Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the ... & Denney... · paradigm (e.g., that...

13
Copyright 2007 Psychonomic Society, Inc. 606 We constantly shift attention from one place to another, and from one object to another, as we look about and in- teract with our environment. This seemingly effortless behavior involves complex contributions from and inter- actions between the perception, attention, and eye move- ment systems. Of particular interest here are contributions to this behavior from attention systems that are thought to operate independently from location- and object-based frames of reference (see, e.g., Leek, Reppa, & Tipper, 2003; Reppa & Leek, 2003; Serences, Liu, &Yantis, 2005; Tipper, Weaver, Jerreat, & Burak, 1994). Whether shifts of attention are location based or object based, they could be described as involving three logical steps or processes associated with visual selective attention: engaging, dis- engaging, and shifting (Posner, 1980). 1 To shift attention from one location or object to another, attention needs to disengage or reorient from the location or object where it is currently deployed (but seeVecera & Flevaris, 2005, and our General Discussion). The present study explores engaging, disengaging, and shifting attention within the location- and object-based systems. A common way to measure contributions from the location- and object-based systems is to use a visual cuing paradigm (e.g., that of Egly, Driver, & Rafal, 1994), in which attention is first drawn to a location or object by flashing a cue, and the reaction time (RT) is then measured for a shift to a subsequently presented target. RTs are faster when the cue and target appear at the same location (valid cue) than when they appear at different locations (invalid cue). The increase in RT with invalid cues is an indicator of the processing time required to disengage and shift at- tention from the cue to the target. The location- and object- based contributions are assessed by comparing invalid cue conditions in which a shift of attention is required either within an object or between objects. A consistent finding in the literature (and in the present study) is that RTs are faster for within-object shifts than for shifts of an equal distance between objects (seeAvrahami, 1999; Egly et al., 1994; Egly, Rafal, Driver, & Starrveveld, 1994; Lavie & Driver, 1996; Law & Abrams, 2002). Why and how the advantage for within-object shifts (or the disadvantage for between-object shifts) occurs has been a topic of consider- able research and d ebate (Avrahami, 1999; Davis, Driver, Pavani, & Shepherd, 2000; Goldsmith & Yeari, 2003; Lamy & Egeth, 2002; Shomstein & Yantis, 2002, 2004; see the General Discussion below for a detailed discussion of alternative theoretical conceptualizations in the con- text of the present study). Lamy and Egeth’s (2002) study using detection, discrimination, and flanker interference tasks illustrated that shifting attention may be a boundary condition for producing object-based effects. We explore this boundary condition further by asking what it is about shifting attention that produces object-based effects. From a location-based perspective, within-object and between-object shifts involve disengaging from one loca- tion, shifting, and engaging on a new location. The only difference is that in some cases these actions are executed within an object and in others between objects. From an object-based perspective, within-object shifts involve dis- engaging attention from one place on an object, shifting, Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the processes underlying the object advantage JAMES M. BROWN AND HOPE I. DENNEY University of Georgia, Athens, Georgia Visual cuing studies have been widely used to demonstrate and explore contributions from both object- and location-based attention systems. A common finding has been a response advantage for shifts of attention oc- curring within an object, relative to shifts of an equal distance between objects. The present study examined this advantage for within-object shifts in terms of engage and disengage operations within the object- and location- based attention systems. The rationale was that shifts of attention between objects require object-based attention to disengage from one object before shifting to another, something that is not required for shifts of attention within an object or away from a location. One- and two-object displays were used to assess object-based con- tributions related to disengaging and engaging attention within, between, into, and out of objects. The results suggest that the “object advantage” commonly found in visual cuing experiments in which shifts of attention are required is primarily due to disengage operations associated with object-based attention. Perception & Psychophysics 2007, 69 (4), 606-618 J. M. Brown, [email protected]

Transcript of Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the ... & Denney... · paradigm (e.g., that...

Page 1: Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the ... & Denney... · paradigm (e.g., that of Egly, Driver, & Rafal, 1994), in which attention is first drawn to a location

Copyright 2007 Psychonomic Society, Inc. 606

Weconstantlyshiftattentionfromoneplacetoanother,andfromoneobjecttoanother,aswelookaboutandin-teractwithourenvironment.Thisseeminglyeffortlessbehaviorinvolvescomplexcontributionsfromandinter-actionsbetweentheperception,attention,andeyemove-mentsystems.Ofparticularinterestherearecontributionstothisbehaviorfromattentionsystemsthatarethoughttooperateindependentlyfromlocation-andobject-basedframesofreference(see,e.g.,Leek,Reppa,&Tipper,2003;Reppa&Leek,2003;Serences,Liu,&Yantis,2005;Tipper,Weaver,Jerreat,&Burak,1994).Whethershiftsofattentionarelocationbasedorobjectbased,theycouldbedescribedasinvolvingthreelogicalstepsorprocessesassociatedwithvisualselectiveattention:engaging,dis-engaging,andshifting(Posner,1980).1Toshiftattentionfromonelocationorobjecttoanother,attentionneedstodisengageorreorientfromthelocationorobjectwhereitiscurrentlydeployed(butseeVecera&Flevaris,2005,andourGeneralDiscussion).Thepresentstudyexploresengaging,disengaging,andshiftingattentionwithinthelocation-andobject-basedsystems.

A common way to measure contributions from thelocation-andobject-basedsystemsistouseavisualcuingparadigm(e.g.,thatofEgly,Driver,&Rafal,1994),inwhichattentionisfirstdrawntoalocationorobjectbyflashingacue,andthereactiontime(RT)isthenmeasuredforashifttoasubsequentlypresentedtarget.RTsarefasterwhenthecueandtargetappearatthesamelocation(validcue)thanwhentheyappearatdifferentlocations(invalidcue).TheincreaseinRTwithinvalidcuesisanindicator

oftheprocessingtimerequiredtodisengageandshiftat-tentionfromthecuetothetarget.Thelocation-andobject-basedcontributionsareassessedbycomparinginvalidcueconditionsinwhichashiftofattentionisrequiredeitherwithinanobjectorbetweenobjects.Aconsistentfindingintheliterature(andinthepresentstudy)isthatRTsarefasterforwithin-objectshiftsthanforshiftsofanequaldistancebetweenobjects(seeAvrahami,1999;Eglyetal.,1994;Egly,Rafal,Driver,&Starrveveld,1994;Lavie&Driver,1996;Law&Abrams,2002).Whyandhowtheadvantageforwithin-objectshifts(orthedisadvantageforbetween-objectshifts)occurshasbeenatopicofconsider-ableresearchanddebate(Avrahami,1999;Davis,Driver,Pavani,&Shepherd,2000;Goldsmith&Yeari,2003;Lamy&Egeth,2002;Shomstein&Yantis,2002,2004;seetheGeneralDiscussionbelowforadetaileddiscussionofalternativetheoreticalconceptualizationsinthecon-textofthepresentstudy).LamyandEgeth’s(2002)studyusingdetection,discrimination,andflankerinterferencetasksillustratedthatshiftingattentionmaybeaboundaryconditionforproducingobject-basedeffects.Weexplorethisboundaryconditionfurtherbyaskingwhatitisaboutshiftingattentionthatproducesobject-basedeffects.

Fromalocation-basedperspective,within-objectandbetween-objectshiftsinvolvedisengagingfromoneloca-tion,shifting,andengagingonanewlocation.Theonlydifferenceisthatinsomecasestheseactionsareexecutedwithinanobjectandinothersbetweenobjects.Fromanobject-basedperspective,within-objectshiftsinvolvedis-engagingattentionfromoneplaceonanobject,shifting,

Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the processes underlying the

object advantage

James m. Brown and Hope I. denneyUniversity of Georgia, Athens, Georgia

Visualcuingstudieshavebeenwidelyusedtodemonstrateandexplorecontributionsfrombothobject-andlocation-basedattentionsystems.Acommonfindinghasbeenaresponseadvantageforshiftsofattentionoc-curringwithinanobject,relativetoshiftsofanequaldistancebetweenobjects.Thepresentstudyexaminedthisadvantageforwithin-objectshiftsintermsofengageanddisengageoperationswithintheobject-andlocation-basedattentionsystems.Therationalewasthatshiftsofattentionbetweenobjectsrequireobject-basedattentiontodisengagefromoneobjectbeforeshiftingtoanother,somethingthatisnotrequiredforshiftsofattentionwithinanobjectorawayfromalocation.One-andtwo-objectdisplayswereusedtoassessobject-basedcon-tributionsrelatedtodisengagingandengagingattentionwithin,between,into,andoutofobjects.Theresultssuggestthatthe“objectadvantage”commonlyfoundinvisualcuingexperimentsinwhichshiftsofattentionarerequiredisprimarilyduetodisengageoperationsassociatedwithobject-basedattention.

