POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

27
POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK: LINKING ORGANIZATIONS AND PHYSIOLOGY EMILY D. HEAPHY McGill University JANE E. DUTTON University of Michigan Human physiological systems are highly responsive to positive social interactions, but the organizational importance of this finding largely has been unexplored. After reviewing extant research, we illustrate how consideration of the physiology of positive social interactions at work opens new research questions about how positive social interactions affect human capacity and how organizational contexts affect employee health and physiological resourcefulness. We also address the practical implications of integrating physiological data into organizational research. Our paper invites a fuller consideration of how employees’ bodies are affected by everyday work interactions and, in so doing, encourages a stronger tie between human physiology and organizational research. For medical researchers, the importance of understanding human physiology is obvious. After all, medical researchers’ primary goal is to improve the health of patients through under- standing health and disease processes. For or- ganizational researchers, human physiology is a distant concern. We typically seek to under- stand social life in organizations using cogni- tive, affective, and behavioral explanations. To the extent that organizational researchers do focus on physiology, we tend to use physiologi- cal markers as indicators of job stress and strain (Cooper, Dewe, & O’Driscoll, 2001; Karasek & Theorell, 1990). In this paper we argue that human physiology deserves greater attention in organizational re- search. Our physiological systems are con- stantly shifting and adjusting to the social world, yet the organizational importance of this finding largely has been unexplored. We bring together unfamiliar physiological research and familiar organizational theories to propose a new agenda in organizational research. Positive social interactions provide the bridge between these two research areas. Specifically, we build on twenty years of research to show that posi- tive social interactions at work have beneficial physiological effects. We propose pathways through which the physiology of positive social interactions builds human capacity and path- ways through which organizations shape the physiology of employees by creating, facilitat- ing, or minimizing opportunities for positive so- cial interactions. This effort is consistent with organizational scholars who call for a focus on specific aspects of human physiological responses (Wright & Diamond, 2006), with researchers who want a more holistic treatment of leaders and employ- ees (Boyzatis, Smith, & Blase, 2006), with sociol- ogists who are trying to bring the biological body into the study of sociology (Fitzhugh & Leckie, 2001; Freund, 1988), and with economists who are trying to understand the micropro- cesses of decision making (Breiter, Aharon, Kahneman, Dale, & Shizgal, 2001; Platt & Glim- cher, 1999). All of these researchers see scholar- ship that does not take physiological processes We thank Rita Benn, Ruth Blatt, Marlys Christianson, Cedric de Leon, Adam Grant, Andy Molinsky, Ryan Quinn, and Scott Sonenshein for comments on earlier drafts of this paper, and Grace Chen for help in preparing the manuscript. We also thank Art Brief and three anonymous reviewers whose comments contributed to the quality of our argu- ments. In addition, we thank the Faculty Scholars Program in the Center for Integrative Medicine at the University of Michigan for its support. Amy Saunders and Cara Sand- elands provided assistance in many ways. We presented an earlier version of the paper at the 2003 annual meeting of the Academy of Management, in Seattle. Academy of Management Review 2008, Vol. 33, No. 1, 137–162. 137 Copyright of the Academy of Management, all rights reserved. Contents may not be copied, emailed, posted to a listserv, or otherwise transmitted without the copyright holder’s express written permission. Users may print, download, or email articles for individual use only.

Transcript of POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

Page 1: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS ANDTHE HUMAN BODY AT WORK:

LINKING ORGANIZATIONS AND PHYSIOLOGY

EMILY D. HEAPHYMcGill University

JANE E. DUTTONUniversity of Michigan

Human physiological systems are highly responsive to positive social interactions,but the organizational importance of this finding largely has been unexplored. Afterreviewing extant research, we illustrate how consideration of the physiology ofpositive social interactions at work opens new research questions about how positivesocial interactions affect human capacity and how organizational contexts affectemployee health and physiological resourcefulness. We also address the practicalimplications of integrating physiological data into organizational research. Our paperinvites a fuller consideration of how employees’ bodies are affected by everyday workinteractions and, in so doing, encourages a stronger tie between human physiologyand organizational research.

For medical researchers, the importance ofunderstanding human physiology is obvious.After all, medical researchers’ primary goal is toimprove the health of patients through under-standing health and disease processes. For or-ganizational researchers, human physiology isa distant concern. We typically seek to under-stand social life in organizations using cogni-tive, affective, and behavioral explanations. Tothe extent that organizational researchers dofocus on physiology, we tend to use physiologi-cal markers as indicators of job stress and strain(Cooper, Dewe, & O’Driscoll, 2001; Karasek &Theorell, 1990).

In this paper we argue that human physiologydeserves greater attention in organizational re-search. Our physiological systems are con-stantly shifting and adjusting to the social

world, yet the organizational importance of thisfinding largely has been unexplored. We bringtogether unfamiliar physiological research andfamiliar organizational theories to propose anew agenda in organizational research. Positivesocial interactions provide the bridge betweenthese two research areas. Specifically, we buildon twenty years of research to show that posi-tive social interactions at work have beneficialphysiological effects. We propose pathwaysthrough which the physiology of positive socialinteractions builds human capacity and path-ways through which organizations shape thephysiology of employees by creating, facilitat-ing, or minimizing opportunities for positive so-cial interactions.

This effort is consistent with organizationalscholars who call for a focus on specific aspectsof human physiological responses (Wright &Diamond, 2006), with researchers who want amore holistic treatment of leaders and employ-ees (Boyzatis, Smith, & Blase, 2006), with sociol-ogists who are trying to bring the biologicalbody into the study of sociology (Fitzhugh &Leckie, 2001; Freund, 1988), and with economistswho are trying to understand the micropro-cesses of decision making (Breiter, Aharon,Kahneman, Dale, & Shizgal, 2001; Platt & Glim-cher, 1999). All of these researchers see scholar-ship that does not take physiological processes

We thank Rita Benn, Ruth Blatt, Marlys Christianson,Cedric de Leon, Adam Grant, Andy Molinsky, Ryan Quinn,and Scott Sonenshein for comments on earlier drafts of thispaper, and Grace Chen for help in preparing the manuscript.We also thank Art Brief and three anonymous reviewerswhose comments contributed to the quality of our argu-ments. In addition, we thank the Faculty Scholars Programin the Center for Integrative Medicine at the University ofMichigan for its support. Amy Saunders and Cara Sand-elands provided assistance in many ways. We presented anearlier version of the paper at the 2003 annual meeting of theAcademy of Management, in Seattle.

� Academy of Management Review2008, Vol. 33, No. 1, 137–162.

137Copyright of the Academy of Management, all rights reserved. Contents may not be copied, emailed, posted to a listserv, or otherwise transmitted without the copyrightholder’s express written permission. Users may print, download, or email articles for individual use only.

Page 2: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

into account as incomplete and see promise inbringing the body back into accounts of sociallife.

Our perspective builds on the established andrelatively simple linkage between individuals’experience of positive social interactions andsalutary physiological processes and estab-lishes its relevance to organizational contexts.This paper provides evidence that people’s sub-jective experience of their connections with oth-ers has immediate, enduring, and consequentialeffects on their bodies. This focus adds a criticalnew dimension to understanding why and howsocial interactions matter for individual and or-ganizational functioning. It documents and ex-plains the variety of ways that social interac-tions with others leave lasting imprints throughhow they affect the functioning of key bodilysystems. In this way, our focus on the body asphysiology affirms the fundamentally importanteffect of work contexts—for better or for worse.We see this depiction as a theoretical and prac-tical view that more completely recognizes em-ployees’ embodied existence as a complicatedand consequential bearer of the effects of orga-nizational systems and the social interactionsthey cultivate.

The paper is organized around four questionsgermane to organizational research. The first is“What do we know about the physiological ef-fects and correlates of positive social interac-tions at work?” The vast majority of physiologi-cal studies of social interactions focus onmarital relationships, caregivers of chronicallyill patients, and people recovering from a majorhealth event (such as a heart attack), or theytake place in lab settings (Uchino, Cacioppo, &Kiecolt-Glaser, 1996). Yet there are important dif-ferences between these contexts and social in-teractions at work. Workplace social interac-tions tend to be more instrumental, lessdiscretionary, and more short-lived than manyof the relationships studied by health research-ers (Duck, 2007). Thus, answering this question isa critical first step.

Second, we ask, “What do the findings aboutthe physiological correlates and effects of posi-tive social interactions contribute to organiza-tional research?” Medical researchers andhealth psychologists’ interests in these findingsare tied to their disciplinary focus on diseaseprevention and health promotion. In sociology,the study of physiological functioning is more

controversial (Freese, Li, & Wade, 2003). For or-ganizational scholars, the theoretical implica-tions are only beginning to be discussed outsideof the stress paradigm (e.g., White, Thornhill, &Hampson, 2006). We argue that a focus on thephysiology of social interactions provides theo-retical room for broader consideration of thebody. Most of our theories assume that workersare “bodiless” (Acker, 1990; Hassard, Holliday, &Wilmott, 2000) or that the body is under the con-trol of the mind (Joas, 1996). The result is that wedon’t know very much about how the body con-tributes to human action and capability. Con-ceptualizing the body as a physiological systemthat can create physiological resourcefulnessand physical health provides one way of theo-rizing the body not as a burden but as a valu-able resource and potential source of agency(Heaphy, 2007).

The third question is “How can organizationalscholars contribute to physiological research?”One limitation of the research so far is a lack ofattention to the social and cultural contexts inwhich social interactions occur (Uchino, 2004).Organizational scholars have a wealth of theo-ries that speak to how organizational contextsshape our relationship experiences. We build onresearch on organizational practices, culture,and leadership behavior to illustrate organiza-tional scholars’ potential contributions to thisinterdisciplinary conversation. The researchagenda we suggest stands in contrast to schol-ars who draw on evolutionary psychology. In-stead of conceptualizing physiological pro-cesses as shaped by the evolutionary past(Freese et al., 2003; Ilies, Arvey, & Bouchard,2006), we focus on the more local, social con-struction of physiological resources and physi-cal health.

Finally, we ask, “What are the practical andmethodological implications of integratingphysiological measures into our research?” Un-derstanding physiology and how to measure itis not typically part of organizational research-ers’ toolbox. We provide a brief overview ofsome general properties of physiology and sug-gest that organizational researchers begin bycollaborating with experts in other disciplinesto conduct this interdisciplinary research.

The principal contributions of this paper areto assemble and critically review organization-ally relevant empirical literature, to identifyhow organizational researchers can both learn

138 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review

Page 3: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

from and contribute to research on the physiol-ogy of social interactions, and to introduce basicmeasurement principles involved in conductingphysiological research. Overall, this paper de-picts a novel view of organizational life as phys-iologically embodied. The model for our argu-ments is presented Figure 1.

THE PHYSIOLOGY OF POSITIVE SOCIALINTERACTIONS AT WORK

Key Assumptions

Our paper builds on the robust finding thatsocial relationships have significant effects onhuman health1 (Seeman & McEwen, 1996). Wefocus specifically on positive social interactionsat work because recent research has shown thatpositive and negative relationships functionthrough bivariate, not bipolar, processes. Posi-tive social interactions are appetitive, charac-terized by the pursuit of rewarding and desiredoutcomes, whereas negative ones are aversive,characterized by unwelcome and punishing out-comes (Reis & Gable, 2002). This means, for ex-ample, that the psychological experiences ofpositive social interactions, such as mutuality

and growth (Miller & Stiver, 1997), are not theopposite of the experiences of negative ones,such as jealousy and isolation (Cacioppo et al.,2002; Fleischmann, Spitzberg, Andersen, &Roesch, 2005). Instead, these psychological ex-periences of relationships are characterized bydifferent eliciting conditions, mechanisms, andoutcomes.2 Putting the appetitive and aversiveprocesses together, we assume that people viewtheir encounters at work as positive, negative,neutral, or ambivalent (Uchino et al., 2001).Within this framework, a focus on positive socialinteractions can obviously only tell one part ofthe story. Therefore, we see this paper as oneimportant first step.

Throughout the paper we use the terms con-nection and relationship to refer to specifictypes of social interactions. A connection is themicrounit of a relationship. It implies that twopeople have interacted and are mutually awareof the interaction (Dutton & Heaphy, 2003; Miller& Stiver, 1997).3 Connections vary in length, last-ing one moment or many, and they may be re-

1 Social relationships are an independent predictor of hu-man health, even when controlling for stressful events, de-pression, and health-related behaviors such as smoking,exercise, and diet (Seeman & McEwen, 1996; Uchino, Holt-Lunstad, Uno, & Flinders, 1996).

2 While we have illustrated this perspective with psycho-logical examples, there are physiological parallels (Gable,Reis, & Elliot, 2003).

3 We also use the word “connection” differently than JeanBaker Miller and colleagues, who define connections aspositive and growth fostering (Miller, 1988; Miller & Stiver,1997). This allows for the possibility that connections can begrowth fostering (life giving) or growth depleting (life deplet-ing).

FIGURE 1The Physiology of Positive Social Interactions at Work

2008 139Heaphy and Dutton

Page 4: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

curring (Berscheid & Lopes, 1997). When connec-tions between two people recur, they are oftencalled relationships (e.g., Gutek, 1995). Thevalue of discussing connections as well as rela-tionships is that connections suggest that briefencounters with another person can be conse-quential moments of interpersonal contact. This,in turn, allows us to attend to the body’s “exquis-ite responsiveness” to positive connections (See-man, 2001: 204). In the studies we review, re-searchers assess both connections (e.g.,participants evaluate the quality of a particularinteraction with a work colleague) and relation-ships (e.g., employees rate the overall quality oftheir relationships with their supervisor).

Key Definitions and Distinctions

A key term in our argument is physiologicalresourcefulness, a form of positive health inwhich the body can build, maintain, and repairitself during times of rest and can more easilydeal with challenges when they occur (Epel,McEwen, & Ickovics, 1998). We argue that posi-tive social interactions build people’s physio-logical resourcefulness by fortifying the cardio-vascular, immune, and neuroendocrine systems.Over time, the physiological resourcefulnesscreated in positive social interactions contrib-utes to better physical health through individualphysiological pathways, as well as in combina-tion, since many diseases affect multiple sys-tems. This argument is related to but distinctfrom both existing physiological constructs andthe literature on social support.

Physiological constructs. Physiological re-sourcefulness builds on previous work on allo-static load (McEwen, 1988), physical thriving(Epel et al., 1998), and physiological toughness(Dienstbier, 1989). All four constructs documenthow people’s life experiences shape the capac-ity of key physiological systems, which, over thelong term, affect physical health. However, threeof the constructs (allostatic load, physical thriv-ing, and physiological toughness) focus on indi-viduals’ responses to stress, with a particularfocus on how particular types of stress (e.g.,acute versus chronic) or cognitive appraisals ofstress (e.g., challenge versus threat) lead tomore or less resilient physiological responses.In contrast, stress is not a necessary conditionfor physiological resourcefulness. It can be builtdirectly through exposure to positive social in-

teractions. Theoretically, this allows us to un-derstand a broad range of relational settings(e.g., mentoring, collaboration), which may ormay not be stressful.

