Personality and Social Psychology Review -...

39
http://psr.sagepub.com/ Review Personality and Social Psychology http://psr.sagepub.com/content/15/2/142 The online version of this article can be found at: DOI: 10.1177/1088868310373752 2011 15: 142 originally published online 17 August 2010 Pers Soc Psychol Rev Susan E. Cross, Erin E. Hardin and Berna Gercek-Swing of Self-Construal What, How, Why, and Where The Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com On behalf of: Society for Personality and Social Psychology can be found at: Personality and Social Psychology Review Additional services and information for http://psr.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Email Alerts: http://psr.sagepub.com/subscriptions Subscriptions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav Permissions: http://psr.sagepub.com/content/15/2/142.refs.html Citations: at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011 psr.sagepub.com Downloaded from

Transcript of Personality and Social Psychology Review -...

http://psr.sagepub.com/Review

Personality and Social Psychology

http://psr.sagepub.com/content/15/2/142The online version of this article can be found at:

 DOI: 10.1177/1088868310373752 2011 15: 142 originally published online 17 August 2010Pers Soc Psychol Rev

Susan E. Cross, Erin E. Hardin and Berna Gercek-Swing of Self-ConstrualWhat, How, Why, and WhereThe

  

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of: 

  Society for Personality and Social Psychology

can be found at:Personality and Social Psychology ReviewAdditional services and information for     

  http://psr.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

 

http://psr.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:  

http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:  

http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:  

http://psr.sagepub.com/content/15/2/142.refs.htmlCitations:  

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Personality and Social Psychology Review15(2) 142 –179© 2011 by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navDOI: 10.1177/1088868310373752http://pspr.sagepub.com

The What, How, Why, andWhere of Self-Construal

Susan E. Cross1, Erin E. Hardin2, and Berna Gercek-Swing1

Abstract

Since the publication of Markus and Kitayama’s pivotal article on culture and the self, the concepts of independent, relational, and interdependent self-construal have become important constructs in cultural psychology and research on the self. The authors review the history of these constructs, their measurement and manipulation, and their roles in cognition, emotion, motiva-tion, and social behavior. They make suggestions for future research and point to problems still to be sorted out. Researchers interested in these constructs have many opportunities to make important contributions to the literature in a variety of fields, including health psychology, education, counseling, and international relations.

Keywords

culture, ethnicity, self, identity, social cognition

When research on the self burst onto the social psychological scene more than three decades ago, it reflected the cultural assumptions and beliefs of the researchers’ backgrounds. These researchers were largely from European, Canadian, and European American backgrounds, and they assumed that the self was autonomous, independent of the influence of others, bounded, unique, integrated, and fundamentally prior to society and social relationships (Geertz, 1975). In the inter-vening decades, both the researchers studying the self and the assumptions about the self have become more culturally diverse. This diversity has resulted in the articulation and development of the concept of self-construal. First coined by Markus and Kitayama (1991), research on self-construal has come to have a prominent place among influential concepts in psychology, especially in regard to research on self-related concepts and processes, social cognition, close relationships, and cultural psychology.

This article reviews the existing research on self-construal and its role in cognition, emotion, motivation, and social behavior. Our overarching goal is to provide a guidebook for researchers—an integrated look at where research and theory on self-construal are after 20 years and where we believe they are going and need to go. To achieve this goal, we first address the What is it? question with a brief review of the history of this construct and its development since the original statement by Markus and Kitayama (1991). Second, we address How is it investigated? with a review of the most common measure-ment tools and manipulations. Third, we address the question, Why is self-construal important? by detailing the accumulated evidence regarding its role in psychological processes. Through-out, we also seek to point out gaps, problems, or inconsistencies

in the research record to explore Where to from here? with the hopes that researchers will continue to further refine the methods used to investigate this topic and further explore the implications of self-construal for human behavior.