Perception & Psychophysics2007, 69 (4), 606-618

J. M. Brown, [email protected]

Page 2: Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the ... & Denney... · paradigm (e.g., that of Egly, Driver, & Rafal, 1994), in which attention is first drawn to a location

Shifting Attention 607

andthenengagingitonanotherplacewithinthesameobject.Atopicofcriticalimportancetothepresentstudy,however,iswhatoccursduringbetween-objectshifts.Ifengaginganddisengagingattentionfromalocationandfromanobjectinvolvedifferentorseparateprocesses,thenengaginganddisengagingattentionfromanobjectismorethansimplyengaginganddisengagingattentionfromalocation.Fromthisperspective,between-objectshiftswouldinvolveseparateengage,disengage,andshiftoperationsforboththelocation-basedandobject-basedsystems.Thus,theobjectadvantagefoundincuingstudiesmayresultfromadditionalprocessesoftheobject-basedsystemthatareinvolvedindisengagingfromtheinvalidlycuedobject,engagingontheobjectthetargetappearsin,orboth.InadditiontoLamyandEgeth’s(2002)findings,twootherstudieshavespecificallysuggestedthisidea.Vecera(1994)heldthewithin-objectdistanceconstantwhiletestingabetween-objectdistancethatwasshorterthanthewithin-objectdistance.RTsforbetween-objectshiftswereshorterwhenthebetween-objectdistancewaslessthanthewithin-objectdistance,buttheywerestillnotasfastasRTsforwithin-objectshifts.Conversely,Brown,Breitmeyer,Leighty,andDenney(2006)heldthebetween-objectdistanceconstantwhiletestingawithin-objectdistancethatwasgreaterthanthebetween-objectdistance.TheyfoundthatRTsforwithin-objectshiftsin-creasedwhenthewithin-objectdistancewasmorethanthebetween-objectdistance,butwerestillshorterthanRTsforbetween-objectshifts.Inbothstudies,theobjectadvantagepersisted,eventhoughitwasreducedinmag-nituderelativetocontrolconditionsinwhichthewithin-andbetween-objectdistanceswereequal.Together,theseresultssuggestedtousthatsomethingspecifictoshiftsofattentionbetweenobjectsmakesthemslowerthanshiftswithinobjects.Acommonthreadlinkingthesestudiesisthatresponseswereslowerwhenattentionhadtodisen-gagefromanobjectbeforeshiftingthanwhenitdidnot,irrespectiveofdistance.

Visualcuingstudiesoftencomparethecostsforinvalidcuesthatrequireshiftsofattentionwithinanobjectwiththecostsforshiftsbetweenobjects.Weareunawareofanycuingstudiesdirectlycomparingshiftsfromanobjecttoalocation,fromalocationtoanobject,andfromaloca-tiontoanotherlocation(butseeLeek,Reppa,&Tipper’s[2003]studyoninhibitionofreturn).Ourgoalwastodeterminewhethertheobjectadvantagefoundincuingstudiesisdrivenbyattention’shavingtodisengagefromanobjectand/orhavingtoengageonanewobjectduringbetween-objectshiftsbycomparingconditionsinwhichattentionshiftswithinobjects,betweenobjects,“outof ”objects(object-to-location),“into”objects(location-to-object),andbetweentwolocations(location-to-location).

ExpEriMEnt 1

Inordertotestforseparateprocessesofengaginganddisengagingattentionfromalocationandfromanobject,weusedone-andtwo-objectstimuli(Figures1A–1H).The two-object condition was important because weneededtoestablishthatatypicalobjectadvantagecould

befoundunderthepresentexperimentalconditions,andalsobecauseitprovidedaperformancebaselineagainstwhichspecificone-objectconditions(describedindetailbelow)couldbecompared,asameansofseparatingoutthecontributionsoftheengageanddisengageoperationsofthelocation-andobject-basedsystems.Thetwo-objectstimuliwereusedtoshowabasicattention-cuingeffect,inwhichvalidRTsareexpectedtobefasterthaninvalidRTs,andtoestablishanobjectadvantage,inwhichin-validwithin-objectRTs(foranexample,seeFigure1A)areexpectedtobefasterthaninvalidbetween-objectRTs(Figure1B).Tohelpdistinguishbetweentheone-objectandtwo-objectconditions,wewillrefertothetwo-objectvalidconditionastwo–validandthetwo-objectinvalidconditionsastwo–withinandbetween-object.Thespe-cificone-objectconditions,withtheirpurposesandpre-dictedoutcomes,arelistednext.

One-Object Valid ConditionsValid-in.Whenthecueandtargetappearedinthesame

placeatoneendofthesingleobjectpresentedinatrial,itwascalledavalid-intrialbecausebothcueandtargetappearedinanobject(Figure1C).Thepurposeofthisconditionwastwofold.First,thisconditionwasneces-saryinordertoshowabasicattention-cuingeffectbycomparingRTsinthisconditionwiththoseinone-objectinvalidconditions.Second,itwasimportanttoshowthatRTsonone-objectvalidtrialswerenodifferentfromthoseontwo-objectvalidtrials,sothatwecouldmakedirectcomparisonsoftheone-andtwo-objectconditions.Sincethesequencesofeventswereidenticalinallvalidtrials,ifone-objectvalid-inRTswereslowerorfasterthantwo-objectvalidRTs,itwouldsuggestthatperformancewasinfluencedbythenumberofobjectsinthedisplay.

Valid-out.Whenthecueandtargetappearedinthesameplaceontheoppositesideofthescreenfromthesingleobjectpresentedinatrial,itwascalledavalid-outtrialbecausebothcueandtargetappearedoutsideofanyobject.Thecueandtargetpositionswerethesameaswhentheyappearedinanobject,justonthesideofthescreenwherenoobjectwaspresent(Figure1F).ThemainpurposeofthisconditionwastoensurethatanyincreasesinRTsfoundforone-objectinvalidconditionsinwhichtargetsappearedoutsideofanobjectcouldnotsimplybeexplainedbythatfact.RTsinthisconditionwereexpectedtobethesameas,orpossiblyfasterthan,valid-inandtwo–validRTs.Therationaleforwhytheymightbefasterwasthat,fromasignal-to-noiseperspective,thecueandtargetmightbesomewhateasiertodetectwithoutobjectcontoursnearby.

One-Object invalid ConditionsWithin-object.Whenthecueandtargetappearedat

oppositeendsofthesingleobject, itwasaone-object within-objecttrial(Figure1D;thisconditionwillhereafterbereferredtoastheone–withincondition).Thesequenceofeventswouldbeidenticaltothetwo–withincondition,butwithonlyoneobjectpresent.RTswereexpectedtobesimilartotwo–withinRTsbecausetheengageanddisen-gageoperationswouldbeidentical.

Page 3: Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the ... & Denney... · paradigm (e.g., that of Egly, Driver, & Rafal, 1994), in which attention is first drawn to a location

608 Brown And denney

Location-to-location.Whenthecueandtargetap-pearedonthesideoppositethesingleobjectpresentedinatrial,thiswasalocation-to-locationtrial(Figure1H;here-after,locloc).Thecueandtargetpositionswerethesameasinone–withintrials,excepttheyappearedonthesideofthescreenoppositewheretheobjectappeared.Thiscon-ditionprovidedasituationinwhichobject-basedengageanddisengageoperationswouldnotbeinvolved,becausethecueandtargetappearedoutsideofanyobjects.RTswereexpectedtobesimilartoone–withinandtwo–withinRTsbecause,asinthoseconditions,object-basedattentionwouldnothavetodisengagefromanobjectinordertoshift,norwoulditengageonanotherobjectaftershifting.

Location-to-object.Whenthecueappearedonthesideoppositethesingleobjectpresentedinatrialandthe

targetappearedintheendoftheobjectclosesttothecue,thiswasalocation-to-objecttrial(Figure1G;hereafter,locobj).Thisconditionwouldnotrequireattentiontodisengagefromanobjectbeforeshifting,justasinbothwithin-objectconditionsandthelocloccondition,butunlikethoseconditions,itwouldentailengagingonanobjectaftershifting.Thus,ifRTsinthisconditionweregreater than those inbothof thewithin-objectcondi-tionsandthelocloccondition,thiscouldbetakenasevidencethatanobject-basedengageoperationoccurredaftershifting.

Object-to-location.Whenthecueappearedinoneendofthesingleobjectpresentedinatrialandthetargetap-pearedintheclosestcomparablepositionontheemptysideof thedisplay, thiswasanobject-to-location trial

Figure 1. An illustration of the different conditions of our experiments. (A and B) two-object invalid conditions. (C–H) the various one-object cue–target conditions with objects presented in the vertical orientation. (i and J) two one-object invalid conditions with the object tilted to the right.

Cue

Target Condition

Two–Within(A)

Between-Object(B)

Cue

Target Condition

Valid-In(C)

One–Within(D)

Object-to-Location(E)

Cue

Target Condition

One–Within(I)

Object-to-Location(J)

Cue

Target Condition

Valid-Out(F)

Location-to-Object(G)

Location-to-Location(H)

Page 4: Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the ... & Denney... · paradigm (e.g., that of Egly, Driver, & Rafal, 1994), in which attention is first drawn to a location

Shifting Attention        609

(Figure 1E; hereafter, objloc). This condition would require attention to disengage from an object before shift-ing, making it most similar to the between-object con-dition. It is different from that condition, though, in not requiring that attention engage on another object after shifting. Thus, if RTs were greater in this condition than in both within-object conditions and the locloc condi-tion, it would be evidence of an object-based disengage operation. In addition, if RTs were similar to those in the between-object condition, it would provide evidence that the typical object advantage is driven by the necessity of disengaging object-based attention from the cued object before shifting.

Comparisons of RTs in this condition and in the locobj condition would also provide information on the relative influences of object-based engage and disengage opera-tions. If RTs were greater for objloc than for locobj shifts, this would indicate that disengaging has a greater influence than engaging object-based attention. Likewise, RTs in the opposite direction would indicate greater in-fluence for engaging than for disengaging object-based attention. No difference in RTs would indicate that both operations have similar influence.

