reporting. Journal of AccountingResearch, Spring, 167189.

    Rau, S. E., & Moser, D. V. (1999). Does performing other

    tasks affect going concern judgments? The Accounting

    Review, October, 493508.

    Ricchiute, D. N. (1992). The effect of audit seniors decisions on

    working paper documentation and on partners decisions.

    Accounting Organizations and Society, 24(February), 155171.

    Ricchiute, D. N. (1999). Working-paper order effects and

    auditors going-concern decisions. The Accounting Review,

    67(January), 4658.

    Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. (1977). Controlled and automatic

    human information processing: detection, research, and

    attention. Psychological Review, 84, 166.

    Shelton, S. E. (1999). The effect of experience on the use of

    irrelevant evidence in auditor judgment. The Accounting

    Review, April, 217224.

    Smith, J. F., & Kida, T. (1991).Heuristics andbiases: expertise and

    task realism in auditing. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 472489.

    Swieringa, R. J., & Weick, K. E. (1982). An assessment of

    laboratory experiments in accounting. Journal of Accounting

    Research, 20(Suppl.), 56101.

    Tan, H. (1995). Effects of expectations, prior involvement, and

    review awareness on memory for audit evidence and judg-

    ment. Journal of Accounting Research, 33(Spring), 113135.

    Taylor, S. E., & Crocker, J. (1981). Schematic bases of social

    information processing. In E. T. Higgins, et al. (Eds.), Socialcognition, the Ontario symposium. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Thibodeau, J. (2001). The development and transferability of

    task knowledge. Working paper, Bentley College.

    Trotman, K. T., & Sng, J. (1989). The effect of hypothesis

    framing, prior expectations and cue diagnosticity on audi-

    tors information choices. Accounting, Organizations and

    Society, 14(December), 565576.

    Trotman, K. T., & Wright, A. (1996). Recency effects: task

    complexity, decision mode, and task-specific experience.

    Behavioral Research in Accounting, 8, 175193.

    Trotman, K. T., & Wright, A. (2000). Order effects and

    recency: where do we go from here? Accounting and Finance,

    40, 169182.

    Tubbs, R. M. (1992). The effect of experience on auditors

    organization and amount of knowledge. The Accounting

    Review, 67(October), 783801.

    Waller, W. S., & Felix, W. L. Jr. (1984). The auditor and

    learning from experience: Some conjectures. Accounting,

    Organizations and Society, 9(June), 383408.

    Yates, F. J. (1990). Judgment and decision making. Englewood

    Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    714 V.B. Hoffman et al. / Accounting, Organizations and Society 28 (2003) 699714