Students’ Perspective into the Apathy and Social ...citadel.sjfc.edu/faculty/hsashittal/PUBS/DSJIE...

34
Decision Sciences Journal of Innovative Education Volume 10 Number 3 July 2012 Printed in the U.S.A. C 2012, Decision Sciences Institute Journal compilation C 2012, Decision Sciences Institute EMPIRICAL RESEARCH Students’ Perspective into the Apathy and Social Disconnectedness They Feel in Undergraduate Business Classrooms Hemant C. Sashittal Bittner School of Business, St. John Fisher College, NY 14618, e-mail: [email protected] Avan R. Jassawalla and Peter Markulis School of Business, State University of New York, Geneseo, NY 14454, e-mail: [email protected], [email protected] ABSTRACT Apathy and social disconnectedness among undergraduate business students remain poorly understood and under-researched—despite evidence that they produce an ad- verse impact on learning-related outcomes. Qualitative research was initially conducted among a sample of undergraduate business students to identify the antecedents and learning-related consequences of apathy and social disconnectedness, develop grounded definitions, hypotheses, and scales. This was followed by a survey that aimed to test a conceptual model that emerged from qualitative data. The study finds evidence to suggest that high levels of anxiety among students antecedes social disconnectedness and powerlessness, which trigger apathy or the lack of caring about being a student or attending college. These psychosocial problems are severe enough, the study finds, to adversely impact the quality of students’ learning experiences. Implications for new thinking and research are discussed, and implications for improving instruction are derived from the findings. Subject Areas: Learning Outcomes, Student Anxiety, Student Apathy, Student Powerlessness, Student Social Disconnectedness. INTRODUCTION Escalating fragmentation of the American social fabric (Putnam, 2000), and grow- ing apathy in the society has concerned scholars for over a decade (Besharov & Gardiner, 1998; Eliasoph, 1998). Recent reports show that student apathy effec- tively frustrates attempts to reform secondary education (Friedman, 2010). Col- leges are not immune to these societal problems; disturbing presence of apathy and social disconnectedness is reported among undergraduate business students as well (Bjornsen, Scepansky, & Suzuki, 2007; Jassawalla, Sashittal, & Malshe, 2009). Instructors are often familiar with these problems. Regardless of the selectivity employed in admissions, most have seen evidence of: (a) student apathy; i.e., the Corresponding author. 413

Transcript of Students’ Perspective into the Apathy and Social ...citadel.sjfc.edu/faculty/hsashittal/PUBS/DSJIE...

Decision Sciences Journal of Innovative EducationVolume 10 Number 3July 2012Printed in the U.S.A.

C©2012, Decision Sciences InstituteJournal compilation C©2012, Decision Sciences Institute

EMPIRICAL RESEARCH

Students’ Perspective into the Apathyand Social Disconnectedness They Feel inUndergraduate Business Classrooms

Hemant C. Sashittal†Bittner School of Business, St. John Fisher College, NY 14618, e-mail: [email protected]

Avan R. Jassawalla and Peter MarkulisSchool of Business, State University of New York, Geneseo, NY 14454,e-mail: [email protected], [email protected]

ABSTRACT

Apathy and social disconnectedness among undergraduate business students remainpoorly understood and under-researched—despite evidence that they produce an ad-verse impact on learning-related outcomes. Qualitative research was initially conductedamong a sample of undergraduate business students to identify the antecedents andlearning-related consequences of apathy and social disconnectedness, develop groundeddefinitions, hypotheses, and scales. This was followed by a survey that aimed to testa conceptual model that emerged from qualitative data. The study finds evidence tosuggest that high levels of anxiety among students antecedes social disconnectednessand powerlessness, which trigger apathy or the lack of caring about being a studentor attending college. These psychosocial problems are severe enough, the study finds,to adversely impact the quality of students’ learning experiences. Implications for newthinking and research are discussed, and implications for improving instruction arederived from the findings.

Subject Areas: Learning Outcomes, Student Anxiety, Student Apathy,Student Powerlessness, Student Social Disconnectedness.

INTRODUCTION

Escalating fragmentation of the American social fabric (Putnam, 2000), and grow-ing apathy in the society has concerned scholars for over a decade (Besharov &Gardiner, 1998; Eliasoph, 1998). Recent reports show that student apathy effec-tively frustrates attempts to reform secondary education (Friedman, 2010). Col-leges are not immune to these societal problems; disturbing presence of apathy andsocial disconnectedness is reported among undergraduate business students as well(Bjornsen, Scepansky, & Suzuki, 2007; Jassawalla, Sashittal, & Malshe, 2009).Instructors are often familiar with these problems. Regardless of the selectivityemployed in admissions, most have seen evidence of: (a) student apathy; i.e., the

†Corresponding author.

413

414 Students’ Perspective of Apathy and Social Disconnectedness

general lack of motivation and disinterest in learning and performance outcomes,and (b) students’ social disconnectedness (henceforth disconnectedness); i.e., thesocial distance, disengagement, and alienation that can inhibit collaboration andlearning with others in a classroom environment. While these are not universalpsychosocial afflictions, the Jassawalla et al. (2009) study suggests that even a fewapathetic, disconnected undergraduate business students can produce a stronglydisconcerting and disruptive social environment and inhibit learning in classrooms.

The concern with students’ psychosocial health is not new. The literature isrich with discussions of student loneliness (Asendorpf, 2000), shyness (Mountset al., 2006), isolation (Berman & Sperling, 1991), depression (Wolf, Scurria, &Webster, 1998), social self-efficacy and social skills (DiTommaso et al., 2003),and apathy toward national politics (Leob, 1994; Pinkleton & Weintraub-Austin,2004; Speckman, 2004). Very little is known, however, about how undergraduatebusiness students make sense of apathy and disconnectedness; what they identifyas the principal reasons for their emergence, and how they define their effects onthe quality of their learning experiences.

The principal purpose of this article is to discuss key findings of a two-stage study that sheds light on apathy and disconnectedness from the perspectiveof undergraduate business students. In the first stage, 68 students enrolled in anundergraduate business program were asked to describe their experiences with apa-thy and disconnectedness, identify their key antecedents, and define a high-qualitylearning experience. Grounded in their voices, definitions, a conceptual framework,scales and testable hypotheses were developed. Similarly, the definition of qualityof student learning experiences (QSLE) construct–a self-assessed, subjective, andaffective learning outcome—was derived from student voices as the dependentvariable of the study (Brown, 2005). In the second stage, 537 students enrolledin a business program were surveyed to test the conceptual model that emergedfrom qualitative data. The study focused on student perceptions and their subjec-tive attributions of antecedents and learning-related consequences, i.e., it differsfrom attempts to identify neurobiological triggers of apathy and social disconnect-edness, and from attempts to assess causal linkages using experimental designs.Instead, the purpose of the study is threefold. First, it aims to simultaneously assesslinkages between student attributed antecedents of apathy and disconnectednessand the quality of their learning experiences using a structural equation modelprocedure. In so doing, it aims to explicate the students’ perspective into con-sequential and under-researched psychosocial problems that are interfering witheffective learning. Secondly, it aims to stimulate new thinking and trigger researchby showing the linkage between the findings and current thinking in the literature,and raising new questions that deserve additional academic scrutiny. Thirdly, itaims to derive implications that can speak to the practical realities of instructorsand administrators of business programs.

CONCEPTUAL BACKGROUND

Apathy

Much is known about apathy and its antecedents and consequences—even thoughvery little is written about the specific experiences of undergraduate business

Sashittal, Jassawalla, and Markulis 415

students. Apathy is defined as “the lack of attention, concentration, and controlleading to disruption in consciousness and to the waste of psychic resources andskills,” (Delle Fave, & Massimini, 2005, p. 270). In the Jassawalla et al. (2009)study, students view apathetic students as lazy, disinterested, and inconsideratetoward others. The antecedents of apathy have received some attention; it is knownto emerge as a psychological defense against feelings of hopelessness, and emo-tional and physical deprivation (Okada, 1995). In the higher education context, it iscurrently attributed to leaving home to live alone in college (Guernina, 1992; Taoet al., 2000; Wintre & Yaffe, 2000), and to the stress associated with transitioningfrom adolescence to adulthood (Coffield, 1981; Coffield & Buckalew, 1986) andfrom high-school to college (Pancer et al., 2000).

Curiously, apathy among college students in Japan has attracted considerableattention. Scholars have examined how it emerges during pre-college days (e.g.,Shimosaka, 2001), crimps the motivation to enter college (Lee, 2004), and impactsstudents during college years (Matsubara, 1993). Scholars attribute this heightenedinterest in Japan to racial and cultural homogeneity, where psychosocial problemsseemingly stand out in sharp relief (Bjornsen et al., 2007). If this line of thinkingis extended, it is likely that symptoms of apathy are overlooked, masked, orerroneously explained away as a benign affliction in multicultural environments.

The consequences of apathy are more clearly identified. In recent discussions,apathy is viewed as the principal reason for failure of educational reform. Friedman(2010) notes:

“The larger cause of failure (in secondary education reform) is almost unmen-tionable: shrunken student motivation. . . “Motivation is weak because morestudents (of all races and economic classes, let it be added) don’t like school,don’t work hard and don’t do well. In a 2008 survey of public high schoolteachers, 21 percent judged student absenteeism a serious problem; 29 percentcited ‘student apathy,’ ” (Friedman, 2010).

Apathetic students learn poorly (Larson, 2000), feel psychologically debil-itated (Besharov & Gardiner, 1998; Larson, 2000; Lee, Draper, & Lee, 2001),and struggle with despair, emptiness, helplessness, and powerlessness (Campling,2002). Apathy directly impacts student interactions with others; apathetic studentsare less extroverted, less trusting (Bjornsen et al., 2007), and more dissatisfiedand fatigued than others (Shimosaka, 2001). Because apathetic people fail to learnimportant skills of give and take, they fail to develop social capital, and fail toparticipate in collective processes even when they want (Putnam, 2000). They arealso less likely to adjust to new environments, less likely to seek self-actualization(Davidson, Bromfield, & Beck, 2007), and less likely to re-enroll after their fresh-men year (Davidson & Beck, 2006).

Social Disconnectedness

Of the two psychosocial problems of interest to the study, disconnectedness remainsthe more underdeveloped construct. In the Jassawalla et al. (2009) study, studentscharacterize others as disconnected based on the extent to which they do not likeor get along with others, or do not fit in or belong to the cliques in classrooms.Putnam’s (2000) seminal study on the social disengagement in American societyremains the most comprehensive work in this area. He demonstrates how social

416 Students’ Perspective of Apathy and Social Disconnectedness

bonds underlie a satisfied life, and shrinking social capital and Web of close socialnetworks in America are threatening personal health. He notes:

“We tell pollsters that we wish we lived in a more civil, more trustworthy, morecollectively caring community. The evidence from our inquiry shows that thislonging is not simply nostalgia or “false consciousness.” Americans are rightthat the bonds of our communities have withered, and we are right to fear thatthis transformation has very real costs.” (Putnam, 2000, p. 302, emphasis inoriginal).