MethodParticipants

A total of 30 University of Georgia undergraduates (15 male, 15 female) participated for introductory psychology course credit. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, were classi-fied as right-handed according to the Annett Handedness Scale, and reported no history of attention deficit disorder.

Stimuli and ApparatusStimulus presentation and data collection were carried out using 

commercially available software (SuperLab Pro) running on a PC with a VGA monitor. Responses were collected from a response box that interfaced with the computer. The participants sat in a darkened room 70 cm from the monitor and used a chinrest.

All stimuli were white on a black background. The fixation cross was 0.76º 3 0.76º in size. Objects (whether one or two) were cen-tered 2.39º from fixation and subtended 5.33º 3 0.57º. The lines constructing the objects subtended 0.19º. Targets consisted of a solid square (0.57º on each side) that either filled one end of an object or, when it appeared outside the object in one-object conditions, oc-cupied the same location on the opposite side of the screen. It was the same shade of white as the lines making up the objects. Cues consisted of a slightly whiter line, 0.57º long and 0.19º thick, which 

again appeared either at the end of an object or in the same location on the side opposite a single object.

ProcedureAll one- and two-object conditions were randomly intermixed 

within each block during the experiment. Following a brief intro-duction in which the various trial conditions were shown once, par-ticipants completed 9 practice trials before starting the experiment. Trials were presented in six blocks of 120 trials with a short break between blocks, for a total of 720 trials.

Each block of 120 trials consisted of 72 (60%) valid trials, in which the cue and target appeared in the same location; 24 (20%) invalid trials, in which the cue and target did not appear in the same location; and 24 (20%) catch trials, in which no target appeared. The 72 valid trials consisted of 24 two-object trials and 24 trials each in which a single object appeared on the left or on the right of the screen. The 24 invalid trials consisted of 4 trials from each of the six invalid conditions: two–within, between-object, one–within, locloc, locobj, and objloc.

An example of the sequence of events is depicted in Figure 2 for a two-object valid trial. Each trial began with a white fixation cross at the center of the screen. After a participant initiated a trial with a keypress, either one or two objects appeared with the fixation cross for 1,000 msec, followed by a cue that appeared for 50 msec. After 150 msec, the target appeared until the participant responded or until 1,500 msec had elapsed. On catch trials, no target appeared. After either a response or 1,500 msec, there was a 500-msec blank inter-val before the fixation stimulus returned, signaling the next trial. RTs were measured from the onset of the target until a response was made. Participants received a warning screen for RTs less than 150 msec or false alarms on catch trials.

Results and Discussion

RTs less than 150 msec and greater than 1,000 msec (2%) were  trimmed from all analyses. The mean false alarm rate on catch trials was 4%. Mean RTs were calcu-lated for each participant and each condition for the sub-sequent analyses (see Figure 3). In the discussion below, RTs in parentheses indicate mean RT 6 SE.

We first ran tests for the basic cuing and object ad-vantage effects, separately for the two- and one-object conditions. A one-way ANOVA on two-object valid and invalid RTs  indicated a  significant 29-msec cuing ef-fect [F(1,29) 5 20.06, p , .001], with valid RTs (326 6 10 msec) faster than invalid RTs (355 6 13 msec). A one-way ANOVA on two–within and between-object RTs indi-cated a significant 13-msec object advantage [F(1,29) 5 8.47, p , .007] due to two–within RTs (348 6 12 msec) 

Fixation1,000 msec

Cue50 msec

ISI150 msec

Time

UntilResponse

or 1,500 msec

Figure 2. An example of a vertical two-object valid trial, illustrating the sequence of events.

Page 5: Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the ... & Denney... · paradigm (e.g., that of Egly, Driver, & Rafal, 1994), in which attention is first drawn to a location

610 Brown And denney

beingfasterthanbetween-objectRTs(361614msec).Totestforacuingeffectwiththeone-objectstimuli,weranaone-wayANOVAonone-objectvalidversusinvalidRTs.Todothis,wecollapsedacrossbothone-objectvalidcon-ditions(-inand-out)andthefourone-objectinvalidcon-ditions(one–within,locloc,locobj,andobjloc).Anoverall25-mseccuingeffectwasevidentinwhichtheone-objectvalidRTs(330610msec)weresignificantlyfasterthantheinvalidRTs(355613msec)[F(1,29)521.51,p,.001].

Aone-wayANOVAonRTsforthethreevalidcondi-tionsallowedustotestwhethervalidRTswereinfluencedbythepresenceofanobject(e.g.,valid-invs.valid-out)orthepresenceofasecondobject(e.g.,valid-invs.two–valid).Posthoccomparisonsindicatedthatthesignifi-canteffectofcondition[F(2,58)515.42,p,.001]wasduetothetwo–valid(326610msec)andvalid-in(322610msec)RTsbeingshorterthanthevalid-outRTs(338610msec).Allposthoc tests throughout the studyusedNewman–Keulstestsandasignificancelevelofp,.05.Thevalid-inandtwo–validRTmeanswereequivalent,in-dicatingthatthepresenceorabsenceofasecondobjectdidnotinfluencevalidresponsestotargetsappearinginanob-ject.Findinglongervalid-outRTswasunexpected.Wehadpredictedsimilar(orpossiblyfaster)RTsforthiscondition.Wewilldiscussthevalid-inandvalid-outconditionsfurtherfollowingExperiment2andintheGeneralDiscussion.

Wehadpredictedthatthethreeinvalidconditionsinwhichobject-basedattentionneitherhad todisengagefrom an object to shift nor engage on another objectaftershiftingwouldbesimilar,andthispredictionwasconfirmedthroughaone-wayANOVA:Theone–within(3416 12msec), two–within (3496 12msec), andlocloc(347613msec)RTswerenotdifferentfromeachother[F(2,58)50.99,p..38].

Aone-wayANOVAcomparingRTsinallone-objectinvalidconditions(one–within,locobj,objloc,andlocloc)wassignificant[F(3,87)515.05,p,.001].Theconditionofprimaryinterestwastheobjloccondi-tion,becauseonlyinthisconditionwouldattentionhavetodisengagefromanobjectbeforeshifting.Aspredicted,objlocRTs(376614msec)weresignificantlylongerthanthoseintheone–within(341612msec),locloc(347613msec),andlocobj(354614msec)condi-tions,withnodifferencesbetweenthelatterthree.Theseresultssuggestthatdisengagingobject-basedattentionfromanobjectbeforeshiftingplaysaprimaryroleintheobjectadvantageandthatengagingonanobjectafterashiftofattentiondoesnot.Ifengagingonanobjectafterashiftdidplayanimportantrole,locobjRTsshouldhavebeensignificantlylongerthanloclocRTs.

Ourfindingthatvalid-outRTswerelongerthanvalid-inRTsmightsuggestthatwetakecautionininterpretingtheresultsoftheobjlocconditionassupportfortheimportanceofdisengagingobject-basedattentionfromanobjectbeforeashift,becausethetargetappearedoutsideofanobjectinthevalid-outcondition,too.However,ifthelongervalid-outRTsindicatedgenerallyslowedre-sponsestotargetsthatappearbythemselvesratherthaninanobject,thisslowinginfluenceshouldhavebeenevidentforallinvalidconditionsinwhichthetargetappearedbyitself.Forexample,inthelocloccondition,inwhichthetargetappearedbyitself,RTsshouldhavebeenlongerthanthoseinone–withintrials,inwhichthetargetap-pearedwithinanobject;thiswasnotthecase,however.Thesecomparisonssuggestthat,whateverthereasonthatvalid-outRTswerelongerthanvalid-inRTs,thatfactorlikelydidnotcontributetothedifferencesinRTsbetweenthevariousone-objectinvalidconditions(seetheResultsbelowforExperiments2and3).

Figure 3. reaction times for all vertically oriented two- and one-object valid and invalid conditions (Experiment 1).

Vertical Condition

Reaction Time (msec)

300 320

2-Obj Valid

2-Obj Invalid

2-Obj Invalid Within

2-Obj Invalid Between

1-Obj Valid-In

1-Obj Valid-Out

1-Obj Invalid Within

1-Obj Invalid Obj�Loc

1-Obj Invalid Loc�Loc

1-Obj Invalid Loc�Obj

340 360 380 400

Page 6: Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the ... & Denney... · paradigm (e.g., that of Egly, Driver, & Rafal, 1994), in which attention is first drawn to a location

Shifting Attention 611

Althoughtheseresultswereencouraging,weshouldmakeoneobservationaboutourstimuliandprocedure.Wechosetouseonlyverticallyorientedstimuliforourtestbecauseofthenumberofone-andtwo-objectcondi-tionswewantedtotestinawithin-subjectsdesign.Usingsuchstimuliallowedustokeeptheexperimentatarea-sonablelengthforparticipantstofinishinasinglevisit.Anotherfactthatcontributedtothisdecisionwasthat,inprevioustwo-objectexperimentsinwhichwehadusedbothverticalandhorizontalstimuli(Brownetal.,2006),wehadneverfoundanyeffectofobjectorientationontheobjectadvantage.Althoughthesewerelegitimaterea-sonsforusingonlyverticalstimuli,thebetween-objectandobjlocconditionsthatproducedthelongestRTswerealsotwoofthethreeconditionsinwhichattentionhadtoshiftfromonevisualfield/cerebralhemispheretoanother(e.g.,fromtherightvisualfield/lefthemispheretotheleftvisualfield/righthemisphere,orviceversa).Allwithin-objectshifts(withoneobjectorwithtwo)andloclocshiftstookplacewithinavisualfield.Itwaspos-sible,then,thatthisfactorcouldhavehadsomeinfluenceonthepatternofresults.Oursecondexperimentwasde-signedtoruleoutthispossibility.