What Is Self-Construal?Psychological researchers have long been interested in the existence and implications of multiple representations of the self. William James (1890), for example, distinguished the material, social, and spiritual selves. One of the most produc-tive distinctions has been made between private and public selves (Baumeister, 1986; Greenwald & Pratkanis, 1984). This distinction between inner, unobservable selves and the outer selves we portray to others provided a useful conceptual framework for understanding a range of representations of the self and their implications for cognition, affect, and behavior. Not surprisingly, however, this early work did not explicitly consider the ways in which culture shapes the development and expression of these different types of self. In one of the earliest efforts to do so, Triandis (1989) elaborated on the distinctions among the private, public, and collective selves (cf. Greenwald & Pratkanis, 1984) and argued that cultural differences reliably lead to differential expression or sampling

1Iowa State University, Ames, IA, USA2Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX, USA

Corresponding Author:Susan E. Cross, Iowa State University, Department of Psychology, W117 Lagomarcino Hall, Ames, IA 50011Email: [email protected]

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Cross et al. 143

of these three selves. Around the same time, Markus and Kitayama (1991) similarly reviewed research that showed that members of European or American and Asian cultures con-structed differing self-views. As they illustrated, Western cultures prioritize the individual over the group, and individuals seek independence, autonomy, and separateness from others. In East Asian cultures, the group is prioritized over the indi-vidual, and individuals seek to fit into the group and maintain harmony in the group. Although other researchers had broached the issue of the self as critical in understanding cultural dif-ferences, Triandis (1989) and Markus and Kitayama’s (1991) articles were the first to make this argument clearly and to make it accessible to a wide audience.

Markus and Kitayama (1991) coined the term self-construal in describing the ways that Americans and Japanese define and make meaning of the self. To construe means to “show or explain the meaning or intention of” (Random House Diction-ary, 1980); thus, self-construal refers to how individuals define and make meaning of the self. Markus and Kitayama identified two such self-construals, independent and interdependent. Although they noted that these are only two of many possible self-construals, the term self-construal has become virtually synonymous with independence and interdependence; thus, self-construal is typically defined as how individuals see the self in relation to others.

Markus and Kitayama (1991) proposed that Europeans and Americans construe the self as fundamentally individual and separate from others, and they labeled this the independent self-construal (IndSC). For persons with high IndSC, the ques-tion, “Who am I?” is likely to be answered with reference to internal traits that are stable across situations (e.g., outgoing, shy, intelligent, ambitious) or that set one apart from others (e.g., especially creative, a straight-A student). Demonstrating one’s uniqueness is an important basis of self-esteem. Being “the same person” across situations and communicating assert-ively are considered signs of maturity. Interpersonal relation-ships are, of course, important to those with an IndSC, but significant others are typically important foremost for how they benefit the individual (e.g., for the support or esteem they provide to the independent individual). Furthermore, others are a source of social comparison for confirming one’s uniqueness (compared to my friends, I am more creative) and internal traits (I demonstrate my compassion in my relationships with my family).

In contrast, Markus and Kitayama (1991) pointed out that the Japanese tend to construe the self as fundamentally con-nected to others and defined by relationships with others, which they labeled the interdependent self-construal (InterSC). For persons with high InterSC, the question, “Who am I?” is likely to be answered with reference to important relationships (e.g., daughter, friend, coworker) or group memberships (fraternity member, Asian American). Demonstrating one’s ability to fit into the group is an important basis of self-esteem. Changing one’s behavior to respond to the demands of different situations

and regulating emotional expression to maintain group har-mony are considered signs of maturity. Interpersonal relation-ships are of utmost importance to those with an InterSC, with the individual concerned primarily with how he or she benefits the group. Others become a source of definition for the self (I am a responsible student and an impulsive friend), and social comparison is used to determine whether one is fulfilling obli-gations within those relationships.

Markus and Kitayama (1991) and others (e.g., Singelis, 1994; Triandis, 1989) have argued that individuals possess both IndSC and InterSC but that cultural context typically promotes the development of one or the other self-construal more strongly. As noted above, the United States and Japan are generally considered prototypical cultures that promote development of IndSC and InterSC, respectively, with IndSC tending to be more well elaborated and salient in Western countries and InterSC tending to be more well elaborated and salient in non-Western countries, including parts of Asia, Africa, and Central and South America. Not surprisingly then, IndSC and InterSC are typically identified as corresponding to individualist and collectivist cultures. However, Markus and Kitayama never make this connection explicitly.