Social support. Our argument is also distinctfrom the social support literature, which sug-gests two ways in which positive social interac-tions affect health. Proponents of the “main ef-fect” model argue that positive connectionsaffect health by prompting the exchange of re-sources or increases in healthy behaviors(Uchino, 2004). In contrast, our argument sug-gests that positive connections directly impactthe body’s physiological resourcefulness. Thismeans that physiological changes can be cre-ated in interactions, without prompting achange in access to external resources (e.g.,emotional or instrumental support) or a changein healthy behaviors (e.g., increases in exerciseor eating more nutritious meals).

Proponents of the second social support modelargue that having social support promoteshealth by protecting people from stress. Thereare two major mechanisms in this account. First,social support changes how individuals viewstressful stimuli (e.g., seeing a disease as achallenge as opposed to a threat), and, second, itprovides instrumental resources that allow aperson to cope with the stressful situation (Co-hen & Willis, 1985; Uchino, 2004). While the ar-gument we develop here does not rely on stressas a necessary condition for physiological re-sourcefulness, we suggest that the physiologi-cal strengthening that occurs in positive socialinteractions improves individuals’ responses toa stressor.

Scope of the Reviewed Studies

Study settings. To the best of our knowledge,this is the first paper to focus solely on the phys-iology of social interactions at work. As westated earlier, most studies focus on nonworkcontexts. To capture relevant findings, we se-lected studies measuring actual work connec-tions and, in three cases, lab settings approxi-mating them. We located these studies throughthe ancestry method, expert recommendations,and literature searches in psychological, busi-ness, and medical databases.

Physiological systems. To understand howpositive social interactions at work effect phys-iological functioning, we examine three major

140 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review

Page 5: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

TABL

E1

Car

diov

ascu

lar

Stud

ies

wit

hR

elat

ions

hip

Mea

sure

san

dD

ayan

dEv

enin

gC

ardi

ovas

cula

rM

easu

res

Stud

yW

ork

Con

text

and

Part

icip

ants

Posi

tive

Rel

atio

nshi

pC

onst

ruct

and

Mea

sure

Phys

iolo

gyM

easu

reR

esul

ts

Un

den

,Ort

h-G

omer

,&E

lofs

son

(199

1)14

8m

emb

ers

of7

occu

pa

tion

al

gro

up

s(3

1w

omen

):p

hys

icia

ns,

tea

cher

s,m

usi

cia

ns,

pol

ice

offi

cers

,tra

inen

gin

eers

,pri

son

per

son

nel

,sa

wm

ill

wor

kers

5it

ems

inte

nd

edto

oper

ati

ona

lize

ath

ree-

pa

rtd

efin

itio

nof

SS

(hig

h-q

ua

lity

rela

tion

ship

sw

ith

cow

orke

rs,g

ood

wor

kin

gen

viro

nm

ent,

stro

ng

gro

up

coh

esio

n):

“Ih

ave

ag

ood

rela

tion

ship

wit

hsu

per

viso

r,”

Ia

mg

etti

ng

onw

ell

wit

hm

yco

wor

kers

,”“T

her

eis

ap

lea

san

ta

tmos

ph

ere

at

my

wor

kpla

ce,”

“Th

ere

isg

ood

gro

up

coh

esio

na

tm

yw

orkp

lace

,”“T

her

ea

reof

ten

con

flic

tsa

nd

arg

um

ents

at

wor

k”(n

oC

ron

ba

ch’s

�p

rovi

ded

)

SB

P,D

BP

,HR

mea

sure

dev

ery

5m

inu

tes

for

24-

hou

rp

erio

d,i

ncl

ud

ing

one

nor

ma

lw

orkd

ay

•H

igh

erp

erce

ived

SS

ass

ocia

ted

wit

hlo

wer

HR

du

rin

gw

ork,

leis

ure

,an

dsl

eep

;SB

Pn

ota

ssoc

iate

dw

ith

SS

•S

ub

ject

sw

ith

low

SS

ha

dh

igh

erS

BP

tha

nth

ose

wit

hh

igh

orm

idle

vels

ofsu

pp

ort

Itu

art

e,K

am

arc

k,T

hom

pso

n,&

Ba

can

u(1

994)

120

emp

loye

esof

un

iver

siti

es,

hos

pit

als

,cit

ya

nd

cou

nty

offi

ces,

an

dp

riva

tefi

rms

inth

eP

itts

bu

rgh

met

rop

olit

an

are

a(2

6b

lack

men

,30

wh

ite

men

,34

bla

ckw

omen

,30

wh

ite

wom

en)

6it

ems

for

cow

orke

ra

nd

wor

ksu

per

viso

rsu

pp

ort

wit

hS

Ssu

bsc

ale

ofJo

bC

onte

nt

Qu

esti

onn

air

e(K

ara

sek,

Gor

don

,Pie

trov

osky

,&F

rese

,19

85).

Item

sn

otsp

ecif

ied

(Cro

nb

ach

’s�

�.8

4);s

ourc

eof

sup

por

tn

otse

pa

rate

din

an

aly

sis

24-h

our

am

bu

lato

ryS

BP

,D

BP

,HR

onon

en

orm

al

wor

kda

y.D

epen

den

tva

ria

ble

:noc

turn

al

dip

pin

g,c

alc

ula

ted

by

sub

tra

ctin

gn

igh

ttim

eB

Pfr

omd

ayt

ime

BP

;hig

her

scor

esin

dic

ate

gre

ate

rd

ipp

ing

Inp

art

ial

corr

ela

tion

an

aly

sis,

gre

ate

rw

ork

SS

wa

ssi

gn

ific

an

tly

ass

ocia

ted

wit

hn

octu

rna

lsy

stol

icd

ipp

ing

;no

dif

fere

nce

inw

ork

SS

bet

wee

nb

lack

an

dw

hit

ep

art

icip

an

ts(r

esea

rch

ers’

inte

rest

wa

sin

raci

al

dif

fere

nce

s,so

no

furt

her

an

aly

sis

wa

sco

nd

uct

edon

wor

kS

S)

Lan

dsb

erg

is,S

chn

all

,Wa

rren

,P

icke

rin

g,&

Sch

wa

rtz

(199

4)26

2m

ena

t8

New

Yor

kC

ity

wor

ksi

tes

4it

ems

for

cow

orke

rsu

pp

ort

(com

pet

ent

cow

orke

rs,f

rien

dly

cow

orke

rs,c

owor

kers

hel

pfu

lin

get

tin

gth

ejo

bd

one,

cow

orke

rsta

kin

ga

per

son

al

inte

rest

inm

e)a

nd

4it

ems

for

sup

ervi

sor

sup

por

t(s

up

ervi

sor

get

tin

gp

eop

leto

wor

kto

get

her

,su

per

viso

rh

elp

ing

get

the

wor

kd

one,

sup

ervi

sor

pa

yin

ga

tten

tion

tow

ha

tI

am

sayi

ng

,su

per

viso

rco

nce

rned

ab

out

wel

fare

ofth

ose

un

der

him

orh

er).

Ba

sed

onm

odif

ied

Job

Con

ten

tQ

ues

tion

na

ire

(Ka

rase

ket

al.,

1985

).(C

ron

ba

ch’s

��

.70)

;sou

rce

ofsu

pp

ort

not

sep

ara

ted

ina

na

lysi

s

24-h

our

am

bu

lato

ryS

BP

an

dD

BP

onon

en

orm

al

wor

kda

y

•H

igh

SS

did

not

ha

vea

ma

inef

fect

onS

BP

orD

BP

;did

not

rep

ort

HR

•H

igh

SS

ha

dm

ild

bu

tin

con

sist

ent

effe

cts

un

der

hig

hjo

bst

rain

con

dit

ion

s

Eva

ns

&S

tep

toe

(200

1)61

nu

rses

an

d32

acc

oun

tan

ts(5

3w

omen

)R

epli

cati

onof

Un

den

eta

l.(1

991)

;se

esu

mm

ary

ab

ove

(Cro

nb

ach

’s�

�.7

6)

HR

,SB

P,a

nd

DB

Pm

easu

red

five

tim

esd

uri

ng

the

da

ya

nd

even

ing

wit

hse

lf-

mon

itor

ing

equ

ipm

ent,

onth

ree

nor

ma

lw

orkd

ays

an

dtw

ole

isu

red

ays

Hig

hS

Sa

ssoc

iate

dw

ith

low

erH

Rd

uri

ng

da

ya

nd

even

ing

ofw

orkd

ays

;not

rela

ted

toS

BP

an

dD

BP

Ra

u,G

eorg

iad

es,F

red

riks

on,

Lem

ne,

&d

eFa

ire

(200

1)75

wor

kin

gm

enw

ith

bor

der

lin

eh

yper

ten

sion

from

sma

llS

wed

ish

tow

n;7

4a

ge-

ma

tch

edh

ealt

hy

con

trol

s

4it

ems

reg

ard

ing

the

pos

sib

ilit

yof

inte

ract

ing

wit

hco

wor

kers

an

dh

ab

itof

seei

ng

coll

eag

ues

outs

ide

ofw

ork.

No

furt

her

info

rma

tion

pro

vid

ed(n

oC

ron

ba

ch’s

�p

rovi

ded

)

SB

P,D

BP

,HR

mea

sure

dev

ery

15m

inu

tes

for

24h

ours

onon

ew

orkd

ay

Hig

hS

Sre

late

dto

low

erH

Ra

tw

ork,

nig

ht,

an

dd

uri

ng

reco

very

at

nig

ht;

SB

Pa

nd

DB

Pn

otre

late

dto

SS

;eff

ects

did

not

dif

fer

by

hyp

erte

nsi

onst

atu

s

Not

e:S

S�

soci

al

sup

por

t;S

BP

�sy

stol

icb

lood

pre

ssu

re;D

BP

�d

iast

olic

blo

odp

ress

ure

;HR

�h

eart

rate

;BP

�b

lood

pre

ssu

re.

2008 141Heaphy and Dutton

Page 6: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

physiological systems: cardiovascular, immune,and neuroendocrine. While physical health anddisease are likely created through multiplephysiological systems, these three are the mostfrequently studied. Each section begins with abrief overview of the system and then describeswhat existing research can tell us about thephysiological correlates and effects of positivesocial interactions at work.

Social interaction measures. The studies wereview measured social interactions in a varietyof ways, reflecting the diversity of the research-ers. We include every study measuring the phys-iological effects of the quality of social relation-ships and connections. Common measuresinclude individuals’ perceptions of emotionalsupport, social closeness, high-quality relation-ships, or positivity of the interaction (details canbe found in Tables 1 through 4). All of the studiescited used a definition that falls under the broaddefinition of positive connections subjectivelyperceived as rewarding or desirable in someway, and all of them focused on one person’sperception of the interaction.

The nine cardiovascular studies assessedwork relationships or connections in naturalisticsettings, whereas the two immune studies wereset in naturalistic contexts but measured onlywork relationships. In our review of the neuroen-docrine system, we report on three lab studiesthat modeled work contexts, and a fourth in anaturalistic context. All of the naturalistic stud-ies used self-report measures of social interac-tions, consistent with our emphasis on individ-uals’ experiences of connection quality. Incontrast, the three lab studies manipulated so-cial interaction quality. With the exception ofthe three lab studies, the reviewed research al-lows us to identify significant associations butdoes not support causal claims.

Protective Cardiovascular Effects During andAfter Work

Cardiovascular overview. The cardiovascularsystem’s major pump (the heart) and distributionsystem (e.g., veins, arteries, and lymphatics) areresponsible for distributing nutrients (i.e., oxy-gen) and removing waste (i.e., carbon dioxide)from every cell in the body. The studies we re-view in this section relied on three cardiovascu-lar measures: systolic blood pressure, diastolicblood pressure, and heart rate. Blood pressure

(BP) and its components, systolic and diastolicblood pressure (SBP and DBP, respectively), areof interest because they have been linked tocardiovascular disease (CVD), hypertension,and other cardiovascular illnesses. SBP is con-sidered a more reliable predictor of cardiovas-cular disease than DBP (Asmar, Darne, el As-saad, & Topouchian, 2001) because it isregulated by the sympathetic nervous system(SNS), which governs the body’s “fight or flight”response to stress. Heart rate (HR) is also com-monly measured, with elevated HR linked tocardiovascular health risks, such as plaques inthe carotid artery (Asmar et al., 2001). However,HR is a more difficult variable to interpret be-cause it is governed by both the SNS and theparasympathetic nervous systems (PNS). There-fore, the underlying causes and consequences ofHR are more difficult to determine.

Tables 1 through 3 reflect all the studies that,to our knowledge, measure positive social inter-actions and cardiovascular activity according tocurrent methodological standards. Ambulatoryblood pressure (ABP)—readings obtained at reg-ular intervals during everyday life—are partic-ularly strong predictors of future cardiovascularproblems (Uchino, 2004), in part because theyare more reliable than one-time measures. Bylooking at cardiovascular patterns over time, re-searchers can make stronger claims about thelinks between social interactions and the car-diovascular system. The studies in each tableare categorized by the duration of the measure-ment period (day only or day and night) andwhether relationships or connections were mea-sured. We excluded studies that had work-related samples but did not specifically mea-sure work interactions (e.g., Steptoe, 2000), aswell as six studies that used cardiovascularmeasures in ways not allowing for strong infer-ences (Fletcher & Jones, 1993; Houben, Diederiks,Kant, & Notermans, 1990; Kaufmann & Beehr,1986; Lercher, Hortnagel, & Kofler, 1993; Thomas& Ganster, 1995; Winnubst, Marcelissen, & Kle-ber, 1982).4

4 These six studies assessed cardiovascular measuresonly once and/or outside of the course of the normal work-day. Current psychophysiological methods advise againstthese practices, for two reasons. First, it is now known thatone-time cardiovascular measures are unreliable (Semmer,Grebner, & Elfering, 2004). Second, when they are takenoutside the context of one’s normal environment, they are

142 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review

Page 7: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

Empirical evidence. Our review shows thatpositive social interactions at work have bothimmediate and enduring effects on the cardio-vascular system, in the form of lower HR and BP.The first group of studies reported the effects ofpositive work relationships on cardiovascularmeasures taken during the day and evening (Ta-ble 1). Researchers assessed cardiovascular pat-terns beyond the workday to see if work rela-tionships have lasting effects. Researchers hadtwo strategies for studying this. Some took fre-quent measures beyond the workday (e.g., everytwenty minutes for twenty-four hours), whereasothers focused on “nocturnal dipping” (Ituarte,Kamarck, Thompson, & Bacanu, 1994)—the dif-ference in BP during waking hours and sleep.Nondipping, or small differences, is associatedwith cardiovascular damage, because the car-diovascular system is working at more taxing,daytime levels for longer periods of time. It hasbeen associated with a number of cardiovascu-lar and circulatory problems, such as organdamage (Palatini et al., 1992; Staessen et al.,2001). Both approaches reveal the degree towhich the cardiovascular system is able to re-cover from the day’s exertions. While these re-searchers assessed participants’ experiences ofspecific relationships (e.g., manager, coworker),they created composite measures of positivework relationships for analysis.

Four of five studies utilized the first strategy—frequent measures during the day and intoevening hours. The three studies that reportedHR found that perceptions of positive work rela-tionships were significantly correlated withlower HR during the day and evening and, whenit was measured, during sleep (Evans & Steptoe,2001; Rau, Georgiades, Fredrikson, Lemne, &

deFaire, 2001; Unden, Orth-Gomer, & Elofsson,1991). None of the studies found a significantrelationship between perceptions of positivework relationships and SBP and DBP. The fifthstudy in this group measured nocturnal dippingand found that greater work social support wascorrelated with greater nocturnal dipping (Itu-arte et al., 1994).

