Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

26
Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion Representing variation The view from phonological theory Marc van Oostendorp Meertens Instituut Reading Tobias Scheer

Transcript of Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Page 1: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Representing variationThe view from phonological theory

Marc van Oostendorp

Meertens Instituut

Reading Tobias Scheer

Page 2: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Where is syntactic variation?

• In syntactic theory, there has been a lively debate aboutthe locus of variation

• Roughly two positions:• Variation is in the computational system (’parameters’)• The computational system is universal; variation is in the

representations, specifically in the choice of features infunctional categories (the ’Chomsky-Borer’ hypothesis)

• The second hypothesis is attractive; it leaves computationuniversal, so that it does not have to be learned. the onlything that needs to be learned is the lexicon (and that isnecessary anyway).

Page 3: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Where is phonological variation?

• Phonologists have always been empirically interested invariation (possibly more so than syntacticians)

• Yet they do not seem to participate much in the discussionon the locus of variation in syntactic theory.

• There are roughly three positions, but all of them putvariation in the computational component:

• Different systems correspond to different rule systems(SPE, variation in individual rules, their ordering; etc.) plusthe relevant representations.

• Parameter theory (e.g. Hayes’ work in the 1980s on stress;Government Phonology)

• Constraint ranking (OT, Harmonic Grammar)

Page 4: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Learning phonology: an exercise

• One thing a child definitely has to learn while acquiringphonology is: which phonetic properties are contrastive?

• In other words, she has to learn what the phonologicalprimitives are (features, elements)

• What if we assume that this is the only thing the childlearns: the set of features and possibly some properties ofthose features?

• In syntax, the primitives are functional items, i.e.morphosyntactic feature bundles (I think); in phonology itwould be elements or features.

• That would lead to something similar to theBorer-Chomsky Conjecture in phonology.

Page 5: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

The set-up of the grammar

• The idea is that computation is universal.• I assume this means that there is a set of operations such

as autosegmental linking and delinking, projection ofprosodic structure, etc.

• These apply “whenever their conditions are met” (across-linguistic generalization of Kaye’s MinimalistHypothesis, that is usually supposed to be true within agiven language)

Page 6: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Two case studies

• Vowel harmony• Final devoicing

Page 7: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Turkic vowels

1 ∅ i {FRONT}a {LOW} e {FRONT, LOW}u {ROUND} y {FRONT, ROUND}o {LOW, ROUND} ø {FRONT, LOW, ROUND}

(work with Kathrin Linke)

Page 8: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Building Block 1: Licensing Constraints

• Charette and Goksel (1996) observe that the patterns ofVowel Harmony follow the same ‘licensing constraints’ asthe underlying inventory in Turkic.

• However, their background assumption is that there is auniversal generator which takes all the available elementsand combines them freely, and then there is a set oflanguage-specific constraints which rules out certaincombinations both syntagmatically and paradigmatically

• Furthermore, presumably the way in which theparadigmatic relations are established is probably alsolanguage-specific.

• Variation is still mostly in the computation; the elementsstay the same.

Page 9: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Building Block 2: Needy Vowels

• Nevins (2010) argues that Vowel Harmony is triggered bythe target vowel, which is ‘needy’, i.e. it does not have avalue for a (binary) feature, and it is specified as needingone.

• This creates the possibility that VH is a universal process,which however only applies if a vowel is needy. Somelanguages just do not have needy vowels.

• Since Nevins works in a binary feature framework, there isno specific prediction as to which features can be needy.

Page 10: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Building Block 3: Emptyness as a condition on VowelHarmony

• Pochtrager (2010) shows that disharmonic roots in Turkishobey certain restrictions, which mostly have to do withemptyness: I elements only spread to empty-headedpositions, U only to empty expressions (a famous paper byClements and Sezer 1982 makes similar observations)

• Pochtrager notes that a problem of ordering arises (if Ispreading applies first, this evacuates U spreading), but heassumes that spreading is just a matter of (simultaneousinterpretation): the target stays empty

• This makes (phonetic?) interpretation of phonological intosomething that is variable / the object of parametricvariation.

Page 11: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Building Block 4: Strength of individual segments

• Inkelas (ms) proposes that learners give an individual‘strength’ index to segments on a word-by-word basis. Thestrength corresponds to the confidence that a learner hasabout the identity of such a segment.

• Typically, the strength will be higher for segments withinmorphemes than at edges; that explains (certain) DEE

• Inkelas proposes a ranking FAITHstrong�M�FAITHweak forsuch situations

• Notice that this ranking could be universal: learners of alanguage in which M is completely irrelevant learn that allsegments are strong, learners of a language in which Malways apply, make all segments weak

Page 12: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Putting the blocks together

• We accept that a language consists of unary elements thatcan be freely combined into segments

• We accept that VH is a matter of ‘needy vowels’• We propose that emptiness leads to neediness (a

completely full vowel is never needy)• The issue is: what is neediness?

(There are some similarities also with Van der Hulst 2012;which may hopefully be discussed during the discussionperiod.)