Nonetheless, the connection between IndSC and individual-ism and between InterSC and collectivism is clear—so clear, in fact, that it can be difficult to distinguish between self-construal and individualism–collectivism (IND-COL). Con-ceptually, IND-COL is a dimension used to describe cultures, whereas self-construal describes individuals. IND-COL, then, is not an individual differences variable, whereas self-construal is. This distinction is complicated by the fact that where a country falls on the IND-COL dimension is typically deter-mined by the average responses of individuals on self-report surveys. Indeed, many self-report measures of IND-COL and self-construal share items. Despite these measurement prob-lems, the conceptual distinction remains: IND-COL describes cultures; self-construal describes individuals. Sports analogies are helpful in understanding this distinction. We might refer to a football team as having a good defense, whereas we describe the individual members of the defense as quick, agile, and ath-letic. Without these individual traits of quickness, agility, and athleticism, the team could not have a strong defense, just as a culture can not be individualistic if the majority of its mem-bers are not independent. Similarly, the individual members of the football team vary in the degree to which they possess these traits, just as individuals within a culture vary in the degree to which they espouse an independent versus interde-pendent self-construal.

A strong defense in football requires more than just skilled players; it also requires strategies and game plans that go beyond the individual players’ abilities. Similarly, self-construal is only one of several components that distinguish individual-istic and collectivistic cultures. For example, Triandis (1995) identified three additional attributes that distinguish individu-alistic and collectivist cultures: (a) variation in structure of goals

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

144 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

(in-group focused vs. individual focused), (b) behavior as a function of social norm versus individual attitudes, and (c) a focus on the needs of the in-group versus social exchanges. These attributes may in some ways be related to self-construal (e.g., variation in goals may derive from variation in self-construal), but in this article, we will define self-construal as one component of IND-COL. Unfortunately, the two terms are often confused or used interchangeably. Where relevant, we comment on this confusion and strive to articulate the source of the confusion and clarify the important differences.

Tripartite Self-Construal: Relational–Interdependent and Collective–Interdependent Self-ConstrualsThe distinctions made by Markus and Kitayama (1991) and Triandis (1989) began to stimulate investigation into other vari-eties of self-construal. Triandis described private, public, and collective selves, but this tripartite distinction has unfortunately received relatively little empirical attention. Instead, researchers have focused more attention on self-construal based on close relationships as a third dimension of self-construal (Brewer & Gardner, 1996; Y. Kashima et al., 1995). Y. Kashima and his colleagues (1995) were the first to demonstrate that independent, collective, and relational views of the self were empirically separable. In addition, they argued that whereas individualism and collectivism differentiated Western from East Asian cultural groups, across cultures men and women were best differentiated on the relatedness dimension.

In a separate line of research, Cross and Madson (1997; Cross, Bacon, & Morris, 2000) argued that gender differences in behavior among Western participants could also be explained by differences in self-construal. Integrating research by gender theorists (e.g., Gilligan, 1982; Jordan & Surrey, 1986; Stewart & Lykes, 1985) with the newer work on culture and self, they suggested that women in Western societies were more likely than men to construct self-views that incorporated close relationships with other people. Men, on the other hand, were more likely to conceive of themselves as independent or separate from their close relationships (but for a different perspective on gender differences in self-construal, see Baumeister & Sommer, 1997; Gabriel & Gardner, 1999). Their article went on to detail how these differences in self-construals could explain many observed gender differences in cognition, motivation, emotion, and social behavior.

Although Cross and Madson (1997) used the term inter-dependent self-construal to refer to ways in which women in the West may define themselves in terms of close relationships, they acknowledged that the form of interdependence experi-enced by Western women could be quite different from the group-oriented interdependence of East Asians. As they wrote then, “[F]or U.S. adults, a self-construal based on relationships with others may be more likely to focus on individual relation-ships (e.g., with one’s spouse, close friends, siblings) than on group memberships or social roles” (p. 8). In later work, Cross and her colleagues (2000), referred to this as the

relational–interdependent self-construal (often shortened to relational self-construal [RelSC]). They defined RelSC as the extent to which people define themselves in terms of close relationships and differentiated it from the group-centered collective–interdependent self-construal described by Markus and Kitayama (1991; often measured by the Singelis, 1994, Interdependent Self-Construal Scale).