Together, these studies provide some supportfor the hypothesis that when employees experi-ence their work relationships positively, the car-diovascular system is less taxed not only atwork but also into nonwork and resting times.These studies’ samples drew people from multi-ple occupations, suggesting that this pattern islikely to be found for employees in many set-tings. However, none of the studies found signif-icant results for BP. Physiologically, BP has anarrower homeostatic range than HR, meaningthat the body more tightly regulates BP com-pared to HR. To capture BP’s more subtle andfleeting changes, we may need more finelygrained measures of social interactions than thegeneral ones used in these studies. We couldalso be more confident of the health impact ofthese effects with studies that demonstrated re-sults for BP variables. The next sets of studiesaddress these issues.

The second group of cardiovascular studiesreports on the effect of positive work relation-ships on cardiovascular measures during theday only (Table 2). The studies differ from theprevious set in that their samples consist of onetype of employee in a single organization, in-stead of the multiple occupations and organiza-tions represented in the Table 1 studies. Theyalso analyze the effect of specific positive rela-tionships (e.g., coworker, immediate supervisor).

First, a study of New York City traffic agentsrevealed the relative effect of coworkers, imme-diate supervisors, and unit supervisors andfound that not all positive relationships havecardiovascular benefits (Karlin, Brondolo, &Schwartz, 2003). For women, immediate supervi-sor support was associated with lower averageSBP and DBP, but not HR. For men, coworkersupport was associated with lower average SBPlevel, but not DBP or HR. Under high-stress con-ditions, immediate supervisor support and co-worker support were associated with lower SBPfor both men and women. Relationships withunit supervisors never had cardiovascular ef-fects. Second, a study of health care assistants

especially vulnerable to “whitecoat hypertension,” or higherreadings that result from the stress of having one’s bloodpressure read in, for example, a medical context (Stern, Ray,& Quigley, 2001). More recent studies guard against thesetwo methodological weaknesses by taking an initial base-line measure as well as frequent measures over the courseof the time period of interest (Semmer et al., 2004). Becausethese six studies do not meet these criteria, we did notinclude them. It is important to note that Uchino et al. (1996:491) based their statement that work relationships may notbe related to blood pressure on four of the six studies weexcluded for methodological reasons. More recent studies,which we review here, have stronger research designs andprovide more valid and reliable evidence that workplacesocial interactions are consequential for cardiovascularfunctioning.

2008 143Heaphy and Dutton

Page 8: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

TABL

E2

Car

diov

ascu

lar

Stud

ies

wit

hR

elat

ions

hip

Mea

sure

san

dFr

eque

ntD

ayti

me

Car

diov

ascu

lar

Mea

sure

s

Stud

yW

ork

Con

text

and

Part

icip

ants

Posi

tive

Rel

atio

nshi

pC

onst

ruct

and

Mea

sure

Phys

iolo

gyM

easu

reR

esul

ts

Ka

rlin

,Bro

nd

olo,

&S

chw

art

z(2

003)

70N

ewY

ork

Cit

ytr

aff

ica

gen

ts(3

6w

omen

)

Per

ceiv

edem

otio

na

lsu

pp

ort

from

(1)

cow

orke

rs(7

item

s;C

ron

ba

ch’s

��

.83)

,(2)

imm

edia

tesu

per

viso

rs(1

6it

ems;

Cro

nb

ach

’s�

�.7

7),(

3)u

nit

sup

ervi

sors

(7it

ems;

Cro

nb

ach

’s�

�.8

6);b

ase

don

mod

ifie

dJo

bC

onte

nt

Qu

esti

onn

air

e(K

ara

sek

eta

l.,19

85).

Item

sin

clu

ded

thos

eli

sted

inLa

nd

sber

gis

eta

l.(1

994)

,as

wel

la

s“M

yco

wor

kers

care

ab

out

me,

”“T

he

peo

ple

Iw

ork

wit

hen

cou

rag

eea

chot

her

tow

ork

tog

eth

er,”

“My

sup

ervi

sor

giv

esm

ecr

edit

for

thin

gs

Id

ow

ell,”

“My

sup

ervi

sor

care

sa

bou

tm

e,”

“My

sup

ervi

sor

ap

pre

cia

tes

me”

SB

P,D

BP

,HR

coll

ecte

dd

uri

ng

one

8-h

our

wor

kda

y;m

easu

red

ever

y15

min

ute

s

•F

orw

omen

,im

med

iate

sup

ervi

sor

sup

por

ta

ssoc

iate

dw

ith

low

erS

BP

an

dD

BP

(not

HR

)•

For

men

,cow

orke

rsu

pp

ort

ass

ocia

ted

wit

hlo

wer

SB

Ple

vel

(not

DB

Pa

nd

HR

)•

Un

der

hig

hst

ress

con

dit

ion

s,im

med

iate

sup

ervi

sor

an

dco

wor

ker

sup

por

tb

oth

ass

ocia

ted

wit

hlo

wer

SB

P

Wa

ger

,Fel

dm

an

,&

Hu

ssey

(200

3)28

fem

ale

hea

lth

care

ass

ista

nts

;13

wor

ked

for

two

div

erg

entl

yp

erce

ived

sup

ervi

sors

;15

wor

ked

for

sim

ila

rly

per

ceiv

edsu

per

viso

r

47-i

tem

sup

ervi

sor

inte

ract

ion

al

styl

e.It

ems

incl

ud

ed“M

ysu

per

viso

ren

cou

rag

esd

iscu

ssio

nb

efor

em

aki

ng

ad

ecis

ion

,”“I

am

trea

ted

fair

lyb

ym

ysu

per

viso

r”(C

ron

ba

ch’s

��

.98)

.Su

per

viso

rin

tera

ctio

na

lst

yle

def

ined

as

the

ma

nn

erin

wh

ich

info

rma

tion

,mea

nin

gs,

an

dfe

elin

gs

are

con

veye

dto

the

sub

ord

ina

teth

rou

gh

the

com

mu

nic

ati

onof

bot

hve

rba

la

nd

non

verb

al

mes

sag

es;f

our

fact

ors

ofth

ein

stru

men

t:co

nsi

der

ati

on,i

nte

rper

son

al

fair

nes

s,so

cia

lm

atu

rity

,em

pow

erm

ent

SB

Pa

nd

DB

Pm

easu

red

ever

y30

min

ute

sov

erth

ree

12-

hou

rp

erio

ds:

wor

kin

gw

ith

(1)

favo

rab

lyp

erce

ived

sup

ervi

sor,

(2)

less

favo

rab

lyp

erce

ived

sup

ervi

sor,

(3)

non

-wor

kda

y

Wit

hin

-gro

up

dif

fere

nce

:hig

her

SB

Pa

nd

DB

Pw

hen

wor

kin

gfo

rle

ssfa

vore

dsu

per

viso

rco

mp

are

dto

favo

red

sup

ervi

sor

Not

e:S

BP

�sy

stol

icb

lood

pre

ssu

re;D

BP

�d

iast

olic

blo

odp

ress

ure

;HR

�h

eart

rate

.

144 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review

Page 9: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

(Wager, Feldman, & Hussey, 2003) found thataverage levels of SBP and DBP were lower whenthe assistants worked for a direct supervisorwhom they viewed positively compared to onethey viewed negatively. This study reinforcesthe importance of examining the quality of rela-tionships and not assuming that two people’sstructural positions within an organization aresufficient for understanding either beneficial orharmful cardiovascular effects.

The final group of cardiovascular studies pro-vides insight into the physiological effects ofconnections as opposed to relationships (Table3). In both studies participants rated the qualityof specific social interactions at work. Holt-Lunstad, Uchino, Smith, Olson-Cerny, andNealey-Moore (2003) used event-contingent sam-pling of social interactions over three days,where the event was any interaction over fiveminutes. Participants averaged 6.3 interactions

TABLE 3Cardiovascular Studies with Connection Measures

StudyWork Context andParticipants

Positive ConnectionConstruct andMeasure Physiology Measure Results

Brondolo et al. (2003) 104 employees orstudents at aNew York Citymedical centerand its affiliatedschool (65women); meanage � 30(SD � 7.70,range � 18–46)

• Positive interactions:participants’interaction exceededindividual’s averagepositive intensityscore (rating ofinteraction aspleasant, friendly,agreeable;Cronbach’s � � .94)

• Negativeinteractions: anger,discomfort, upset, ortension ininteraction(Cronbach’s � � .84)

BP and HR measuredevery 20 minutesthroughout oneworkday, withparticipantsrecordinginformation aboutsocial interactionsand activities atthe time of BP andHR readings(random samplingof socialinteractions)

• Participants averaged 16interactions (SD � 8.13,range � 2–36) with othersat time of cuff inflation

• Positive interactionsassociated with DBP only;negative interactionsassociated with DBP andSBP

Holt-Lunstad, Uchino,Smith, Olson-Cerny,& Nealey-Moore (2003)

102 participants(53 women); 86percent wereemployed,others were full-time students;includednontraditionalcollege students(working andattendingschool); meanage � 24(range � 18–46)

• Individuals’ generalfeelings of positivityand negativity aboutinteraction partner

• Perceptions ofinteraction: positiveaffect, negativeaffect, intimacy, self-disclosure

BP 5 minutes intosocial interactions,in which the twopeople weremutually engagedwith one another,on three days(approximately 12-hour days); datacollection occurredon two work/schooldays and one non-work/school day(event-contingentsampling of socialinteractions)

• Participants had anaverage of 6.3 interactions/day including 2.14 withcoworkers, .62 with boss,13 with client or customer

• Work relationships wereviewed more negativelythan interactions withnonwork relationships andwere associated withlower positive affect,greater negative affect,lower intimacy, and lowerself-disclosure

• There was no significantdifference between workand nonwork interactionsin predicting SBP, DBP, orHR

• Participants in ambivalentinteractions had higherSBP and DBP, but not HR,compared to all otherrelationship categories

Note: BP � blood pressure; HR � heart rate; SBP � systolic blood pressure; DBP � diastolic blood pressure.

2008 145Heaphy and Dutton

Page 10: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

TABL

E4

Imm

une

and

Neu

roen

docr

ine

Stud

ies

Stud

yW

ork

Con

text

and

Part

icip

ants

Posi

tive

Con

nect

ion

Mea

sure

Phys

iolo

gyM

easu

reR

esul

ts

Levy

,Her

ber

ma

n,W

hit

esid

e,S

an

zo,L

ee,&

Kir

kwoo

d(1

990)

61b

rea

stca

nce

rp

ati

ents

(fro

ma

larg

ersa

mp

leof

120)

wh

ore

cen

tly

com

ple

ted

surg

ery

Per

ceiv

edem

otio

na

lsu

pp

ort

from

fam

ily

mem

ber

s,d

octo

rs,n

urs

es,a

nd

oth

ers.

Fiv

eso

urc

esof

sup

por

tco

nsi

der

ed:s

pou

seor

inti

ma

teot

her

,fa

mil

ym

emb

er,f

rien

d,n

urs

e,d

octo

r.M

easu

resp

ecif

ica

lly

des

ign

edfo

rsa

mp

leof

bre

ast

can

cer

pa

tien

ts(C

ron

ba

ch’s

��

.79

to.9

5)

NK

cell

act

ivit

yH

igh

erp

erce

ived

emot

ion

al

sup

por

tfr

omsp

ouse

an

dd

octo

ra

ssoc

iate

dw

ith

low

erN

Kce

llly

sis

inca

nce

rp

ati

ents

;p

erce

ived

emot

ion

al

sup

por

tfr

omn

urs

en

ota

ssoc

iate

dw

ith

NK

cell

lysi

sT

heo

rell

,Ort

h-G

omer

,&E

ner

oth

(199

0)49

sub

ject

s(1

0w

omen

)fr

omsi

xoc

cup

ati

ons

(mu

sici

an

,air

tra

ffic

con

trol

ler,

ph

ysic

ian

,fre

igh

th

an

dle

r,w

ait

er,m

ech

an

ic)

Ava

ila

bil

ity

an

da

deq

ua

cyof

SS

,in

clu

din

gb

ut

not

lim

ited

tow

ork

rela

tion

ship

s(m

easu

red

once

an

du

sed

as

aco

nst

an

t;a

na

lysi

sd

idn

otse

pa

rate

sou

rces

ofsu

pp

ort)

.Ba

sed

onm

easu

reb

yO

rth

-Gom

era

nd

Un

den

(198

7)

Mea

sure

sof

imm

un

oglo

bu

lin

Gm

easu

red

fou

rti

mes

over

the

cou

rse

ofon

eye

ar

(im

mu

nog

lob

uli

nG

cha

ng

esa

sa

resu

ltof

lon

g-l

ast

ing

stre

ssor

s,n

otsh

ort-

term

ones

)

Job

stra

in(p

sych

olog

ica

ld

ema

nd

sa

nd

dec

isio

nla

titu

de)

ass

ocia

ted

wit

hh

igh

erle

vels

ofim

mu

nog

lob

uli

nG

for

pa

rtic

ipa

nts

wit

hlo

wle

vels

ofS

Sb

ut

not

for

thos

ew

ith

inte

rmed

iate

orh

igh

leve

lsof

SS

Ad

am

&G

un

na

r(2

001)

70m

idd

lecl

ass

mot

her

sof

2-ye

ar-

old

chil

dre

nin

Min

nes

ota

Com

pos

ite

mea

sure

ba

sed

onfo

ur

rela

tion

ship

mea

sure

s:A

du

ltA

tta

chm

ent

Inte

rvie

w,S

ocia

lC

lose

nes

sS

cale

from

the

Mu

ltid

imen

sion

al

Per

son

ali

tyQ

ues

tion

na

ire,

Rel

ati

onsh

ipw

ith

Sp

ouse

Sca

leof

the

Pa

ren

tin

gS

tres

sIn

dex

;F

am

ily

Su

pp

orts

an

dS

tres

ses

Sca

le.C

omb

ined

con

stru

ctd

efin

eda

s“t

he

exte

nt

tow

hic

hth

ep

art

icip

an

tte

nd

sto

ha

vep

osit

ive

feel

ing

sa

bou

tre

lati

onsh

ips

an

def

fect

ivel

yu

ses

them

for

sup

por

ta

nd

com

fort

”(p

.194

)

Sa

liva

ryco

rtis

olm

easu

red

six

tim

esa

da

yfo

rtw

od

ays

(fro

mju

stb

efor

ew

ake

-up

toju

stb

efor

esl

eep

)

Mor

ep

osit

ive

rela

tion

al

fun

ctio

nin

gp

red

icts

hig

her

mor

nin

gco

rtis

olva

lues

an

dst

eep

erd

ecli

ne

inco

rtis

olva

lues

over

the

cou

rse

ofth

ed

ay

Hei

nri

chs,

Ba

um

ga

rtn

er,K

irsc

hb

au

m,&

Eh

lert

(200

3)37

hea

lth

ym

enin

lab

ora

tory

sett

ing

;Tri

erS

ocia

lS

tres

sT

est

(5-m

inu

tem

ock

job

inte

rvie

ww

ith

an

un

know

np

an

el,

foll

owed

by

5-m

inu

tep

ub

lic

men

tal

ari

thm

etic

task

)