As a consequence, acutely disconnected people, “often see themselves asoutsiders, feel misunderstood by others, have difficulty sharing with the socialworld, and are uncomfortable in social situations” (Lee et al., 2001, p. 310).They experience high levels of anxiety, loneliness, depression, and other negativeemotions (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Lee & Robbins, 1998). Conversely, whenstudents feel connected with others, it is reflected in the awareness of a lastingpersonal relationship and closeness with others in the social environment (Leeet al., 2001; Lee & Robbins, 1995, 2000).

Disconnectedness is currently attributed to early attachment behaviors in thenursing literature (Hagerty et al., 1993). The popular press attributes disconnected-ness to proliferating online resources that get in the way of students congregatingwith others in libraries (Scott, 2001), and to Web-based social networking tools andvideo games that get in the way of face-to-face interactions (Crawford, 2005). De-spite these notions, causal insights are hard to identify; it remains unclear whetherdisconnected people gravitate toward the Web, or immersion in the Web causespeople to disconnect with others, or both. Some insights into the likely antecedentsof disconnectedness can be derived from studies of closely related constructs suchas loneliness (see Cacioppo & Patrick (2008) for elaboration on the lonelinessconstruct; see Wei, Russell, & Zakalik (2005) for complex linkages between dis-connectedness, loneliness, depression, and underdeveloped social skills amongcollege students; and Russell (1996) for a scale to assess loneliness). For in-stance, loneliness emerges from students’ failure to learn important social skills(DiTomasso et al., 2003) because early college years can be stressful (Berman &Sperling, 1991), and cause feelings of depression (Wolf et al., 1998). Lonelinessalso emerges from early attachment anxiety, i.e., fear of rejection, and from attach-ment avoidance, i.e., fear of dependency and intimacy (Brennan, Clark, & Shaver,1998). The business education literature, however, largely fails to acknowledge theexistence of this psychosocial problem, and has little to say about its causes andconsequences in undergraduate business programs.

Quality of Student Learning Experiences (QSLE)

The initial conceptualization of this construct was aligned with current notions ofaffective learning outcomes (Kraiger, Ford, & Salas, 1993), student satisfaction(Sitzman et al., 2010), student happiness (Voss, Gruber, & Reppal, 2010), andstudent confidence about translating their learning into actions (Bandura, 1977).To develop a grounded definition, however, students were asked to define QSLEand developed this construct as a dependent variable for the second-stage study.

At present, there are two opposing views regarding effective assessment ofstudent learning outcomes. One view holds a dismal view of student self-reports

Sashittal, Jassawalla, and Markulis 417

and perceptions of affect; they are regarded as invalid and unworthy indicators ofstudent learning (Goodman & Beenen, 2008; Pringle & Mitri, 2007). Even thoughthe Sitzman et al. (2010) meta-analysis finds 43% of studies hold self-assessmentas a valid indicator of learning, self-assessment of knowledge is suspect becausepeople overwhelmingly overestimate what they have learned (Kruger & Dunning,1999). Definition of student learning outcomes, this stream of thinking holds,is a faculty and institutional prerogative. Even the notion of asking students todescribe what they want to learn is held in low regard by the adherents of this‘‘faculty prerogative” view. Speaking of college freshmen, Goodman and Beenen(2008) note that students ask for “‘solid,’ quality, competitive education that helpsthem develop mastery in their field and helps them find a job,” (p. 522). Theycharacterize these students’ notions as merely general expressions of commonlyheld beliefs and expectations, unrelated to any particular institution, and powerlessfor guiding their learning. Aligned with this thinking, scholars have defined whatstudents ought to learn (Chen, Donahue, & Klimosky, 2004; Friga, Bettis, &Sullivan, 2003), and written about how institutions should articulate what, how,and when students will learn what they learn (Goodman & Beenen, 2008). Thestrong concern for what students ought to learn and for objective measures ofstudent learning outcomes helps explain the literature’s underdeveloped interest instudents’ subjective perceptions and evaluation of learning experiences.

The other view holds that self-reported student perceptions are stronglyindicative of the actual learning that has occurred (Arbaugh & Benbunan-Fich,2006; Endres & Hurtubis, 2009), and reflect many desirable outcomes of stu-dents’ on-campus experiences. For instance, student perceptions are viewed to:(a) represent a global measure of the multidimensional student experience on cam-pus (Arbaugh, 2005), and (b) reflect the quality of their social interactions withtheir team members (Shipley, Johnson, & Hashemi, 2009) and instructors (Miles,2001). They are also viewed as valid indicators of: (a) students’ interactive experi-ences with curriculums (Arbaugh, 2000; Benbunan-Fich, Hiltz, & Harasim, 2005);(b) their program’s conduciveness to learning (Arbaugh & Benbunan-Fich, 2006);and (c) their experiences and satisfaction with their institution (Astin, 1993; Hu &Kuh, 2003; Rode et al., 2005; Sanchez, Bauer, & Paronto, 2006).

The QSLE construct was derived from student voices not only because it re-flects student learning, but also because it determines the well-being of instructorsand program administrators. First, for instance, poor learning experiences shape theway students evaluate instructors when asked to complete the “end of semester”teaching evaluations. Even though scholars are quick to note that this evalua-tion is not an objective indicator of a faculty member’s value in the classroom,program administrators rely overwhelmingly on this instrument while evaluatinginstructors. Worse yet, they discount or fail to conduct classroom observationsor analysis of students’ written materials (Lang, 2007). In other words, despitearguments in the literature, good teaching evaluations, word of mouth, and highQSLE scores—all highly subjective and affective measure of liking, preference,satisfaction, and instructors’ popularity—exert an enormous impact on instructors’tenure and promotion decisions (Marsh, 1983; Sitzman et al., 2010).

Second, poor learning experiences can raise the cost of administering busi-ness programs—an issue of great concern to program administrators. Poor learningexperiences have the potential to trigger decisions to transfer in an environment

418 Students’ Perspective of Apathy and Social Disconnectedness

when about half of all undergraduates are already transferring to another college(Goldrick-Rab & Pfeffer, 2009; Knapp et al., 2007; McCormick, 2003, for more ontransfer decisions). Poor learning experiences can also exacerbate dropout rates—which trigger a host of undesirable outcomes. Presently, a quarter of all enrolledundergraduates do not finish their degrees, and only 35% of full time universitystudents complete their programs in 4 years (Morisano et al., 2010). Reducedretention rates raise disconcerting questions about the quality (Perry, 2003), andrankings of academic programs (Morse & Flanagan, 2009; Tinto, 2007)—problemsthat business programs can ill afford. Dropout rates also contribute to the disturb-ing and growing income gap in the society (Carey, 2004). Finally, a poor qualitylearning experience can adversely impacts alumni giving (Cary, 2001) an issue ofgreat concern to outreach efforts. Student dissatisfaction with their learning expe-riences, despite scholarly opinions about their incidental nature, exert a somewhatoverwhelming impact on the well being of instructors and business programs.

METHOD

Stage 1. Qualitative Research

Although Jassawalla et al. (2009) identified apathy and disconnectedness as keyissues that trigger social loafing in classroom teams and interfere with studentlearning, the literature is mostly silent when it comes to explicating the apathyand disconnectedness constructs from the perspective of undergraduate businessstudents. The absence of a critical mass of research findings precluded theoryderived hypotheses tests, and implicated a qualitative study. Initially, therefore,small group discussions were conducted in three sections of an undergraduateOrganizational Behavior class. To prepare students, one of the co-authors beganby defining apathy and disconnectedness in classrooms based on the Jassawallaet al. (2009) study. Students were informed that these definitions: (a) reflect thecurrent understanding of these constructs, but do not adequately reflect students’perspectives, and particularly, their experiences, (b) are meant to stimulate newand original thinking on their part. They were also informed that they would beasked to provide concrete illustrations to substantiate their definitions (i.e., theywould be asked to explain why they defined the constructs as they did). Alignedwith the interest in identifying antecedents, grounded definitions, and a conceptualframework showing the linkages among key constructs, students were asked toanswer the following questions for homework, and prepare for a discussion in thesubsequent class:

a. What do the concepts of student apathy and social disconnectedness inthe classroom environment mean to you? How would you define them?

b. What do students experiencing strong apathy and disconnectedness do?c. What do you think leads to or causes students’ apathy and social discon-

nectedness in your classroom? What specific examples would illustrateyour answer?

d. How would you define a high quality learning experience as a student ofthis business program?

In the subsequent class, students were initially assigned to groups and asked todiscuss their answers to the above questions. An appointed scribe in each team was

Sashittal, Jassawalla, and Markulis 419

required to turn in a written record of their team’s discussion. A total of 68 studentsparticipated in the small group discussions, somewhat evenly split across threeundergraduate sections of Organizational Behavior classes (a required course forall undergraduate business students). After the scribes had turned in their reports,one of the co-authors facilitated a class discussion. The discussion began with thepresentation by each team’s spokesperson; other students were invited to join in thediscussion as well. The co-author asked probing and follow up questions, soughtclarifications and explanations when necessary, and asked for concrete illustrationsof the issues being raised. The specific instances and inferences were recorded onthe white board and later transcribed by the co-authors. The class discussions lastedapproximately 60 minutes (of a 75-minute class).

Qualitative Data Analysis and Findings

The first stage of the study culminated in written reports of team interactionsfrom scribes and the notes taken from class discussions by one co-author. Thesewritten reports were content analyzed in three stages. First, each of the co-authors,working separately, categorized student responses according to each of thequestions (i.e., by questions related to apathy, disconnectedness, its antecedents,and notions about quality of learning experiences). Then they independentlyidentified the key themes in student responses to each question, and developedmulti-item measurement scales grounded in these data. The independently derivedanalyses were compared and contrasted over two meetings among the co-authors.Discussions continued until a consensus was reached regarding themes, inferences,and scales. The reconciled list of themes was translated into measurement scales.

Students had the most to say about disconnectedness, i.e., their responses ledto an 11-item Likert scale (see Table 1 for resulting scales and alphas). Questionsabout disconnectedness retrieved dense schematics that included loneliness, socialdistance, feelings of alienation, and lack of friends as key concepts. Similarly, stu-dent responses to questions about apathy yielded a five-item Likert scale. Studentsdefined apathy in classrooms, much as the literature suggests, primarily as thelack of concern and caring for grades, learning, being a student, or their lives asstudents. The following grounded definitions of apathy and disconnectedness inundergraduate business classroom teams were derived:

Apathy of undergraduate business students is the extent of disinterest in learn-ing, in their involvement in the class, and in their learning outcomes.