RelSC, therefore, is a general individual difference construct and is distinct from other similar-sounding constructs that focus on specific close relationships. For example, the Inclusion of Other in the Self scale (Aron, Aron, Tudor, & Nelson, 1991) taps commitment to a specific relationship rather than a general ori-entation to represent oneself in terms of close relationships (as does the Relational-Interdependent Self-construal Scale [RISC]). Andersen’s (Andersen & Chen, 2002) theory of relational selves also focuses on specific relationships and on individuals’ distinc-tive and persistent ways of interacting in particular relationships. For instance, Leigh’s interactions with her peers may be marked by confidence and positive emotions, but the particular relational self (in Andersen’s terms) that has been created in her interactions with her overbearing older brother may be uniquely marked by resentment and anger. RelSC, instead, is conceived as a relatively global dimension that captures the extent to which people con-ceptualize themselves as defined by their close relationships.

Several researchers have sought to differentiate RelSC and collective–interdependent self-construal. For example, Gabriel and Gardner (1999; also see Baumeister & Sommer, 1997) investigated the hypothesis that American men and women differ in the extent to which they are characterized by these two forms of InterSC. Using a variety of methods, they found that American women were more likely than men to character-ize themselves in relational terms but men were more likely than women to describe themselves in terms of larger, group collectives. Thus, InterSC may be considered to have two com-ponents: a relational component (RelSC) and a collective or group-oriented component (Coll-InterSC).

Unfortunately this distinction is rarely observed by research-ers; many times, the term interdependent self-construal is used without regard to whether close relationships or group member-ships are the focus of attention. This is understandable, given the history of the concept of InterSC. With a focus on East–West differences, Triandis (1995) and Markus and Kitayama (1991) defined collectivism and InterSC in terms of close relationships and important in-groups, including families and work groups (thus, they grouped together relational and group-oriented com-ponents of self-construal). More recently, Yuki, Maddux, Brewer, and Takemura (2005) showed that East Asian in-groups tend to be based on relational ties whereas Western groups are based on categorical distinctions between in-groups and out-groups. Thus, they suggest that InterSC among East Asians primarily focuses on close relationships (and so emphasizes the RelSC) whereas InterSC among European Americans primarily focuses on groups and collectives (and so emphasizes Coll-InterSC). Building on this work and taking the perspective of social identity theory,

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Cross et al. 145

Brewer and Chen (2007) argued that collectivism is best under-stood as membership in large shared social categories (e.g., eth-nicity, nationality) rather than in groups of relationship connections, and they showed that most measures of collectivism or InterSC focus on interpersonal relationships. As this debate illustrates, there is significant confusion over the boundaries between RelSC and Coll-InterSC. We view InterSC as a super-ordinate category, with RelSC and Coll-InterSC as subcompo-nents within this larger category. In many cases researchers have used InterSC to refer to both relational and collective orientations; we have chosen to adopt the researchers’ usage in these cases except where one or the other subcomponents is made explicit. Despite these distinctions, most research continues to focus on the more general consequences of InterSC and IndSC articulated by Markus and Kitayama (1991). In the next section, we briefly review their hypotheses.

Hypothesized Consequences of Cultural Differences in Self-ConstrualMarkus and Kitayama (1991) offered detailed discussions of the implications of self-construal for cognition, emotion, and motivation, reviewing cross-cultural literature that supported their hypotheses. Of course, such literature did not directly measure self-construal, so Markus and Kitayama relied on the assumption that Japanese people were more likely to have an InterSC, whereas those from Western cultures were more likely to have an IndSC (cf. Matsumoto, 1999). Markus and Kita-yama demonstrated that a broad range of cultural differences in cognition, emotion, and motivation could be explained by invoking cultural differences in self-construal. We briefly review their hypotheses concerning the effects of self-construal on cognition, emotion, and motivation.