Bes

tfr

ien

dp

rovi

din

gsu

pp

ort

inp

rep

ara

tion

for

task

,or

don

ea

lon

e;in

stru

ctio

ns

tofr

ien

dw

ere

tob

ea

sh

elp

ful

as

pos

sib

led

uri

ng

10-m

inu

tep

rep

ara

tion

for

spee

chta

ska

nd

toof

fer

bot

hin

stru

men

tal

an

dem

otio

na

lsu

pp

ort

Intr

an

asa

lO

T;s

ali

vary

cort

isol

mea

sure

da

tota

lof

eig

ht

tim

es,b

oth

bef

ore

an

da

fter

test

Com

bin

edS

Sa

nd

OT

con

dit

ion

ha

dlo

wes

tle

vels

ofco

rtis

old

uri

ng

an

da

fter

task

;no

SS

(alo

ne)

an

dn

oO

Th

ad

hig

hes

tle

vels

.Wh

enp

art

icip

an

tsh

ad

SS

an

dO

T(o

ne

orb

oth

),th

eysh

owed

incr

ease

dca

lmn

ess

an

dd

ecre

ase

da

nxi

ety

scor

esd

uri

ng

stre

ssp

roce

ss;

sub

ject

sw

ith

out

SS

an

dw

ith

pla

ceb

oex

per

ien

ced

dec

rea

sed

calm

nes

sa

nd

incr

ease

da

nxi

ety

Sch

nor

pfe

il,N

oll,

Sch

ulz

e,E

hle

rt,F

rey,

&F

isch

er(2

003)

324

emp

loye

esa

ta

Ger

ma

nm

an

ufa

ctu

rin

gp

lan

t(5

2w

omen

)S

Sb

yco

wor

ker

(4it

ems)

,su

pp

orti

vesu

per

viso

rb

eha

vior

(4it

ems)

sca

les

from

larg

er“s

alu

tog

enic

sub

ject

ive

wor

ka

na

lysi

s,”

orS

ALS

Aq

ues

tion

na

ire

(Rim

an

n&

Ud

ris,

1997

)

14it

ems

tom

easu

rea

llos

tati

clo

ad

,in

clu

din

gov

ern

igh

tu

rin

ary

cort

isol

excr

etio

n

Wor

kers

exp

erie

nci

ng

hig

hS

Sex

per

ien

ced

sig

nif

ica

ntl

ylo

wer

cort

isol

tha

nth

ose

wit

hlo

wS

S

Zak,

Ku

rzb

an

,&M

atz

ner

(200

4)82

ma

lea

nd

fem

ale

stu

den

ts●

Pos

itiv

eso

cia

lcu

e(t

rust

):In

vest

orco

nve

yed

tru

stto

pa

rtn

erb

ych

oosi

ng

am

oun

tof

mon

eyg

iven

toth

etr

ust

ee.P

resu

ma

bly

,th

etr

ust

eep

erce

ived

the

exh

an

ged

am

oun

ta

sa

sig

na

lof

inve

stor

’sg

ood

inte

nti

ons

●Im

per

son

al

cue

(con

trol

):A

mou

nt

exch

an

ged

wa

sp

ub

licl

ya

nd

ran

dom

lya

ssig

ned

.Th

etr

ust

eekn

ewth

at

the

exch

an

ge

invo

lved

no

ben

efic

ent

inte

nti

ons

OT

(pla

sma

);b

lood

dra

wn

aft

ersu

bje

cts

ma

de

thei

rd

ecis

ion

ing

am

e●

Tru

stee

’sO

Tle

vels

twic

ea

sh

igh

wh

enre

ceiv

edso

cia

lsi

gn

al

oftr

ust

com

pa

red

top

ure

lyex

cha

ng

e-b

ase

dre

lati

onsh

ipco

nd

itio

n●

Tru

stee

tra

nsf

erre

db

ack

toin

vest

or53

per

cen

tof

the

mon

eyre

ceiv

ed,c

omp

are

dto

no

mon

eyin

con

trol

con

dit

ion

Kos

feld

,Hei

nri

chs,

Zak,

Fis

chb

ach

er,&

Feh

r(2

005)

121

ma

lest

ud

ents

intr

ust

exp

erim

ent;

61m

ale

stu

den

tsin

risk

exp

erim

ent

●T

rust

exp

erim

ent:

Pa

rtic

ipa

nts

ass

ign

edto

role

of“i

nve

stor

”or

“tru

stee

.”B

oth

are

giv

enin

itia

la

mou

nt

of12

mon

eta

ryu

nit

s(M

Us)

.In

vest

ora

lloc

ate

s0,

4,8,

12M

Us

totr

ust

ee.T

rust

eea

lloc

ate

sa

mou

nt

ofre

turn

toin

vest

or.I

nve

stor

pa

yoff

isca

lcu

late

da

s[i

nit

ial

am

oun

ttr

an

sfer

totr

ust

ee�

am

oun

tre

ceiv

edfr

omtr

ust

ee].

Tru

stee

pa

yoff

isca

lcu

late

da

s[i

nit

ial

am

oun

t�

3(tr

an

sfer

from

inve

stor

)a

mou

nt

retu

rned

toin

vest

or]

●R

isk

exp

erim

ent:

“In

vest

or”

all

oca

tes

0,4,

8,12

MU

sto

a“p

roje

ct.”

Am

oun

tre

turn

edra

nd

omly

det

erm

ined

(not

by

an

oth

erp

erso

n).

Pa

yoff

dis

trib

uti

onsa

me

as

intr

ust

exp

erim

ent

●B

oth

:Ha

lfof

the

pa

rtic

ipa

nts

giv

enO

T,h

alf

giv

enp

lace

bo.

Rep

eate

dfo

ur

tim

es,a

lwa

ysw

ith

dif

fere

nt

“tru

stee

”or

“pro

ject

Intr

an

asa

lO

Tve

rsu

sin

tra

na

sal

pla

ceb

o(w

ith

sam

ein

act

ive

ing

red

ien

tsa

sO

T)

●O

Tsi

gn

ific

an

tly

incr

ease

dth

ea

mou

nt

inve

stor

str

an

sfer

red

intr

ust

exp

erim

ent

only

,not

inri

skex

per

imen

t●

OT

did

not

aff

ect

ba

cktr

an

sfer

am

oun

tof

tru

stee

(in

tru

stex

per

imen

t)●

Pa

rtic

ipa

nts

’op

tim

ism

ab

out

outc

ome

did

not

vary

acr

oss

con

dit

ion

s,so

OT

did

not

ap

pea

rto

aff

ect

peo

ple

’sb

elie

fsa

bou

tot

her

s

Not

e:N

K�

na

tura

lki

ller

;SS

�so

cia

lsu

pp

ort;

OT

�ox

ytoc

in.

146 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review

Page 11: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

per day, which is similar to studies using time-based definitions of interactions (Tschan, Sem-mer, & Inversin, 2004). Eighteen percent of totalinteractions involved work relationships. Ana-lytically, the researchers were interested in find-ing if work and nonwork relationships affectedcardiovascular rates differently; they did not.Brondolo’s et al.’s (2003) study of social interac-tions of employees and students at a medicalcenter is more illuminating for our purposes.Their analysis was based on a random sam-pling of cardiovascular measures throughoutone workday, and participants averaged sixteeninteractions per day (at the time of cuff infla-tion). These researchers found that positive con-nections were associated with lower SBP andDBP, compared to negative ones.

Taken together, the cardiovascular studiesprovide evidence from a variety of contexts thatpositive relationships at work have both imme-diate (Table 2) and enduring (Table 1) effects onthe cardiovascular system. Research on the ef-fects of positive connections (Table 3) was lessclear, with one of two studies providing evi-dence that positive connections affect BP. Froma measurement perspective, it is important tonote that studies that assessed specific relation-ships were associated with BP, which is moreclearly linked to health outcomes than HR. Thissuggests that, in future work, measures of spe-cific interactions may capture more powerful ev-idence for the relationship between positive so-cial interactions on physiological resource-fulness and health.

Strengthening the Capacity of the ImmuneSystem

Immune overview. The immune system de-fends the body against such challenges as dis-ease and tissue damage (Cacioppo, Bernston,Sheridan, & McClintock, 2000). The study of thepsychological modulation of the immune systemis a field called psychoneuroimmunology (PNI).PNI researchers typically study health-relatedoutcomes, such as the rate of wound healing, orthe mechanisms underlying those outcomes,such as testing how specific immune cells, pro-teins, or functions respond to challenges likeexposure to a virus (Kiecolt-Glaser & Glaser,1995; Kiecolt-Glaser, McGuire, Robles, & Glaser,2002). One of the most robust findings of PNI isthat positive social relationships can strengthen

the immune system (Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 2002;Uchino et al., 1996).

Empirical evidence. Two studies5 focus on thelink between quality of work relationships andthe immune system (Table 4). Levy et al. (1990)found that breast cancer patients recoveringfrom surgery had healthier immune responseswhen they gave higher ratings of emotional sup-port to their doctor and their spouse, whereasthe ratings of nurses did not show significanteffects. While this is not a study of employees,doctors and nurses do have a form of workingrelationship with patients. Theorell, Orth-Gomer, and Eneroth (1990) found that workers’immune systems were buffered from the immu-nological effects of job strain when they viewedtheir social support as adequate, but not whenthey viewed their social support as inadequate.

Consistent with studies of nonwork relation-ships (Uchino et al., 1996), both studies suggestthat positive connections strengthen the im-mune system during periods of acute andchronic stress. But there are a number of unan-swered questions that future research can ad-dress. First, the two studies discussed here illus-trate only the buffering effects of positive socialinteractions, or how social interactions protectagainst the deleterious effects of significantstress on immune functioning. Future researchis needed to determine whether work relation-ships also have direct physiological effects (out-side the context of a challenge) on immune func-tioning. Second, because both studies measuredrelationships, not connections, they can only in-form us about the physiological effects of moreenduring positive relationships. However, theimmune system is responsive to short-termstressors of as little as five minutes (e.g., givinga speech in front of strangers; Segerstrom &Miller, 2004), so future research may be able tocapture immune strengthening of shorter posi-tive connections. Finally, like the cardiovascu-lar studies, the study of breast cancer patientsprovided additional evidence that not all workrelationships are associated with physiologicaloutcomes.

5 We excluded Jemmott and Magloire (1988) because ofquestions about the methodology previously identified byStone, Cox, Vladimarsdottir, and Neale (1987) and Herbertand Cohen (1993).

2008 147Heaphy and Dutton

Page 12: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

Healthier Patterns of Neuroendocrine Response

Neuroendocrine overview. The neuroendo-crine system refers to the interactions betweenthe nervous system and the hormones of theendocrine system. Researchers in this area seekto understand the functions, distribution, andeffects of these biologically active agents(Snowdon & Ziegler, 2000). The neuroendocrinesystem helps activate a number of other physi-ological processes, including the immune andcardiovascular systems. Social scientists areparticularly interested in the neuroendocrinesystem because hormones are highly respon-sive to the quality of social relationships (See-man & McEwen, 1996) and have effects on be-havior (Lovallo & Thomas, 2000). The existingneuroendocrine research related to work rela-tionships focuses on oxytocin (OT) and cortisol.We do not address the other hormones that havebeen studied in relation to a broader set of hu-man relationships, such as marital relation-ships (Kiecolt-Glaser, Bane, Glaser, & Malarkey,2003; Kiecolt-Glaser & Newton, 2001).

All hormones have multiple physiologicalfunctions. It is the patterns and proportionalityof their response that can have health-enhanc-ing or -damaging effects (Epel et al., 1998). Forexample, cortisol is released when people expe-rience certain types of psychological stress, andit then breaks down stored sugars (glucogen-esis) and suppresses some anabolic processes(e.g., growth and digestion). A healthy responseto stress includes both a spike in cortisol and areturn to baseline levels, a process governed byother hormones. Both cortisol under- and over-activity are associated with health outcomes(Adam & Gunnar, 2001; Dickerson & Kemeny,2004).

Researchers studying positive social interac-tions are particularly interested in the neuroen-docrine system because of recent research thatshows that the neuropeptide OT may be an im-portant physiological substrate of affiliation be-haviors (Carter, 1998), increasing social behav-iors and attenuating hormone responses (e.g.,cortisol) to stress (DeVries, Glasper, & Detillion,2003). OT has been featured in many prominentsocial and medical scientists’ theories of socialinteractions, including theories of love (Carter,1998; Porges, 1998), maternal and mate attach-ment (Bartels & Zeki, 2004; Insel, 2000), adult at-tachment and affiliation (Henry & Wang, 1998),

positive social interactions (Uvnas-Moberg,1998), and Taylor’s (2002) tend-and-befriendstress response theory. Although it was previ-ously thought of as a “women’s hormone” be-cause of its role in such female reproductiveprocesses as birth and lactation, it is found inboth men and women, and recent research hasshown that it is associated with a broad array ofsocial activities, including social elements oflearning and memory (Engelmann, Wotjak, Neu-mann, Ludwig, & Landgraf, 1996; Ferguson,Young, & Insel, 2002).

In spite of OT’s promise, we are only begin-ning to understand how to measure and studyOT in humans. No reliable salivary measure hasbeen developed thus far (Horvat-Gordon,Granger, Schwartz, Nelson, & Kivlighan, 2005),making expensive and intrusive blood samplinga necessity for measuring OT.6 As a result, mostof the studies to date have relied on animalsamples. Given these challenges, the only stud-ies of OT applicable to work connections havetaken place in lab settings. In addition, theoret-ical interest in neuroendocrine responses arosefrom studies of the human stress response, witha tendency to view social relationships as anevolutionary means for ensuring species sur-vival and reproduction. These theories focus onromantic, reproductive, or parental relation-ships. Therefore, importing these theories andtheir evolutionary logic into the study of rela-tionships at work should be done with caution(Newton, 2003).

Empirical evidence. Neuroendocrine studiesrelevant for positive social interactions focus onthe hormones OT and cortisol. We include threelab experiments because they modeled organi-zationally relevant social interactions. Twostudies used a paradigm from experimental eco-nomics designed to learn about trust and reci-procity in exchange relationships. In this para-digm, one person plays the role of “investor” andthe other the “trustee.” Both people are given aninitial sum of money. Only one exchange is

6 OT can be introduced intranasally, a technology thatwas developed to stimulate labor and induce lactation inwomen. Manufactured by Novartis, it is not available in theUnited States. It is rarely used to manipulate OT levels forresearch because of disagreement about whether intranasalOT functions in the same way as OT released naturally inthe brain (for a discussion of these issues, see Born et al.,2002; Carter & Altemus, 1997; Stockhorst & Pietrowsky, 2004).

148 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review

Page 13: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

made in any investor-trustee dyad, with the in-vestor making the first move. Any money theinvestor allocates to the trustee will triple; thetrustee then has the choice of returning anyamount of money (including zero) to the investor.Each person then keeps any remaining money.(See Table 4 and Berg, Dickhaut, and McCabe[1995] for more details.)

Zak, Kurzban, and Matzner (2004) used thisparadigm to examine the effects of positive so-cial cues of trust on trustees’ OT levels. Theyfound that when trustees received a positivesocial cue from an investor, their OT levels weretwice as high as trustees who received an im-personal cue. Even though trustees in both thepositive and impersonal conditions received (onaverage) the same amount of money from theirinvestor partners, trustees in the positive socialcue/high OT group returned 53 percent of themoney they had received, whereas the imper-sonal cue group returned zero. This suggeststhat even momentary positive connections haveconsequential behavioral, as well as neuroen-docrine, effects.