Disconnectedness of undergraduate business students refers to the extent towhich they are disengaged, lonely, and distant from their peers in their class-room’s and campus’ social environment.

Antecedents

The first inference drawn by the co-author present during the class discussionsrelated to the relevance of age on the emergence of apathy and disconnectedness.Younger students (under 21) seemed to experience apathy and disconnectednessdifferently from older traditional age students (22–24 year olds). Older studentswere more articulate, described how they felt in greater detail, and providedmore concrete illustrations in response to probing questions. Age seemed a likely

420 Students’ Perspective of Apathy and Social Disconnectedness

Table 1: Scales and items (all scales are five-point Likert).

Scale Items

Anxiety ANXIETY1. I find working in team environments really(α = .8) intimidating.

ANXIETY2. I rarely volunteer for activities in class.(deleted after CFA 3)

ANXIETY3. It is hard for me to initiate conversationswith other students.

ANXIETY4. I am often afraid of saying what is on mymind in the classroom.

ANXIETY 5. I feel like an outsider in my classes.(deleted after CFA1)

ANXIETY6. I am uncomfortable when it comes tointeracting with other people. (deleted after CFA0)

Powerlessness POWLES1. Even if there is something bothering me in(α = .753) my classes, I do not have any power to change the

situation.POWLES2. I find it difficult to concentrate in class.POWLES3. I am often bored and sleepy in classes.POWLES4. I find that I am not really interested in the

courses I am taking. (deleted after CFA1)POWLES5. I am often frustrated with what occurs in my

classes. (deleted after CFA0)Social Dis-connectedness SD1. I am often lonely on campus(α = .946) SD2. I do not think people around here understand me.

(deleted after CFA4)SD3. I mostly keep to myself on campus. (deleted after

CFA4)SD4. I see myself as a loner.SD5. I feel disconnected from the general campus

environment.SD6. I feel distant from other students. (deleted after

CFA5)SD7. Even around other students that I know, I do not

feel I really belong.SD8. I do not feel I participate with anyone or a group

on this campus.SD9. I cannot really relate to other students on this

campus.SD10. I do not like hanging out with other students.

(deleted after CFA2)SD11. I always get negative feedback from other people.

Apathy APATHY1. I do not really care about the grades I am(α = .81) receiving. (deleted after CFA0)

APATHY2. I do not really care about being a studenthere.

APATHY3. I do not care much about what happens inCollege.

Continued.

Sashittal, Jassawalla, and Markulis 421

Table 1: Continued

Scale Items

APATHY4. Attending College is not high on my prioritylist.

APATHY5. I am not sure I value the education I amgetting at College. (deleted after CFA0)

Teamwork-related learning TRL1. I am better able to interact with others in a team(α = .842) environment. (deleted after CFA2)

TRL 2. I have learned a lot because I worked in teamswith other students.

TRL 3. I think classroom teams have enhanced mylearning as a student.

TRL 4. I have learned to work as a team player.TRL 5. I am generally enthusiastic about team projects.

Academic Achievement AA 1. Overall, I am learning a lot at (name of the(α = .794) the University). (deleted after CFA2)

AA 2. The content of the courses I have taken havereally challenged me.

AA 3. My coursework has helped me develop ameaningful philosophy of life.

AA 4. I have a better appreciation of the complexity ofthe real world.

AA.5 I am gaining knowledge that I will find useful inthe real world.

AA.6 I am learning the kinds of things that I believe willbe useful for a career.

Academic Performance (GPA) My cumulative GPA is: ______Age I am:

� 18 years old or less � 19 years old � 20 years old� 21 years old � 22 years old � 23 years old� 24 years old � 25 or older

antecedent because older students reported a greater propensity for apathy anddisconnectedness.

Content analysis showed that students attribute apathy and disconnectednessto feelings of anxiety and powerlessness. A six-item scale emerged from theirdescriptions of anxiety, which included a combination of feelings of intimidationabout working in teams, failing to volunteer, and experiencing difficulty in ini-tiating conversations with others. Their descriptions of powerlessness related tothe frustration they felt about their inability to change what was occurring, theirinability to concentrate and the extreme ennui in the classroom. The followinggrounded constitutive definitions for the antecedents were developed:

Anxiety of undergraduate business students refers to the felt intimidation inteam environments, and fear of initiating conversation, saying what was ontheir mind, and volunteering for class activities.

Feelings of powerlessness as a student refers to the concern about their inabilityto change what occurs in the classroom and the futility of trying, that is

422 Students’ Perspective of Apathy and Social Disconnectedness

Figure 1: Hypothesized model.

manifested in the inability to concentrate, and in the persistent feelings offrustration and ennui within the classroom environment.

Quality of student learning experiences (QSLE)

Student responses highlight the expansive domain of this construct. A high qualitylearning experience, they note, includes: (a) academic achievement, i.e., high levelsof content-based learning derived in the business program, (b) teamwork-relatedlearning, i.e., high levels of learning about working with others in teams, and (c)academic performance, i.e., high level learning related to course content reflected intheir GPA. High levels of academic achievement or content-based learning occurswhen they: (a) learn a lot in terms of breadth and depth of subject matter, (b) arechallenged by the relative difficulty of gaining mastery over important concepts,(c) develop a meaningful philosophy of life, i.e., gain knowledge that allowsthem to make sense of the complex environment in which they find themselvesand develop informed and useful perspectives for functioning effectively, and (d)gain necessary knowledge and skills to pursue a career in the business world.From their descriptions, a five-item scale to measure student perceptions of idealcontent-based learning outcomes was developed. In a similar vein, high levelsof teamwork-related learning occurs if, as a result of participating in classroomteams, they can: (a) interact better with others in teams, (b) learn a lot as a result ofworking in the team, (c) learn about functioning better as a team member, and (d)feel higher levels of enthusiasm for working in teams. Their descriptions led to thedevelopment of a six-item scale to measure student perceptions of ideal teamworkrelated learning (see Table 1 for alphas and scales).

Conceptual model and hypotheses

Figure 1 shows the conceptual model derived from the qualitative research. Themodel relates to the following guiding hypotheses that were simultaneously testedin the second stage of the study (all in the context of the perceptions of full time,traditional age (18–25) undergraduate students enrolled as majors or minors inundergraduate business programs):

H1a: Higher level of anxiety is linked to higher levels of apathy.

H1b: Higher level of powerlessness is linked to higher levels of apathy.

Sashittal, Jassawalla, and Markulis 423

H1c: Older students will report higher levels of apathy.

H2a: Higher level of anxiety is linked to higher levels of socialdisconnectedness.

H2b: Higher level of powerlessness is linked to higher levels of socialdisconnectedness.

H2c: Older students will report higher levels of social disconnectedness.

Higher level of apathy is linked to lower levels of:

H3a: Academic achievement.

H3b: Teamwork-related learning.

H3c: Academic performance.

Higher level of social disconnectedness is linked to lower levels of:

H4a: Academic achievement.

H4b: Teamwork-related learning.

H4c: Academic performance.

Stage 2. Hypotheses/Model TestingSurvey

The data-derived scales were translated into a questionnaire and administered to537 students in 33 undergraduate sections offered in a business school of a regionalstate university located in Northeastern United States. Instructors were askedto circulate the questionnaire while making the following points: (a) the surveyaimed to assess student experiences and perspectives, (b) the survey would take10 minutes to complete and should be returned at the end of the class, (c) studentsshould complete the entire questionnaire, as incomplete questionnaires wouldbe discarded, and (d) students should not fill a questionnaire if they had alreadycompleted one in another class. No extra credit was offered for completing thequestionnaire, and no student declined to participate (the response rate was 100%).

Sample

After all the questionnaires were collected, an SPSS data file was created by awork-study student assigned to a co-author. The profile of students participating inthe survey was as follows. Nearly all (99%) were traditional age, full time studentswith the average age of 20.6 (SD = 1.66), average cumulative GPA of 3.16 (SD =.74). They had taken an average of 5.58 classes at the university that required themto function in teams and engage in team-based learning. Of the 56% male and 44%female sample, 36% were seniors, 34% were juniors, 22% were sophomores, and8% were freshmen. Most (51%) had spent at least 2 years at the college (22% weretransfer students). Most students were pursuing a business major (43% manage-ment and 31% accounting). The remaining students were pursuing a managementminor, and majoring in other degree programs (math/sciences (6%), economics(3.4%), communication (3.1%), political science (3%), and psychology (2.5%)).

424 Students’ Perspective of Apathy and Social Disconnectedness

Table 2: Model modification process for purifying the measurement model.

Action (variablesModel SB Chi-sq Df NNFI CFI IFI RMSEA dropped)a

CFA0 1664.45 650 .856 .867 .868 .054 Apathy1, Apathy5,Anxiety6, Powless5

CFA1 1248.76 512 .881 .891 .892 .052 Anxiety5, Powless4CFA2 1096.82 449 .888 .898 .899 .052 AA1, SD10, TRL1CFA3 811.95 361 .912 .921 .922 .048 Anxiety2CFA4 708.77 334 .922 .931 .932 .046 SD2, SD3CFA5 556.71 283 .933 .942 .943 .043 SD6CFA6 499.49 259 .933 .942 .943 .042 Proceeded to

modification processfor purifying thetheoretical model

aDeleted variables cross loaded on latent variables based on LMs Test.

Purifying the measurement model

EQS 6.1 software was used to construct the structural equation model (Anderson &Gerbing, 1988). First, the measurement model was purified by conducting six suc-cessive confirmatory factor analyses (CFA). To assess whether a structural modelexists, and has an acceptable goodness of fit, the base model included latent factorsand measured variables for anxiety, powerlessness, apathy, disconnectedness, stu-dent perceptions of ideal content-based learning and teamwork-related learning.The goodness of fit for the first CFA model was reasonable (RMSEA = .054, CFI =.867, BNNFI = .856, IFI = .868). The goodness of fit was determined using robustestimation on the sample of 537 respondents because Mardia’s co-efficient was343.7, Satorra-Bentler scaled chi-square was 1664.4 (650 degrees of freedom,p = .000), and because non-normality can impact multivariate procedures.Lagrange Multiplier (LM) tests were used to identify the items that cross-loadedon several latent variables, and deleted them from the model. Table 2 shows thedetails of the six CFA tests conducted and the items of the latent variables elimi-nated as a result of the LM tests; i.e., see the italicized variables in Table 1 deletedbefore a theoretical model was fitted on the data. The final purified base model hadacceptable fit indices (see CFA6, BNNFI = .933, CFI = .942, RMSEA = .042).