ExpEriMEnt 2

Totestwhetherthepatternofone-andtwo-objectre-sultsinExperiment1wasinsomewayinfluencedbyat-tentionhavingtoshiftfromonevisualfieldtoanother,thestimuliweusedinExperiment2wererotated45ºeitherclockwise(forexamples,seeFigures1Iand1J)orcoun-terclockwise,withorientationabetween-subjectsfactor.Inthisexperiment,shiftsforallone-andtwo-objectvalidandinvalidconditionsalwaysoccurredwithinavisualfieldandneverinvolvedcrossingtheverticalmeridian.Ifthetrendsofresultschangedremarkablyinthesecondi-

tions,thatfindingwouldsupportthevisualfieldhypoth-esisandgoagainstourhypothesesconcerningengaginganddisengagingattention.Ifthetrendsofresultsremainedthesame,thiswouldreplicateExperiment1andconfirmtheinvolvementwehypothesizedofobject-basedengageanddisengageoperations.

Methodparticipants

Atotalof60UniversityofGeorgiaundergraduates(30male,30female)participatedforintroductorypsychologycoursecredit.Allparticipantshadnormalorcorrected-to-normalvision,wereclassi-fiedasright-handedaccordingtotheAnnettHandednessScale,andreportednohistoryofattentiondeficitdisorder.

Stimuli and ApparatusThedetailsofthestimuliandapparatusremainedthesameas

inExperiment1,withtheexceptionthatallstimuliwerepresentedtilted45ºeitherclockwiseorcounterclockwisefromvertical.

procedureAllproceduraldetailswereidenticaltothoseinExperiment1,

exceptthatonegroupof30participants(15male,15female)werepresentedstimulitiltedtotheright,andanothergroupof30(also15male,15female)werepresentedstimulitiltedtotheleft.

results and Discussion

RTslessthan150msecandgreaterthan1,000msec(2%)were trimmedfromallanalyses.Themeanfalsealarmrateoncatchtrialswasonceagain4%.MeanRTswereagaincalculatedforeachconditionforthesubse-quentanalyses(seeFigure4).

InitialANOVAsonthevariouscomparisonsmadeinExperiment1,usingbarorientationasabetween-subjectsfactor,revealednomaineffectsoforientationorinterac-tionsoforientationwithotherfactors,sotheresultsofthefollowinganalysesarecollapsedacrossorientations.Aone-wayANOVAontwo-objectvalidandinvalidRTs

Figure 4. reaction times, collapsed across left- and right-tilted orientations, for all two- and one-object valid and invalid conditions (Experiment 2).

Combined Tilted Conditions

Reaction Time (msec)

300 320 340 360 380 400

2-Obj Valid

2-Obj Invalid

2-Obj Invalid Within

2-Obj Invalid Between

1-Obj Valid-In

1-Obj Valid-Out

1-Obj Invalid Within

1-Obj Invalid Obj�Loc

1-Obj Invalid Loc�Loc

1-Obj Invalid Loc�Obj

Page 7: Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the ... & Denney... · paradigm (e.g., that of Egly, Driver, & Rafal, 1994), in which attention is first drawn to a location

612 Brown And denney

indicatedasignificant22-mseccuingeffect[F(1,59)572.45,p,.001],withvalidRTs(32368msec)fasterthaninvalidRTs(35569msec).Aone-wayANOVAontwo–withinandbetween-objectRTsindicatedasignifi-cant28-msecobjectadvantage[F(1,59)575.52,p,.001]due towithin-objectRTs (3416 9msec)beingfasterthanbetween-objectRTs(369610msec).Totestforacuingeffectwiththeone-objectstimuli,weranaone-wayANOVAonone-objectvalidversusinvalidRTs,asinExperiment1,andfoundoverallasignificant26-mseccuingeffect,withtheone-objectvalidRTs(32667msec) faster than the invalid RTs (3526 9msec)[F(1,59)593.81,p,.001].AcomparisonoftheresultsfromExperiment1,inwhichhalfoftheinvalidcondi-tionsinvolvedcrossingtheverticalmeridian,withthosehereindicatesthatcrossingtheverticalmeridianwasnotasignificantfactorinfluencingtheearlierresults.

Aone-wayANOVAcomparingRTsforthevalid-in,valid-out,andtwo–validconditionswassignificant[F(2,118)515.87,p,.001].Posthoccomparisonsshowednodif-ferenceinRTsbetweenthevalid-in(32368msec)andtwo–valid(32368msec)conditions,aspredicted.Thisisimportant,becausebothexperimentsshowthatvalidresponsestotargetsappearingwithinanobjectarenotin-fluencedbythenumberofobjectsinthedisplay.However,valid-outRTswereagainslightly,butsignificantly,longer(33067msec)thanthoseintheothertwovalidcondi-tions.Whyvalid-outRTsweresignificantlylongerinbothexperiments,despitethesmallerdifference(7msec)inthisexperiment,isnotclear.Onespeculationisthatbot-tom-up,stimulus-driveninformationmayautomaticallydrawattentiontothesingleobjectinone-objectdisplaysduring thesecond that theobject isvisiblebefore thecue–targetsequencebegins.Thisattentiontothesingleobjectmightcontributetotheslightprocessingadvantageofacuedobjectoveracuedemptylocation,resultinginslightlyshortervalid-inRTs(butseetheOverallAnalysessectionbelowandExperiment3).

ReplicatingtheresultsofExperiment1,one–within(34269msec),two–within(34169msec),andlocloc(34069msec)RTswerenodifferentfromeachother[F(2,118)50.14,p..87].Again,thisresultsupportedourpredictionthatRTsinthesethreeinvalidconditionsshouldbesimilarbecauseobject-basedattentionwouldneitherhavetodisengagefromanobjecttoshiftnoren-gageonanotherobjectaftershifting.

Aone-wayANOVAcomparingRTsinallone-objectinvalidconditions(one–within,locobj,objloc,andlocloc)wassignificant[F(3,177)530.54,p,.001].Again, theobjlocconditionwasofprimary interestbecauseonlyinthisconditionwouldobject-basedatten-tionhavetodisengagefromanobjectbeforeshifting.AsinExperiment1,theobjlocconditionhadthelongestRTsofanycondition.ObjlocRTs(37369msec)weresignificantly longer thanone–within (3426 9msec),locloc(34069msec),andlocobj(351610msec)RTs.NoteparticularlythatRTsinthelocobjconditionweresignificantlylongerthanloclocRTs.Thisfind-ingisdiscussedfurtherinthenextsection.Aspredicted,theone-objectinvalidconditionthatproducedthelon-

gestRTsinbothexperimentswastheobjloccondition,whichrequiredobject-basedattentiontodisengagefromanobjectbeforeshifting.

Overall AnalysesThreeadditionalanalyseswereconducted,incorporat-

ingthedatafrombothexperimentsandusingorientationasabetween-subjectsfactorwiththreelevels,vertical(Experiment1)and tilted leftand tiltedright (Experi-ment2).First,a3(orientation) 3 6(invalidconditions)ANOVAonRTsfromallinvalidconditionsshowednomaineffectoforientation[F(2,87)50.77,p..55]orinteraction with invalid condition [F(6,261)5 0.56,p..75],buttherewasasignificantmaineffectofin-validcondition[F(3,261)544.37,p, .001].RTsforthebetween-object(36868msec)andobjloc(37567msec)conditionsweresignificantlylongerthanthoseinallotherconditions,butnotsignificantlydifferentfromeachother.Thetwo–within(34567msec),one–within(34367msec),andlocloc(34468msec)conditionsproducedtheshortestRTsandwerenotdifferentfromeachother.RTsinthelocobj(35368msec)conditionwerebetweentheseextremesanddifferentfromthoseinallotherconditions.

A3(orientation) 3 3(validconditions)ANOVAonRTsfromthetwo–valid,valid-in,andvalid-outconditionsshowednomaineffectoforientation[F(3,177)530.54,p,.001],buttherewasasignificantmaineffectofvalidcondition[F(2,174)530.87,p, .001]andasignifi-cantinteraction[F(4,174)52.40,p,.052].Two–valid(32466msec)andvalid-in(32366msec)RTswerenotdifferentfromeachotherandwereshorterthanvalid-out(33366msec)RTs.AsisevidentinTable1,thepri-maryreasonsfortheorientationinteractionaretheoverallshorterRTsandsmallerRTdifferencesacrossvalidcon-ditionsforthetilted-rightorientation.Althoughvalid-outRTsweresignificantlylongerthanvalid-inRTsforthisorientation,thisdifferencewaslessthanintheothertwoorientations,andthevalid-outandtwo–validRTswerenotdifferentfromeachother.Fortheothertwoorientations,valid-outRTsweresignificantlylongerthanbothvalid-inandtwo–validRTs.Weshouldemphasizethetenuousna-tureofboththespeculationsearlierandthosebelowabouttheoverallslowervalid-outRTs,sincevalid-outRTswerenotdifferentfromtwo–validRTs(inwhichthetargetap-pearsinanobject)forthetilted-rightorientation.