Kosfeld, Heinrichs, Zak, Fischbacher, and Fehr(2005) used the same paradigm but made twoimportant modifications. First, investors allo-cated money either to a trustee (which theycalled the “trust” version) or to a “project” (the“risk” version). Both the trust and risk versionsprovided equal opportunity for profit, but in the“project” condition, the investor’s return on in-vestment was randomly determined. Second,these researchers manipulated OT levels, withhalf of the participants receiving intranasal OTand the other half receiving an intranasal pla-cebo. The results showed that OT significantlyincreased the amount investors transferred totrustees in the trust experiment, but not in therisk experiment. Participants in both conditionshad similar mood and calmness measures be-fore and after the administration of OT, whichsuggests that OT does not have general psycho-logical and behavioral effects but, instead, op-erates specifically in the context of social inter-actions.

It is important to note that Kosfeld and col-leagues found that intranasally administeredOT did not affect trustees’ behavior; in both theOT and placebo conditions, trustees returnedthe same amount of money to investors. Thus,these researchers claim that OT does not affectreciprocity. The most likely explanation for

these contradictory findings is that the research-ers did not take into account the natural in-crease in OT levels that we saw in Zak et al.’sstudy, in which trustees’ OT levels increasedsimply as a result of receiving an initial amountfrom the investor. If that is the case, then it is notsurprising that the experimentally induced OTwould fail to have an effect. In fact, all trustees’OT levels may have been saturated. However,replication and more detailed testing of mecha-nisms underlying these effects can resolve thesediscrepancies.

In a third experiment using a different para-digm, men were asked to give a mock job inter-view to a panel of unfamiliar interviewers (Hein-richs, Baumgartner, Kirschbaum, & Ehlert, 2003).This study was a 2 (alone versus emotional andinstrumental support from a best friend) � 2(intranasal OT versus placebo) design. The de-pendent variable was cortisol level, which is amarker of stress and SNS activation. The re-searchers found that both OT and social supportindependently decreased cortisol levels, in-creased calmness, and decreased anxiety.When OT and social support were combined, theeffects on cortisol were amplified, providingsupport for the argument that OT is a mecha-nism through which positive social interactionsbuild health by decreasing cortisol levels dur-ing stress.

Two studies have addressed the link betweenpositive social interactions and cortisol’s diur-nal patterns. Health outcomes are associatedwith the degree to which one’s cortisol levelsfollow the normal pattern (high before waking,low from evening to 2 to 3 a.m.; Adam, 2005;Antoni, 1987; Lovallo & Thomas, 2000). First,Adam and Gunnar (2001) looked at the effect ofrelationship functioning and home and work de-mands on the cortisol levels of seventy mothersof toddlers. They created a composite measureof general relationship functioning based onfour qualitative and quantitative measures.However, the measures did not focus explicitlyon work relationships, so this only has sugges-tive implications for the neuroendocrine effectsof work relationships. The researchers foundthat higher scores on positive relationship func-tioning, as well as lower numbers of childrenand fewer hours of work, were associated withhealthier cortisol rhythms. A second study ofGerman workers found that those workers expe-riencing high social support had healthier cor-

2008 149Heaphy and Dutton

Page 14: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

tisol patterns compared to those with low socialsupport (Schnorpfeil et al., 2003).

Together, these five studies suggest that pos-itive social interactions have beneficial effectson neuroendocrine functioning, either throughthe release of OT or through contributing tohealthier diurnal cortisol response, and theseeffects, in turn, are associated with better phys-ical health. The OT findings also point towardintriguing physiological mechanisms of trustand reciprocity. While this research confirms thegeneral theories about the benefits of positivesocial interactions on neuroendocrine function-ing, future research will have to answer ques-tions about the relative contribution of work re-lationships and determine the mechanisms thatunderlie these effects.

Summary

Collectively, this review illustrates humans’exquisite physiological responsiveness (See-man, 2001) to positive social interactions atwork. The cardiovascular studies provide evi-dence that positive work relationships are asso-ciated with decreases in cardiovascular reactiv-ity at work and beyond. Over time, elevated HRand BP can have serious effects on cardiovascu-lar health (Uchino, 2004). Positive work relation-ships appear to strengthen the immune systemby bolstering components of immune response.Because the immune system is always alert forchallenges, such as viruses, the strength of theimmune system can have effects on short-(Kiecolt-Glaser, Garner, Speicher, Penn, & Gla-ser, 1984) and long-term health (Esterling,Kiecolt-Glaser, Bodnar, & Glaser, 1994; Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 2002). The neuroendocrine studiessuggest that positive work relationships contrib-ute to healthier hormone patterns. Positive so-cial connections appear to prompt the release ofOT, which has been shown to have short-termeffects on attenuating hormonal reactions tostress and long-term effects on anabolic pro-cesses such as growth (Uvnas-Moberg, 1998).The cortisol findings suggest that positive rela-tionships contribute to more proportional re-sponses to stress and build long-term reservesof health. In the next sections we consider theimplications of these findings for organizationalresearch.

CONTRIBUTIONS TO ORGANIZATIONALRESEARCH

Our review reveals that experiences of posi-tive social interactions at work directly affectthe body’s physiological processes. The focus onthe physiology–social interaction link helps ussee health not as a gross outcome measured indays absent or health care costs (Danna & Grif-fin, 1999) but as momentary ebbs and flows thatcan be built up or whittled down through socialinteractions. This suggests that health can bestrengthened or weakened across multiple in-teractions and that this process occurs over time(Epel et al., 1998). Organizational research cur-rently lacks a way of capturing this form ofhealth.

To capture this more finely grained form ofhealth, we propose that positive social interac-tions build people’s physiological resourceful-ness by fortifying the cardiovascular, immune,and neuroendocrine systems through immedi-ate and enduring decreases in cardiovascularreactivity, strengthened immune responses, andhealthier hormonal patterns. As we arguedabove, these physiological responses increasethe body’s ability to build, maintain, and repairitself during times of rest and to deal more eas-ily with challenges when they occur. Over time,the physiological resourcefulness created inpositive social interactions contributes to betterphysical health through individual physiologi-cal pathways as well as in combination, sincemany diseases affect multiple systems. To illus-trate how physiological resourcefulness cancontribute to organizational research, we usethe examples of work recovery and engagement.

Work Recovery

Recent research has shown that relationshipsare one key pathway through which work andnonwork domains influence one another (e.g.,Greenhaus & Powell, 2003), yet organizationalresearchers have overlooked the physiologicalsubstrates of relationships. Considering thephysiological effects of work relationships pro-vides us with a new window into understandingwhy and how work and nonwork contexts affectone another. We can see this through researchon work recovery.

Recovery refers to the process by which anindividual’s functioning returns to its pre-

150 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review

Page 15: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

stressor levels and the experience of strain isreduced (Sonnentag & Natter, 2004). Work recov-ery focuses specifically on employees’ recoveryfrom the workday during evening hours, and it isan important window into understanding how toprevent depletion from work (Rothbard, 2001;Sonnentag, 2001). Studies have shown that lackof work recovery results in more health com-plaints and absence due to sickness two yearslater (DeCroon, Sluiter, & Frings-Dresen, 2003;Elfering, Grebner, Semmer, & Gerber, 2002;Jansen, Kant, & van den Brandt, 2002; Sluiter,Van der Beek, & Frings-Dresen, 1999). One studyfound that cortisol levels during work recoverywere related to subsequent health care costs,while cortisol levels during work were not (Gan-ster, Schaubroeck, Sime, & Mayes, 1991), sug-gesting that the physiological factors contribut-ing to work recovery contribute to physicalhealth over the long term.

Workplace social interactions, and their phys-iological correlates, have not yet been studied inthis literature (Sonnentag & Fritz, 2006). How-ever, the studies reviewed here suggest thatpositive social interactions play an importantrole in the physiological basis of work recovery.First, the cardiovascular studies summarized inTable 1 suggest that when people experiencepositive social interactions at work, their cardio-vascular systems are more likely to return toresting levels after work. Second, the cortisolstudy of working mothers found that positiverelationship functioning is associated withhealthier cortisol rhythms, which includes therecovery time after work (Adam & Gunnar, 2001).This suggests that positive social interactions atwork leave a lasting physiological imprint, onethat affects people after they have left work.

Proposition 1: The physiological re-sourcefulness generated during posi-tive social interactions facilitates thework recovery process and, over time,contributes to better physical health.

Engagement

Engagement describes a form of intentionalparticipation and involvement in a work role(Adam, 2005; Benner, 1984, 2000; Kahn, 1990; Roth-bard, 2001). People experience moments of en-gagement or disengagement in their work rolesthroughout each day (Britt, Dickinson, Greene, &

McKibben, 2007; Kahn, 1990). Engagement re-quires effort (Nes, Segerstrom, & Sephton, 2005)or vigor (Schaufeli & Bakker, 2004). One anteced-ent of engagement is the availability of re-sources, including physical or bodily resources(Kahn, 1990; Loehr & Schwartz, 2003). Research-ers have considered how physical resources canbe depleted from working in a taxing environ-ment, such as under the hot sun (Kahn, 1990) orunder stressful conditions (Frankenhaeuser,1978), and can be replenished through exerciseregimens (Loehr & Schwartz, 2003; Sonnentag &Natter, 2004).

We propose that the physiological resource-fulness and physical health generated in posi-tive social interactions are an additional bodilysource of engagement at work. As the studieshave shown, positive social interactions haveboth immediate and enduring effects on physi-ological functioning. This physical strengthen-ing builds up a foundation for engagement, sup-porting the body’s ability to expend effort, adaptto changing circumstances, and utilize energyefficiently (Epel et al., 1998). The research re-viewed from all three physiological systemsalso suggests that positive social interactionscontribute to less physiologically taxing experi-ences of stress. This suggests, therefore, thatwhen people experience positive social interac-tions at work, they have a stronger physiologicalfoundation for the effort needed to engage intheir work role.

Proposition 2: The physiological re-sourcefulness generated in positivesocial interactions contributes tohigher levels of physiological re-sources for engagement in a workrole.

These two examples illustrate the potentialtheoretical contributions of considering thephysiological correlates and effects of positivesocial interactions at work. Physiology has pri-marily been incorporated into stress research, inwhich scholars view physiology primarily as adependent variable—one that is negatively af-fected by workplace experiences (Cooper et al.,2001). In contrast, we suggest that physiologicalvariables can measure the construct of physio-logical resourcefulness, which, in turn, functionsas an independent variable in explaining indi-vidual-level processes of work recovery and en-gagement.

2008 151Heaphy and Dutton

Page 16: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

CONTEXTUALIZING EMBODIED POSITIVESOCIAL INTERACTIONS IN ORGANIZATIONS

Research into the link between positive socialinteractions at work and physiology has the po-tential to contribute to the fields emerging at theintersection of the social and medical sciences.We suggest new ways of theorizing about howorganizational contexts shape employees’ bod-ies through experiences of positive social inter-actions at work. In particular, we build on re-searchers’ claims that organizational contexts(e.g., organizational practices, culture, and lead-ership) shape patterns of social networks (Baker,2000; Cross & Parker, 2004) and patterns of socialinteractions (Dutton, 2003), which, in turn, shapeemployee attitudes and behaviors.

However, our focus on the connection betweenpositive relationships and employee physiologydirects attention to the features of organization-al contexts that would be particularly potent inaffecting employees’ physiological resourceful-ness because of their capacity to foster thebuilding or sustaining of the experience of pos-itive social interactions. Below we develop aseries of research propositions illustrating howorganizational behavior can enrich the study ofhuman physiology by specifying how social (inthis case, organizational) conditions foster pro-cesses (social interactions) that directly affecthuman physiology.

Organizational Practices

Baker and Dutton (2007) have identified sevenclusters of practices that foster the building ofpositive connections in organizations. By “prac-tices,” they mean, drawing from Orlikowski(2002), recurrent, materially bounded, situatedactivities of a particular unit or organization.The seven sets of practices fall into two majorgroups: (1) human resource practices (selection,socialization, evaluation, rewards) and (2) every-day work practices (conduct of meetings, collab-orative technologies, practices of interpersonalhelping). They theorize that certain forms ofthese practices increase the experience of posi-tive interactions through how they shape themotivation to connect and the opportunity struc-tures for connection. We use two examples of anorganizational practice (e.g., socialization prac-tices and meeting practices) to illustrate how apositive social interaction–physiology link

encourages new theorizing about how organiza-tional contexts (in this case, patterns of prac-tices) affect employees’ physiological resource-fulness through positive social interactions.

Organizations vary considerably in the waysthat they help employees become members ofnew units, new projects, or the organization it-self. Although much of organizational researchhas focused on the sensemaking and interpre-tative side of socialization (e.g., Louis, 1980),there is a very important relational componentto socialization practices. By “relational,” wemean conducive to building positive connec-tions between people. In particular, organiza-tions vary in how easily and broadly they facil-itate employees’ connection with old and othernew members of the organization during thiscrucial onboarding process (Baker & Dutton,2007).

For example, some organizations routinely ro-tate people through multiple departments aspart of the socialization process (Cross & Parker,2004). If this is done in a way that allows peopleto meet each other in affirming ways, then theyare more likely to experience positive social in-teractions during this process. Some organiza-tions create specified roles, such as new mem-ber advocates or mentors, who help newcomerslearn about the organization and interact inways that are culturally appropriate, often in-creasing the chances positive social interac-tions will occur (Dutton, 2003; Rollag, Parise, &Cross, 2005).

Organizations also vary in their practices ofrewarding people for successfully locating andbringing on board new organizational members(e.g., Fernandez, Castilla, & Moore, 2000), which,in many situations, directly increases new em-ployees’ chances of having positive social inter-actions with others during their socializationprocess. When organizations reward employeesfor locating new members, “old-timers” are moreinvested in having people get integrated andexert greater effort in helping them meet othersand get up to speed more quickly. These morerelational socialization practices make it morelikely that new employees will be physiologi-cally strengthened and more resourceful duringthe socialization process because they havemore opportunities to engage in positive socialinteractions.

152 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review

Page 17: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

Proposition 3: The greater the extent ofrelational socialization practices usedin an organization (or unit), thegreater the level of physiological re-sourcefulness of employees.

Virtual and face-to-face meetings are frequentforums for social connecting at work, and, thus,the meeting practices that typify organizationsor units shape patterns of social interacting. Weposit that meeting practices shape employees’physiological resourcefulness in several ways.First, as a critical conversational space, organi-zations can have practices that effectively pre-pare people at the meeting to understand andappreciate meeting members’ contributions toeach other. For example, Cohen and Prusak(2001) have suggested that UPS and Hewlett-Packard explicitly encourage and design phys-ical spaces that foster face-to-face meetings asan explicit means of fostering positive socialinteractions. Dutton (2003) has argued that meet-ings can affect the degree to which they encour-age respectful interactions, trust, and task en-abling, any one of which facilitates theexperience of positive social interactions. If acontext exhibits more relational meeting prac-tices (marked by the encouragement of listen-ing, supporting and equipping meeting mem-bers to contribute, more respectful engagement,task enabling, and trust), then we would expectto see increased physiological resourcefulness(Baker & Dutton, 2007).

Proposition 4: The greater the extent ofrelational meeting practices used inan organization (or unit), the greaterthe level of physiological resourceful-ness of employees.