Refining the theoretical model

The Wald test identifies the paths in the theoretical base model that should bedropped from the model because of smaller than acceptable t-statistics (Bentler &Wu, 2002). The LM test shows the multiple parameters that can be added to themodel (Bentler & Wu, 2002). Based on these tests, a series of revisions wereconducted to the base model. At every step, the model was modified; paths wereadded and deleted based on the Wald and LM tests. The final model emergedat the third iteration, and has acceptable fit indices (BNNFI = .918, IFI = .926,RMSEA = .043); all path coefficients are significant. Table 3 summarizes the

Sashittal, Jassawalla, and Markulis 425

Table 3: Model modification process for purifying the theoretical model.

Model SB Chi-sq Df NNFI CFI IFI RMSEA Action

Step 1 618.61 313 .891 .903 .904 .05 Dropped linksa:Disconnectedness → academic

Perf.Age → disconnectednessPowerlessness →

disconnectednessAdded linksb

Disconnectedness → apathyAnxiety → powerlessness

Step 2 633.34 315 .915 .923 .923 .44 Added linkb

Academic achievement →teamwork-related learning

Step 3 578.26 316 .924 .931 .932 .42 All remaining links aresignificant

Final model reported

aBased on Wald’s test.bBased on LM test.

Figure 2: Revised theoretical model.

changes made at each iteration and the corresponding fit indices. Figure 2 showsthe revised theoretical model; Table 4 shows the fit indices.

Key Findings

The contrast between Figures 1 and 2, and the list of hypotheses supported andunsupported by the data serve as a basis for this discussion (Table 5 includes

426 Students’ Perspective of Apathy and Social Disconnectedness

Table 4: Fit indices of the theoretical model.

Parameter Estimate

Satorra-Bentler zcaled Chi-square 602.6125 (317 degrees of freedom)Bentler-Bonett non-normed fit index (NNFI) .924Comparative fit index (CFI) .931Bollen’s fit index (IFI) .932Root mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) .04290% confidence interval of RMSEA .036–.047

means, alphas, and correlations; also see Table 6 for measurement and structuralparameters from the revised theoretical model). The comparison highlights thekey contributions of the study; i.e., the revised theoretical model explicates thenature of apathy and disconnectedness from the students’ perspective, isolatestheir significant antecedents, and shows their significant consequences. Briefly, thefollowing links are significant as hypothesized:

a. Higher level of powerlessness is associated with higher levels of apathy(H1b is validated).

b. Older students report greater levels of apathy (H1c is validated).c. Higher levels of anxiety are associated with higher levels of social dis-

connectedness (H2a is validated).d. Higher levels of social disconnectedness are associated with lower levels

of teamwork-related learning (H4a is validated).e. Higher levels of apathy are associated with lowered academic achieve-

ment and performance (H3b and H3c are validated).

However, contrary to qualitative research-derived hypotheses, the study sug-gests that:

a. Anxiety and apathy are unrelated (H1a is disproved).b. Powerlessness and social disconnectedness are unrelated (H2b is

disproved).c. Older students do not report higher levels of social disconnectedness (H2c

is disproved).d. Social disconnectedness does not lower academic achievement nor aca-

demic performance (H4b and H4c are disproved).e. Apathy does not lower teamwork-related learning (H3a is disproved).

Moreover, the study highlights three new relationships not identified byqualitative research:

a. Higher levels of social disconnectedness are associated with higher levelsof apathy.

b. Higher levels of anxiety are associated with higher levels of powerless-ness.

c. Higher levels of academic achievement are associated with higher levelsof teamwork-related learning.

Sashittal, Jassawalla, and Markulis 427

Tabl

e5:

Mea

ns,s

tand

ard

devi

atio

n,al

phas

,and

corr

elat

ions

(fina

lmod

el).

Cor

rela

tions

Soci

alTe

amw

ork-

Pow

er-

disc

onne

cted

-R

elat

edA

cade

mic

Aca

dem

icV

aria

ble

Alp

haM

ean

SDA

nxie

tyle

ssne

ssA

gene

ssA

path

yL

earn

ing

Ach

ieve

men

tPer

form

ance

Anx

iety

.732

2.45

8.7

281

Pow

erle

ssne

ss.7

532.

833

.806

.339

∗∗1

Age

a20

.56

2.07

9.0

1.0

341

S.D

isc.

.916

1.88

.743

.413

∗∗.1

77∗∗

.017

1A

path

y.7

631.

582

.634

.251

∗∗.2

50∗∗

.122

∗∗.4

34∗∗

1T-

RL

earn

ing

.831

3.35

5.8

21−.

084

−.11

6∗∗.0

4−.

254∗∗

−.20

3∗∗1

Aca

d.A

chie

v..7

493.

662

.628

−.08

4.2

95∗∗

−.04

−.16

1∗∗−.

296∗∗

.344

∗∗1

Aca

d.Pe

rfor

m.

3.16

5.7

41−.

09∗

−.10

8∗.0

81−.

121∗∗

−.22

2∗∗−.

091∗

.061

1

a No

alph

aca

lcul

ated

.

428 Students’ Perspective of Apathy and Social Disconnectedness

Table 6: Measurement and structural parameters from the Revised TheoreticalModel.

Structural Model Betas (t-values)

Powerlessness → anxiety .344 (4.484∗)Social disconnectedness → anxiety .534 (6.898∗)Apathy → powerlessness .255 (4.426∗)Apathy →social disconnectedness .465 (7.275)Apathy → age .124 (2.943)Teamwork-Related Learning → Social Disconnectedness −.180 (−3.871∗)Teamwork-Related Learning → Academic Achievement .350 (5.202∗)Academic Achievement → Apathy −.387 (−4.917)Academic Performance (GPA) → Apathy −.282 (−4.174∗)Measurement ModelApathy → I do not really care about being a student here 1∗∗

Apathy → I do not care much about what happens in College 1.103 (10.791)Apathy → Attending College is not high on my priority list .768 (8.566)

Anxiety → I find working in team environments really intimidating 1∗∗

Anxiety → It is hard for me to initiate conversations with other students. 1.816 (8.723)Anxiety → I am often afraid of saying what is on my mind in the classroom. 1.774 (8.568)

Acad. Achievement → The content of the courses I have taken really challengedme.

1∗∗

Acad. Achievement → The coursework has helped me develop a meaningfulphilosophy of life.

1.9 (6.518)

Acad. Achievement → I have a better appreciation of the complexity of the realworld.

2.342 (6.406)

Acad. Achievement → I am gaining knowledge that I will find useful in the realworld.

2.29 (6.182)

Acad. Achievement → I am learning the kinds of things that I believe will beuseful for a career.

2.188 (6.198)

Powerlessness → Even if there is something bothering me in my classes, I do nothave any power to change the situation.

1∗∗

Powerlessness → I find it difficult to concentrate in class. 1.836 (8.327)Powerlessness → I am often bored and sleepy in classes. 1.732 (7.95)

S.Disc → I am often lonely on campus. 1∗∗

S.Disc → I see myself as a loner. .921 (17.039)S.Disc → I feel disconnected from the general campus environment. 1.077 (22.249)S.Disc → Even around other students that I know, I do not feel I really belong. .979 (21.353)S.Disc → I do not really participate with anyone or a group on this campus. .818 (14.17)S.Disc → I cannot really relate to other students on this campus. .915 (18.081)S.Disc → I always get negative feedback from other people. .592 (11.853)

T-R Learning → I have learned a lot because I worked in teams with otherstudents.

1∗∗

T-R Learning → I think classroom teams have enhanced my learning as a student. 1.128 (25.15)T-R Learning → I have learned to work as a team player. .592 (12.107)T-R Learning → I am generally enthusiastic about team projects. .807 (14.839)

∗Parameter estimates are standardized with t-values shown in parentheses; all values aresignificant at p < .05.∗∗Indicant loading fixed at 1 to set the scale (t-values, all significant at p < .05).

Sashittal, Jassawalla, and Markulis 429

COMPARING FINDINGS AND CURRENT THEORY

In this discussion, key findings of the study are compared and contrasted withcurrent notions in the literature. The purpose is to place the findings in perspective,highlight the unique contributions, and aid future theory development. The dis-cussion culminates therefore in a list of research questions raised by the findings.Briefly, student attributions emerge as highly perceptive, and reinforce some andcontradict other views in the literature. In a similar vein, the findings about anxietyand powerlessness are aligned with some, and challenge other notions prevalent inthe literature.

The Perceptiveness and Value of Student Attributions

Among the reasons that students’ subjective perceptions, attributions, and self-reported learning remain under-represented in the business education literature arethe concerns that: (a) students may not know enough to articulate their psychosocialproblems nor make valid attributions about their causes and consequences, and(b) students’ self reports are likely to overstate their learning. The findings providesome evidence to suggest that student definitions and attributions are remarkablyperceptive because they resonate with empirical findings in many ways.

First, for instance, their attribution of anxiety as a key antecedent of apathyand disconnectedness has a basis in research findings. Anxiety seems prevalent inthe society and likely lies at the root of many psychosocial problems. For instance,generalized anxiety disorder afflicts about 40 million Americans (Henig, 2009). Upto 20% of healthy children are born with a temperamental predisposition towardanxiety and fear of social situations (Kagan & Snidman, 1999), who later becomeemotionally subdued, cautious, and wary (Biederman et al., 2001). Anxious peopleare shy (Cheek & Buss, 1981; Crozier, 2000), fail to initiate friendships (Gecas,1989), and lonely and disconnected (Brennan et al., 1998; DiTommaso et al.,2003; Mounts et al., 2006; Riggio, Watring, & Throckmorton, 1993; Schmidt &Fox, 1995).

Second, the identification of powerlessness as an antecedent of apathy anddisconnectedness is similarly perceptive. Powerlessness has concerned scholarsinterested in pedagogy (Luechauer & Schulman, 2002). Educational research alsoshows that powerlessness produces alienation, a notion aligned with student def-initions of disconnectedness (Wang, 2010). Research in healthcare organizationsshows that people ascribe belongingness to their immediate work area when theyfeel a sense of control or power over their work (Russell et al., 2010), i.e. the linkagebetween disconnectedness and powerlessness is established. Similarly, research innursing homes suggests that powerless people feel lonely and disconnected fromothers (Duncan, 2007), and become apathetic and non-participative (Meddaugh &Peterson, 1997). Some research finds that alienation, apathy, and powerlessnessare inseparable constructs (Kanungo, 1992).

