Sense of Coherenece _and School Stress

download Sense of Coherenece _and School Stress

of 12

Transcript of Sense of Coherenece _and School Stress

  • 8/13/2019 Sense of Coherenece _and School Stress

    1/12

    Social Science & Medicine 53 (2001) 603614

    Sense of coherence and school-related stress as predictors of

    subjective health complaints in early adolescence: interactive,

    indirect or direct relationships?

    Torbjorn Torsheima,*, Leif Edvard Aaroeb, Bente Wolda

    aResearch Centre for Health Promotion, University of Bergen, Christiesgt.13, N-5015 Bergen, Norwayb Department of Psychosocial Sciences, University of Bergen, Christiesgt.12, N-5015, Bergen, Norway

    Abstract

    The role of sense of coherence (SOC) on the relationship between adolescent school-related stress and subjective

    health complaints was tested with structural equation modelling. As part of the crossnational WHO-survey Health

    behaviour in school-aged children 1997/98 Norwegian representative samples of 1592 grade 6, 1534 grade 8, and 1605

    grade 10 students completed measures on SOC, school-related stress and subjective health complaints. A test of nested

    structural models revealed that both stress-preventive (D w2 814. 86,p50:001), stress-moderating (D w2 11.74, p50:02)

    and main health-enhancing (D w2 1289.1,p50:001) effects of SOC were consistent with the data. A model including all

    these relationships fitted the data well (CFI=0.91, RMSEA=0.04). Age-group comparisons revealed that the

    association between SOC and stress grew weaker with age (p50:05), whereas the direct association between SOC and

    health complaints grew stronger (p50:001). The main effect of SOC accounted for between 39% (11 year olds) and

    54% (15 year olds) of the variance in subjective health complaints. Findings indicate that SOC may potentially be a

    salutogenic factor in adolescents adaptation to school-related stress, and that relationships between SOC and healthy

    adaptation, may be evident in younger age-groups than previously anticipated. # 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All

    rights reserved.

    Keywords: Sense-of-coherence; Moderator; Stress; Adolescence; Health-complaints; Resilience

    Introduction

    Subjective health complaints like headache, backache,

    and abdominal pains, are common in early adolescence

    (Aro, Paronen, & Aro, 1987; Garralda, 1996; Goodman

    & McGrath, 1991; King, Wold, Tudor-Smith, & Harel,1996; Mikkelsson, Salminen, & Kautiainen, 1997). A

    series of studies have implicated school-related stress in

    the development and maintenance of such health

    complaints (Aro et al., 1987; Garralda, 1996; Hurrel-

    mann, Engel, Holler, & Nordlohne, 1988; Ystgaard

    1997). However, the finding that not all students develop

    complaints from school-related demands has directed

    the attention to factors that moderate the perception of

    stress, and the adverse health impact of stress (e.g.

    Wagner & Compas, 1990; Ystgaard, 1997). In adults,

    one of the stress moderators that has generated

    considerable interest is the sense of coherence (SOC), a

    global orientation to view life situations as comprehen-sible, manageable and meaningful (Antonovsky, 1987).

    In the original theoretical formulation Antonovsky

    (1987) proposed that SOC may influence stress and

    health in three ways: (1) SOC influences whether a

    stimuli is appraised as stressor or not; (2) SOC influences

    the extent to which a stressor leads to tension or not;

    and (3) SOC influences the extent to which tension leads

    to adverse health consequences. While research on

    adults in part support these assumptions (for a review,

    see Antonovsky, 1993), the role of SOC in child and

    adolescent health is largely unexplored. With the view

    that school adaptation has an essential impact on a wide

    *Corresponding author. Tel.: +47 55 58 33 01; fax: +47 55

    58 98 87.

    E-mail address:[email protected]

    (T. Torsheim).

    0277-9536/01/$ - see front matter # 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

    PII: S 0 2 7 7 - 9 5 3 6 ( 0 0 ) 0 0 3 7 0 - 1

  • 8/13/2019 Sense of Coherenece _and School Stress

    2/12

    range of social, psychological, and behavioural out-

    comes, empirical evidence on the stress moderating role

    of SOC during adolescence may offer particularly scope

    for development of prevention policies. The aim of the

    present paper is to examine the ways SOC and school-

    related stress interact in relation to subjective health

    complaints during early adolescence.

    SOC and stress appraisal

    In the processes linking life situations to health, stress

    appraisal is the first process that SOC may influence.

    Stress research indicates that level of ambiguity and

    uncertainty are important dimensions in appraisals of

    life situations. Unpredictable or incomprehensible life

    situations are potent sources of stress (Lazarus &

    Folkman, 1984). As a global orientation to life, the

    sense of coherence (SOC), will influence the degree to

    which people view life demands as chaotic andincomprehensible, or coherent and comprehensible.

    Through the confidence that . . .the stimuli deriving

    from ones internal and external environments are

    structured, predictable and explicable. . . (Antonovsky,

    1987, p. 19), individuals with a strong SOC will be less

    likely to perceive ambiguity in encounters with life

    demands.

    In keeping with the hypothesis that a high SOC may

    help to appraise demands as non-stressful, studies on

    adult populations have reported moderate inverse

    associations between measures of SOC and measures

    of perceived stress. A review of these studies (Anto-

    novsky, 1993) showed that that the associations are

    generally stronger for perceived measures of stress than

    for measures of stressful life events, suggesting a role in

    appraisal processes, and not in the actual exposure to

    stressful events.

    SOC and stress moderation

    As a next step in the stress process, SOC has been

    suggested to influence coping expectancies in encounters

    with stress (Antonovsky, 1987, p.19). According to the

    transactional model of stress (Lazarus & Folkman,

    1984), coping expectancies develop from secondaryappraisal processes, where people assess the means that

    are available to deal with the stressful condition. As a

    global orientation to life, individuals with a strong SOC

    will have a general confidence that resources are

    available to meet the demands posed by stressful

    situations (Antonovsky, 1987, p.19). This confidence

    increases the likelihood of positive coping expectancies.