Effects of self-construal on cognition. Given that those with an InterSC define the self in relation to others and are motivated to fit in to the current social context, Markus and Kitayama (1991) argued that interdependent individuals are more likely to pay attention to others and to social context, resulting in three primary effects on cognition. First, individuals with an InterSC are likely to know more about, and thus to have more elaborate cognitive representations of, others than are those with an IndSC. Second, individuals with an InterSC are more likely to have cognitive representations of the self that incor-porate a social context than are independent individuals. Finally, Markus and Kitayama (1991) argued that self-con-strual also affects cognitive processes that appear to be non-social, such as counterfactual thinking or reasoning about abstract concepts. They argued that interdependent individuals are more likely to consider their role in the relationship between themselves and the interviewer and thus to respond to apparently abstract questions differently than an indepen-dent individual who is much less, if at all, concerned with how his or her answer will be construed.

Effects on emotion. Arguing that most emotions implicate the self, Markus and Kitayama (1991) hypothesized that “emo-tional experience should vary systematically with the construal of the self” (p. 235). More specifically, emotional experience should vary in two ways. First, the same emotion may be elic-ited by different conditions for IndSC and InterSC. Second, people with IndSC versus InterSC will express and experience different emotions. Markus and Kitayama (1991) distinguished between so-called ego-focused emotions (e.g., anger, pride) and other-focused emotions (e.g., shame). They hypothesized that those with an IndSC are more likely to express and to experience ego-focused emotions, whereas those with an InterSC are more likely to express and to experience other-focused emotions. Relatedly, Markus and Kitayama hypoth-esized that people with differing self-construals would express and/or experience the same emotions with differing intensity. For example, not only are interdependent individuals expected to experience anger less often than independent individuals, but they are expected to experience anger less intensely when they do experience it.

Effects on motivation. Markus and Kitayama (1991) offered several hypotheses regarding the effects of self-construal on the types of goals individuals are motivated to pursue. The most intuitive of these is that individuals with an InterSC are more likely to be motivated by socially oriented goals. Impor-tantly, it is not that these individuals have a higher need for affiliation, per se, but rather that they are more likely to be motivated to fulfill their roles within important relationships. Second, Markus and Kitayama argued that agency, or a sense of personal control and efficacy, is experienced differently, depending on one’s self-construal. Specifically,

for those with independent selves, agency will be experienced as an effort to express one’s internal needs, rights, and capacities and to withstand undue social pressure, whereas among those with interde-pendent selves, agency will be experienced as an effort to be receptive to others, to adjust to their needs and demands, and to restrain one’s own inner needs or desires. (p. 240)

In other words, independent and interdependent individuals both experience a sense of being active agents in pursuit of their goals, but these specific goals differ depending on one’s self-construal (cf. Kağitçibaşi, 2005).

Third, Markus and Kitayama (1991) argued that motives such as self-enhancement take on different meanings when the self that is being enhanced is independent versus interdepen-dent. For example, given an InterSC, self-esteem is based on one’s ability to fit in, whereas for those with an IndSC, self-esteem is based on one’s ability to be unique. Thus, to enhance the IndSC, one is motivated to demonstrate uniqueness, result-ing in self-serving biases, which are much less common among

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

146 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

those with an InterSC. These differences in the self and bases for self-esteem also mean that different motives will be expe-rienced as more or less positive or desirable, depending on self-construal. For example, the so-called need for abasement may be desirable for those with an InterSC but considered almost pathological for those with an IndSC.

Finally, given that a sign of maturity for those with an InterSC is the ability to refrain from expressing private attitudes and feelings to maintain social harmony, counterattitudinal behavior is not likely to create dissonance-related distress among those with an InterSC. In other words, those with an InterSC are much less likely to be motivated to reduce cogni-tive dissonance than are those with an IndSC.

In the remainder of this article, we examine the ways that researchers have responded to and elaborated on the original Markus and Kitayama (1991) theses. Because of space limitations, we focus on research that employs either measures or manipulations of self-construal. Unfortunately, therefore, a thorough description of research that uses cul-ture as a proxy for self-construal must be saved for another time. We begin first with a review of the most frequently used means of measuring and manipulating self-construal, followed by a review of the research examining the role of self-construal in cognition, emotion, motivation, and social behavior.