Third, student descriptions of the QSLE construct resonate strongly withcurrent discussions. For instance, the data-derived notion of self-assessed aca-demic achievement (content-based learning) is aligned with the summative, end-of-semester evaluations of faculty, instruction, and curriculum content that studentsare asked to conduct in most classes (Sitzman et al., 2010). Scholars advocate that

430 Students’ Perspective of Apathy and Social Disconnectedness

such self-assessments of knowledge should be embedded into and conducted peri-odically during courses because they can influence the learning process (Dunning,Heath, & Suls, 2004; Sitzman et al., 2010; Sullivan, Hitchcock, & Dunnington,1999). Students’ concern with teamwork-related learning suggests that it factorsinto satisfaction with learning, and manifests itself sufficiently in their educationalexperiences. The notion that learning of teamwork skills is an important part ofbusiness education is widely shared (Hoover et al., 2010; Munoz & Huser, 2008).Finally, students’ concern with GPA as a key component of the QSLE constructseems to fit with the “faculty prerogative” view of learning assessment. GPA notonly serves as a direct measure of learning, it also reflects students’ cognitive abil-ity and motivation (Bartels, Bommer, & Rubin, 2000) and psychosocial well being(Rode et al., 2005; Waldman & Korbar, 2004). Rode et al. (2005) find that GPA re-flected “initiating and maintaining a range of self-regulatory behaviors. . . (which)encapsulated motivation, ability and other aspects of performance in a measurethat was very important to students and reflected the academic performance ofrespondents in a wide variety of subject areas, as graded by various raters. . . . ,”(pp. 425–426). In sum, students’ definition of QSLE can be anchored to theoreticaldevelopments in the field of assessing student learning outcomes.

Finally, student attributions of directionality of linkages between key con-structs are also perceptive. For instance, the revised theoretical model reinforcescurrent notions about linkages between disconnectedness and anxiety (Lee et al.,2001), apathy, and powerlessness (Campling, 2002). Moreover, while the literatureidentifies linkages among concepts included in the study, the model adds value byproviding preliminary evidence of the directionality of the linkages. While lone-liness is attributed to attachment anxiety (Brennan et al., 1998), early attachmentbehaviors (Hagarty et al., 1993), and linked with the need for attachment (Baumeis-ter & Leary, 1995), the study identifies the directionality of these linkages in thecontext of undergraduate business programs; i.e., disconnectedness emerges fromanxiety in social situations. This literature-derived directionality was tested, how-ever, these models either did not converge, or did not produce significant parameterestimates or fit statistics. The best fit of the model was based on the qualitative-dataderived notions of directionality. In sum, findings reinforce the view that studentsare not just active participants in the learning process, but their self-reports, sub-jective definitions and attributions of causes are aligned with current notions in theliterature—and worthy of study in their own right.

Anxiety

Either the tenuousness of campus life or the proclivity toward dysfunctionalanxiety—or the interaction among them—is producing significant levels of apathyamong powerless and disconnected students. Anxious students feel intimidated byothers, fail to volunteer or initiate conversation, or say what is on their mind in theclassroom. Scholars suggest that anxiety can make for attentive friends and goodstudents (Henig, 2009), and that anxiety can sharpen focus on exams (Schmidtet al., 2010). The study does not provide parallel evidence; student descriptions ofanxiety is limited to their general sense of dread about saying or doing anythingthat will attract the scrutiny of others, and about their actions likely to produce

Sashittal, Jassawalla, and Markulis 431

negative social consequences. Similarly, current views implicitly regard anxietyas a consequence and not an antecedent of powerlessness, as does this study.Luechauer and Schulman (2002, p. 42) observe, “. . . [T]oo many faculty viewthemselves as either experts who transmit information and concepts or formal au-thorities who set goals and procedures. They act like bosses. . . . This orientationcreates grade consciousness, dependency, and a real fear of being stupid.” Anxiety,according to their impressionistic learning, is a product of students disempoweredby authoritarian instructors and choice-less curriculums. Student responses suggestand the analysis later confirms that the reverse is likely true, i.e., anxious studentsfeel powerless.

As Figure 2 shows, students’ anxiety triggers disconnectedness and power-lessness, indirectly produces apathy, and eventually hurts the QSLEs, i.e., nega-tively impacts perceptions of content-based and team-related learning, and GPA.While the ultimate responsibility of reducing anxiety likely rests with students,this finding highlights the importance of faculty and administrator-led initiatives toreduce student anxiety as the root cause of key psychosocial and learning-relatedoutcomes. The first order implications of the findings based on students’ defi-nition of anxiety are clear; anxiety among undergraduate business students maybe reduced if their faculty and administrators take steps to create a socially safeenvironment in the classroom and on campus in which students can confront andmanage the intimidation they feel, and initiate conversations and gain feedbackwithout negative socio-emotional consequences.

The findings highlight the importance of new research that can help determinewhether dysfunctional anxiety among undergraduate business students emergesfrom the mismatched square pegs of students’ characteristics and the round holesof undergraduate business curriculums and instruction. First, for instance, themismatch between what is expected of students by instructors in terms of teamworkdeserves examination as a possible source of anxiety. Students in the sample hadparticipated in over five classes that required them to work with others in teams;however specific instructions and guidelines for working in teams were provided inonly three of the 33 sections of the classes from which data were collected. Whetherleaving students to figure out ways of working with and collaborating with otherstriggers high levels of anxiety deserves investigation. Currently, more instructorsassign students to teams than those who provide clear guidelines about functioningeffectively as team members (Bryant & Albring, 2006; Clinebell & Stecher, 2003;Holmer, 2001); i.e., scholars have identified a key mismatch between instructionand expectations in undergraduate business programs. While working in teams,the level of anxiety can escalate if students cannot hold each other accountable orfail to collaborate with each other; i.e., outcomes more likely when they receiveinadequate teamwork-related coaching and instruction (Hansen, 2006). The natureand extent of anxiety triggered by the lack of teamwork-related instruction remainsunder-investigated in business education literature.

Second, the mismatches produced by widely varying student aptitudes andthe increased accreditation-driven standardization of b-school curriculums andtesting deserve fresh examination as a possible source of anxiety. The populationof business students remains at an all-time high. The Bureau of Labor Statisticsreports that of the 3.2 million graduates of American high schools in 2008, 68.6%

432 Students’ Perspective of Apathy and Social Disconnectedness

were enrolled in college. In a related vein, of the approximately 2200 institutionsoffering four-year degree programs, over 90% have non-selective admission poli-cies (Maeroff, 2005). One estimate suggests that 22% of all undergraduates areenrolled in business programs (Chace, 2009). The funneling of approximately halfa million students every year (or approximately 15% of all high school graduates)with wide variances in aptitudes into standardized business programs—where cur-riculums adhere closely to guidelines of accreditation bodies and assessment oflearning outcomes relies strongly on standardized multiple choice exams (ETStests, in-class tests)—may produce mismatches that exacerbate the anxiety. Worseyet, if anxious students perceive others as innately belonging in and comfortablewith the academic environment of business programs, it may serve to worsen theiranxiety—which as the study suggests is triggering powerlessness and disconnect-edness. If future research validates this notion, it will call for a renewed emphasison student selection, if not for the broadening of business curriculums in responseto multiple aptitudes, cognitive abilities, motivations, and interests of the upcomingbatches of 18-year old high school graduates.

Third, the mismatch between the millennial generation’s expectations andlearning styles and the classroom environment deserves investigation as a possiblesource of anxiety. Although a critical mass of insights from rigorous empiricalstudies is overshadowed by writings informed by impressionistic data, most currentthinking about the millennial generation (i.e., those born after 1982 and comprisingthe entire sample) speaks discouragingly of their interest and ability to learnin a traditional college classroom with an emphasis on lectures and one-waycommunication. For instance, this generation: (a) is quick to disengage if theirhigh expectations are not easily and quickly rewarded (Levine, 2005; Twenge,2006), (b) prefers classes with low levels of workload and professes low levels ofinterest in courses that accentuate analytical and computational skills (Milliron,2008), (c) wants good grades without the interest in putting in requisite effort(Seaman, 2005), and (d) has received and come to expect prodigious strokingand positive feedback—and are quick to disengage and disconnect when it is notreceived (Levine, 2005). They are also known for: (a) their lack of social trust andfor the resulting practical world view (Wolburg & Pokrywczynski, 2001); (b) strongbonds with their parents and their inability to delay gratification (Tucker, 2006);and (c) their inextricable link with the digital environment (Wankel, 2009), i.e.,they send and receive an average of 2272 text messages per month, devote over9 hours a month to social networking sites, and have little fluency with nonverbalcommunication (Bauerlein, 2009). Whether these descriptors hold up to empiricalscrutiny, and whether the mismatch between their defining characteristics andcurrent curriculum and instruction offered in undergraduate business programsproduces anxiety are questions for future research.

Powerlessness

The study suggests that powerlessness emerges from anxiety and triggers ap-athy, and adversely affects the quality of students’ learning experiences. Thisfinding differs from the literature’s view in at least two important ways. First,people feeling powerless are known to feel inefficacious and unable to control

Sashittal, Jassawalla, and Markulis 433

interpersonal relationships (Paulhus, 1983). In a similar vein, powerlessness wasfound to produce distancing (Schmidt et al., 2010). There is no evidence in thefindings to suggest that feelings of powerlessness are leading to disconnectedness,although they may impact interpersonal relationships in other ways. Second, pow-erlessness and dissociative behaviors are defined as unrelated constructs (Irwin,1998). To the extent apathy is a dissociative behavior—it seems to emerge as aconsequence of powerlessness. These differences likely relate to the unique natureof the sample (i.e., traditional age undergraduate business students), and the uniqueconcern with powerlessness as it relates to the classroom.

The literature is largely supportive of the notion that the nature of curriculumand instruction is currently heightening feelings of powerlessness among students.For instance, scholars note that students currently feel disempowered because theycannot impact class policies and due dates, find themselves at the end of one-waycommunication, find material irrelevant to their lives, receive no opportunity tovoice their opinions or apply their learning, and their feedback is solicited only atthe end of the semester (Luechauer & Schulman, 2002, p. 43). Conversely, studentsfeel empowered when they: (a) learn on their own, are involved in knowledgecreation, and encouraged to disagree and provide alternative points of view, and(b) can relate to instructors’ experiences, in an environment they feel is conduciveto learning (Thomas & Velthouse, 1990). Luechauer and Shulman (2002) furthernote: “Feelings of powerlessness can be managed by reducing “standardization,memorization and regurgitation” (p. 45). Curriculums and instruction, they suggest,should be more meaningful, help students feel more competent, permit them toimpact the design of the course, and allow them to make choices about what theylearn.

The study, aligned with Luechauer and Schulman (2002) and Thomas andVelthouse (1990), confirms that powerlessness (or disempowerment) is a negativeaffliction. Additionally, the study finds that: (a) powerlessness-triggered apathyhurts the quality of students’ learning experiences in significant ways, and (b) cur-ricular and instructional innovations can reduce powerlessness if they can initiallyreduce anxiety. To the extent curricular and instructional innovations cannot ad-dress the multiple, complex sources of anxiety, new research on contingency factorsis sorely needed to address the question: Who should be empowered, how, when,and by whom? Sole reliance on empowering curriculum and instruction may forcestudents to make decisions about content, schedules, testing, and evaluation thatthey are unable to make—worsening their anxiety, or produce evidence of learn-ing unacceptable to faculty, administrators, potential employers, and accreditationbodies.