    In related conceptual formulations (e.g. Bandura, 1986;

    Kobasa, 1979; Ursin, 1988) coping expectancies are

    assumed to moderate reactions to stress. In line with

    these models, Antonovsky (1987) proposes that a strong

    SOC may help to prevent stress from turning into

    potentially harmful tension. From this perspective SOC

    acts as a classic moderator of life stress.

    Empirical studies on the stress-moderating role of

    SOC show mixed findings. In a study of Finnish adult

    workers, Feldt (1997) found that the relationship

    between work demands and health complaints was

    stronger for workers with a low SOC, but in statisticalterms the interaction was weak. In a similar vein,

    Vahtera and colleagues (1996) found that job demands

    from active jobs lead to sickness spells in workers with

    low SOC, but not in workers with a high SOC. In

    contrast, a number of other studies have failed to detect

    stress-distress moderation (e.g. Anson, Carmel, Leven-

    son, Bonneh, & Maoz, 1993; Flannery & Flannery,

    1990), leaving the issue of SOC as a moderator

    unresolved.

    SOC and stress-termination

    As a third mechanism, Antonovsky suggested that a

    high SOC may prevent stress-associated tension from

    developing into health problems. Stressing the point that

    SOC is not a particular coping style, Antonovsky (1987)

    proposed that individuals with a high SOC are more

    likely to select the coping strategy that is efficient for

    dealing with the stressor. High SOC individuals tend to

    use problem-focused strategies, they are flexible in their

    choices of strategies, and they are skilled in using

    feedback to redirect coping attempts. As a consequence,

    individuals with a high SOC are, in general, more likely

    to remove the source of stress, and to terminate the

    associated tension. Over time, individuals with a strong

    SOC will experience shorter periods of harmful tension

    than individuals with a weak SOC, suggesting a main

    effect between level of SOC and health.

    In line with the tension-termination hypothesis, a high

    SOC has been strongly associated with measures of self-

    reported health and well-being, as well as low scores on

    markers of disease (for a review see Antonovsky, 1993).

    While these findings are in line with the tension-

    termination hypothesis, authors have suggested that

    the strong associations to some extent may reflect

    methodological confounding between measures of SOC

    and measures of self-reported health (Geyer, 1997;Korotkov, 1993).

    SOC and the mechanisms of health complaints

    Through the influence on stress appraisals, coping

    expectancies and coping behaviour, SOC may affect

    processes that are essential in the development and

    maintenance of subjective health complaints. Uncer-

    tainty is a potent stimuli for the stress response. Once

    initiated, the profile of the stress response is moderated

    by response-outcome expectancies and control beliefs

    (Ursin & Hytten, 1992). Low perceived control over

    T. Torsheim et al. / Social Science & Medicine 53 (2001) 603614604

  • 8/13/2019 Sense of Coherenece _and School Stress

    3/12

    stressful conditions have been associated with a general

    tonic activation involving all biological response sys-

    tems, including changes in neuro-endocrine, vegetative,

    neuro-muscular, central-nervous and immune system

    functioning (Ursin, 1997). Activation is sustained when

    coping efforts to remove the stressor are unsuccessful, or

    when no attempts are made to remove the stressor (i.e.helplessness). Sustained activation causes long term

    sensitisation of neural transmission (Antelman, Soares,

    & Gershon, 1997; Dubner & Ruda, 1992; Woolf &

    Thompson, 1991). Recent contributions view long-term

    sensitisation as a candidate mechanism for chronically

    elevated levels of health complaints (Ursin, 1997). In

    sum, these finding suggest that stressful appraisals,

    negative coping expectancies and unsuccessful coping

    behaviour are associated with physiological processes

    that may permanently lower the threshold for experien-

    cing subjective health complaints.

    SOC in adolescence

    In view of the pervasive impact that is claimed for the

    adult SOC, surprisingly little is known about the role of

    SOC in the normal adaptation of general adolescent

    populations. In the original theoretical formulation,

    Antonovsky (1987) emphasises that SOC is a develop-

    mental construct that becomes crystallised at the age of

    30, suggesting a more fluctuating and less essential role

    for SOC in earlier age-groups: The adolescent, at the

    very best can only have gained a tentative strong SOC,

    which may be useful for short-range prediction about

    coping with stressors and health status. (Antonovsky,

    1987, p. 107).

    The hypothesised limited role for SOC in adolescent

    health, has been paralleled by a limited research focus

    on these groups. Some authors view adolescent experi-

    ences as important for the development of SOC in

    adulthood (e.g. Lundberg, 1997; Cederblad, Dahlin,

    Hagnell, & Hansson, 1994) but few contributions have

    addressed the potential health impact SOC may have

    during adolescence. The few studies on child and

    adolescent SOC, have to a large degree focused on

    particular risk groups, such as adolescents experiencing

    evacuation stress (Antonovsky & Sagy, 1986), learningdisabled children (Margalit & Efrati, 1996), and

    adolescents with chronic disease (Baker, 1998). Contrary

    to the idea of a fluid and weak adolescent SOC, these

    studies indicate that a young SOC may contribute to

    stress and coping in much the same way as does the

    mature adult SOC.

    While the above studies offer preliminary evidence

    that SOC may help adolescent risk groups to cope with

    particular difficult life conditions, the health impact of

    SOC may be even more far-reaching for normative

    demands that every adolescents encounter during the

    course of normal development. School-related demands

    are potent sources of stress in adolescent normal

    populations (Eme, Maisak, & Goodale, 1979; Greene,

    1988; Henker, Whalen, & ONeil, 1995). Elevated levels

    of such stress is associated with psychological distress

    (Wagner & Compas, 1990; Ystgaard, 1997) and somatic

    complaints (Aro et al., 1987; Garralda, 1996; Hurrel-

    mann et al., 1988). As schooling is mandatory in mostcountries, exposure to school demands is beyond the

    control of adolescents. Identification of resources that

    may help to prevent stressful appraisals, or moderate the

    adverse health impact of stress, may serve as an

    important first step in developing preventive strategies.