Organizational Culture

An organization’s shared values, beliefs,norms, and artifacts (Schein, 2004) are distin-guishing and critical features of organizationalcontexts. Variance in shared values and beliefs,for example, is associated with different pat-terns of interrelating among organizationalmembers (Perlow, Gittell, & Katz, 2004). This as-sertion allows us to build illustrative proposi-tions about how organizational culture canshape employees’ physiological resourceful-ness through shaping patterns and perceptionsof positive social interactions.

For example, Brickson (2005, 2007) recently ar-gued that organizations vary systematically inwhat she calls “identity orientation,” which isthe shared assumptions about how an organiza-tion relates to stakeholders. One can conceptu-alize identity orientation as a form of sharedcultural belief. Brickson’s empirical researchsuggests that an organization’s identity orienta-tion toward outside stakeholders is empiricallyrelated to its identity orientation toward internalstakeholders (what she calls “internal identityorientation”; Brickson, 2005). Her research fur-ther suggests three distinct types of identity ori-entations. A relational identity orientation isone in which members define the organizationas a partner to its stakeholders, which, in turn, isoften associated with a motivation to care andtrust partners.

We posit that organizations that have a strongrelational identity orientation will have morefrequent positive social interactions, buildingthe physiological resourcefulness of members.This proposition aligns with Brickson’s (2007) ar-gument that organizations with a more rela-tional organizational identity will have strongerdyadic ties between members and internal andexternal stakeholders.

Proposition 5: The stronger the relationalidentity orientation of an organization,the greater the level of physiologicalresourcefulness of the organization’semployees.

Organizational Leadership

For as long as there have been organizationalresearchers, there has been an interest in theimpacts of organizational leadership. A physio-logical take on social interactions invites con-sideration of how leadership behaviors shapepatterns of positive social interactions and,thus, employee health and resourcefulness. Wedevelop two propositions that explore this pos-sibility as another way of seeing how studies oforganizational behavior and human physiologyenrich one another.

The assertion that leaders’ behaviors affectpatterns of social interactions is not surprising,given that many researchers construe leader-ship as fundamentally about relationships (e.g.,Fletcher, 2007; Graen & Uhl-Bien, 1995; Pearce &Conger, 2003). However, certain leadership be-

2008 153Heaphy and Dutton

Page 18: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

haviors may be particularly powerful means ofencouraging and facilitating positive social in-teractions, and these, in turn, enable physiolog-ical resourcefulness of the persons in the inter-action. Relational attentiveness captures aleader’s capacity to perceive and respond toother people’s emotional state and has been ar-gued to be an important component of emotionalintelligence (Salovey, Mayer, & Caruso, 2002).Frost (2003) has argued that the relational atten-tiveness of leaders is particularly important forthe detection of and response to the emotionalpain of followers, which serves to foster positiveinteractions through activating processes of re-lational repair. Dutton has argued that rela-tional attentiveness fosters the creation of high-quality connections between people inorganizations, because leaders with thisstrength “can play a key role in sustaining andrepairing the connective tissue of a work group,department or organization” (2003: 164).

Proposition 6: The more a leader isrelationally attentive, the greater thelevel of employee physiological re-sourcefulness.

Leaders behave in ways that not only directlyshape social interaction patterns but also shapeinteractions by creating and galvanizing mean-ing. Podolny, Khurana, and Hill-Popper (2005)recently emphasized this path of leadership im-pact by articulating the different ways leadersshape organizational action through meaningmaking. We argue that when leaders propagatemeaning that evokes collaboration, cooperation,and interdependence between people—what wecall “relational images”—they motivate peopleto connect and ease connection possibilities.This, in turn, should increase physiological re-sourcefulness. For example, in studies of collab-oration among organizations with conflictingviews on environmental protection, Wondolleckand Yaffee (2000) showed that inclusive lan-guage and meaning that calls forth a commonidentity fosters positive social interactions be-tween previously warring groups. When leadersare able to foster a sense of shared fate, commonidentity, and interdependence, differences be-tween people become less extreme, and theground for positive interactions is tilled.

Proposition 7: The more leaders culti-vate and spread relational images,

the greater the level of physiologicalresourcefulness of employees.

In summary, we suggest that organizationalscholars can contribute to research on the phys-iology of social interactions by using organiza-tional research to understand how organiza-tions shape people’s opportunities to experiencepositive social interactions at work. Theoreti-cally, our perspective responds to the call for agreater understanding of the social and culturalembeddedness of the physiology of social inter-actions (Uchino, 2004). These propositions arenot exhaustive but are meant to illustrate theunique contributions that organizational schol-ars can offer to this vibrant research area. In theprocess, we recast organizations as consequen-tial vessels of physiological resourcefulnessand physical health.

IMPLICATIONS OF INTEGRATINGPHYSIOLOGICAL DATA INTO

ORGANIZATIONAL RESEARCH

The time is ripe to integrate physiology intoorganizational research because of technologi-cal innovations that make measures morewidely available, of higher quality, and less in-trusive for potential research participants. Inthis section we briefly review key issues for in-corporating physiological measures into organi-zational research.

First, it is important to remember that “physi-ological systems . . . follow their own basic lawsand are only loosely coupled with each otherand with other response systems such as psy-chological and behavioral reactions” (Semmeret al., 2004: 225). Thus, it is important to under-stand how a particular physiological measure isaffected by larger physiological systems. To il-lustrate, we can consider the example of thehormone cortisol. There are at least three waysof measuring cortisol. Cortisol is released undersome type of stress (a cortisol spike) and thenreturns to baseline levels. Cortisol also has astrong diurnal pattern, which is influenced byboth “trait” or stable factors and “state” or time-varying factors. One study found that between24 and 36 percent of variation in morning corti-sol levels was due to stable factors, about 62 to74 percent was due to state factors, and the restwas due to measurement error (Shirtcliff,Granger, Booth, & Johnson, 2005, as cited in

154 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review

Page 19: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

Adam, 2005). All three ways of measuring corti-sol—cortisol spikes, return to baseline after aspike, and diurnal patterns—are associatedwith health outcomes, albeit different ones.

If one is interested in measuring cortisolspikes in reaction to stress (e.g., Fischer, Cal-ame, Dettling, Zeier, & Fanconi, 2000), it is im-portant to control for the time of day (the diurnalpattern). If one is interested in explaining thestate variation in diurnal patterns of cortisol(e.g., Adam, 2005), then it is important to takemultiple measures throughout the day, in orderto compare not just levels but changes in thelevels (or slopes). Ideally, one should measuremore than one day to see if the pattern is stable.Each of the issues mentioned here in relation tocortisol could be said about any physiologicalmeasure or construct, such as allostatic load.

Second, the relationships among social, psy-chological, physiological, and behavioral vari-ables are “loosely coupled.” Researchers havefound that the correlations among physiologicaldata, self-rated health, physician-rated health,subjective experiences of one’s body (e.g., symp-toms), and measures of subjective well-beingare generally quite low (Brief, Butcher, George,& Link, 1993; Pennebaker, 1983). They are notinterchangeable measures, even though theyare sometimes treated as such (Jex & Beehr,1991). In addition, in order to interpret physiolog-ical measures, it is important to couple themwith other kinds of data, such as self-report andobservation (Semmer et al., 2004). For researchon positive social interactions, self-report datathat capture peoples’ subjective views of thequality of connections and relationships are es-pecially important. Finally, there are often setsof standard control variables that are essentialto making sense of the data, including medica-tion and current health status, smoking and caf-feine intake, and physical activity, among oth-ers (Cacioppo, Tassinary, & Berntson, 2000;Kiecolt-Glaser & Glaser, 1988).

Third, some physiological measures are eas-ier to assess in naturalistic settings than others.Technology to measure the cardiovascular sys-tem is relatively portable and unintrusive. As-sessing the immune and neuroendocrine sys-tems typically requires collecting saliva orblood, so these data are more difficult to collectin the field (for examples, see Adam, 2005; Ma-rucha, Kiecolt-Glaser, & Favagehi, 1998;Schaubroeck & Ganster, 1993).

Stepping back from the specific consider-ations of measurement and research design, theresearch agenda that we have outlined requiresinterdisciplinary collaboration. This brief over-view of the practical considerations of usingphysiological data in research can only scratchthe surface of this complex topic. The wealth ofknowledge that has developed in the last twentyyears has occurred in large part through thecollaboration of experts from many fields (e.g.,Ohio State’s Institute for Behavioral MedicineResearch). In classic fish-scale fashion (Camp-bell, 1969), scientific innovation has occurredthrough the overlapping of fields. Our hope isthat this paper will provoke interest in a physi-ological perspective on social interactions in or-ganizations, prompting continuing innovationsacross fields.

DISCUSSION

This paper has suggested that positive socialinteractions at work are associated with imme-diate and enduring effects on the cardiovascu-lar, immune, and neuroendocrine systems. Aphysiological perspective invites considerationof the body as a source of human action andcapability, because positive social interactionsat work can build physiological resourcefulnessand physical health. We illustrate this possibil-ity with propositions about work recovery andengagement. Organizational scholars can makevaluable contributions to the extant literatureby bringing in our unique perspective on howorganizational contexts shape employees’ expe-riences of social interactions. We have begun todescribe the organizational embeddedness ofhuman physiology at work with propositionsabout organizational practices, culture, andleadership. Positive social interactions are onecore pathway through which human physiologyis embedded in organizations. This kind of re-search involves learning about a kind of dataunfamiliar to most organizational researchers,and collaboration with experts in other fieldsmay be the best way to start such a researchprogram.

Establishing the link between positive socialinteractions and physiology is an important firststep, and it provokes a number of questions andextensions for future research. While we havefocused on positive social interactions, futureresearch will have to address questions about

2008 155Heaphy and Dutton

Page 20: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

people’s larger relational landscape. Employ-ees’ relational contexts are full of connectionsand relationships, some of which are positive,whereas others are undoubtedly neutral, harm-ful, or ambivalent (Holt-Lunstad et al., 2003). Re-search has suggested that negative interac-tions—for example, undermining behaviors—can be more potent in their impact on employeesthan positive relationships—for example, socialsupport (Duffy, Ganster, & Pagon, 2002). Giventhe potent effect of negative interactions at work(e.g., Pearson, Andersson, & Wegner, 2001; Tep-per, 2000), it may be especially important to un-derstand how to remedy or nullify these effectsby creating occasions for positive interactionsthat are health building rather than health de-pleting. Researchers, for example, can examinehow protective or health enhancing positive re-lationships can be in the context of a toxic workenvironment (Frost, 2003; Kahn, 2001). Onestream of research suggests that examining theratio of positive to negative interactions overtime (e.g., over the course of a meeting) can be auseful strategy for understanding how negativeand positive interactions combine to shape out-comes (Gottman, 1993; Losada & Heaphy, 2004).

It is important to note that several cardiovas-cular and immune studies have shown thatsome but not all work relationships are associ-ated with physiological outcomes. This raisesimportant questions about which work relation-ships matter and why. These questions are onlybeginning to be addressed by researchers, whosuggest that people are physiologically reactiveto those relationships that are meaningful to theself, which, in turn, is shaped by social andcultural forces (Dressler, Balieiro, & Dos Santos,1997; Kiecolt-Glaser & Newton, 2001).

By paying attention to physiological-levelchanges, we can begin to consider physicalhealth in more nuanced ways. Typically, it ismeasured as the absence of disease (Patrick &Begner, 1990). In organizational research this of-ten means using such coarse-grained measuresas days absent and health insurance costs(Danna & Griffin, 1999). Consistent with this fo-cus, most research on physical health has fo-cused on stress, which often has negative effectson physical health (Cooper et al., 2001). But re-cent critiques of this model have pushed fordefinitions of positive health, measures that cancapture it, and interventions that can build it(Bower & Segerstrom, 2004; Patrick & Begner,

1990; Ryff & Singer, 2000). We believe that ourpaper provides one such alternative. Physiolog-ical measures can help us capture more finelygrained measures of health, such as immunesystem strength, healthy cortisol patterns, andcardiovascular reactivity, and physiological re-sourcefulness is one of a number of ideas (e.g.,physical thriving; Epel et al., 1998) that can helpus see how physical health can be strengthened.Our paper suggests a series of organizationalpathways through which these measures of pos-itive health can be built.

A focus on how an organizational contextshapes the physiological resourcefulness of in-dividuals is part of a broader quest for organi-zational scholars to better understand endoge-nous resourcefulness—or the processes throughwhich resources are generated from within aperson, dyad, group, or organization (see Dutton& Ragins, 2007; Feldman, 2005; Rousseau & Ling,2007). Such a view assumes that resources arenot fixed assets but are valuable elements thatare created and changed by people in the waythat they behave. A focus on processes of endog-enous resourcefulness is consistent with posi-tive organizational scholarship’s goal of detail-ing conditions and processes that contribute tooptimal functioning in individuals, dyads,groups, and organizations (Cameron, Dutton, &Quinn, 2003; Dutton, Worline, Frost, & Lilius,2006). By linking features of an organizationalcontext to physiological resourcefulness, re-searchers and practitioners alike are drawn toconsider how organizational conditions contrib-ute to measures of optimal functioning (like en-gagement, health, etc.) through how they affectpositive social interactions and physiologicalprocesses.

This perspective on relationships as cultivarsof physiological resourcefulness is particularlyimportant to the study of work. Work is increas-ingly characterized by greater degrees of collab-oration, cooperation, and cross-boundary work,and the economy is increasingly based in ser-vice as opposed to manufacturing industries(National Research Council, 1999). It is not sur-prising, then, that social interactions often ac-count for the largest proportion of individuals’work time (Waldron, 2000). Accordingly, organi-zational scholars who wish to bring work backin to how we theorize and study employee andorganizational experiences (Barley & Kunda,2001) would do well to find ways to study how

156 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review

Page 21: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

interactions shape work processes and workoutcomes. Our paper also invites researchers tobring the body back into the study of work, notas an instrument of physical labor but by seri-ously considering how social interactions leaveconsequential physiological imprints (Heaphy,2007). If scholars answer this invitation, we seenew possibilities for how organizational schol-arship can inform not only our own disciplinebut studies of human physiology as well.

CONCLUSION

This paper began with the acknowledgmentthat physiology is not typically considered to bewithin organizational researchers’ domain. So-cial interactions, however, are at the heart ofmany streams of organizational research (Dut-ton & Heaphy, 2003). We link physiology andsocial interactions together by establishing thatsocial interactions at work, whether brief con-nections or enduring relationships, have physi-ological correlates and effects. We can then seehow the physiological resourcefulness built inpositive social interactions shapes micro-organizational behavior, such as engagementand work recovery, and how organizationsshape employees’ bodies by providing (or fail-ing to provide) opportunities for these positivesocial interactions to occur. At a fundamentallevel, these arguments invite organizational re-searchers to add a bodily, physiological dimen-sion to their research on organizational life, andto offer organizational explanations to the ongo-ing interdisciplinary conversations about thephysiology of social relationships occurringthroughout the academy.

REFERENCES

Acker, J. 1990. Hierarchies, jobs and bodies: A theory of gen-dered organizations. Gender and Society, 42: 139–158.

Adam, E., & Gunnar, M. 2001. Relationship functioning andhome and work demands predict individual differencesin diurnal cortisol patterns in women. Psychoneuroen-docrinology, 26: 189–208.