Questions for Future Research

For future attempts that aim to assess student anxiety, social disconnectedness,powerlessness, apathy, and the QSLE construct, we present refined measurementscales in the Appendix, Exhibit 1. The following list encapsulates the questionsraised by our study that appear to deserve additional academic scrutiny:

a. What faculty and administrator-led initiatives (both campus- and class-related actions) can trigger anxiety among students?

434 Students’ Perspective of Apathy and Social Disconnectedness

b. Is the mismatch between students’ aptitudes, preparedness, and attitudes,and the demands of the college curriculum triggering anxiety? If so, inwhat way?

c. Is the lack of teamwork-related instruction, coupled with the requirementto function in teams, triggering anxiety among students?

d. Is insufficiently rigorous selection of students based on objective criteriasuch as GPA and SAT scores triggering anxiety among students?

e. Is student anxiety related to the expectations and characteristics of themillennial generation?

f. While student empowerment is strongly advocated in the literature, whichstudents should be empowered to make choices about curriculum andinstructions, when, and by whom?

g. Why are older students reporting higher levels of apathy? What are theimplications for socialization processes led by campus administrators?

h. What are normal levels of anxiety, social disconnectedness, powerless-ness, and apathy on a college campus?

IMPLICATIONS FOR INSTRUCTORS AND PROGRAMADMINISTRATORS

This discussion of implications should be viewed in the context of the followingfeatures of students at the university at which this research was conducted; theyindicate the nature of the population to which the findings are more generalizable.The average incoming SAT of all freshmen is 1329, average ACT is 29.2, highschool average is 93.5, and 79% of the students make up the top 20% of their highschool class. Sophomores apply to the business program and are admitted only iftheir GPA is 2.85 or better (transfer students need GPA of 3.0 or better).

Implications for Innovative Educational Practice

The study produces evidence to support the following notions: (a) undergradu-ate business students are likely afflicted with significant levels of dysfunctionalanxiety, powerlessness, and disconnectedness which trigger apathy and lack ofinterest in learning and participating in class, and (b) these afflictions are ad-versely affecting students’ learning experiences. As such, these are disquietingfindings given the escalating cost of attending college and the resources directedto promoting psychosocial health of students, i.e., resources directed to all extra-curricular activities. In light of the gravity and prevalence of the problems, threepotential areas for innovative educational practice can be identified from thefindings.

First, to the extent college policies or instructor actions are exacerbatingexisting psychosocial problems, or worse yet, creating anxiety and powerless-ness where none existed, re-evaluation of curriculums and instruction is stronglyadvised. For instance requiring students to work in teams without providing coach-ing and instruction about working in teams likely exacerbates anxiety—and trig-gers a host of other psychosocial dysfunctions. Courses that require teamworkfrom students should, the study suggests, provide coaching and instruction for

Sashittal, Jassawalla, and Markulis 435

functioning effectively in teams without assuming that all students are equallypre-skilled. New instruction is strongly implicated because the socialization of themillennial generation has likely rendered them less skilled for formal teamworkwithout adequate adult supervision (Jassawalla, Malshe, & Sashittal, 2008). Whenleft on their own, and without adequate coaching, they seem less likely to buildsocial relationships with others via face-to-face interaction (preferring cell phonesand Web-based social networks), or work well in teams. Similarly, efforts to em-power students, as some scholars suggest, are also likely to produce beneficialresults (Luechauer & Schulman, 2002,). In a related vein, curriculums that showthe linkage between classroom-related learning opportunities and career-relatedsuccess might help students find meaning in what they are learning.

Second, to the extent students are pre-afflicted, and bring dysfunctional levelsof anxiety, powerlessness, and social disconnectedness into the classroom, apathyseems likely to result regardless of the curriculum and instruction. Current researchsuggests that applicants to colleges and business programs are very likely pre-afflicted by high levels of social (Putnam, 2000), and social anxiety disorder(Henig, 2009). Hence, there is merit to thinking about careful evaluation andselection, and preventing some students from infecting the rest of the student body.Careful in-class assessment and monitoring to weed out dysfunctional students isalso an implication because of the high cost remedying dysfunctional psychosocialproblems that is unfairly borne by all students.

Third, given that apathetic students do not care about being a student orattending college, nor rate college attendance as a priority—it becomes importantto ask how and why these students remain enrolled in business programs. In thisregard, new thinking about increasing academic rigor in classrooms to increasethe consequences of disconnection, disengagement, apathy toward being a studentor attending college seems important. It seems similarly important to evaluatewhether the variance in the rigor across the curriculum is triggering anxiety amongstudents. For instance, student anxiety may be exacerbated if some classes aretoo close to therapy; i.e., instructors overtly validate individual student’s feelings,opinions, and world views without judging their merits or providing feedback abouttheir validity whereas other classes are too close to a boot camp; i.e., instructorsinvalidate individual creativity and contributions, expect unquestioning adherence,and allow no flexibility.

Implications for Program Administrators

The findings produce two major implications for undergraduate business programadministrators (see Appendix, Exhibit 2 for a list). First, the sample seemed ho-mogenous in terms of age at first glance (i.e., 96% of sample is 23 or younger,and 100% of the sample is 25 or younger; population average estimate is 20.64±1.66). However, even within this narrow variation in age, older students reportedsignificantly higher levels of apathy—which produce a significant negative impacton the quality of students’ experiences in the business program. Moreover, basedon the qualitative research, it was expected that older students—who had spentmore time developing and utilizing socialization skills—would report higher levelsof disconnectedness. This, too, was not the case: age and disconnectedness were

436 Students’ Perspective of Apathy and Social Disconnectedness

unrelated in the findings. This far-reaching impact of age among traditional-agecollege students has yet to receive any attention from scholars. The findings sug-gest that administrative effort to socialize and assimilate students should continuebeyond freshmen and sophomores; juniors and seniors can benefit from oppor-tunities for interacting and collaborating with others via curricular (e.g. learningcommunities) and extra-curricular initiatives that alleviate the apathy they feeltoward learning.

In a related vein, even though there was no specific hypothesis regarding gen-der, some findings deserve a brief mention. Female students report significantlylower apathy than male students (male mean = 1.7706, female mean = 1.5055).Female students also report higher levels of content-based learning (male meancontent-based learning = 3.67, female mean content-based learning = 3.79), al-though there is no significant difference in their GPAs. There were no other notabledifferences among the genders; the structural equation models that emerge fromsplitting the data by gender are significant, and yield comparable parameters andfit statistics.

Second, the findings highlight the importance of defining normal levels ofapathy, disconnectedness, anxiety, and powerlessness among undergraduate busi-ness student, i.e., issues relevant to both student selection processes and man-agement of student life. Definition of normal can help evaluate current lev-els of psychosocial problems and develop remedial action. In the study, theaverage student apathy (population average estimate with 95% confidence =1.65 ± .027), and average student disconnectedness (population average estimatewith 95% confidence = 1.92 ± .032) are sufficient to produce an adverse im-pact on students’ learning experiences. These averages are somewhat lower andcompare favorably to reports about similar constructs; average shyness is re-ported as 2.65 and average loneliness as 2.02 on a five-point scale among firstsemester college freshmen (Mounts et al., 2006). Moreover, population esti-mates of average students’ anxiety (2.31 ± .029) and powerlessness (2.65 ± .03)suggest that they exist at a higher level than both disconnectedness and ap-athy. Defining what is normal and what to do if the numbers are abnormalemerge as key challenges related to improving the quality of students’ learningexperiences.

CONCLUSIONS

Aligned with the interest in student perceptions, links between constructs definedby students were examined. While hypothesized links were simultaneously testedusing structural equation model procedure, the study does not provide evidenceof causal linkages because it did not identify the true neurobiological or chemicalcauses of apathy and disconnectedness, nor conduct an experiment using timeseries data. Identification of causes is left to future research efforts. While thestudy provides a cross-sectional snapshot, new studies that can track changes inapathy and disconnectedness, and in their antecedents and consequences, are sorelyneeded. Similarly, while the study indicates the directionality of linkages basedon qualitative-data derived inferences, experimental research is necessary beforecausal linkages are identified. The validity of the model requires testing acrossmultiple campuses (private and state schools of multiple sizes) and heterogeneous

Sashittal, Jassawalla, and Markulis 437

populations (traditional age and adult students), and in multiple settings (differentlyranked business programs) before generalizable, actionable insights are produced.

The key motivator of the present study was the finding that apathy and dis-connectedness are: (a) present among students enrolled in undergraduate businessprograms; (b) associated with the prevalent, if not universal, incidence of socialloafing in classroom teams; and (c) negatively associated with student learning(Jassawalla et al., 2009). The grossly underrepresented student perspective alsoled to the exploration of their subjectively defined experiences, perspectives, andattributions. Apathy and disconnectedness seem alive in business programs, linkedto unhealthy levels of anxiety and powerlessness, and hurt the quality of students’learning experiences. Innovations in educational practices and program admin-istration are necessary to help students realize the full potential of their collegeeducation.

References

Anderson, J. C., & Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in prac-tice: A review and recommended two-step approach. Psychological Bulletin,103(3), 411–423.

Arbaugh, J. B. (2000). Virtual classroom characteristics and student satisfac-tion in Internet-based MBA courses. Journal of Business education, 24(1),32–54.

Arbaugh, J. B. (2005). Is there an optimal design for online MBA courses? Academyof Management Learning & Education, 4(2), 135–149.

Arbaugh, J. B., & Benbunan-Fich, R. (2006). An investigation of epistemologicaland social dimensions of teaching online learning environments. Academyof Management Learning & Education, 5(4), 435–451.

Asendorpf, J. B. (2000). Shyness and adaptation to the social world of university.In W. R. Crozier (Ed.), Shyness: Development, consolidation, and change.New York, NY: Routledge, 103–120.

Astin, A. W. (1993). What matters in college: Four critical years revisited. SanFrancisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. New York, NY: General LearningPress.

Bartels, L. K., Bommer, W. H., & Rubin, R. S. (2000). Student performance: As-sessment centers versus traditional classroom evaluation techniques. Journalof Education for Business, 75, 198–201.

Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interper-sonal attachments as a fundamental human emotion. Psychological Bulletin,112, 461–484.

Bauerlein, M. (2009). Taste: Why Gen-Y Johnny can’t read nonverbal cues. TheWall Street Journal, August 28: W11.

Benbunan-Fich, R., Hiltz, S. R., & Harasim, L. (2005). The online interactionlearning model: An integrated theoretical framework for learning networks.

438 Students’ Perspective of Apathy and Social Disconnectedness

In S. R. Hiltz & R. Goldman (Eds.), Learning together online. Mahwah, NJ:Erlbaum, 19–37.

Bentler, P. M., & Wu, E. J. C. (2002). EQS6 for Windows: Users Guide. Encino:CA. Multivariate Software Inc.