    The present study

    Antonovskys (1987) original contribution suggests

    that the adolescent SOC may affect level of health

    complaints indirectly by preventing school-related stressappraisals, interactively by moderating the impact of

    stress, and directly by reducing the likelihood of

    sustained activation. A potential shortcoming in pre-

    vious research, is the failure to compare the relative

    importance of these mechanisms at given developmental

    stages in life. To gain more knowledge on the role of

    SOC in early adolescents adaptation to school-related

    stress, the present paper investigates each of these

    assumed relationships:

    1. SOC is inversely related to appraisals of school-

    related stress.

    2. SOC moderates the relationship between school-

    related stress on subjective health complaints.

    3. SOC is inversely related to health complaints.

    Method

    Sample and sampling procedure

    As part of the WHO survey Health behaviour in

    school-aged children 1997/98 a representative sample of5026 Norwegian 11, 13 and 15 year olds took part in the

    study, representing a response rate of 78%. 1733 was

    from grade 6 (mean age 11.46), 1623 was from grade 8

    (mean age 13.46), and 1670 was from grade 10 (mean

    age 15.48). The sample was obtained using a clustered

    sampling procedure with school-class as the sampling

    unit (Currie, 1998). Clustered samples may potentially

    underestimate measurement error due to non-indepen-

    dent observations. However, previous documenta-

    tion (King et al., 1996, P. 215) suggests there to be

    essentially no such design effect for self-reported health

    measures.

    T. Torsheim et al. / Social Science & Medicine 53 (2001) 603614 605

  • 8/13/2019 Sense of Coherenece _and School Stress

    4/12

    Instruments

    The HBSC symptom checklist

    This is an 8-item scale on reported symptoms

    (headache, abdominal pain, backache, depressed

    mood, irritability, nervousness, sleeping difficulties,

    dizziness). In the last 6 months: how often haveyou had the following? Symptoms are rated on a five-

    point frequency scale: about every day; more than

    once a week; about every week; about every month;

    rarely or never. (Cronbachs alpha : .76; test-retest:

    0.80)

    Perceived school-related stress

    The HBSC school-related stress subscale measured

    this (Samdal, Wold, & Torsheim, 1998b). The subscale

    comprises three items, see Appendix B (Cronbachs

    alpha: 0.75; test-retest 0.80).

    Sense of coherence

    Antonovskys (1987) Orientation to Life Question-

    naire, short form (SOC-13), was adapted to fit early

    adolescents (see Appendix A). A description of the

    adaptations are made in Torsheim and Wold (1998). The

    13-items tap into three components of comprehensibil-

    ity, manageability and meaningfulness (Cronbachs

    alpha: 0.85, test-retest: 0.78).

    Procedure

    The survey was carried out according to a standar-

    dised protocol (Currie, 1998). Prior to the distribution of

    material, teachers received instructions how to admin-

    ister the survey. Questionnaires were distributed and

    filled out during a regular school-hour. Students were

    informed that participation was voluntary, and that

    their responses were anonymous.

    Statistical analysis

    The fit of the models were assessed with Structural

    equation modeling (SEM) procedures using EQS for

    windows ver. 5.4 (Bentler, 1995). When a theoretically

    founded causal model exists, SEM offers severaladvantages compared to linear regression analysis.

    SEM offers more sophistication in detecting violation

    of the model assumptions, and in comparison of

    alternative causal models. SEM may be particularly

    advantageous for the kind of interaction that the

    moderator model implies. In multiple regression, mea-

    surement errors tend to be high in interaction terms

    (Busemeyer & Jones, 1983), thus increasing the prob-

    ability of type II errors in the tests of interaction. In

    SEM the latent variables are measured without mea-

    surement error, which reduces the problem of type II

    error in tests of interaction. Several procedures for latent

    variable modeling of interaction (i.e. moderation) effects

    have been developed during the last decade (e.g. Jaccard

    & Wan, 1995; Kenny & Judd, 1984; Ping, 1996). The

    present study implemented the two-step procedure

    developed by Ping (1996). Prior to model estimation,

    the observed variables were centered to ensure uncorre-

    lated main and interaction terms. A product termbetween the average of the indicators of the school-

    related stress factor and the average of the indicators of

    the latent sense of coherence factor was computed.

    Estimation of the latent school-related stress *SOC

    interaction factor was done in two steps. Firstly, the

    errors and the loadings for the latent stress and the

    latent SOC factor was estimated. In the second step, the

    estimated errors and loadings were used to compute

    fixed error and loading for the measurement equation of

    the latent interaction factor, leaving only the structural

    model to be estimated. Under the assumptions of

    normality and unidimensional measures, the methodprovides estimated solutions that are essentially identical

    to more elaborate methods (for a practical example see

    Ping, 1998).

    Goodness-of-fit criteria

    Several indices of goodness of fit exist. In the

    present study, the comparative fit index (CFI) and

    the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA)

    were used as the goodness-of-fit-criteria. By convention,

    a CFI above 0.90 and a RMSEA lower than 0.05 is

    taken to reflect adequate model fit. Comparisons of

    nested models were made by testing the significanceof the difference in w2. According to Baron and

    Kenny (1986), a moderator effect would be indicated

    by a significant improvement in model fit when

    adding the effect of interaction factor to the main effects

    model.

    Measurement models

    The latent factors of the structural models were based

    on the following congeneric measurement models: For

    the latent school-related stress factor, the three items of

    the school-related stress subscale were used as three

    observed indicators of school-related stress. For theadapted SOC-13 scale, the summed scores for each of

    the three subcomponents meaningfulness, manageability

    and comprehensibility were used as indicators of the

    latent SOC factor. Feldt and Rasku (1998) showed that

    the three subfactors may reflect one underlying factor.

    For the latent SOC*stress interaction factor, the product

    of the average of the three stress indicators and the

    average of the three SOC indicators were used as the

    observed indicator, with fixed loadings and error

    variances. For the subjective health complaints latent

    variable, the eight observed complaint variables were

    used as indicators.

    T. Torsheim et al. / Social Science & Medicine 53 (2001) 603614606

  • 8/13/2019 Sense of Coherenece _and School Stress

    5/12

    Missing values

    Cases with more than 30% missing values were

    excluded from the analysis. For the rest, missing values

    were replaced with variable means. The final sample thus

    consisted of 1592 11 year olds, 1534 13 year olds, and

    1605 15 year olds.

