Poster Print Size: Speech Perception in Noise Using Non ...€¦ · Aids: Phonak Naida IX BTEs, set...

1
Speech Perception in Noise Using Non-Linear Frequency Compression with a Cognitive Load Task James Shehorn, AuD/PhD Student, Nicole Marrone, PhD, and Thomas Muller, AuD Department of Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences James Shehorn Email: [email protected] Phone: (520) 626 - 8549 Contact 1. Gatehouse, S., Naylor, G., & Elberling, C. (2003). Benefits from hearing aids in relation to the interaction between the user and the environment. Int J Audiol, 42 (Suppl 1): S77-S85. 2. Kalluri, S. & Humes (2012). Hearing Technology and Cognition. Amer J Audiol, 21(2): 338-343. 3. Simpson, A. (2009). Frequency-lowering devices for managing high-frequency hearing loss: A review. Trends in Amp, 13(2): 87-106. 4. Alexander, J.M. (2013). Individual variability in recognition of frequency-lowered speech. Semin Hear, 34: 86-109. 5. Arehart, K. H., Souza, P., Baca, R., & Kates, J. M. (2013). Working Memory, Age, and Hearing Loss: Susceptibility to Hearing Aid Distortion. Ear and Hear; (Jan):3 6. Pichora‐Fuller, M. K., Schneider, B. A., & Daneman, M. (1995). How young and old adults listen to and remember speech in noise. J Acoust Soc Am, 97: 593. 7. Sarampalis, A., Kalluri, S., Edwards, B., & Hafter, E. (2009). Objective measures of listening effort: Effects of background noise and noise reduction. JSLHR, 52(5): 1230. 8. Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. J Verb Learn Verb Beh, 19(4): 450-466. References Test Environment: Signal and noise presented from 0º azimuth. Loudspeaker 1m from listener in 3.65 m x 3.65 m (12’ x 12’) soundbooth. Hearing Aids: Phonak Naida IX BTEs, set to omnidirectional mode with all noise management off, fit using real-ear verification to NAL-NL2 targets. Two listening programs (non-linear frequency compression “On” or “Off”) individually fit using manufacturer’s software; blinded to the listener. “Cognitive Load Task”: Adaptation of procedures of Pichora-Fuller et al. (1995) and Sarampalis et al. (2009). SNR set individually. Methods Although the sample size is small, it appears that greater benefit from frequency compression may be observed when cognitive demand is increased either by reducing context or requiring more of working memory. Individuals may demonstrate no benefit on one task and substantial benefit on another, making it difficult to define the efficacy of frequency compression from performance on only one task. An individual listener’s working memory capacity appears to be predictive of benefit from frequency compression for our recall task and low-level speech recognition task. These data add to the growing literature suggesting the need for fitting protocols that take into account individuals’ cognitive processing capabilities. These might include measurement of working memory. Discussion Introduction Within cognitive hearing science, an emerging area of research is how cognitive factors influence hearing aid outcomes. 1,2 Non-linear frequency compression is a signal processing strategy that compresses (lowers) high-frequency acoustic input above a certain cut-off frequency. 3 While increasingly available in the U.S., there is inconclusive evidence regarding the effectiveness of this strategy, with wide individual variability reported. 4 A recent study suggests that people with low working memory capacity perform more poorly with frequency compression than those with higher capacity. 5 The purpose of this study is to examine the efficacy of non-linear frequency compression across multiple outcome measures with varying cognitive and acoustic complexity. Results Aided Speech Recognition in Quiet Aided Speech Recognition in Noise (Dual-task) Aided Recall Speech in Noise (Dual-task) Recognition in Quiet Conversational Level Recognition in Quiet Low-level† Recognition in Noise (Dual-task) High Context Recognition in Noise (Dual-task) Low Context* Recall in Noise Dual Task*† 11 adults (age 64-85 years) with symmetric sensorineural hearing loss Low-level (40-55 dBA) Individual Differences Example: Spectrogram of the word “SCIENCE” R 2 = 0.09, p = 0.37 60 40 20 Reading Span Score Age (years) 70 75 80 85 60 65 Frequency (Hz) Time (sec) Second International Conference on Cognitive Hearing Science for Communication, June 16-19, 2013, Linköping, Sweden Data Collection Summary Cognitive Demand Lower Higher Speech Recognition in Quiet AZ Bio sentences 70 dBA 40-55 dB A Speech Recognition in Noise R-SPIN Dual-task High context Low context Speech Recall R-SPIN Dual-task 6 words ADDITIONAL MEASURES: Audiogram, Reading span (a measure of working memory capacity; Daneman & Carpenter, 1980) Participants High context Low context Conversational level (70 dBA) Listeners sorted by Reading Span Score Reading Span Score: 4 8 13 13 22 22 31 33 40 58 Benefit of frequency compression was observed for the low context speech in noise recognition and recall, however, not all subjects experienced benefit for those tasks. *P<0.05 RMANOVA main effect of freq comp †P<0.05 Correlation between reading span and outcome (No significant interaction effect)