Page 13: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Feature Cooccurrence Requirements

We have to assume that features can have the followingproperties

• F⊃G: the feature F ‘needs’ the feature G• *{F,G}: the feature F ‘repels’ the feature G

Note that this means that features have properties (just like‘uninterpretable’ etc. features in classical minimalism). This issomething which needs to be formalized. I assume that actuallyonly features can have such properties, not segments.(I will from now on talk about features, just because that is morein line with syntactic parlance.)

Page 14: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Vowel harmony

nom.sg. gloss gen.sg. nom.pl.k1z ‘girl’ k1z-1n k1z-larip ‘rope’ ip-in ip-lersap ‘stalk’ sap-1n sap-larel ‘hand’ el-in el-ler

pul ‘stamp’ pul-un pul-larjyz ‘face’ jyz-yn jyz-lerson ‘end’ son-un son-larkøj ‘village’ køj-yn køj-ler

Page 15: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Spreading properties

We have to assume that Turkish features have the followingproperties

• V⊃FRONT

• V⊃ROUND

• FRONT and ROUND need lexical support (cannot be justinserted to satisfy the needs of V)

Spreading of features would be a universal process, but subjectto the properties of the vowels in question.The third property might be a universal (the Non-ArbitrarinessCondition of Government Phonology)The result is that empty vowels will start attracting FRONT andROUND whenever they can; but they will stay needy on thesurface if those features are not available.

Page 16: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Final Devoicing

• Catalan:• gris ‘grey (M)’ - griz@ ‘grey (F)’• gos ‘dog (M)’ - gos@ ‘dog (F)’

• Dutch:• kwaa[t] ‘angry (PRED.)’ - kwad@ ‘angry (ATT)’• laat ‘late (PRED.)’ - lat@ ‘late (ATT)’

• German:• Rad ‘wheel (NOM. SG.)’ - Rades ‘wheel (GEN.SG.)’• Rat ‘council (NOM.SG.)’ - Rates ‘council (GEN.SG.)’

(work with Bjorn Kohnlein)

Page 17: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Two Romance dialects

• Standard French has obligatory devoicing (only) beforevoiceless obstruents: distin[kt]tif (distin[g]uer), su[pt]ropical(su[b]alpin), pro[St]er (pro[Z]et), a[ps]or[ps]ion (absor[b]er),la[ts]us (par[de]sus), but [regArd] ‘look’

• Walloon: wade-lu ‘keep it’ [wO:tly] vs. wad’-ler ‘to supportmine walls with billets’ [wOdle] (Liege)

(Van Oostendorp, to appear)

Page 18: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Final Devoicing as a property of [Voice]

• FINDEV: [Voice] needs to be in an onset.

Page 19: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

laide

French Walloonσ

O

l

N

E

O

d

N

[voice]

σ σ

O

l

N

E

C

t

Page 20: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Independent evidence

• The most important argument in favour of the assumptionthat word final consonants are onsets in French, is thatthey can form clusters

• However, these clusters have been simplified in Walloon• This gives strong support to the assumption that they are

syllabified differently, i.e. in codas

underlying form isolation prevocalic/trist/ [tris] ‘sad’ [tristEs] ‘sadness’/mEspl/ [mEs] ‘meddlar’ [mEspli:] ‘meddlar-tree’

Page 21: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

What’s the difference?

• Are (final) empty nuclei allowed? (the Piggott parameter)• This again can be formalized as a property of a feature

(e.g. V⊃F)

Page 22: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Four West Germanic languages

• Dutch: /rad/→ [rat] (voicing distinction, FD)• German: /rat/→ [rath] (spread glottis distinction, FD)• Yiddish: /rOd/→ [rOd] (voicing distinction, no FD)• English: /bæt/→ [bæt] (spread glottis distinction, no FD)

Page 23: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Four West Germanic languages: Voicing assimilation

• Dutch: /frœyt/+/bo:m/→ [frœydbo:m] (VA)• German: /opst/+/paum/→ [opsthpaum] (no VA)• Yiddish: /bAk/+/bein/→ [bagbein] (VA)• English: /bæk/+/bon/→ [bækbone] (no VA)

Page 24: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Analysis

• Dutch and German have a syllable structure like Walloon• English and Yiddish have a syllable structure like French• Dutch and Yiddish have a feature VOICE

• English and German have a feature SPREAD

• Coda consonants are needy for any laryngeal feature• VOICE is needy for onsets• SPREAD can be provided by the end of the word

Page 25: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Projection

• We encounter a specific type of neediness, i.e. needinessof a feature for a certain position.

• This has been formalized in Van Oostendorp (1995, 2000)as ‘projection’: certain features project to certain prosodicpositions and vice versa. In that work, however, projectionis a (violable) constraint of the grammar

• We need to reconceptualize it as a property of the feature

Page 26: Representing variation - The view from phonological theory

Introduction Vowel harmony Final devoicing Conclusion

Conclusion

• I have presented an exercise in finding a more preciselocus of variation in phonology

• The idea is that the only thing which is language-specific isthe set of features and their properties; everything elsefollows from them

• Under this model, the acquisition task would be restrictedto learning the set of phonological features and theirproperties (in particular, neediness and repulsion)

• An issue: we have to stipulate ‘properties of features’, oftwo types: relations to other features, and projection toprosodic structure.

• It is not immediately clear what this means, and to whatextent this can be extended to other domains (e.g. stressshould be seen entirely as projection of features).