Thefinalanalysiswasacostanalysisofcueconditionsacrossorientations.Acostanalysisnormalizesresponsestotargetsintheone-object invalidconditions,relative

table 1 Mean reaction times (in Milliseconds) for the Valid Conditions

of Each Orientation in Experiments 1 (Vertical) and 2 (tilted Left or right)

Vertical Left Right

Valid 326 333 315Valid-in 322 333 313

Valid-out 338 343 319

Note—Thestandarderrorforallconditionsis610msec.

Page 8: Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the ... & Denney... · paradigm (e.g., that of Egly, Driver, & Rafal, 1994), in which attention is first drawn to a location

Shifting Attention 613

bothtoeachotherandtothetwo-objectconditions,bysubtractingtheappropriatevalidconditionasabaseline.Thus,thecostindicateshowmuchadditionaltimewasnecessarytomakearesponsetoatargetappearingataparticularpositionwhenashiftofattentiontothatposi-tionwasnecessaryversuswhenitwasnot.Assuch,valid-inRTsweresubtractedfromRTsinthetwoconditionsinwhichthetargetappearedintheobject(one–withinandlocobj).Valid-outRTs,ontheotherhand,weresub-tractedfromthoseinthetwoconditionsinwhichthetar-getappearedoutsidetheobject(objlocandlocloc).Two-objectcostswerecalculatedbysubtractingtwo–validRTsfromthetwo–withinandbetween-objectRTs.A3(orientation)36(cuecondition)mixed-designANOVAconductedon thecosts,withorientation thebetween-subjectsfactor,showedasignificantmaineffectofcuecondition[F(5,435)536.9,p,.001]butnomainef-fectoforientation[F(2,87)50.31,p..74]orinterac-tion[F(10,435)51.12,p..33].Table2summarizesthecosts(6SE)overthedifferentinvalidconditions.

Costcomparisonsacrosscueconditionswereconsis-tentwiththeRTanalyses,butoneresultraisedaconcernrelatingbacktothevalid-in/valid-outRTdifference.Con-sistentwiththeRTanalyses,shiftsoutofanobject(i.e.,between-objectandobjlocshifts)producedthegreatestcostsofanyoftheconditions,andthepresenceofasec-ondobjectdidnotinfluencethetimetoshifteitherwithin(i.e.,two–withinvs.one–within)oroutof(i.e.,between-objectvs.objloc)anobject.Inaddition,locobjshiftcostsweredifferentfromthoseinallotherconditions,suggestingapossibleinfluenceofobject-basedattentionengagingonanobjectafterashift.

Oneresult,however—thefindingthatcostsforloclocshiftsweredifferentfromthoseinallotherconditions—didraiseaconcernthatrelatedbacktothevalid-in/valid-outRTdifference.Acomparisonofthelocloccondi-tionwithbothwithin-objectconditionssuggestedthatshiftswithinanobjectweresomehowslowerthanthosebetweentwolocations.Thereasonforourconcernwasthat,whateveradvantageanobjectmayhaveaffordedre-sponsestovalidlycuedtargetswithinit,onceacuehaddrawnattentiontotheobjectandashiftofattentionwasrequiredwithinit,theshiftseemedtotakemoretimethandidshiftsofthesamedistancebetweentwopositionsout-sideanobject.Thispotentiallytheoreticallyprovocativefindingsuggestedtheneedforacloserexaminationofthesequenceofeventsineachtrialfromtheperspectivesofengage,disengage,andshiftoperations.Inparticular,becauseloclocandobjloccostsarecalculatedusingvalid-outRTsasthebaseline,whatshouldbemadeoftheslightlybutsignificantly(7msec)shorterRTsforvalidlycuedtargetsthatappearedinsideratherthanoutsidean

object?Althoughwewilldescribetwopossibleexplana-tions,itshouldbenotedthatwebelievethatneither(orboth)oftheprocessesweproposemayhavecontributedtotheresults(see,e.g.,Experiment3).First,theeffectmightbeakintoanobjectsuperiorityeffect,inwhichthepresenceofanobjectfacilitatesperceptualprocess-ingofatargetappearingwithinit(Weisstein&Harris,1974),evenincomparisonwithatargetappearingalone(Williams&Weisstein,1978).Thiscouldexplainwhyvalid-inandtwo–validRTscouldbefasterrelativetothevalid-outcondition.However,asnotedabove,valid-outandtwo–validRTswerenotdifferentwiththetilted-rightorientation,whichraisesdoubtsaboutanyspeculationsconcerningthevalid-in/valid-outdifferences.

AnotherpossibilitycomesfromVeceraandBehrmann’s(2001)biasedcompetitionperspectiveonobjectsegre-gationandattention.Accordingtotheiraccount,objectsegregationandselectionforprocessinginvolveacom-petitionbetweenbottom-upinformationfromthephysicalstimulusandtop-downinformationrelatedtoanobserver’sgoals.Itseemslesslikelythattop-downinfluencescouldhavedeterminedtheresultshere,becauseboththeinstruc-tionsandthecue–targetprobabilityalwaysindicatedthattargetsweremostlikelytoappearatcuedlocations,andthisprobabilitywasthesamewhetherthelocationwasinoroutofanobject.Fromthisperspective,thefacilita-tionofresponsesforthevalid-inrelativetothevalid-outconditionmayhavebeenduetobottom-upinformation,consistentwithanobjectsuperiorityaccount.Duringthepreviewperiodofeachtrial,bottom-upcues(i.e.,thepres-enceofthesingleobject)mayhaveautomaticallydrawnattentiontotheobject(andtobothobjectswhenthereweretwo).Whenacueappearedinanobject,theprocessofen-gagingonthepositionofthecuewasfacilitatedbecauseattentionhadalreadybeendrawntotheobject.Whenthecueappearedoutsideofanobject,thelocation-baseden-gageoperationassociatedwiththecuewouldhavehadto,inasense,overcomeattentionalreadyengagedatthelocationoftheobjectduringthepreviewperiod.Thus,re-sponseswerefacilitatedwhenvalidcue–targetsequencesappearedwithinratherthanoutsideanobjectbecauseat-tentionhadbeendrawnbeforehandtothesingleobject.Asonereviewernoted,thevalid-in/valid-outdifferencemightbeconsideredabiascostassociatedwiththecuewhenitappearsoutsideasingleobject.Toaddressthisissue,afinal,controlexperimentwasrunwithnoobjectsinthedisplay.

ExpEriMEnt 3

ThemainpurposeofthisexperimentwastomeasureRTs tovalidlyand invalidlycued targetsappearing in

table 2 Average Costs for All invalid Conditions, Collapsed Across Orientations

TwoObjects OneObject

Within Between One–Within Obj→Loc Loc→Obj Loc→Loc

2063 4364 2063 4163 3063 1063

Page 9: Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the ... & Denney... · paradigm (e.g., that of Egly, Driver, & Rafal, 1994), in which attention is first drawn to a location

614        Brown and denney

open space, devoid of any potentially biasing influence of a single nearby object. This measurement would allow for comparisons of (1) no-object valid RTs with the valid-in and valid-out RTs from Experiment 2, and (2) the costs for shifts in open space, with no objects present, with those in the various one- and two-object invalid conditions from Experiment 2.

MethodParticipants

A total of 20 University of Georgia undergraduates (12 male, 8 female) participated for introductory psychology course credit. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, were classi-fied as right-handed according to the Annett Handedness Scale, and reported no history of attention deficit disorder.

Stimuli and ApparatusThe no-object condition was run under the same viewing condi-

tions as in Experiment 2 with cues and targets appearing at the same four positions relative to each other and to the fixation cross.

ProcedureThe total of 240 trials consisting of 144 (60%) validly cued, 48 

(20%) invalidly cued, and 48 (20%) catch trials. As before, invalid targets only appeared at the two positions closest to the cues. All other procedural details were identical to those in Experiment 2, except for the absence of objects in the display.

Results and Discussion

RTs less than 150 msec and greater than 1,000 msec (2%) were  trimmed from all analyses. The mean false alarm rate on catch trials was 5%. Valid RTs were aver-aged over position, and invalid RTs were averaged over all shift directions between cues and targets. A one-way ANOVA for the no-object valid and invalid RTs indicated a significant 15-msec cuing effect [F(1,19) 5 10.41, p , .004], with valid RTs (320 6 10 msec) faster than invalid RTs (335 6 10 msec). Costs were calculated as before, with RTs to validly cued targets at a position subtracted from RTs to invalidly cued targets at that same position. No-object RTs and costs were then compared with those from Experiment 2 (collapsed across left/right orienta-tion) using independent t tests (two-tailed). Only the Ex-periment 2 data were used for comparison because the positions of cues and targets relative to fixation and to each other were identical in both experiments. No-object valid RTs (320 6 10 msec) were not different from either the valid-out RTs [331 6 7 msec; t(78) 5 0.80, p 5 .43] or the valid-in RTs [323 6 8 msec; t(78) 5 0.25, p 5 .81]. This lack of a difference between the no-object valid RTs and both the valid-in and valid-out RTs suggests that, al-though the result was statistically significant, the slightly 

longer (7 msec) valid-out RTs in Experiment 2 likely did not reflect the presence of the single object, because there was no object present in this control experiment to influ-ence RTs. The present RT results and a comparison of them with the RTs in Experiment 2 suggest that our con-cern and speculations about the differences between valid-in and valid-out RTs were unwarranted. If valid-out RTs had been influenced by the presence of the single object, removing that object should have removed the influence, and no-object RTs should then have been different from valid-out RTs; this was not found. Similarly, if the single object had facilitated valid responses within it, a differ-ence should be found between valid-in and no-object valid RTs, and this difference was not found, either.