    Results

    Table 1 shows the correlation matrix and the means

    and standard deviations that was used for the estimation

    of the structural equations.

    Preliminary analysis of measurement model

    Unidimensional measurement models is a prerequisite

    for obtaining unbiased results. The fit of the measure-

    ment model for SOC, school-related stress and sub-jective health complaints was tested simultaneously by

    allowing the three latent factors to correlate. Unidimen-

    sionality for each of the latent factors would be

    indicated by a high overall goodness of fit. The

    correlated factors model obtained a CFI of 0.96 and a

    RMSEA=0.03, suggesting a satisfactory, yet not perfect

    degree of unidimensionality. Close scrutiny of the

    measurement model suggested a minor departure from

    perfect unidimensionality in the observed indicators of

    the latent health complaints factor. Modelling the

    symptoms as two correlated latent factors, rather than

    the proposed one-factor model, provided a small

    improvement in the overall measurement model

    (CFI=0.98, RMSEA=0.02) but did not alter the error

    terms and factor loadings for the observed indicators of

    SOC and stress. To check for potential bias in the

    proposed one-factor model of health complaints the

    structural models to be presented in the next sections

    were estimated for both the one-factor solution and the

    two-factor measurement model. The substantive find-

    ings did not differ for these solutions, suggesting no

    essential bias for the one-factor model. For parsimony,

    the results to be presented in the next sections is for the

    proposed one-factor measurement model of health

    complaints only.

    Model selection

    Structural models were tested as a series of nested

    models, moving from the most restricted model to

    models with less restricted assumptions.

    The most restricted model in the present study, was a

    model were school-related stress predicts health com-

    plaints but where SOC has no effects. This model served

    as a null model to compare the fit of the three stress-

    health mechanisms that SOC was assumed to influence.

    As shown in Table 2, the null model showed a poor fit

    to the data, indicated by a CFI of 0.79. In this model,

    school-related stress was moderately associated with

    health complaints (Standardised path coefficients, 11

    year olds 0.35; 13 year olds 0.38; 15 year olds 0.32; p5

    0:001 for all paths).

    To assess the fit of the three mechanism, each of the

    proposed relationships were added in a stepwise manner.Model 1 included indirect effects only, model 2 included

    indirect and direct effects, and model 3 included indirect,

    direct, and interactive effects on health complaints.

    Model 1 (M1) tested the assumption that SOC

    influences level of stress, but SOC was assumed to have

    no interaction with stress and no main effects on health

    complaints. M1 produced a relatively poor fit to the data

    (CFI=0.84), but the fit was better than the null model

    (Ddf 3, Dw2 814:86, p50:001), suggesting that the

    modelled relationship between SOC and perceived

    school-related stress was consistent with the data.

    Model 2 added the assumption that SOC has a directeffect on health, but SOC was not assumed to interact

    with stress. This model fitted the data well, indicated by

    a CFI of 0.91, and a RMSEA of 0.04. Model 2 fitted the

    data better than model 1 (Ddf 3, Dw2 1289:1,

    p50:001), suggesting that the assumption of a main

    effect of SOC on health complaints was consistent with

    the data.

    Model 3 tested the additional assumption that SOC

    and school-related stress interact in predicting health

    complaints. A significance test of the difference in w2,

    revealed a marginally better fit for model 3 (df 3,

    Dw2 11:74,p50:02), suggesting that also the modelled

    interaction effects fitted the data.

    Invariance across age

    The best-fitting model, model 3, incorporating both

    indirect, direct, and interactive effects of SOC on health

    complaints, was chosen as baseline model for testing

    invariance of structural coefficients across age groups.

    To test if the path coefficients were invariant across age

    groups, constraints of equality were imposed on the

    structural equations.

    Four equality constraints were entered simultaneously:

    Equality across groups for (1) the path coefficientbetween SOC and school-related stress; (2) the path

    coefficient between SOC and subjective health com-

    plaints; (3) the path coefficient between school-related

    stress and subjective health complaints; and (4) the

    path coefficient between the SOC*stress interaction

    factor and subjective health complaints.

    The constrained baseline model had a poorer fit to the

    data than the unconstrained model (Dw2, 31.40, df 8,

    p50:001), suggesting that the assumption of invariant

    path coefficients did not fit the data. To detect which of

    the path coefficients that varied across age groups,

    constraints were released in a sequential manner. The

    T. Torsheim et al. / Social Science & Medicine 53 (2001) 603614 607

  • 8/13/2019 Sense of Coherenece _and School Stress

    6/12

  • 8/13/2019 Sense of Coherenece _and School Stress

    7/12

    school-related stress was different across age. For the

    remaining two path coefficients, release of constraints

    did not improve model fit (models 3d and 3e), which is

    consistent with the assumption of invariance across age

    groups.

    Path coefficients and effect decomposition

    Fig. 1 shows the path coefficients of model 3c.

    Sense of coherence (SOC) was moderately associated

    with school-related stress, with the strongest inverse

    Table 2

    Model summary for the tested models, multisample analysis

    Model df w2 CFI RMSEA Comparison Ddf Dw2 P

    (M0) Null model 273 3985.40 0.79 0.05

    (M1) Stress appraisal 270 3170.54 0.84 0.05 M1 vs M0 3 814.86 50.001

    (M2) Health resistance 267 1881.44 0.91 0.04 M2 vs M1 3 1289.1 50.001(M3) Stress moderation 264 1869.70 0.91 0.04 M3 vs M2 3 11.74 50.02

    Table 3

    Testing constraints of equal path coefficients across groupsa

    Hypothesis df w2 CFI RMSEA Comparison df Dw2 p

    (M3a) Path coefficients equal across groups 272 1901.10 0.91 0.04

    (M3b) SOC!Health complaints6across groups 270 1885.14 0.91 0.04 M3b vs M3a 2 15.96 50.001

    (M3c) SOC! stress6across groups 268 1877.78 0.91 0.04 M3c vs M3b 2 7.36 50.05

    (M3d) Stress moderation6across groups 266 1874.13 0.91 0.04 M3d vs M3c 2 3.65 ns

    (M3e) Stress!Health Complaints6across groups 266 1873.40 0.91 0.04 M3e vs M3c 2 4.38 nsaNote. ns nonsignificant at the 0.05 level.