Transcript of Poster Print Size: Speech Perception in Noise Using Non ...€¦ · Aids: Phonak Naida IX BTEs, set...

  • Poster Print Size: This poster template is 48” high by 36” wide. It can be used to print any poster with a 4:3 aspect ratio.

    Placeholders: The various elements included in this poster are ones we often see in medical, research, and scientific posters. Feel free to edit, move, add, and delete items, or change the layout to suit your needs. Always check with your conference organizer for specific requirements.

    Image Quality: You can place digital photos or logo art in your poster file by selecting the Insert, Picture command, or by using standard copy & paste. For best results, all graphic elements should be at least 150-200 pixels per inch in their final printed size. For instance, a 1600 x 1200 pixel photo will usually look fine up to 8“-10” wide on your printed poster.

    To preview the print quality of images, select a magnification of 100% when previewing your poster. This will give you a good idea of what it will look like in print. If you are laying out a large poster and using half-scale dimensions, be sure to preview your graphics at 200% to see them at their final printed size.

    Please note that graphics from websites (such as the logo on your hospital's or university's home page) will only be 72dpi and not suitable for printing.

    [This sidebar area does not print.]

    Change Color Theme: This template is designed to use the built-in color themes in the newer versions of PowerPoint.

    To change the color theme, select the Design tab, then select the Colors drop-down list.

    The default color theme for this template is “Office”, so you can always return to that after trying some of the alternatives.

    Printing Your Poster: Once your poster file is ready, visit www.genigraphics.com to order a high-quality, affordable poster print. Every order receives a free design review and we can delivery as fast as next business day within the US and Canada.

    Genigraphics® has been producing output from PowerPoint® longer than anyone in the industry; dating back to when we helped Microsoft® design the PowerPoint software.

    US and Canada: 1-800-790-4001

    Email: [email protected]

    [This sidebar area does not print.]

    Speech Perception in Noise Using Non-Linear Frequency Compression with a Cognitive Load Task James Shehorn, AuD/PhD Student, Nicole Marrone, PhD, and Thomas Muller, AuD

    Department of Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences

    James Shehorn Email: [email protected] Phone: (520) 626 - 8549

    Contact 1. Gatehouse, S., Naylor, G., & Elberling, C. (2003). Benefits from hearing aids in relation to the interaction between the user and the

    environment. Int J Audiol, 42 (Suppl 1): S77-S85. 2. Kalluri, S. & Humes (2012). Hearing Technology and Cognition. Amer J Audiol, 21(2): 338-343. 3. Simpson, A. (2009). Frequency-lowering devices for managing high-frequency hearing loss: A review. Trends in Amp, 13(2): 87-106. 4. Alexander, J.M. (2013). Individual variability in recognition of frequency-lowered speech. Semin Hear, 34: 86-109. 5. Arehart, K. H., Souza, P., Baca, R., & Kates, J. M. (2013). Working Memory, Age, and Hearing Loss: Susceptibility to Hearing Aid Distortion.