Berman, W. H., & Sperling, M. B. (1991). Parental attachment and emotionaldistress in the transition to college. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20(4),427–440.

Besharov, D. J., & Gardiner, K. N. (1998). Preventing youthful disconnectedness.Children and Youth Services Review, 20(9–10), 797–818.

Biederman, J., Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R., Rosenbaum, J. F., Herot, C., Friedman,D., Snidman, N., Kagan, J., & Faraone, S. V. (2001). Further evidence ofassociation between behavioral inhibition and social anxiety in children. TheAmerican Journal of Psychiatry, 158(10), 1673–1679.

Bjornsen, C. A., Scepansky, J. A., & Suzuki, A. (2007). Apathy and personalitytraits among college students: A cross-cultural comparison. College StudentJournal, 41(3), 668–675.

Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement ofadult attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes(Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships. New York, NY: GuildfordPress, 46–76.

Brown, K. G. (2005). An examination of the structure and nomological networkof trainee reactions: A closer look at “smile sheets.” Journal of AppliedPsychology, 90(5), 991–1001.

Bryant, S. M., & Albring, S. M. (2006). Effective teambuilding: Guidance foraccounting educators. Issues in Accounting Education, 21(3), 241–265.

Cacioppo, J. T., & Patrick, W. (2008). Loneliness: Human nature and the need forsocial connection. New York, NY: W.W. Norton & Co. Inc.

Campling, P. (2002). Connection and catastrophe, hope and despair in our border-line world. British Journal of Psychotherapy, 19(2), 235–245.

Carey, K. (2004). A matter of degrees: Improving graduation and universities.Washington, DC: Education Trust.

Cary, P. (2001). The challenges of striving to measure academic excellence: Whatcounts. U.S. News & World Report, 131(10), 105.

Chace, W. M. (2009). The decline of the English department: How it happenedand what could be done to reverse it. The American Scholar, 78(4), 32–42.

Cheek, J. M., & Buss, A. H. (1981). Shyness and sociability. Journal of PersonalSocial Psychology, 41, 330–339.

Chen, G., Donahue, L. M., & Klimosky, R. J. (2004). Training undergradu-ates to work in organizational teams. Academy of Management learning &Education, 3(1), 27–40.

Clinebell, S., & Stecher, M. (2003). Teaching teams to be teams: An exerciseusing the Myers-Briggs type indicator and the Five-Factor personality traits.Journal of Business education, 27(3), 362–383.

Sashittal, Jassawalla, and Markulis 439

Coffield, K. E. (1981). Student apathy: A comparative study. Teaching of Psychol-ogy, 8(1), 26–28.

Coffield, K. E., & Buckalew, L. W. (1986). Student apathy: An analysis of relevantvariables. College Student Journal, 22(2), 211–214.

Crawford, W. (2005). The coming of the borgs. EContent, 28(5), 43.

Crozier, W. R. (2000). Shyness and social relationships: Continuity and change.In W. R. Crozier (Ed.), Shyness: Development, consolidation, and change.New York, NY: Routledge, 103–120.

Davidson, W. B., & Beck, H. P. (2006). Survey of Academic Orientations scoresand persistence in college freshmen. Journal of College Student Retention:Research, Theory and Practice, 8(3), 297–305.

Davidson, W. B., Bromfield, J. M., & Beck Hall, P. (2007). Beneficial academicorientations and self-actualization of college students. Psychology Reports,100(2), 604–612.

Delle Fave, A., & Massimini, F. (2005). The investigation of optimal experienceand apathy. European Psychologist, 10(4), 267–274.

DiTommaso, E., Brennen-McNulty, C., Ross, L., & Burgess, B. (2003). Attachmentstyles, social skills, and loneliness in young adults. Personality and IndividualDifferences, 35(2), 303–312.

Duncan, C. (2007). Loneliness, helplessness, and boredom. Nursing Homes, 56(9),86–87.

Dunning, D., Heath, C., & Suls, J. M. (2004). Flawed self assessment: Implicationsfor health, education, and the workplace. Psychological Science in the PublicInterest, 5(3), 69–106.

Eliasoph, N. (1998). Avoiding politics: How Americans produce apathy in everydaylife. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University press.

Endres, M. L., & Hurtubis, C. A. (2009). The multifaceted nature of online MBAstudent satisfaction and impacts on behavioral intention. Journal of Educa-tion for Business, 84(5), 304–313.

Friedman, T. L. (2010). We’re No. 1(1)! New York Times. http://www.nytimes.com/2010/09/12/opinion/12friedman.html?ref=thomaslfriedman

Friga, P. N., Bettis, R. A., & Sullivan, R. S. (2003). Changes in graduate businesseducation and new business school strategies for the 21st century. Academyof Management Learning & Education, 2(3), 223–249.

Gecas, V. (1989). The social psychology of self-efficacy. Annual Review ofSociology, 15, 291–316.

Goldrick-Rab, S., & Pfeffer, F. T. (2009). Beyond access: Explaing socioeconomicdifferences in college transfer. Sociology of Education, 82(2), 101–125.

Goodman, P. S., & Beenen, G. (2008). Organizational learning contracts andbusiness education. Academy of Management Learning & Education, 7(4),521–534.

Guernina, Z. (1992). The assessment of adolescents’ behavior problems. Interna-tional Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 4(1), 85–92.

440 Students’ Perspective of Apathy and Social Disconnectedness

Hagerty, B. M., Lynch-Sauer, J., Patusky, K., & Bouwsema, M. (1993). An emerg-ing theory of human relatedness. IMAGE: Journal of Nursing Scholarship,25(4), 291–296.

Hansen, R. S. (2006). Benefits and problems with student teams: Suggestions forimproving team projects. Journal of Education for Business, 82(1), 11–19.

Henig, R. M. (2009). Anxiety! The New York Times Magazine, October 4, 1-L.

Holmer, L. L. (2001). Will we teach leadership or skilled incompetence? Thechallenge of student project teams. Journal of Business education, 25(5),590–605.

Hoover, D. J., Giambatista, R. C., Sorenson, R. L., & Bommer, W. H. (2010).Assessing the effectiveness of the whole person learning pedagogy inskill acquisition. Academy of Management Learning & Education, 9(2),192–207.

Hu, S., & Kuh, G. D. (2003). Maximizing what students get out of college: Testing alearning productivity model. Journal of College Student Development, 44(2),185–203.

Irwin, H. J. (1998). Attitudinal predictors of dissociation: Hostility and powerless-ness. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 132(4), 389–400.

Jassawalla, A. R., Sashittal H. C., & Malshe, A. (2009). Students’ perceptions ofsocial loafing, its antecedents and consequences in undergraduate businessclassroom teams. Academy of Management Learning & Education, 8(1),42–54.

Jassawalla, A. R., Malshe, A., & Sashittal, H. (2008). Student perceptions ofsocial loafing in undergraduate business classroom teams. Decision SciencesJournal of Innovative Education, 6(2), 392–415.

Kagan, J., & Snidman, N. (1999). Early childhood predictors of adult anxietydisorder. Biological Psychiatry, 46(11), 1536–1541.

Kanungo, R. N. (1992). Alienation and empowerment: Some ethical imperativesin business. Journal of Business Ethics, 11(5,6), 413–422.

Knapp, L. G., Kelly-Reid, J. E., Whitmore, R. W., & Miller, E. (2007). Enrollmentin postsecondary institutions, fall 2005; graduation rates 1999 and 2002cohorts; and financial statistics, fiscal year 2005. Publication No: NCES2007–154. Washington DC: National Center for Education Statistics, U.S.Department of Education. http://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/pubsinfo.asp?pubid=2007154.

Kraiger, K., Ford, J. K., & Salas, E. (1993). Application of cognitive skill based, andaffective theories of learning outcomes to new methods of training evaluation.Journal of Applied Psychology, 78(2), 311–328.

Kruger, J., & Dunning, D. (1999). Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficultiesin recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1121–1134.

Lang, J. M. (2007). Did you learn anything? Chronicle of Higher Education,53(March 9): C1–C4.

Sashittal, Jassawalla, and Markulis 441

Larson, R. W. (2000). Toward a psychology of positive youth development.American Psychologist, 55(1), 170–183.

Lee, R. M., Draper, M., & Lee, S. (2001). Social connectedness, dysfunctional in-terpersonal behaviors, and psychological distress: Testing a mediator model.Journal of Counseling Psychology, 48(3), 310–318.

Lee, R. M., & Robbins, S. B. (1995). Measuring belongingness: The social connect-edness and the social assurance scales, Journal of Counseling Psychology,42(2), 232–241.

Lee, R. M., & Robbins, S. B. (1998). The relationship between social connect-edness and anxiety, self-esteem, and social identity, Journal of CounselingPsychology, 45(3), 338–345.

Lee, R. M., & Robbins, S. B. (2000). Understanding social connectedness incollege women and men. Journal of Counseling and Development, 78(4),484–491.

Lee, S. (2004). Apathy in Japanese and Korean high school students: Careerdevelopment. Japanese Jornal of Developmental Psychology, 15(3), 302–312.

Leob, P. R. (1994). Generation at the crossroads: Apathy and action on the Amer-ican campus. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

Levine, M. (2005). Ready or not, here life comes. New York, NY: Simon &Schuster.

Luechauer, D. L., & Shulman, G. M. (2002). Creating empowered learners: Adecade of trying to practice what we preach. Organizational DevelopmentJournal, 20(3), 42–51.

Maeroff, G. I. (2005). The media: Degrees of coverage. In R. H. Hersh & J. Merrow(Eds.), Declining by degrees: Higher education at risk. New York, NY:Palgrave MacMillan.

Marsh, H. W. (1983). Multidimensional ratings of teaching effectiveness bystudents from different academic settings and their relation to stu-dent/course/instructor characteristics. Journal of Educational Psychology,75, 150–166.

Matsubara, T. (1993). Characteristics of research trends of student apathy. JapaneseJournal of Counseling Science, 26(2), 163–177.

McCormick, A. C. (2003). Swirling and double-dipping: New patterns of studentattendance and their implications for higher education. New Directions forHigher Education, 121(Spring), 13–24.

Meddaugh, D. I., & Peterson, B. (1997). Removing powerlessness from the nursinghome. Nursing Homes, 46(8), 32–35.

Miles, L. (2001). Marketers warm to electronic learning. Marketing, February 1,35.

Milliron, V. C. (2008). Exploring millennial student values and societal trends:Accounting course selection preferences. Issues in Accounting Education,23(3), 405–419.

442 Students’ Perspective of Apathy and Social Disconnectedness

Morisano, D., Hirsch, J. B., Peterson, J. B., Pihl, R. O., & Shore, B. M. (2010).Setting, elaborating, and reflecting on personal goals improves academicperformance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(2), 255–264.