Adam, E. K. 2005. Momentary emotions and physiologicalstress levels in the everyday lives of working parents. InB. Schneider & L. J. Waite (Eds.), Being together, workingapart: Dual-career families and the work-life balance:105–133. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Antoni, M. H. 1987. Neuroendocrine influences on psychoneu-roimmunology and neoplasia: A review. Psychology andHealth, 1: 3–24.

Asmar, R., Darne, B., el Assaad, M., & Topouchian, J. 2001.Assessment of outcomes other than systolic and dia-stolic blood pressure: Pulse pressure, arterial stiffnessand heart rate. Blood Pressure Monitoring, 6: 329–333.

Baker, W. 2000. Achieving success through social capital.San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Baker, W., & Dutton, J. E. 2007. Enabling positive social cap-ital. In J. E. Dutton & B. R. Ragins (Eds.), Exploring posi-tive relationships at work: Building a theoretical andresearch foundation: 325–345. Mahwah, NJ: LawrenceErlbaum Associates.

Barley, S. R., & Kunda, G. 2001. Bringing work back in. Or-ganization Science, 12: 76–95.

Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. 2004. The neural correlates of maternaland romantic love. NeuroImage, 21: 1155–1166.

Benner, P. 1984. From novice to expert: Excellence and powerin clinical nursing practice. Menlo Park, CA: Addison-Wesley.

Benner, P. 2000. The roles of embodiment, emotion and life-world for rationality and agency in nursing practice.Nursing Philosophy, 1: 5–19.

Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., & McCabe, K. 1995. Trust, reciprocity andsocial history. Games and Economic Behavior, 10: 122–142.

Berscheid, E., & Lopes, J. 1997. A temporal model of relation-ship satisfaction and stability. In R. J. Sternberg &M. Hojjat (Eds.), Satisfaction in close relationships: 129–159. New York: Guilford Press.

Born, J., Lange, T., Kern, W., McGregor, G. P., Bickel, U., &Fehm, H. L. 2002. Sniffing neuropeptides: A transnasalapproach to the human brain. Nature Neuroscience, 5:514–516.

Bower, J. E., & Segerstrom, S. C. 2004. Stress management,finding benefit, and immune function: Positive mecha-nisms for intervention effects on physiology. Journal ofPsychosomatic Research, 56: 9–11.

Boyatzis, R. E., Smith, M. L., & Blaize, N. 2006. Developingsustainable leaders through coaching and compassion.Academy of Management Learning & Education, 5: 8–24.

Breiter, H. C., Aharon, A., Kahneman, D., Dale, A., & Shizgal,P. 2001. Functional imaging of neural responses to ex-pectancy and experience of monetary gains and losses.Neuron, 30: 619–639.

Brickson, S. L. 2005. Organizational identity orientation:Forging a link between organizational identity and or-ganizations’ relations with stakeholders. AdministrativeScience Quarterly, 50: 576–609.

Brickson, S. L. 2007. Organizational identity orientation: Thegenesis of the role of the firm and distinct forms of socialvalue. Academy of Management Review, 32: 864–888.

Brief, A. P., Butcher, A. H., George, J. M., & Link, K. E. 1993.Integrating bottom-up and top-down theories of subjec-tive well-being: The case of health. Personality and So-cial Psychology, 64: 646–653.

Britt, T. W., Dickinson, J. M., Greene, T. M., & McKibben, E. S.2007. Self engagement at work. In C. L. Cooper & D. Nel-son (Eds.), Positive organizational behavior: Accentuat-

2008 157Heaphy and Dutton

Page 22: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

ing the positive at work: 143–158. Thousand Oaks, CA:Sage.

Brondolo, E., Rieppi, R., Erickson, S. A., Bagiella, E., Shapiro,P. A., McKinley, P., & Sloan, R. P. 2003. Hostility, inter-personal interactions, and ambulatory blood pressure.Psychosomatic Medicine, 65: 1003–1011.

Cacioppo, J. T., Bernston, G. G., Sheridan, J. F., & McClintock,M. K. 2000. Multilevel integrative analyses of humanbehavior: Social neuroscience and the complementingnature of social and biological approaches. Psycholog-ical Bulletin, 126: 829–843.

Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., Crawford, E., Ernst, J. M.,Burleson, M. H., Kowalewski, R. B., Malarkey, W. B., VanCauter, E., & Bernston, G. G. 2002. Loneliness and health:Potential mechanisms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64: 407–417.

Cacioppo, J. T., Tassinary, L. G., & Berntson, G. B. 2000.Handbook of psychophysiology (2nd ed.). Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.

Cameron, K. E., Dutton, J. E., & Quinn, R. E. 2003. Foundationsof positive organizational scholarship. In K. E. Cameron,J. E. Dutton, & R. E. Quinn (Eds.), Positive organizationalscholarship: 3–13. San Francisco: Berrett-Koehler.

Campbell, D. T. 1969. Ethnocentrism and the fish-scalemodel of omniscience. In M. Sherif & C. W. Sherif (Eds.),Interdisciplinary relationships in the social sciences:328–348. Chicago: Aldine.

Carter, C. S. 1998. Neuroendocrine perspectives on socialattachment and love. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23:779–818.

Carter, C. S., & Altemus, M. 1997. Integrative functions oflactational hormones in social behavior and stress man-agement. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,807: 164–174.

Cohen, D., & Prusak, L. 2001. In good company: How socialcapital makes organizations work. Boston: Harvard Busi-ness School Press.

Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. 1985. Stress, social support, and thebuffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98: 310–357.

Cooper, C. L., Dewe, P. J., & O’Driscoll, M. P. 2001. Organiza-tional stress: A review and critique of theory, research,and applications. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Cross, R., & Parker, A. 2004. The hidden power of socialnetworks: Understanding how work really gets done inorganizations. Cambridge, MA: Harvard BusinessSchool Press.

Danna, K., & Griffin, R. W. 1999. Health and well-being in theworkplace: A review and synthesis of the literature.Journal of Management, 25: 357–384.

de Croon, E. M., Sluiter, J. K., & Frings-Dresen, M. H. W. 2003.Need for recovery after work predicts sickness absence:A 2-year prospective cohort study in truck drivers. Jour-nal of Psychosomatic Research, 55: 331–339.

DeVries, A. C., Glasper, E. R., & Detillion, C. E. 2003. Socialmodulation of stress responses. Physiology and Behav-ior, 79: 399–407.

Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. 2004. Acute stressors andcortisol responses: A theoretical integration and synthe-sis of laboratory research. Psychological Bulletin, 130:355–391.

Dienstbier, R. A. 1989. Arousal and physiological toughness:Implications for mental and physical health. Psycholog-ical Bulletin, 96: 84–100.

Dressler, W. W., Balieiro, M. C., & Dos Santos, J. E. 1997. Thecultural construction of social support in Brazil: Associ-ations with health outcomes. Culture, Medicine, Psychi-atry, 21: 303–335.

Duck, S. 2007. Finding connections at the individual/dyadiclevel. In J. E. Dutton & B. R. Ragins (Eds.), Exploringpositive relationships at work: Building a theoreticaland research foundation: 179–186. Mahwah, NJ: Law-rence Erlbaum Associates.

Duffy, M. K., Ganster, D. G., & Pagon, M. 2002. Social under-mining in the workplace. Academy of Management Jour-nal, 45: 431–451.

Dutton, J. E. 2003. Energize your workplace: How to create andsustain high-quality connections at work. San Francisco:Jossey-Bass.

Dutton, J. E., & Heaphy, E. D. 2003. The power of high qualityconnections at work. In K. Cameron, J. Dutton, &R. Quinn (Eds.), Positive organizational scholarship:Foundations of a new discipline: 263–278. San Francisco:Berrett-Koehler.

Dutton, J. E., & Ragins, B. R. (Eds.). 2007. Exploring positiverelationships at work: Building a theoretical and re-search foundation. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum As-sociates.

Dutton, J. E., Worline, M., Frost, P. J., & Lilius, J. 2006. Explain-ing compassion organizing. Administrative ScienceQuarterly, 51: 59–96.

Elfering, A., Grebner, S., Semmer, N. K., & Gerber, H. 2002.Time control, catecholomines and back pain amongyoung nurses. Scandinavian Journal of Work Environ-ment and Health, 28: 386–393.

Engelmann, M., Wotjak, C. T., Neumann, I., Ludwig, M., &Landgraf, R. 1996. Behavioral consequences of intra-cerebral vasopressin and oxytocin: Focus on learningand memory. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews,20: 341–358.

Epel, E., McEwen, B. S., & Ickovics, J. R. 1998. Embodyingpsychological thriving: Physical thriving in response tostress. Journal of Social Issues, 54: 301–322.

Esterling, B. A., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Bodnar, J., & Glaser, R.1994. Chronic stress, social support, and persistent al-terations in the natural killer cell response to cytokinesin older adults. Health Psychology, 13: 291–299.

Evans, O., & Steptoe, A. 2001. Social support at work, heartrate, and cortisol: A self-monitoring study. Journal ofOccupational Health Psychology, 6: 361–370.

Feldman, M. 2005. Explaining endogeneity. Paper presentedat the annual meeting of the Academy of Management,Honolulu.

Ferguson, J. N., Young, L. J., & Insel, T. R. 2002. The neuroen-

158 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review

Page 23: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

docrine basis of social recognition. Frontiers in Neuroen-docrinology, 23: 200–224.

Fernandez, R., Castilla, E., & Moore, P. 2000. Social capital atwork: Networks and employment at a phone center.American Journal of Sociology, 105: 1288–1356.

Fischer, J. E., Calame, A., Dettling, A. C., Zeier, H., & Fanconi,S. 2000. Experience and endocrine stress responses inneonatal and pediatric critical care nurses and physi-cians. Critical Care Medicine, 28: 3281–3288.

Fitzhugh, M. L., & Leckie, W. H. 2001. Agency, postmodernismand the causes of change. History and Theory, 40: 59–81.

Fleischmann, A. A., Spitzberg, B. H., Andersen, P. A., &Roesch, S. C. 2005. Tickling the monster: Jealousy induc-tion in relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Re-lationships, 22: 49–73.

Fletcher, B. C., & Jones, F. 1993. A refutation of Karasek’sdemand-discretion model of occupational stress with arange of dependent measures. Journal of OrganizationalBehavior, 14: 319–330.

Fletcher, J. 2007. Leadership, power and positive relation-ships. In J. E. Dutton & B. R. Ragins (Eds.), Exploringpositive relationships at work: Building a theoreticaland research foundation: 347–371. Mahwah, NJ: Law-rence Erlbaum Associates.

Frankenhaeuser, M. 1979. Psychoneuroendocrine ap-proaches to the study of emotion as related to stress andcoping. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation 1978, 26:123–161.

Freese, J., Li, J. A., & Wade, L. D. 2003. The potential relevanceof biology to social inquiry. Annual Review of Sociology,29: 233–256.

Freund, P. E. S. 1988. Bringing society into the body: Under-standing socialized human nature. Theory and Society,17: 839–864.

Frost, P. J. 2003. Toxic emotions at work. Cambridge, MA:Harvard Business School Press.

Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., & Elliot, A. J. 2003. Evidence forbivariate systems: An empirical test of appetition andaversion across domains. Journal of Research in Person-ality, 37: 349–372.

Ganster, D. C., Schaubroeck, J., Sime, W. E., & Mayes, B. T.1991. The nomological validity of the Type A personalityamong employed adults. Journal of Applied Psychology,76: 143–168.

Gottman, J. M. 1993. The roles of conflict engagement, esca-lation, and avoidance in marital interaction: A longitu-dinal view of five types of couples. Journal of Consultingand Clinical Psychology, 61: 6–15.

Graen, G. B., & Uhl-Bien, M. 1995. Development of leader-member exchange (LMX) theory of leadership over 25years: Applying a multi-level, multi-domain perspec-tive. Leadership Quarterly, 6: 219–247.

Greenhaus, J. H., & Powell, G. N. 2003. When work and familycollide: Deciding between competing role demands. Or-ganizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,90: 291–304.

Gutek, B. A. 1995. The dynamics of service. San Francisco:Jossey-Bass.

Hassard, J., Holliday, R., & Wilmott, H. 2000. Body and orga-nization. London: Sage.

Heaphy, E. 2007. Bodily insights: Three lenses for positiveorganizational relationships. In J. E. Dutton & B. R. Ra-gins (Eds.), Exploring positive relationships at work:Building a theoretical and research foundation: 47–71.Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Heinrichs, M., Baumgartner, T., Kirschbaum, C., & Ehlert, U.2003. Social support and oxytocin interact to suppresscortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress.Biological Psychiatry, 54: 1389–1398.

Henry, J. P., & Wang, S. 1998. Effects of early stress on adultaffiliative behavior. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23: 863–875.

Herbert, T. B., & Cohen, S. 1993. Stress and immunity inhumans: A meta-analytic review. Psychosomatic Medi-cine, 55: 364–379.

Holt-Lunstad, J., Uchino, B. N., Smith, T. W., Olson-Cerny, C.,& Nealey-Moore, J. B. 2003. Social relationships and am-bulatory blood pressure: Structural and qualitative pre-dictors of cardiovascular function during everyday so-cial interactions. Health Psychology, 22: 388–397.

Horvat-Gordon, M., Granger, D. A., Schwartz, E. B., Nelson,V. J., & Kivlighan, K. T. 2005. Oxytocin is not a validbiomarker when measured in saliva by immunoassay.Physiology and Behavior, 84: 445–488.

Houben, G. J., Diederiks, J. P. M., Kant, Y., & Notermans,J. V. H. 1990. Rationalization in garages in the Nether-lands and its effects on occupational health. Work andStress, 4: 179–189.

Ilies, R., Arvey, R. D., & Bouchard, T. J. 2006. Darwinism,behavioral genetics, and organizational behavior: A re-view and agenda for future research. Journal of Organi-zational Behavior, 27: 121–141.

Insel, T. R. 2000. Toward a neurobiology of attachment. Re-view of General Psychology, 4: 176–185.

Ituarte, P. H. G., Kamarck, T. W., Thompson, H. S., & Bacanu,S. 1994. Psychosocial mediators of racial differences innighttime blood pressure dipping among normotensiveadults. Health Psychology, 18: 393–402.

Jansen, N. W. H., Kant, I., & van den Brandt, P. A. 2002. Needfor recovery in the working population: Description andassociations with fatigue and psychological distress.International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 9: 322–340.

Jemmott, J. B., & Magloire, K. 1988. Academic stress, socialsupport, and secretory immunoglobin A. Journal of Per-sonality and Social Psychology, 55: 803–810.

Jex, S. M., & Beehr, T. A. 1991. Emerging theoretical andmethodological issues in the study of work-relatedstress. Research in Personnel and Human ResourcesManagement, 9: 311–365.

Joas, H. 1996. The creativity of action. Chicago: University ofChicago Press.

Kahn, W. A. 1990. Psychological conditions of personal en-

2008 159Heaphy and Dutton

Page 24: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

gagement and disengagement at work. Academy ofManagement Journal, 33: 692–724.

Kahn, W. A. 2001. Holding environments at work. Journal ofApplied Behavioral Science, 37: 260–279.

Karasek, R., & Theorell, T. 1990. Healthy work: Stress, produc-tivity, and the reconstruction of working life. New York:Basic Books.