Cost comparisons (see Table 3) indicated that shift times for no-object invalid trials (15 6 5 msec) were not dif-ferent from those in the locloc [10 6 4 msec; t(78) 5 20.69, p 5 .49], one–within [20 6 4 msec; t(78) 5 0.67, p 5 .51], and two–within [18 6 3 msec; t(78) 5 0.53, p 5 .60] conditions. These results suggest that caution should be used when  interpreting,  and  speculating about,  the seemingly faster shift times in the overall analyses above for the locloc than for the within-object condition (see Table 2). If, as we speculated earlier, shifts in open space were somehow faster than those within an object, costs should then have been less in the no-object than in the within-object condition, but this was not the case. Rela-tive to the no-object condition, shift times were actually longer for the locobj [29 6 4 msec; t(78) 5 1.99, p 5 .05], objloc [43 6 3 msec; t(78) 5 4.47, p 5 .001], and  between-object [47 6 5 msec; t(78) 5 3.60, p 5  .001] conditions. Finding costs in those conditions that were greater than in the no-object condition is also consistent with the overall analyses in Experiment 2, in which those costs were also greater  than  those  in  the  locloc and within-object conditions.

In light of the results of the overall analyses above and of the comparisons with the no-object condition, the com-parisons of the results in the locobj condition with those in the locloc and both within-object conditions suggest possible influences from the engage operation in object-based attention. Although the operations associated with engaging on and disengaging from the cue and shifting would all be the same for the locobj and locloc con-ditions, those associated with engaging on the target after a shift would be different, since an object-based engage operation would only become involved when the target appeared in the object. Thus, engaging on a position after a shift would be less costly than engaging on an object, because only location-based attention would be involved 

Table 3 Average Costs for the No-Object Condition (Experiment 3) and the

Invalid Conditions of Experiment 2, Collapsed Across Left and Right Orientations

Two Objects One Object No ObjectWithin   Between   One–Within   Obj→Loc   Loc→Obj   Loc→Loc   (Loc→Loc)

18 6 4   47 6 5   20 6 4   43 6 3   29 6 4   10 6 4   15 6 5

Page 10: Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the ... & Denney... · paradigm (e.g., that of Egly, Driver, & Rafal, 1994), in which attention is first drawn to a location

Shifting Attention 615

fortheposition,butbothlocation-andobject-basedat-tentionwouldbeinvolvedfortheobject.Althoughfuturepsychophysicaltestsofthisaccountareneededinordertotestitdirectly,theaccountdoesseemreasonable,anditre-ceivessupportfromneuroimaging.Usinganevent-relatedfMRIparadigm,Arrington,Carr,Mayer,andRao(2000)foundgreateractivityacrossanumberofbrainareasdur-ingthedeploymentofobject-basedratherthanlocation-basedattention,andneverviceversa.Theyinterpretedthesefindingsasindicatingthatobject-baseddeployment“isgainedbyaddingtoorsupplementingthebrainactiv-ityneededtoallocateattentiontoasimilarlysituatedbutfuzzilyboundedspatialregiondefinedbyenvironmentalcoordinatesratherthanobjectshape”(p.111).Asimilarargumentcouldbemadeforwhythecostsassociatedwiththelocobjconditioncouldbegreaterthanthoseforthewithin-object conditions.Again, only in the locobjconditionwouldobject-basedattentionhavetoengageonanobjectafterashift.Inthewithin-objectconditions,object-basedattentionwouldalreadybeengagedontheobjectbecauseofthecue,butinthelocobjconditiontheobject-basedengageoperationwouldnotbeactivateduntilthetargetappearedintheobject.Becausethedis-tanceacrosswhichshiftsoccurredwasheldconstant,thegreatercostsforthelocobjconditioncouldberelatedtotheneedfortheobject-basedengageoperationafterashift.FromArringtonetal.’sperspective,thismightbeat-tributedtotheadditionalbrainactivationassociatedwithobject-basedattentionbecominginvolvedinthelocobjconditiononcethetargetappearsintheobject,incompar-isonwiththewithin-objectconditions,inwhichsimilarbrainactivationwouldalreadyhavebeeninvolvedfromthetimethecueappearedintheobject.

Finally,costswerelessforlocobjshiftsthanforei-therobjlocorbetween-objectshifts,butthecostsinthelattertwoconditionsweresimilar.Thisparticularfindingmightbeconsideredevidenceagainsttheinvolvementofobject-basedengageoperations,becausetheywouldhavebeeninvolvedinthelocobjandbetween-objectcondi-tions,butnotintheobjloccondition.Thereareafewexplanationsofwhythismightoccur.First,themethodusedheretomeasuretheseinfluences(RTs)maynothavebeensufficientlysensitivetoseparateouttheinfluencesofdisengageandengageoperationswhenbothwerein-volved.Thiscouldcertainlyhavebeenthecaseifobject-baseddisengageoperationshaveagreaterinfluenceonperformancethandoengageoperations.Thus,thecostsofengageoperationsareevidentintheabsenceofdisen-gageoperationsinthelocobjcondition,andthecostsofdisengageoperationsareevidentintheabsenceofengageoperationsintheobjloccondition,buttheinvolvementofdisengageoperationsinthebetween-objectconditionmaybesuchthattheyobscureevidenceoftheengageop-erations.Finally,itisalsopossiblethatthesespeculationsaboutengageoperationsmaybeincorrect.

Tosummarize,thebetween-objectandobjloccondi-tionsconsistentlyproducedthelongestRTsandgreatestcostsofalloftheinvalidconditions,andtheformercon-ditionsweretheonlyonesinwhichobject-basedatten-

tionshouldhavehadtodisengagefromanobjectbeforeshifting.TheresultsofExperiment2ruledoutshiftingattentionfromonevisualfieldtoanotherasanaccountoftheresultsofExperiment1,showedthatthebasicfindingsofExperiment1arereplicable,andstronglyimplicatedobject-baseddisengageoperationsasanimportantcon-tributortothedisadvantagefoundforshiftsofattentionbetweenratherthanwithinobjectsincuingstudies.Fi-nally,thegreatercostsforthelocobjconditionincom-parisonwiththeloclocandanyofthewithincondi-tionsmayreflecttheinvolvementofengageoperationsinobject-basedattention.

GEnErAL DiSCuSSiOn

Thepresentstudyexploredtheengageanddisengageoperationsofbothobject-andlocation-basedformsofattentionandtherolethoseoperationsplayintheobjectadvantagecommonlyfoundinattention-cuingstudies.Previousresearchshowedthatshiftingattentionmightbecriticalforproducingobjecteffects(Lamy&Egeth,2002)andthattheobjectadvantageincuingstudiesisrobustwhenbetween-objectdistance(Vecera,1994)andwithin-objectdistance(Brownetal.,2006)aremanipulated.Acommonthreadlinkingthesestudiesisthatresponsesareslowerwhenattentionmustdisengagefromanobjectbe-foreshiftingthanwhenitdoesnothaveto.Thisresultledtoourhypothesisthatdisengageoperationsassociatedwithobject-basedattentionplayaprimaryroleinpro-ducingtheobjectadvantageincuingstudies.Ourresultssupportthishypothesisandalsoindicatethatthe“objectadvantage”incuingstudiesmaymoreaccuratelybede-scribedasadisadvantageassociatedwithattentionshift-ingoutof,orawayfrom,anobject(seeLamy&Egeth,2002,forasimilardisadvantageargument).

WeinterpretourresultsasbuildingonandexpandingLamyandEgeth’s(2002)findingthatshiftingattentionisanessentialtaskrequirementforproducingobject-basedeffects.Ashiftofattentionwasrequiredforallinvalidtri-alsinourexperiments.Byvaryingwhereattentionshiftedfromandwhereitshiftedto,wecoulddirectlycompareRTsandcostsinone-andtwo-objectinvalidconditions.Asnotedintheintroduction,fromtheperspectiveoflocation-basedattention,alloftheinvalidconditionstestedshouldhaveproducedthesameresults,becauseattentionalwayshadtodisengagefromthecuelocationbeforeshiftingtothetargetlocation,andthecue-to-targetdistancewasthesameinallinvalidconditions.Anydifferencesfoundbetweenthevariousone-andtwo-objectinvalidcondi-tionsmust,therefore,beattributedtoinfluences(orlackof influences)ofobject-basedattention.Followinganevaluationoftheseinfluencesintermsoftheengageanddisengageoperationsoftheobject-andlocation-basedattentionsystems,wewillconsiderspreading-attention(Abrams&Law,2000;Avrahami,1999;Brownetal.,2006),biased-competition(Vecera,1994,2000;Vecera&Behrmann,2001;Vecera&Flevaris,2005),andprioritiza-tion(Shomstein&Yantis,2002,2004)perspectivesontheobjectadvantageandonourresults.