    Fig. 1. Structural path model for direct, indirect and interactive relationships for SOC and stress on health complaints, multisample

    analysis on 11 year olds, 13 year olds and 15 year olds. aPath coefficient: upper number 11 year olds; middle number 13 year olds; lower

    number 15 year olds. bPath coefficients when no effects of SOC is included (null model).

    T. Torsheim et al. / Social Science & Medicine 53 (2001) 603614 609

  • 8/13/2019 Sense of Coherenece _and School Stress

    8/12

    association among 11 year olds (standardised path

    coefficients: 11 year olds 0.47; 13 year olds 0.41; 15

    year olds 0.40). SOC was inversely related to health

    complaints, with stronger relationship with increasing

    age (standardised path coefficients: 11 year olds 0.59;

    13 year olds 0.67; 15 year olds 0.71; p50:001 for all

    age groups).When controlling for the effects of SOC, the relation-

    ship between school-related stress and health complaints

    was very weak (standardised path coefficients: 11 year

    olds 0.08; 13 year olds 0.07; 15 year olds 0.07; p50:05

    for all age groups).

    An effect decomposition was done to assess the

    relative contribution for the different relationships

    regressed on subjective health complaints. Across age,

    as shown in Table 4, the main effect of SOC accounted

    for an increasing share of variance in health complaints,

    from 39% in the group of 11 year olds up to 54% in the

    group of 15 year olds. The interactive effects of SOC andschool-related stress accounted for between 0.5 and 1%

    of the variance in health complaints.

    Discussion

    In brief, we found that both stress-mediated effects,

    stress-interactive effects, and direct effect of SOC on

    health complaints were consistent with the data.

    SOC and school-related stress

    The reason for investigating the role of SOC was

    twofold. Firstly, a large literature indicate that many

    students perceive school-demands to be stressful. Sec-

    ondly, the variance in these perceptions predicts

    variance in psychological and physical distress. Factors

    that prevent stressful appraisals, or that moderate the

    impact of stressful appraisals may be potential targets

    for preventive action.

    One of the assumptions made by Antonovsky (1987),

    is that individuals with a high SOC will tend to appraise

    demands as predictable and comprehensible. Based on

    Antonovsky, we expected SOC to be inversely related to

    perceived school-related stress. Students with a high

    SOC would be more inclined to view school demands as

    comprehensible and predictable, and less threatening to

    well-being. In line with these expectations moderate-to-

    strong inverse relationships were found in all three age

    groups. The subcomponent of comprehensibility has

    been suggested to be an important factor in stress

    appraisal of recurring demands (Antonovsky, 1987).

    The role of comprehensibility may be particularly

    relevant in relation to school related demands, as theambiguities posed by schoolwork is closely related to a

    lack of comprehension. The expectations of comprehen-

    sible demands may provide a cognitive set that organises

    the appraisal of such demands as they occur.

    The relationship between SOC and school-related

    stress grew weaker with age. With the notion of an

    emerging, and more and more crystallised SOC, this

    pattern was somewhat surprising. One possible explana-

    tion is that academic demands increase over age, and

    become less susceptible to benign appraisals. In the

    context of qualification to higher education, the

    potential threatening aspects of school-demands maybecome more salient. Such contextual factors may

    influence stress appraisals more strongly than the level

    of SOC. As such, the relative contribution of SOC in

    appraisals of school-related stress may become lower

    when contextual factors dominate.

    Several authors have argued that school may be

    regarded as a work setting for adolescents (Rudd &

    Walsh, 1993; Samdal, Nutbeam, Wold, Kannas, 1998a).

    The size of the path coefficients between SOC and

    school-related stress, was strikingly similar to what has

    been found in adult studies for work-related demands

    (Larsson & Setterlind, 1990; Ryland & Greenfeld, 1991).

    If we assume that schoolwork and paid work share

    essential features, such as performance demands and

    time pressure, our findings may reflect that that the

    adolescent SOC operates in much the same way as does

    the adult SOC. While several authors have stressed the

    labile character of the adolescent SOC (e.g. Antonovsky,

    1987; Lundberg, 1997), the parallel findings in this study

    do not support a strong distinction between the function

    of the adolescent and the adult SOC in appraisals of

    work demands.

    Does SOC moderate the impact of school-related stress?

    In the original theoretical formulation SOC is

    assumed to moderate the impact of stress. Individuals

    Table 4

    Effect decomposition of associations with subjective health complaints, best-fitting model (standardised solution)

    Direct effects of SOC Interaction Direct effects of stress Disturbance Total variance

    11 year olds 0.3944 0.0119 0.0060 0.5883 1.001a

    13 year olds 0.4886 0.0079 0.0046 0.4998 1.001

    15 year olds 0.5388 0.0055 0.0042 0.4529 1.001

    a

    Total variance exceeds 1.0 due to rounding errors.

    T. Torsheim et al. / Social Science & Medicine 53 (2001) 603614610

  • 8/13/2019 Sense of Coherenece _and School Stress

    9/12

    high on SOC would expect stressors to be manageable.

    According to cognitive activation theory (Ursin &

    Hytten, 1992) such expectations may prevent the

    initiation of harmful activation during stress.

    The model that included interaction effects fitted the

    data better than the model including main effects only.

    For students with a high SOC, school-related stress wasless strongly associated with health complaints. It is

    tempting to interpret this in support for a stress-

    moderating role for SOC, but several methodological

    factors speaks against such an interpretation. When

    judging the present interaction-effects one has to

    consider that the study employed a method that

    minimises measurement error in the interaction term.

    The interactive effects were still only marginal, account-

    ing for between 0.5 and 1% of the true variance in health

    complaints. Notably, the main effect of SOC on health

    complaints was strong. As suggested in the seminal

    paper by Baron and Kenny (1986), interaction effectsmay be difficult to interpret when the assumed

    moderator has a strong main effect on the outcome.