Karasek, R. A., Gordon, G., Pietrokovosky, C., & Frese, M.1985. Job Content Instrument: Questionnaire and user’sguide. Los Angeles: University of Southern California,Department of Industrial and Systems Engineering.

Karlin, W. A., Brondolo, E., & Schwartz, J. 2003. Workplacesocial support and ambulatory cardiovascular activityin New York City traffic agents. Psychosomatic Medi-cine, 65: 167–176.

Kaufmann, G. M., & Beehr, T. A. 1986. Interactions betweenjob stressors and social support: Some counterintuitiveresults. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71: 522–526.

Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Bane, C., Glaser, R., & Malarkey, W. B.2003. Love, marriage, and divorce: Newlyweds’ stresshormones foreshadow relationship changes. Journal ofConsulting and Clinical Psychology, 71: 176–188.

Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Garner, W., Speicher, C. E., Penn, G., &Glaser, R. 1984. Psychosocial modifiers of immunocom-petence in medical students. Psychosomatic Medicine,46: 7–14.

Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Glaser, L. 1988. Methodological issuesin behavioral immunology research with humans. Brain,Behavior and Immunity, 2: 67–68.

Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Glaser, L. 1995. Measurement of im-mune response. In S. Cohen, R. C. Kessler, & L. U. Gor-don (Eds.), Measuring stress: A guide for health andsocial scientists: 215–229. New York: Oxford UniversityPress.

Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., McGuire, L., Robles, T. F., & Glaser, R.2002. Psychoneuroimmunology: Psychological influ-ences on immune function and health. Journal of Con-sulting and Clinical Psychology, 70: 537–547.

Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Newton, T. L. 2001. Marriage andhealth: His and hers. Psychological Bulletin, 127: 472–503.

Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr,E. 2005. Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature, 435:673–676.

Landsbergis, P. A., Schnall, P. L., Warren, K., Pickering, T. G.,& Schwartz, J. E. 1994. Association between ambulatoryblood pressure and alternative formulations of jobstrain. Scandinavian Journal of Work Environment andHealth, 20: 349–363.

Lercher, P., Hortnagel, J., & Kofler, W. W. 1993. Work noiseannoyance and blood pressure: Combined effects withstressful working conditions. International Archives ofOccupational and Environmental Health, 65: 23–28.

Levy, S. M., Herberman, R. B., Whiteside, T., Sanzo, K., Lee, J.,& Kirkwood, J. 1990. Perceived social support and tumorestrogen/progesterone receptor status as predictors of

natural killer cell activity in breast cancer patients. Psy-chosomatic Medicine, 52: 73–85.

Loehr, J., & Schwartz, T. 2003. Power of full engagement:Managing energy, not time, is the key to high perfor-mance and personal renewal. New York: Free Press Pa-perbacks.

Losada, M., & Heaphy, E. 2004. The role of positivity andconnectivity in the performance of business teams: Anonlinear dynamics model. American Behavioral Scien-tist, 47: 740–765.

Louis, M. 1980. Surprise and sensemaking: What newcomersexperience in entering unfamiliar organizational set-tings. Administrative Science Quarterly, 25: 226–251.

Lovallo, W. R., & Thomas, T. L. 2000. Stress hormones inpsychophysiological research: Emotional, behavioraland cognitive implications. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassi-nary, & G. B. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysi-ology (2nd ed.): 342–367. Cambridge: Cambridge Univer-sity Press.

Marucha, P. T., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Favagehi, M. 1998.Mucosal wound healing is impaired by examinationstress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 60: 362–365.

McEwen, B. S. 1998. Protective and damaging effects of stressmediators. New England Journal of Medicine, 338: 171–179.

Miller, J. B. 1988. Connections, disconnections, and violations.Stone Center Working Paper Series, Work in ProgressNo. 33, Wellesley, MA.

Miller, J. B., & Stiver, I. P. 1997. The healing connection: Howwomen form relationships in therapy and in life. Boston:Beacon Press.

National Research Council. 1999. The changing nature ofwork: Implications for occupational analysis. Washing-ton, DC: National Academy Press.

Nes, L. S., Segerstrom, S. C., & Sephton, S. E. 2005. Engage-ment and arousal: Optimism’s effects during a briefstressor. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31:111–120.

Newton, T. 2003. Truly embodied sociology: Marrying thesocial and the biological? Sociological Review, 51: 550–561.

Orlikowski, W. J. 2002. Knowing in practice: Enacting a col-lective capability in distributed organizing. Organiza-tion Science, 13: 249–273.

Orth-Gomer, K., & Unden, A.-L. 1987. The measurement ofsocial support in population surveys. Social ScienceMedicine, 24: 83–94.

Palatini, P., Penzo, M., Racioppa, A., Zugno, E., Guzzardi, G.,Anaclerio, M., & Pessina, A. C. 1992. Clinical relevanceof nighttime blood pressure and of daytime blood pres-sure variability. Archives of Internal Medicine, 152: 1855–1860.

Patrick, D. L., & Begner, M. 1990. Measurement of healthstatus in the 1990s. Annual Review of Public Health, 11:165–183.

Pearce, C., & Conger, J. (Eds.). 2003. Shared leadership: Re-framing the hows and whys of leadership. London: Sage.

160 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review

Page 25: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

Pearson, C., Anderrson, L., & Wegner, J. W. 2001. When work-ers flout convention: A study of workplace incivility.Human Relations, 54: 1387–1419.

Pennebaker, J. W. 1983. Physical symptoms and sensations:Psychological causes and correlates. In J. Cacioppo &R. E. Petty (Eds.), Social psychophysiology: 543–564. NewYork: Guilford Press.

Perlow, L. A., Gittell, J. H., & Katz, N. 2004. Contextualizingpatterns of work group interaction: Toward a nestedtheory of structuration. Organization Science, 15: 520–536.

Platt, M. L., & Glimcher, P. W. 1999. Neural correlates ofdecision variables in the parietal cortex. Nature, 400:233–238.

Podolny, J., Khurana, R., & Hill-Popper, M. 2005. Revisiting themeaning of leadership. Research in Organizational Be-havior, 26: 1–36.

Porges, S. W. 1998. Love: An emergent property of the mam-malian autonomic nervous system. Psychoneuroendo-crinology, 23: 837–861.

Rau, R., Georgiades, A., Fredrikson, M., Lemne, C., & deFaire,U. 2001. Psychosocial work characteristics and per-ceived control in relation to cardiovascular rewind atnight. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 6:171–181.

Reis, H. T., & Gable, S. L. 2002. Toward a positive psychologyof relationships. In C. L. M. Keyes, J. Haidt, & M. E. P.Seligman (Eds.), Flourishing: Positive psychology andthe life well-lived: 129–160. Washington, DC: AmericanPsychological Association.

Rimann, M., & Udris, I. 1997. Subjektive arbeitsanalyse: DerFragebogen SALSA. In O. Ulizh & O. Strohm (Eds.), Un-ternehmen arbeitspsychologisch bewerten. Zurich: udfHochschulverlag.

Rollag, K., Parise, S., & Cross, R. 2005. A relational view ofrapid on-boarding: Getting newcomers connected andproductive quickly. Sloan Management Review, 46(2):35–41.

Rothbard, N. P. 2001. Enriching or depleting? The dynamicsof engagement in work and family roles. AdministrativeScience Quarterly, 46: 655–684.

Rousseau, D. M., & Ling, K. 2007. Commentary: Following theresources in positive organizational relationships. InJ. E. Dutton & B. R. Ragins (Eds.), Exploring positiverelations at work: Building a theoretical and researchfoundation: 373–384. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum As-sociates.

Ryff, C. D., & Singer, B. 2000. Interpersonal flourishing: Apositive human health agenda for the new millennium.Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4: 30–44.

Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., & Caruso, D., 2002. The positivepsychology of emotional intelligence. In C. R. Snyder &S. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology: 159–171. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Schaubroeck, J., & Ganster, D. 1993. Chronic demands andresponsivity to challenge. Journal of Applied Psychol-ogy, 78: 73–85.

Schaufeli, W. B., & Bakker, A. B. 2004. Job demands, jobresources, and their relationship with burnout and en-gagement: A multi-sample study. Journal of Organiza-tional Behavior, 25: 293–315.

Schein, E. H. 2004. Organizational culture and leadership.San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Schnorpfeil, P., Noll, A., Schulze, R., Ehlert, V., Frey, K., &Fischer, J. 2003. Allostatic load and work conditions.Social Science and Medicine, 57: 647–656.

Seeman, T. 2001. How do others get under our skin?: Socialrelationships and health. In C. D. Ryff & B. Singer (Eds.),Emotion, social relationships and health: 189–210. NewYork: Oxford University Press.

Seeman, T., & McEwen, B. S. 1996. Impact of social environ-ment characteristics on neuroendocrine regulation. Psy-chosomatic Medicine, 58: 459–471.

Segerstrom, S. C., & Miller, G. E. 2004. Psychological stressand the human immune system: A meta-analytic studyof 30 years of inquiry. Psychological Bulletin, 130: 601–630.

Semmer, N. K., Grebner, S., & Elfering, A. 2004. Beyond self-report: Using observational, physiological, and situa-tion-based measures in research on occupational stress.In P. L. Perrewe & D. C. Ganster (Eds.), Emotional andphysiological processes and positive intervention strat-egies: Research in occupational stress and well-being,vol. 3: 205–263. Boston: JAI Press.

Shirtcliff, E. A., Granger, D. A., Booth, A., & Johnson, D. 2005.A latent-state trait model of salivary cortisol and atypi-cal social behavior in normally developing youth. De-velopment and Psychopathology, 17: 167–184.

Sluiter, J. K., Van der Beek, A. J., & Frings-Dresen, M. H. W.1999. The influence of work characteristics on the needfor recovery and experienced health: A study on coachdrivers. Ergonomics, 42: 573–583.

Snowdon, C. T., & Ziegler, T. E. 2000. Reproductive hormones.In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. B. Berntson (Eds.),Handbook of psychophysiology (2nd ed.): 368–396. Cam-bridge: Cambridge University Press.

Sonnentag, S. 2001. Work, recovery activities, and individualwell-being: A diary study. Journal of OccupationalHealth Psychology, 6: 196–210.

Sonnentag, S., & Fritz, C. 2006. Endocrinological processesassociated with job stress: Catecholamine and cortisolresponses to acute and chronic stressors. Employeehealth, coping and methodologies. Research in Occupa-tional Stress and Well Being, 5: 1–60.

Sonnentag, S., & Natter, E. 2004. Flight attendants’ dailyrecovery from work: Is there no place like home? Inter-national Journal of Stress Management, 11: 366–391.

Staessen, J. A., Asmar, R., DeBuyzere, M., Yutaka, I., Parati,G., Shimada, K., Stergiou, G., Redon, J., & Verdecchia, P.2001. Task Force II: Blood pressure measurement andcardiovascular outcomes. Blood Pressure Monitoring, 6:355–370.

Steptoe, A. 2000. Stress, social support and cardiovascularactivity over the working day. International Journal ofPsychophysiology, 37: 299–308.

2008 161Heaphy and Dutton

Page 26: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK

Stern, R. M., Ray, W. J., & Quigley, K. S. 2001. Psychophysio-logical recording (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford UniversityPress.

Stockhorst, U., & Pietrowsky, R. 2004. Olfactory perception,communication, and the nose-to-brain pathway. Physi-ology and Behavior, 83: 3–11.

Stone, A. A., Cox, D. S., Vladimarsdottir, H., & Neale, J. M.1987. Secretory IgA as a measure of immunocompetence.Journal of Human Stress, 13: 136–140.

Taylor, S. E. 2002. The tending instinct: How nurturing isessential for who we are and how we live. New York:Time Books.

Tepper, B. J. 2000. Consequences of abusive supervision.Academy of Management Journal, 43: 178–190.

Theorell, T., Orth-Gomer, K., & Eneroth, P. 1990. Slow-reacting immunoglobin in relation to social support andchanges in job strain: A preliminary note. Psychoso-matic Medicine, 52: 511–516.

Thomas, L. T., & Ganster, D. C. 1995. Impact of family-supportive work variables on work-family conflict andstrain: A control perspective. Journal of Applied Psychol-ogy, 80: 6–15.

Tschan, F., Semmer, N. K., & Inversin, L. 2004. Work relatedand “private” social interactions at work. Social Indica-tors Research, 67: 145–182.

Uchino, B. 2004. Social support and physical health: Under-standing the consequences of relationships. New Haven,CT: Yale University Press.

Uchino, B., Cacioppo, J., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. 1996. The rela-tionship between social support and physiological pro-cesses: A review with emphasis on underlying mecha-nisms and implications for health. PsychologicalBulletin, 199: 488–531.

Uchino, B. N., Holt-Lunstad, J., Uno, D., & Flinders, J. B. 2001.Heterogeneity in the social networks of young and older

adults: Prediction of mental health and cardiovascularreactivity during acute stress. Journal of BehavioralMedicine, 24: 361–382.

Unden, A.-L., Orth-Gomer, K., & Elofsson, S. 1991. Cardiovas-cular effects of social support in the work place: Twenty-four-hour ECG monitoring of men and women. Psycho-somatic Medicine, 53: 50–60.

Uvnas-Moberg, K. 1998. Oxytocin may mediate the benefitsof positive social interaction and emotions. Psychoneu-roendocrinology, 23: 819–835.

Wager, N., Feldman, G., & Hussey, T. 2003. The effect onambulatory blood pressure of working under favourablyand unfavourably perceived supervisors. Occupationaland Environmental Medicine, 60: 468–474.

Waldron, V. R. 2000. Relational experiences and emotions atwork. In S. Fineman (Ed.), Emotion in organizations: 64–82. London: Sage.

White, R. E., Thornhill, S., & Hampson, E. 2006. Entrepreneursand evolutionary biology: The relationship between tes-tosterone and new venture creation. Organizational Be-havior and Human Decision Processes, 100: 21–34.

Winnubst, J. A. M., Marcelissen, F. H. G., & Kleber, R. J. 1982.Effects of social support in the stressor-strain relation-ship: A Dutch sample. Social Science and Medicine, 16:475–482.

Wondolleck, J., & Yaffee, S. L. 2000. Making collaborationwork: Lessons for innovation in natural resource man-agement. Washington, DC: Island Press.

Wright, T. A., & Diamond, W. J. 2006. Getting the pulse of youremployees: The use of cardiovascular research in betterunderstanding behavior in organizations. Journal of Or-ganizational Behavior, 27: 395–401.

Zak, P. J., Kurzban, R., & Matzner, W. T. 2004. The neurobiol-ogy of trust. Annals of the New York Academy of Sci-ences, 358: 1737–1748.

Emily D. Heaphy ([email protected]) is an assistant professor at McGill Uni-versity, Desautels Faculty of Management. She received her Ph.D. in organizationalbehavior from the University of Michigan. Her research focuses on work relationshipsas a means of understanding the embodiment of work, agency, workplace conflict,and professional image.

Jane E. Dutton ([email protected]) is the Robert L. Kahn Distinguished UniversityProfessor of Business Administration and Psychology at the University of Michigan.She received her Ph.D. from Northwestern University. Her research focuses on thrivingand resilience at work, with a particular focus on high-quality relationships, theconstruction and impacts of positive identities, compassion at work, and job crafting.

162 JanuaryAcademy of Management Review

Page 27: POSITIVE SOCIAL INTERACTIONS AND THE HUMAN BODY AT WORK