Spencer Oatts, Ben Gibson, Carl White, Yongli Zou, & Chris Dula Department of Psychology East...

16
Spencer Oatts, Ben Gibson, Carl White, Yongli Zou, & Chris Dula Department of Psychology East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN

Transcript of Spencer Oatts, Ben Gibson, Carl White, Yongli Zou, & Chris Dula Department of Psychology East...

Page 1: Spencer Oatts, Ben Gibson, Carl White, Yongli Zou, & Chris Dula Department of Psychology East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN.

Spencer Oatts, Ben Gibson, Carl White, Yongli Zou, & Chris Dula

Department of Psychology

East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN

Page 2: Spencer Oatts, Ben Gibson, Carl White, Yongli Zou, & Chris Dula Department of Psychology East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN.

Objective The purpose of the current study was to

assess the frequency of safety belt use among students at a southeastern university.

A secondary assessment was conducted to determine the efficacy of video data collection and field data collection.

Page 3: Spencer Oatts, Ben Gibson, Carl White, Yongli Zou, & Chris Dula Department of Psychology East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN.

Introduction A NHTSA study found a 40–50% decrease in risk

of fatal injury with properly used safety belts (NHTSA, 1984, 1996).

The Tennessee “Click It or Ticket” program was found to increase safety belt use by 20%. (Tennessee Department of Transportation, 2001).

Farrell, Cox, & Geller (2007) found that unbuckled male drivers were significantly more likely to buckle up when prompted by a female.

Page 4: Spencer Oatts, Ben Gibson, Carl White, Yongli Zou, & Chris Dula Department of Psychology East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN.

Introduction Research from Rosenblum et al. (2004) has

shown evidence to support the notion that video coding is a viable alternative to field observation.

We hypothesized that there would be a significant difference in reported safety belt use between video coders and field observers.

Page 5: Spencer Oatts, Ben Gibson, Carl White, Yongli Zou, & Chris Dula Department of Psychology East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN.

Participants Drivers (14 women, 22 men, 2 undetermined, age

range 16-65 years) were observed naturalistically at an intersection located at a southeastern university.

Thirty-eight vehicles were recorded.

Researchers recorded color of vehicle, type of vehicle, safety belt use, cell phone use, sex of driver, race of driver, number of passengers, and age range of the driver.

Page 6: Spencer Oatts, Ben Gibson, Carl White, Yongli Zou, & Chris Dula Department of Psychology East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN.

Procedure Two trained field researchers were equipped with a

high definition camera to record video data of the vehicles at an intersection.

Data of the vehicles were also recorded manually via field data collection sheets.

The recorded data on the disc in the high definition camera was later transferred onto a computer.

Page 7: Spencer Oatts, Ben Gibson, Carl White, Yongli Zou, & Chris Dula Department of Psychology East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN.

Procedure Two trained researchers watched the video and

recorded any observations onto similar video data collection sheets.

A verbal cue was used to notify the video coders of the specific targeted vehicle.

Once all four researchers collected their data, the data collection sheets were compared using a Pearson’s correlation.

Page 8: Spencer Oatts, Ben Gibson, Carl White, Yongli Zou, & Chris Dula Department of Psychology East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN.

Results Field Observer 1: Reported 55% of

drivers were wearing a safety belt. Field Observer 2: Reported 79% of

drivers were wearing a safety belt. Video Observer 1: Reported 88% of

drivers were wearing a safety belt. Video Observer 2: Reported 92% of

drivers were wearing a safety belt.

Page 9: Spencer Oatts, Ben Gibson, Carl White, Yongli Zou, & Chris Dula Department of Psychology East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN.

Results The average rates of safety belt use

reported was 79%. Correlation of safety belt use between

Field Observers 1 & 2 r = 0.22 Correlation of safety belt use between

Video Observers 1 & 2 r = 0.80

Page 10: Spencer Oatts, Ben Gibson, Carl White, Yongli Zou, & Chris Dula Department of Psychology East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN.

Discussion We can infer that the difference in the report of

safety belt rates for Field Observers are significant enough to question the methodology involved.

Field Observer 1 controlled the camera, thus causing less time to record the necessary vehicle demographics. Control of camera could influence the data for the Video Observers.

Page 11: Spencer Oatts, Ben Gibson, Carl White, Yongli Zou, & Chris Dula Department of Psychology East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN.

Discussion In future research for the study, there will be a

third observer who will strictly control only the camera and not record vehicle data.

While we had higher agreement rates for Video Observers, we also must question the methodology involved.

Video Observers had the luxury of rewinding, zooming in, and pausing the video if needed to record the correct vehicle demographics.

Page 12: Spencer Oatts, Ben Gibson, Carl White, Yongli Zou, & Chris Dula Department of Psychology East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN.

Discussion This luxury could also have caused the result of

more compliance between Video Observers.

Subtle cues from body language could have caused subjectivity between Video Observers.

In future research for this study, we will require Video Observers to record the vehicle demographics separately to prevent any kind of subjectivity between observers.

Page 13: Spencer Oatts, Ben Gibson, Carl White, Yongli Zou, & Chris Dula Department of Psychology East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN.

Discussion From our results, we cannot infer that one method

of data collection is superior, however, we can revise our methodology and determine more reliable methods for collecting the vehicle demographics.

Once a consistent method is implemented, our results can be more reliable and our conclusion can be well supported.

Page 14: Spencer Oatts, Ben Gibson, Carl White, Yongli Zou, & Chris Dula Department of Psychology East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN.

Limitations All of our data came from the same local region.

Different perceptions of vehicle demographics could lead to inconsistency between researchers.

Tinted windows and unfavorable weather conditions limited visibility of the interior for some vehicles.

Page 15: Spencer Oatts, Ben Gibson, Carl White, Yongli Zou, & Chris Dula Department of Psychology East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN.

Future Implications The results from this study could be used to help

develop programs to increase safety belt use.

Future research should be conducted in a variety of locales.

If we can determine which methodology is superior, we can set a standard data collection protocol for the driving research field.

Page 16: Spencer Oatts, Ben Gibson, Carl White, Yongli Zou, & Chris Dula Department of Psychology East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN.

References Farrell, L.V., Cox, M.G., & Geller, S. G. (2007). Prompting safety belt use in

the context of a belt-use law: The flash-for life revisited. Journal of Safety Research, 38, 407-411.

National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, 1984. Final Regulatory Impact Analysis: Amendment to Federal Motor Vehicle Safety Standard 208 Passenger Car Front Seat Occupant Protection.

National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, 1996a. Presidential Initiative for Increasing Seat Belt Use Nationwide

Rosenblum, K. L., Zeanah, C., McDonough, S., & Muzik, M. (2004). Video-taped coding of working model of the child interviews: a viable

and useful alternative to verbatim transcripts? Infant Behavior &Development, 27, 544-549.