    From this point of view, it seems inappropriate to

    interpret the interaction effect as moderation. In any

    case, taking into account that other studies have shown

    mixed findings of moderation effects, it seems reasonable

    not to overstate the substantive significance of the

    present interaction effects.

    The weak interaction effects found in our study are in

    line with the bulk of studies reporting weak or non-

    significant interaction effects between SOC, stress and

    health outcomes (Anson et al., 1990; Feldt, 1997;

    Vahtera et al., 1996). For studies of stress-moderation

    in general, weak or absent interaction effects, has been

    explained with reference to lack of statistical power, and

    amplification of measurement error (Busemeyer &

    Jones, 1983). However, in the case of SOC, the weak

    interaction may also reside in the generality of the SOC

    construct. In social support research, a key issue has

    been that global support may not be able to match

    specific stressors (Gore & Aseltine, 1995). Thus, global

    resources should not produce strong moderation of

    domain-specific stress. The stressor-resource matching

    argument could also apply to SOC. The point that sense

    of coherence is not a particular coping style, orbehavioural trait (Antonovsky, 1987), suggests that

    SOC may moderate stress through domain-specific

    response outcome beliefs, but these response outcome

    beliefs may be affected by a host of other situational

    factors. The moderating effect of SOC on stress, may

    thus be levelled out in specific situations. While global

    expectancies of being able to manage stress, may be

    relevant in encounters with school-related stressors,

    school-specific efficacy and skills may be even more

    instrumental in shaping coping expectancies. In future

    studies, instead of trying to establish moderation effects

    of SOC on domain-specific stressors, it may well prove

    to be more fruitful to establish main effects between

    SOC and domain-specific self-efficacy and outcome

    beliefs.

    SOC and subjective health complaints

    The effect decomposition revealed that most of thevariance accounted for by SOC could be attributed to

    the direct relationship between SOC and health com-

    plaints. In fact, the direct effects of SOC accounted for

    almost half of the variance in health complaints. The

    indirect and interactive relationships only accounted for

    a marginal proportion of variance.

    The strong main effect of SOC is open to several

    interpretations. According to Antonovsky, people with a

    high SOC use more effective coping strategies, and will

    always be moving from a state of tension to non-tension.

    In a more speculative vein, Antonovsky (1987) proposes

    that SOC affects the tendency for the physiologicalsystem to enter disequilibrium. Interestingly, this hy-

    pothesis converge with recent contributions that

    view subjective health complaints as a physiological

    sensitisation phenomenon (Ursin, 1997). The strong

    association between SOC and health complaints,

    controlling for level of stress, could reflect that

    individuals with a high SOC show a resistance to such

    sensitisation.

    The strong associations do however bring forward the

    issue of a conceptual or methodological confound

    between SOC and subjective health complaints. Several

    authors have argued that measures of SOC in part

    reflect negative affectivity or negative mood dispositions

    (Geyer, 1997; Korotkov, 1993). According to symptom

    perception theory, negative affectivity is associated with

    high introspection and low threshold for symptom

    perception (Watson & Pennebaker, 1989). Conse-

    quently, associations between SOC and health com-

    plaints could reflect common influence of negative

    affectivity. However, as argued by Stru mpfer and

    colleagues (1998), the association between SOC and

    negative affectivity may not primarily be an issue

    of methodological confounding. If low negative affec-

    tivity is viewed as emotional stability, the strong

    associations may well been seen as a validation of thestress-resistance component in SOC (Stru mpfer et al.,

    1998).

    While a high SOC may be a part of a larger

    adaptation pattern that includes low negative affectivity,

    SOC is not necessarily the same as negative affectivity

    (Stru mpfer et al., 1998). One possible interpretation is

    that SOC represents the cognitive-motivational mani-

    festation of depression, and not the affective component

    per se. Cognitive models on depression (e.g. Beck, 1974)

    indicate that depressive beliefs causes affective states.

    The strong association with health complaints could

    mean that a low SOC reflects the depressed belief-system

    T. Torsheim et al. / Social Science & Medicine 53 (2001) 603614 611

  • 8/13/2019 Sense of Coherenece _and School Stress

    10/12

    that sustain a high arousal, which must be clearly

    distinguished from theeffects of such arousal.

    Alternative models

    The present paper investigates causal models for the

    relationship between stress, SOC and subjective healthcomplaints, but the crossectional design does not allow

    for any firm conclusions regarding causality. This

    limitation needs to be particularly addressed when using

    structural equation modelling. While the results are

    consistent with the general mechanisms proposed by

    Antonovsky, several alternative models could fit the

    data equally well.

    As a model worth considering, developmental per-

    spectives suggest that SOC may mediate school-related

    stress. Load experiences from social role performance,

    have been assumed to have a strong impact on the

    development of SOC (Cederblad et al., 1994; Antonovs-ky, 1991). According to this view, experiences of

    persistent overload or underload, is associated with the

    development of a low SOC, whereas experiences of

    balanced load, promote a strong SOC. Following this

    argument, the adolescent SOC could be seen as

    influenced by school-related stress, rather than as

    predicting school-related stress.

    Though conceptually very different, the SOC-as-

    outcome model would fit the present data equally well

    as the models that were tested in our study. The problem

    of interpretation this leaves us with does not change the

    fact that there is a robust covariance structure between

    SOC, school-related stress and subjective health com-

    plaints. Future prospective studies could benefit from

    adopting a transactional perspective on the relationship

    between SOC and stress, where reciprocal effects can be

    modelled.

    Conclusion

    The aim of the present paper was to assess the role of

    SOC in adolescents health adaptation to school-related

    stress. The results provide some support for the general

    stress-health mechanisms that Antonovsky formulated.While the present study did not address stability of SOC,

    the strong cross-sectional consistency across samples,

    indirectly point to a degree of stability in SOC also in

    adolescence. As such the present study underscores the

    need to adopt a life-span perspective when examining

    the role of sense of coherence in health.