    Ear and Hear; (Jan):3 6. Pichora‐Fuller, M. K., Schneider, B. A., & Daneman, M. (1995). How young and old adults listen to and remember speech in noise. J Acoust

    Soc Am, 97: 593. 7. Sarampalis, A., Kalluri, S., Edwards, B., & Hafter, E. (2009). Objective measures of listening effort: Effects of background noise and noise

    reduction. JSLHR, 52(5): 1230. 8. Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. J Verb Learn Verb Beh, 19(4): 450-466.

    References

    Test Environment: Signal and noise presented from 0º azimuth. Loudspeaker 1m from listener in 3.65 m x 3.65 m (12’ x 12’) soundbooth.

    Hearing Aids: Phonak Naida IX BTEs, set to omnidirectional mode with all noise management off, fit using real-ear verification to NAL-NL2 targets. Two listening programs (non-linear frequency compression “On” or “Off”) individually fit using manufacturer’s software; blinded to the listener.

    “Cognitive Load Task”: Adaptation of procedures of Pichora-Fuller et al. (1995) and Sarampalis et al. (2009). SNR set individually.

    Methods

    • Although the sample size is small, it appears that greater benefit from frequency compression may be observed when cognitive demand is increased either by reducing context or requiring more of working memory.

    • Individuals may demonstrate no benefit on one task and substantial benefit on another, making it difficult to define the efficacy of frequency compression from performance on only one task.

    • An individual listener’s working memory capacity appears to be predictive of benefit from frequency compression for our recall task and low-level speech recognition task.

    • These data add to the growing literature suggesting the need for fitting protocols that take into account individuals’ cognitive processing capabilities. These might include measurement of working memory.

    Discussion

    Introduction

    • Within cognitive hearing science, an emerging area of research is how cognitive factors influence hearing aid outcomes.1,2

    • Non-linear frequency compression is a signal processing strategy that compresses (lowers) high-frequency acoustic input above a certain cut-off frequency.3

    • While increasingly available in the U.S., there is inconclusive evidence regarding the effectiveness of this strategy, with wide individual variability reported.4

    • A recent study suggests that people with low working memory capacity perform more poorly with frequency compression than those with higher capacity. 5

    • The purpose of this study is to examine the efficacy of non-linear frequency compression across multiple outcome measures with varying cognitive and acoustic complexity.

    Results Aided Speech Recognition in Quiet

    Aided Speech Recognition in Noise (Dual-task)

    Aided Recall Speech in Noise (Dual-task)

    Recognition in Quiet Conversational Level

    Recognition in Quiet Low-level†

    Recognition in Noise (Dual-task) High Context

    Recognition in Noise (Dual-task) Low Context*

    Recall in Noise Dual Task*†

    11 adults (age 64-85 years) with symmetric sensorineural hearing loss

    Low-level (40-55 dBA)

    Individual Differences

    Example: Spectrogram of the word “SCIENCE”

    R2 = 0.09, p = 0.37

    60

    40 20 R

    ead

    ing

    Span

    Sco

    re

    Age (years) 70 75 80 85 60 65

    Freq

    ue

    ncy

    (H

    z)

    Time (sec)

    Second International Conference on Cognitive Hearing Science for Communication, June 16-19, 2013, Linköping, Sweden

    Data Collection Summary Cognitive Demand

    Lower Higher

    Speech Recognition in Quiet AZ Bio sentences

    70 dBA 40-55 dB A

    Speech Recognition in Noise R-SPIN Dual-task

    High context Low context

    Speech Recall R-SPIN Dual-task

    6 words

    ADDITIONAL MEASURES:

    Audiogram, Reading span (a measure of working memory capacity; Daneman & Carpenter, 1980)

    Participants

    High context Low context

    ---- Conversational level (70 dBA)

    Listeners sorted by Reading Span Score

    Reading Span Score: 4 8 13 13 22 22 31 33 40 58 61

    Benefit of frequency compression was observed for the low context speech in noise recognition and recall, however, not all subjects experienced benefit for those tasks.

    *P