Page 11: Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the ... & Denney... · paradigm (e.g., that of Egly, Driver, & Rafal, 1994), in which attention is first drawn to a location

616 Brown And denney

Shifting Between two Objects Versus Shifting From an Object to a Location

Object-basedattentionwouldhavetodisengagefromanobjectbeforeshiftinginboththebetween-objectandobjloc conditions. In thebetween-object condition,object-basedattentionwouldalsohavetoengageonanewobjectaftershiftingawayfromtheoriginalobject.ThefactthatwefoundnodifferencesinRTsorcostsbetweentheseconditionsisoneindicatorsuggestingthatengagingobject-basedattentiononanobjectafterashiftmaynotbeamajorcontributortothetypicalobjectdisadvantage,butthatdisengagingattentionfromanobjectbeforeshift-ingis.

Shifting From an Object to a Location Versus From a Location to an Object

Acomparisonoftheseone-objectconditionsallowedustodirectlyassessthedifferencebetweendisengagingfromanobjectanddisengagingfromalocation.Ifobject-andlocation-basedattentionoperateindependently(see,e.g.,Leeketal.,2003;Reppa&Leek,2003;Tipperetal.,1994),theprocessesofengaginganddisengagingatten-tionwithinthesesystemswouldmostlikelyoperatein-dependentlyaswell.Ifdisengagingfromacuedobjectinvolvesbothdisengaginglocation-basedattentionfromthecuedobject’slocationanddisengagingobject-basedattentionfromthecuedobject,anadditionaldisengageoperationwouldbeassociatedwithobject-basedattentionforobjlocshiftsrelativetolocobjshifts.ThelongerRTsandgreatercostsfortheobjlocandbetween-objectconditionsrelativetothelocobjconditionareanotherindicatorthatdisengagingobject-basedattentionisapri-maryfactor.

Shifting From One Location to Another Versus From an Object to a Location

A comparison of the locloc and objloc condi-tionsprovidedanopportunitytoexaminetheinfluenceofobject-baseddisengageoperationswithoutanypotentialinfluencesofobject-basedengageoperationsoccurringafterashift,becauseinbothconditionsattentionshiftedtoalocation,andonlyintheobjlocconditionwoulddisengagingobject-basedattentionbeforeshiftinghavebeennecessary.TheconsistentlylongerRTsandgreatercostsfortheobjlocthanfortheloclocconditionalsosupporttheideathatdisengagingobject-basedattentionistheprimarycontributortotheobjectdisadvantage.

Shifting From One Location to Another Versus From a Location to an Object

Comparingtheloclocwiththelocobjconditionprovidedanotherway to assess the contribution fromobject-basedattentionengagingonanobjectafterashift.Thiscomparisoncouldbemadewithoutinfluencesfromobject-baseddisengageoperations,becauseattentional-waysshiftedfromalocationoutsideanobject.Thegreatercostsforthelocobjconditionsuggestaninfluenceofobject-basedengageoperations,althoughthisinfluencewasnotnoticeablewhenobject-baseddisengageopera-

tionswerealsoinvolved(e.g.,inthebetween-objectandobjlocconditions).

Shifting From One Location to Another Versus Within an Object

Althoughlocation-basedattentionwouldbeinvolvedinbothloclocandwithin-objectshifts,object-basedat-tentionwouldonlybeinvolvedinthewithin-objectshift.However,oncethecuearrives,thereisbasicallynodif-ferencebetweentheseconditionsintermsofdisengage,shift,andengageoperations.Forthewithin-objectcon-dition,oncethecuedrawsattentiontoalocationintheobject,theshiftfromcuetotargetoccurswithintheobject(i.e.,disengagingobject-basedattentionisnotinvolved),andthusonlylocation-basedattentionmustdisengageinordertoshiftfromcuetotargetlocation.Likewise,forthe locloccondition,onceattention isdrawn to thecuelocationoutsidethesingleobject,onlydisengaginglocation-basedattentionisrequiredinordertoshifttothetargetlocation.Thus,nodifferencewasfoundbetweentheloclocandwithin-objectRTs,aswellasnorealdif-ferenceinshifttimesbetweenthesetwoconditions,asevidencedbycomparisonswiththeno-objectconditionofExperiment3.

Spreading-Attention, Biased-Competition, and prioritization perspectives on the Object Advantage

Spreading-attention, biased-competition, and pri-oritizationperspectiveseachofferanaccountoftheob-jectadvantage,and thereforeanaccountof the fasterwithin-objectshiftsfoundhereincomparisonwiththebetween-objectandobjlocconditions.Initsownway,eachaccountemphasizesthefacilitativeinfluenceofat-tentiononprocessingwithinanobject,acharacteristicthatdistinguishestheseperspectivesfromthedisengageperspective.

Thespreading-attentionperspectiveattributesthefa-cilitationofwithin-objectshiftstoaprocessofattentionmoving,radiating,orspreadingthroughanobject,guidedbythatobject’scontours(Abrams&Law,2000;Avrahami,1999;Brownetal.,2006).

The biased-competition perspective (Vecera, 1994,2000;Vecera&Behrmann,2001;Vecera&Flevaris,2005)indicatesthatbottom-upandtop-downbiasescontributetotheobjectadvantage.Ingeneral,thesebiasescompeteintheperceptualsegregationandorganizationofthevi-sualfield,aswellasintheallocationofattentionwithinit.Inourone-andtwo-objectdisplays,bottom-upinforma-tionwouldsegregatetheobject(s)fromthebackground,biasingattentionalselectiontowardthemand,therefore,constrainingthefocusofattentionrelativetoemptyspace(Vecera&Behrmann,2001).Althoughthistheorycouldaccountforattentionbeingdrawntotheobject(s)inourexperiments,itcouldnotaccountfortheobjectadvan-tage,becauseourobjectswereidentical,andbottom-upinformationwouldnothavebiasedselectionofoneobjectovertheother(Vecera,2000).Inourtask,thecuewasthemainattention-biasingfactor.AccordingtoVecera(2000),

Page 12: Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the ... & Denney... · paradigm (e.g., that of Egly, Driver, & Rafal, 1994), in which attention is first drawn to a location

Shifting Attention 617

a“spatialprecuemayallowspatialattentiontobiasatten-tioninatop-downmanner”(p.372);when“spatialatten-tionissummonedtoacuedlocation,attentioncanspreadormovewithinaclosedregionmoreeasilythanbetweenclosed regions” (p.364; i.e., cuing anobject leads tospreadingattentionwithinit).“Thespatialprecueactstobiasobjectattentiontowardthecuedobject,allowingob-serverstorespondfastertotargetsappearinginthecuedobjectthanintheuncuedobject”(Vecera,2000,p.372).Inlightofourstimuliandtask,thisperspectiveseemstoincorporatebothspreadingandprioritizationviews:Theenclosedcontoursofthecuedobjectfacilitatethespreadofattentionwithinitinabottom-upmanner,andthecuealsobiasesobject-basedattentiontowardthecuedobjectinatop-downmannerthatcouldbeinterpretedasastra-tegicinfluenceontheallocationofattentiontothatobjectoverothers.

Theprioritizationperspectivealsoemphasizesapro-cessingadvantagewithinobjects(Shomstein&Yantis,2004).Object-basedselectionreflects“animplicitobject-specificattentionalprioritizationstrategythatarisesonlywhenmultiplelocationsinthescenemustbeattended”(Shomstein&Yantis,2004,p.248);“wheneverthereismorethanoneobjectinthescene,regionswithinanat-tendedobjectwill,bydefault,beassignedhigherpriorityforvisualexplorationthanotherobjects”(p.253).Thus,whenattentionisnotnarrowlyfocused,within-objectpo-sitionsareaccordedpriorityinprocessing,leadingtoanobjectadvantage(Shomstein&Yantis,2002).AsLamyandEgeth(2002)havenoted,however,whenShomsteinandYantis(2002)didnotfindanobjecteffectwiththeirinterferenceparadigm,thetargetalwaysappearedatfixa-tion,andthusattentionneverhadtoshiftinordertoac-complishthetask.Intheoneexperimentinwhichtheydidfindanobjecteffect,theymodifiedtheirparadigmsothatshiftsofattentionwereinvolvedinperformingthetask.Thus,theextenttowhichthefocusofattentionand/orshiftsinattentioncontributedtothepresenceorabsenceofanobjecteffectisnotcertain.

A closer comparison of the spreading, biased-competition,andprioritizationviewsintermsoftheal-locationofattentionfollowingacuesuggeststhatlittlemaydistinguishthem,exceptperhapstheallocationofattentionwithinanobject.Theprioritizationviewdefinestheallocationofattentionintermsofanimplicitscanningstrategy,whereasboththespreading-attentionandbiased-competitionperspectivesdescribeitasaspreadingofat-tention.However,bydefinition,peoplewouldnomorebeawareofanimplicitwithin-objectscanningstrategythanofattentionspreadingwithinanobject.Similarly,whereasspreadingattentionisthoughttooccurfromthecuetootherlocationsinthecuedobject,implicitprioritygiventovisualexplorationofwithin-objectpositionswouldalsobeassumedtooccurfromthecuetootherlocationsinthecuedobject.Thus,fromalloftheseperspectives,atten-tionfacilitatesprocessingwithinobjectsinamannerthatproceedsfromcuedtouncuedpositions.

Thedifferentfocusofourdisengageperspective,onadisadvantageforshiftsawayfromobjectsratherthananadvantageforshiftswithinthem,highlightsthedifference

betweenourviewandthespreading,biased-competition,andprioritizationperspectives.Ourperspectivealsoex-pandsonLamyandEgeth’s(2002)emphasisonshiftingattentionbyconsideringthedisengagementofattentionbeforeandtheengagementofattentionaftershifting.Thistheoryisclearlydifferentfromthespreading-attentionandbiased-competitionperspectives,bothofwhichin-corporatespreadingattention.