    Importantly, the present findings were established in

    relation to a kind of stress and health complaints that

    the general population of adolescents are exposed to

    during socialisation into adulthood. The cross-sectional

    design does not permit us to draw strong practical

    implications from the study, but the study does take a

    first step in providing a conceptual framework for how

    salutogenic factors may intervene on adolescent adapta-

    tion to school-related demands.

    Acknowledgements

    The authors would like to thank Candace Currie for

    preparation of the research protocol of the Health

    Behaviour in School-Aged Children 1997/98 survey.

    The writing of the manuscript was made possible

    through a doctoral grant from the Norwegian Research

    Council, division of medicine and health.

    Appendix A. The age-adapted SOC-13

    Here is a series of questions relating to various aspects

    of our lives. Each question has five possible answers. For

    each question, please mark the answer which bestexpresses your feelings about your life.

    1. (Me) How often do you have the feeling that you

    dont really care about what goes on around you?1

    2. (C )How often has it happened in the past that you

    were surprised by the behavior of people who you

    thought you knew well?

    3. (Ma) How often has it happened that people whom

    you counted on disappointed you?

    4. (Me) How do you think you are going to feel about

    the things you will do in the future?

    5. (Ma)How often do you have the feeling that you arebeing treated unfairly?

    6. (C) How often do you have the feeling that you are

    in a unfamiliar situation and dont know what to

    do?

    7. (Me) How do you feel about the things you do

    every day?

    8. (C) How often does it happen that you dont quite

    understand your own feelings and ideas?

    9. (C) How often does it happen that you have feelings

    inside that you would rather not feel?

    10. (Ma) Many people-even those with a strong

    character- sometimes feel like losers in certain

    situations. How often have you felt this way inthe past?

    11. (C) How often does it happen that you have the

    feeling that you dont know exactly whats about to

    happen?

    12. (Me) How often do you have the feeling that there

    is little meaning in the things you do in your daily

    life?

    1Response keys for all questions except no.4 and no. 7 were:

    Very often } Often } Sometimes } Seldom } Never. For

    no.4 and no.7 response keys were : Like it a lot } Like it } Its

    OK}

    Dont like it}

    Dont like it all.

    T. Torsheim et al. / Social Science & Medicine 53 (2001) 603614612

  • 8/13/2019 Sense of Coherenece _and School Stress

    11/12

    13. (Ma)How often do you have feelings that youre

    not sure you can keep under control.

    Appendix B. The HBSC school-related stress subscale

    Do you agree or disagree with the following

    statements? (strongly agree } agree } neither nor }

    disagree } strongly disagree)

    V1. I have too much schoolwork.

    V2. I find schoolwork difficult.

    V3. I find schoolwork tiring.

    References

    Anson, O., Carmel, S., Levenson, A., Bonneh, D., & Maoz, B.

    (1993). Coping with recent life events: The interplay ofpersonal and collective resources. Behavioral Medicine, 18,

    159166.

    Antelman, S. M., Soares, J. C., & Gershon, S. (1997). Time-

    dependent sensitization-possible implications for clinical

    psychopharmacology. Behavioural Pharmacology, 8, 505

    514.

    Antonovsky, A. (1987). Unraveling the mystery of health. How

    people manage stress and stay well. San Fransisco, CA:

    Jossey-Bass Publ.

    Antonovsky, A. (1991). The structural sources of salutogenic

    strengths. In C. L. Cooper, & R. Payne (Eds.), Personality

    and stress: Individual differences in the stress process (pp. 67

    104). London: Wiley.

    Antonovsky, A. (1993). The structure and properties of thesense of coherence scale. Social Science and Medicine, 36,

    725733.

    Antonovsky, H., & Sagy, S. (1986). The development of a sense

    of coherence and its impact on responses to stress situations.

    The Journal of Social Psychology, 126, 213225.

    Aro, H., Paronen, O., & Aro, S. (1987). Psychosomatic

    symptoms among 1416 year old Finnish adolescents.

    Social Psychiatry, 22, 171176.

    Baker, L. K. (1998). Sense of coherence in adolescents with

    cystic fibrosis. In H. I. McCubbin, E. A. Thompson, A. I.

    Thompson, & J. I. Fromer (Eds.), Stress, coping, and health

    in families. Sense of coherence and resiliency (pp. 107124).

    Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action.

    New Jersey: Prentice Hall.

    Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator

    variable distinction in social psychological reasearch:

    Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal

    of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 11731182.

    Beck, A. T. (1974). Cognition, affect, and psychopathology. In

    H. London, & R. E. Nisbett (Eds.), Thought and feeling(pp.

    127140). Chicago, IL: Aldine Publishing Company.

    Bentler, P. M. (1995). EQS structural equations program

    manual. Encino CA: Multivariate Software Inc..

    Busemeyer, J. R., & Jones, L. E. (1983). Analysis of multi-

    plicative combination rules when the causal variables are

    measured with error. Psychological Bulletin, 93, 549562.

    Cederblad, M., Dahlin, L., Hagnell, O., & Hansson, K. (1994).

    Salutogenic childhood factors reported by middle-aged

    individuals: Follow-up of the children experiencing three

    or more childhood psychiatric risk factors. European

    Archives of Clinical Neuroscience, 244, 111.

    Currie, C. (1998). Health behaviour in school-aged children.

    Research protocol for the 1997/98 Survey. University ofEdinburgh.

    Dubner, R., & Ruda, M. A. (1992). Activity dependent

    neuronal plasticity following tissue injury and inflamma-

    tion. Trends in Neurosciences, 15, 96103.

    Eme, R., Maisak, R., & Goodale, W. (1979). Seriousness of

    adolescent problems. Adolescence, 14, 9399.

    Feldt, T. (1997). The role of sense of coherence in well-being at

    work: Analysis of main and moderator effects. Work and

    Stress, 11, 134147.

    Feldt, T., & Rasku, A. (1998). The structure of Antonovskys

    orientation to life questionnaire. Personality and Individual

    Differences, 25, 505516.

    Flannery, R. B., & Flannery, G. J. (1990). Sense of coherence,

    life stress, and psychological distress: a prospective

    methodological study. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 46,

    415420.

    Garralda, M. E. (1996). Somatisation in children. Journal of

    Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 1333.