Morse, R. J., & Flanagan, S. (2009). How we calculate rankings. U.S. News &World Report, 146(8), 101–117.

Mounts, N. S., Valentiner, D. P., Anderson, K. L., & Boswell, M. K. (2006).Shyness, sociability and parental support for the college transition: Rela-tion to adolescents’ adjustment. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 5(1),71–80.

Munoz, C., & Huser, A. (2008). Experiential and cooperative learning: Using asituation analysis project in Principles of Marketing. Journal of Educationfor Business, 83(4), 214–219.

Okada, F. (1995). Student apathy: A footnote in the history of psychiatry or adisorder unique to post-adolescence? The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry,40(1), 51–52.

Pancer, S. M., Hunsberger, B., Pratt, M. W., & Alisat, S. (2000). Cognitive com-plexity of expectations and adjustment to university in the first year. Journalof Adolescent Research, 15, 38–57.

Paulhus, D. (1983). Sphere-specific measures of perceived control. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 44(6), 1253–1265.

Perry, R. P. (2003). Perceived (academic) control and causal thinking in achieve-ment settings. Canadian Psychology, 44, 312–331.

Pinkleton, B. E., & Wientraub-Austin, E. (2004). Media perceptions and publicaffairs in the politically inexperienced. Mass Communication and Society,7(3), 319–337.

Pringle, C., & Mitri, M. (2007). Assessment practices in AACSB-Accredited Busi-ness Schools. Journal of Education for Business, 82(4), 202–211.

Putnam, R. D. (2000). Bowling alone. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.

Riggio, R., Watring, K., & Throckmorton, B. (1993). Social skills, social support,and psychological adjustment. Personality and Individual Differences, 15(3),275–280.

Rode, J. C., Arthaud-Day, M. L., Mooney, C. E., Near, J. P., Baldwin, T. T., Bom-mer, W. H., & Rubin, R. S. (2005). Life satisfaction and student performance.Academy of Management Learning & Education, 4(4), 421–433.

Russell, D. W. (1996). The UCLA loneliness scale (Version 3): Reliability,validity, and factor structure. Journal of Personality Assessment, 66(1),20–40.

Russell, V., Wyness, L. A., McAuliffe, E., & Fellenz, M. (2010). The socialidentity of hospital consultants as managers. Journal of Health Organizationand Management. 24(3), 200–236.

Sanchez, R. J., Bauer, T. N., & Paronto, M. E. (2006). Peer-mentoring fresh-men: Implications for satisfaction, commitment, and retention to graduation.Academy of Management Learning & Education, 5(1), 25–37.

Sashittal, Jassawalla, and Markulis 443

Schmidt, L. A., & Fox, N. A. (1995). Individual differences in young adults’shyness and sociability: Personality and health correlates. Personality andIndividual Differences, 19(4), 455–462.

Schmidt, S., Tinti, C., Levine, L., & Testa S. (2010). Appraisals, emotions andemotion regulation: An integrative approach. Motivation and Emotion, 34(1),63–81.

Scott, C. (2001). The deserted library. Chronicle of Higher Education, 48(12),A35– A38.

Seaman, B. (2005). Binge: What your college student won’t tell you. Hoboken, NJ:John Wiley & Sons.

Shimosaka, T., (2001). Apathy: Junior high, high school, and college students,Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology, 49(3), 305–313.

Shipley, M. F., Johnson, M., & Hashemi, S. (2009). Cognitive learning styleand its effects on perceptions of learing, satisfaction and social interac-tions in virtual teams. Journal of American Academy of Business, 14(2),17–28.

Sitzmann, T., Ely, K., Brown, K. G., & Bauer, K. N. (2010). Self-assessment ofknowledge: A cognitive learning or affective measure? Academy of Manage-ment Learning & Education, 9(2), 169–191.

Speckman, K. R. (2004). Reversing youth apathy. The Quill, 92(August), 30–32.

Sullivan, M. E., Hitchcock, M. A., & Dunnington, G. L. (1999). Peer and selfassessment during problem-based tutorials. American Journal of Surgery,177(March), 266–269.

Tao, S., Dong, Q., Pratt, M. W., Hunsberger, B., & Pancer, M. S. (2000). Socialsupport: Relations to coping and adjustment during the transition to universityin the People’s Republic of China. Journal of Adolescent Research, 15(1),123–144.

Thomas, K., & Velthouse, B. (1990). Cognitive elements of empowerment: An“interpretive” model of intrinsic task motivation. Academy of ManagementReview, 15, 661–681.

Tinto, V. (2007). Research and practice in student retention: What next? Journalof College Student Retention: Research, Theory and Practice, 8, 1–19.

Tucker, P. (2006). Teaching the Millennial generation. The Futurist, 40(3), 7.

Twenge, J. M. (2006). Generation Me. New York, NY: Free Press (Simon &Schuster Inc.).

Voss, R., Gruber, T., & Rappel, A. (2010). Which classroom service encountersmake students happy or unhappy? Insights from an online CIT study. TheInternational Journal of Educational Management, 24(7), 615–631.

Waldman, D. A., & Korbar, T. (2004). Student assessment center performance inthe prediction of early career success. Academy of Management Learning &Education, 3(2), 151–167.

Wankel, C. (2009). Management education using social media. Organization Man-agement Journal, 6(4), 251–162.

444 Students’ Perspective of Apathy and Social Disconnectedness

Wang, Y. (2010). Embracing dissonant voices in English classrooms. Contempo-rary Issues in Education Research, 3(3), 21–27.

Wei, M., Russell, D. W., & Zakalik, R. A. (2005). Adult attachment, social self-sufficiency, self-disclosure, lonekiness, and subsequent depression for fresh-man college students: A longitudinal study. Journal of Counseling Psychol-ogy, 52(4), 602–614.

Wintre, M. G., & Yaffe, M. (2000). First-year students’ adjustment to university lifeas a function of relationship with parents. Journal of Adolescent Research,15(1), 9–13.

Wolburg, J. M., & Pokrywczynski, J. (2001). A psychographic analysis of Gener-ation Y college students. Journal of Advertising Research, 41(5), 33–52.

Wolf, T. M., Scurria, P. L., & Webster, M. G. (1998). A four-year study of anxiety,depression, loneliness, social support and perceived mistreatment in medicalstudents. Journal of Health Psychology, 3(1), 125–136.

APPENDIX

EXHIBIT 1: SURVEY INSTRUMENTThis brief survey is designed to assess the psychosocial health of all studentson campus. Please indicate the extent to which you agree or disagree with thefollowing statements (1 = strongly disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = neither agree nordisagree, 4 = agree, 5 = strongly agree)ANXIETY: 1 2 3 4 5I find working in team environments really intimidating. 1 2 3 4 5It is hard for me to initiate conversations with other students. 1 2 3 4 5I am often afraid of saying what is on my mind in the

classroom.1 2 3 4 5

POWERLESSNESS: 1 2 3 4 5Even if there is something bothering me in my classes, I do

not have any power to change the situation.1 2 3 4 5

I find it difficult to concentrate in class. 1 2 3 4 5I am often bored and sleepy in classes. 1 2 3 4 5

SOCIAL DISCONNECTEDNESS: 1 2 3 4 5I am often lonely on campus 1 2 3 4 5I see myself as a loner. 1 2 3 4 5I feel disconnected from the general campus environment. 1 2 3 4 5Even around other students that I know, I do not feel I really

belong.1 2 3 4 5

I do not feel I participate with anyone or a group on thiscampus.

1 2 3 4 5

I cannot really relate to other students on this campus. 1 2 3 4 5I always get negative feedback from other people. 1 2 3 4 5

APATHY: 1 2 3 4 5I do not really care about being a student here. 1 2 3 4 5I do not care much about what happens in College. 1 2 3 4 5Attending College is not high on my priority list. 1 2 3 4 5

Sashittal, Jassawalla, and Markulis 445

TEAMWORK-RELATED LEARNING 1 2 3 4 5I have learned a lot because I worked in teams with other

students.1 2 3 4 5

I think classroom teams have enhanced my learning as astudent.

1 2 3 4 5

I have learned to work as a team player. 1 2 3 4 5I am generally enthusiastic about team projects. 1 2 3 4 5

ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT 1 2 3 4 5The content of the courses I have taken has really challenged

me.1 2 3 4 5

My coursework has helped me develop a meaningfulphilosophy of life.

1 2 3 4 5

I have a better appreciation of the complexity of the realworld.

1 2 3 4 5

I am gaining knowledge that I will find useful in the realworld.

1 2 3 4 5

I am learning the kinds of things that I believe will be usefulfor a career.

1 2 3 4 5

My Cumulative GPA is: _____ I am a ___ Freshman ____ Sophomore ____ Junior ____SeniorMy major is: __________ My minor is: _______My gender is: ________ Male_________ FemaleMy age is (circle one): 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 or over

EXHIBIT 2: RECOMMENDATIONS FOR INSTRUCTORS ANDADMINISTRATORS

a. Prepare for the prevalence of the anxiety disorder likely toafflict more than 10% of college students, before they arrive oncampus, and campus-related exacerbating factors including:

a. Mismatch between ability, aptitude, motivation, andchosen field of study.

b. Mismatch between the instructions and guidelinesstudents receive for functioning effectively in teams,and the instructor-defined expectations of teamwork.Instructions and guidance commensuratewith expectations can go a long way in reducingstudent anxiety.

c. Mismatch between the millennial generation’sexpectations and lifestyles and the requirements foreffective learning on campus.

446 Students’ Perspective of Apathy and Social Disconnectedness

b. While empowerment of students has received much favorablepress, efforts to empower often calls students to make decisionsabout course content, schedules, testing, and evaluation forwhich they are unprepared, and exacerbate anxiety (which liesat the root of social disconnectedness, powerlessness, andapathy).

c. Incorporate learning communities in junior and seniorcurriculums, and not just limit them to the freshmen andsophomore experiences.

d. Conduct orientation programs at the beginning of junior andsenior years (which address issues of anxiety, powerlessness,social disconnectedness, and apathy).

e. Apathy and disconnectedness can infect the social and thelearning environment in classrooms and on campus. Identifystudents afflicted with dysfunctional levels of apathy anddisconnectedness for counseling and remedial action. Considerquarantines for the seriously afflicted; they can infect thequality of other students’ learning experiences.

Hemant C. Sashittal (PhD, Syracuse University) is a professor of marketing, andchair of the management/marketing department at the Bittner School of Business,St. John Fisher College. His research interests include marketing implementation,product innovation, and pedagogy.

Avan R. Jassawalla (PhD, Syracuse University) is professor of management atthe School of Business, State University of New York at Geneseo, New York. Herresearch interests include product innovation, teamwork, and pedagogy.

Peter Markulis (PhD, University of Buffalo) is a professor of management at theSchool of Business, State University of New York at Geneseo, New York. Hisresearch interests lie in pedagogy.