Itshouldbeacknowledgedthatthepresentstudywasnotspecificallydesignedtodistinguishbetweenthedis-engageandprioritizationperspectives.However,becausethedisengageaccountwasthebasisforthepresentexperi-ments,itdoeshavetheadvantageofincorporatingallthefindingsunderoneconceptualframework.Asnotedabove,however,theprioritizationperspectivedoesprovidearea-sonableaccountfortheobjectdisadvantagesfoundinourone-andtwo-objectconditions.Oneanonymousreviewernotedthatifwithin-objectlocationsarealwaysgivenahigherprioritywhenthecueispartiallyvalid,shiftingfromanobjecttoalocationshouldbeslowerthanshiftingfromalocationtoanobject,becauseintheformercasethereisatendencytosearchthenoncuedwithin-objectlo-cationfirst.Therevieweralsonotedthatshiftingfromanobjecttoalocationwouldbeslowerthanshiftingfromonelocationtoanotherforthesamereason.However,becausethecuesappearedinlocationsnotoccupiedbyanobjectinthelocobjandloclocconditions,theprioritizationperspectivewouldseemtohavenothingtosayabouttheslowerlocobjresponses.Inordertomaintainapriori-tizationaccountforthepresentstudy,amodificationofitsdefiningcharacteristicswouldseemtobenecessary:Whenthereisonlyoneobjectinthescene,yetmultiplelo-cationsmustbeattended,ifattentionisdrawnawayfromtheobject,locationsoutsidetheobjectmayreceivehigherprioritizationthanlocationsassociatedwiththeobject.SuchanadditiontotheprioritizationperspectiveseemsnecessaryinordertoaccountforthelongerRTsinthelocobjthaninthelocloccondition.However,thisad-ditionwouldalsoseemtotaketheprioritizationaccountbeyondtherealmofobject-specificattentionalselection,makingitablendofobject-andlocation-basedprioriti-zation.Thischangemayormaynotfitwiththeauthor’sintentions.

Finally,itshouldalsobenotedthatourpsychophysi-calfindingsareconsistentwithrecentevent-relatedfMRIstudiesoflocation-andobject-basedattention.Thereisgrowingevidencethattransientactivityinslightlydiffer-entregionsofthesuperiorparietallobule,intraparietalsulcus,andprecuneuscoincidewithshiftsofvisualatten-tionbetween(respectively)spatiallocations,stimulusfea-tures,andobjects(Corbetta,Kincade,Ollinger,McAvoy,&Shulman,2000;Corbetta&Shulman,2002;Serencesetal.,2005;Yantisetal.,2002;Yantis&Serences,2003).Transientactivityassociatedwithshiftsofattentionacrosstheseperceptualdimensionshasbeeninterpretedasreori-entingsignalsinordertodisengageandshiftattention(Ser-encesetal.,2005;Yantisetal.,2002;Yantis&Serences,2003)andcauseachangeinthestateofbiasedcompeti-tioninotherbrainregionsassociatedwithstimulusandtaskdemands(Serencesetal.,2005).Contributionsfrom,

Page 13: Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the ... & Denney... · paradigm (e.g., that of Egly, Driver, & Rafal, 1994), in which attention is first drawn to a location

618 Brown And denney

andinteractionsbetween,thetransientcorticalactivityintheregionsrelatedtolocation-andobject-basedattentionshiftsmayunderliethepsychophysicallymeasureddiffer-encesfoundacrosstheconditionsofthepresentstudy.

AutHOr nOtE

CorrespondencerelatingtothisarticlemaybesenttoJ.M.Brown,DepartmentofPsychology,UniversityofGeorgia,Athens,GA30602-3013(e-mail:[email protected]).

rEFErEnCES

Abrams, R. A., & Law, M. B. (2000).Object-basedvisualattentionwithendogenousorienting.Perception & Psychophysics,62,818-833.

Arrington, C. M., Carr, T. H., Mayer, A. R., & Rao, S. M. (2000).Neuralmechanismsofvisualattention:Object-basedselectionofaregioninspace.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,12(Suppl.2),106-117.

Avrahami, J. (1999).Objectsofattention,objectsofperception.Per-ception & Psychophysics,61,1604-1612.

Brown, J. M., Breitmeyer, B. G., Leighty, K. A., & Denney, H. I. (2006).Thepathofvisualattention.Acta Psychologica,121,199-209.

Corbetta, M., Kincade, J. M., Ollinger, J. M., McAvoy, M. P., & Shulman, G. L. (2000).Voluntaryorientingisdissociatedfromtargetdetectioninhumanparietalcortex.Nature Neuroscience,3,292-297.

Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002).Controlofgoal-directedandstimulus-drivenattentioninthebrain.Nature Reviews Neuroscience,3,201-215.

Davis, G., Driver, J., Pavani, F., & Shepherd, A. (2000).Reappraisingtheapparentcostsofattendingtotwoseparatevisualobjects.Vision Research,40,1323-1332.

Egly, R., Driver, J., & Rafal, R. D. (1994).Shiftingvisualattentionbetweenobjectsandlocations:Evidencefromnormalandparietallesionsubjects.Journal ofExperimental Psychology: General,123,161-177.

Egly, R., Rafal, R., Driver, J., & Starrveveld, Y. (1994).Covertori-entinginthesplitbrainrevealshemisphericspecializationforobject-basedattention.Psychological Science,5,380-383.

Goldsmith, M., & Yeari, M. (2003).Modulationofobject-basedat-tentionbyspatialfocusunderendogenousandexogenousorienting.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Perfor-mance,29,897-918.

Lamy, D., & Egeth, H. (2002).Object-basedselection:Theroleofat-tentionalshifts.Perception & Psychophysics,64,52-66.

Lavie, N., & Driver, J. (1996).Onthespatialextentofattentioninobject-based visual selection. Perception & Psychophysics, 58,1238-1251.

Law, M. B., & Abrams, R. A. (2002).Object-basedselectionwithinandbeyondthefocusofspatialattention.Perception & Psychophysics,64,1017-1027.

Leek, E. C., Reppa, I., & Tipper, S. P. (2003).Inhibitionofreturnfor

objectsandlocationsinstaticdisplays.Perception & Psychophysics,65,388-395.

Posner, M. I. (1980).Orientingofattention.Quarterly Journal of Ex-perimental Psychology,32,3-25.

Reppa, I., & Leek, E. C. (2003).Themodulationofinhibitionofreturnbyobject-internalstructure:Implicationsfortheoriesofobject-basedattentionalselection.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,10,493-502.

Serences, J. T., Liu, T., & Yantis, S. (2005).Parietalmechanismsofat-tentionalcontrol:Locations,features,andobjects.InL.Itti,G.Rees,&J.K.Tsotsos(Eds.),Neurobiology of attention(pp.35-41).SanDiego:AcademicPress.

Shomstein, S., & Yantis, S. (2002).Object-basedattention:Sensorymod-ulationorprioritysetting?Perception & Psychophysics,64,41-51.

Shomstein, S., & Yantis, S. (2004).Configuralandcontextualprioriti-zationinobject-basedattention.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,11,247-253.

Tipper, S. P., Weaver, B., Jerreat, L. M., & Burak, A. L. (1994).Object-basedandenvironment-basedinhibitionofreturnofvisualattention.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,20,478-499.

Vecera, S. P. (1994).Groupedlocationsandobject-basedattention:CommentonEgly,Drive,andRafal(1994).Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,123,316-320.

Vecera, S. P. (2000).Towardabiasedcompetitionaccountofobject-basedsegregationandattention.Brain & Mind,1,353-384.

Vecera, S. P., & Behrmann, M. (2001).Attentionandunitformation:Abiasedcompetitionaccountofobject-basedattention.InT.F.Shipley&P.J.Kellman(Eds.),From fragments to objects: Segregation and grouping in vision(pp.145-180).Amsterdam:North-Holland.

Vecera, S. P., & Flevaris, A. V. (2005).Attentioncontrolparametersfollowingparietal-lobedamage:Evidencefromnormalsubjects.Neu-ropsychologia,43,1189-1203.

Weisstein, N., & Harris, C. S. (1974).Visualdetectionoflineseg-ments:Anobject-superiorityeffect.Science,186,752-755.

Williams, A., & Weisstein, N. (1978).Linesegmentsareperceivedbetterinacoherentcontextthanalone:Anobject-lineeffectinvisualperception.Memory & Cognition,6,85-90.

Yantis, S., Schwarzbach, J., Serences, J. T., Carlson, R. L., Stein-metz, M. A., Pekar, J. J., & Courtney, S. M. (2002).Transientneuralactivityinhumanparietalcortexduringspatialattentionshifts.Nature Neuroscience,5,995-1002.

Yantis, S., & Serences, J. T. (2003).Corticalmechanismsofspace-basedandobject-basedattentionalcontrol.Current Opinion in Neu-robiology,13,187-193.

nOtE

1.AlthoughweutilizetheterminologyusedbyPosner(1980)tode-scribetheprocessesinvolvedinshiftsofattention,weremainneutralonwhethertherearespecificneuralmechanismsforeach(e.g.,a“neuraldisengager”).

(ManuscriptreceivedJanuary24,2005;revisionacceptedforpublicationAugust28,2006.)