    Geyer, S. (1997). Some conceptual considerations on the sense

    of coherence. Social Science and Medicine, 44, 17711779.

    Goodman, J. E., & McGrath, P. (1991). The epidemiology of

    pain in children and adolescents: A review.Pain,46, 247264.

    Gore, S., & Aseltine, R. H. (1995). Protective processes in

    adolescence: Matching stressors with social resources.

    American Journal of Community Psychology, 23, 301327.

    Greene, A. L. (1988). Early adolescents perception of stress.

    Journal of Early Adolescence, 8, 391403.Henker, B., Whalen, C. K., & ONeil, R. (1995). Worldly and

    workaday worries: Contemporary concerns of children and

    young adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,

    23, 685702.

    Hurrelmann, K., Engel, U., Holler, B., & Nordlohne, E. (1988).

    Failure in school, family conflicts, and psychosomatic

    disorders in adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 11, 237

    249.

    Jaccard, J., & Wan, C. K. (1995). Measurement error in the

    analysis of interaction effects between continuous predictors

    using multiple regression: Multiple indicator and structural

    equation approaches. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 348357.

    Kenny, D. A., & Judd, C. M. (1984). Estimating the non-linear

    and interactive effects of latent variables. Psychological

    Bulletin, 96, 201210.

    King, A., Wold, B., Tudor-Smith, C., Harel, Y. (1996). The

    health of youth. A cross-national survey. WHO Regional

    Publications, 69.

    Kobasa, S. C. (1979). Stressful life events, personality, and

    health: An inquiry into hardiness.Journal of Personality and

    Social psychology, 37, 111.

    Korotkov, D. L. (1993). An assessment of the (short-form)

    sense of coherence personality measure: Issues of validity

    and well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 14,

    575583.

    Larsson, G., & Setterlind, S. (1990). Work load/work control

    and health: Moderating effects of heredity, self image and

    T. Torsheim et al. / Social Science & Medicine 53 (2001) 603614 613

  • 8/13/2019 Sense of Coherenece _and School Stress

    12/12

    health behavior.International Journal of Health Sciences, 1,

    7988.

    Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal and

    coping. New York: Springer Publishing Company.

    Lundberg, O. (1997). Childhood conditions, sense of coherence,

    social class and adult ill health: Exploring their theoretical

    and empirical relations. Social Science and Medicine, 44,821831.

    Margalit, M., & Efrati, M. (1996). Loneliness, coherence and

    companionship among children with learning disorders.

    Educational Psychology, 16, 6979.

    Mikkelsson, M., Salminen, J., & Kautiainen, H. (1997). Non-

    specific muskuloskeletal pain in pre-adolescents. Prevalence

    and 1-year persistence. Pain, 73, 2935.

    Ping, R. A. (1996). Latent variable interaction and quadratic

    effect estimation. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 166175.

    Ping, R. A. (1998). EQS and LISREL examples using survey

    data. In R. E. Schumacker, & G. A. Marcoulides (Eds.),

    Interaction and nonlinear effects in structural equation

    modeling(pp. 43100). London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associ-

    ates.Rudd, R. E., & Walsh, D. C. (1993). Schools as healthful

    environments: Prerequisite to comprehensive school health?

    Preventive Medicine, 22, 499506.

    Ryland, E., & Greenfeld, S. (1991). Work stress and well being:

    An investigation of Antonovskys Sense of Coherence

    model.Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 6, 3954.

    Samdal, O., Nutbeam, D., Wold, B., & Kannas, L. (1998a).

    Achieving health and educational goals through schools: A

    study of the importance of school climate and students

    satisfaction with school. Health Education Research, 13,

    383397.

    Samdal, O., Wold, B., & Torsheim, T. (1998b). The relationship

    between students perception of school and their reported

    health and quality of life. In C. Currie (Ed.), Health

    behaviour in school-aged children. Research protocol for the

    199798 survey (pp. 5155). Edinburgh: University of

    Edinburgh.

    Stru mpfer, D. J. W., Gouws, J. F., & Viviers, M. R. (1998).

    Antonovskys sense of coherence scale related to negative

    and positive affectivity.European Journal of Personality,12,

    457480.

    Torsheim, T., & Wold, B. (1998). Adolescent coping and health:

    Sense of coherence. In C. Currie (Ed.), Health behaviour in

    school-aged children. Research protocol for the 199798Survey (pp. 7983). Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh.

    Ursin, H. (1988). Expectancy and activation: An attempt to

    systematize stress theory. In I. Florin, & H. Weiner (Eds.),

    Neurobiological Approaches to Human Disease (pp. 313

    334). Toronto: Hans Huber.

    Ursin, H., & Hytten, K. (1992). Outcome expectancies and

    psychosomatic consequences. In B. N. Carpenter (Ed.),

    Personal coping. Theory, Research and Application(pp. 171

    184). Westport: Praeger publishers.

    Ursin, H. (1997). Sensitization, somatization, and subjective

    health complaints. International Journal of Behavioral

    Medicine, 4, 105116.

    Vahtera, J., Pennti, J., & Uutela, A. (1996). The effect of

    objective job demands on registered sickness absence spells;do personal, social and job-related resources act as

    moderators?.Work and Stress, 10, 286308.

    Wagner, B., & Compas, B. E. (1990). Gender, instrumentality,

    and expressivity: Moderators of the relation between stress

    and psychological symptoms during adolescence. American

    Journal of Community Psychology, 18, 383406.

    Watson, D., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Health complaints,

    stress and distress: Exploring the central role of negative

    affectivity. Psychological Review, 96, 234254.

    Woolf, C. J., & Thompson, W. J. (1991). The induction and

    maintenance of central sensitisation is dependent on N-

    methyl-D-aspartatic acid receptor activation: implications

    for the treatment of post-injury pain hypersensitivity states.

    Pain, 44, 293299.

    Ystgaard, M. (1997). Life stress, social support and psycholo-

    gical distress in late adolescence. Social Psychiatry and

    Epidemiology, 32, 277283.

    T. Torsheim et al. / Social Science & Medicine 53 (2001) 603614614