How Is Self-Construal Studied? Approaches to Measuring and Manipulating Self-ConstrualMeasuring Independent and Interdependent Self-Construal

Likert-type scales. The majority of research on self-construal has relied on self-report measures with Likert-type scales. Measures of IND-COL, sociotropy–autonomy, allocentrism, and other related constructs are sometimes used as proxy measures of self-construal. However, several measures have been designed specifically to measure IndSC and InterSC as individual-differences variables as defined by Markus and Kitayama (1991). The first and most common of these is the Self-Construal Scale (SCS; Singelis, 1994). The SCS provides separate scores for IndSC and InterSC, consistent with theo-retical predictions that the two self-construals are orthogonal dimensions rather than opposite ends of a single continuum (Singelis, 1994). An initial 45-item pool included original items and modified items drawn from existing measures of related constructs. Exploratory principal components analysis, fol-lowed by confirmatory factor analyses, in multiethnic Hawaiian samples resulted in two 12-item scales. Scores on the items demonstrated the expected between-group differences, with Asian Americans being more interdependent and less indepen-dent than European Americans. Furthermore, InterSC scores predicted participants’ tendency to make situational attributions

for behaviors described in short vignettes, supporting the pre-dictive validity of the SCS. However, the interitem reliabilities of the two scales tend to be adequate at best. Singelis (1994) reported Cronbach’s alpha reliabilities of .73 and .74 for the IndSC scale and .69 and .70 for the InterSC scale. Most pub-lished research using these scales report comparable reli-abilities, although they have been found to be even lower (e.g., α = .58 and .53 on the InterSC and InterSC scales, respectively, among undergraduates in Hong Kong; Singelis, Bond, Sharkey, & Lai, 1999).

Additional items have been added to the SCS, resulting in various versions being used in the literature, including IndSC scales with 13 (Singelis et al., 1999), 14 (Trafimow & Finlay, 1996), 15 (Hardin, Leong, & Osipow, 2001), and 16 items (Kwan, Bond, & Singelis, 1997) and InterSC scales with 14 (e.g., Yamada & Singelis, 1999) and 15 items (e.g., Hardin et al., 2001; Kwan et al., 1997). The 12- and 15-item versions appear to be most common. In addition, some authors have created a unidimensional self-construal score by reverse scoring the InterSC items (e.g., Aaker, 2000; Pöhlmann & Hannover, 2006); however, such unidimensional scores are clearly con-trary to the intended use of the SCS and theoretical understand-ing of the nature of self-construal (Singelis, 1994). Although most items have good face validity, several have been ques-tioned (e.g., “I value being in good health above everything”; Hardin, Leong, & Bhagwat, 2004; Levine et al., 2003). The SCS has been used in hundreds of studies and translated into numerous languages, including Chinese (Aaker & Schmitt, 2001), Japanese (Ozawa, Crosby, & Crosby, 1996), Greek (Nezlek, Kafetsios, & Smith, 2008), Thai (Polyorat & Alden, 2005), Arabic, and Hebrew (Kurman, 2001).

A second self-report measure of self-construal is that devel-oped by Gudykunst et al. (1996). As with the SCS (Singelis, 1994) a large item pool was drawn from existing measures (including the SCS) and supplemented with original items. Data from four countries (United States, Japan, Korea, and Australia) were submitted to a principal components analysis, after stan-dardizing item responses within culture. On one of the two factors, 29 items had acceptably high loadings, resulting in a 14-item IndSC scale and a 15-item InterSC scale. A total of 3 (6) items on the IndSC (InterSC) scale also appear on the Singelis scales. Cronbach’s alpha reliabilities were adequate to good, ranging from .80 (United States) to .85 (Korea) on the IndSC scale and from .73 (Korea) to .83 (Australia) on the InterSC scale. Although the four cultural groups did not differ as might be expected in mean scores on the IndSC and InterSC scales, self-construal did predict high and low context communication as expected.

Hackman, Ellis, Johnson, and Staley (1999) further tested and refined the Gudykunst et al. (1996) scales. Using confir-matory factor analysis (CFA) with a large sample of under-graduates drawn from three countries (the United States, New Zealand, and Kyrgyzstan), Hackman et al. were unable to get the hypothesized two-factor model to adequately fit the

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Cross et al. 147

data, despite numerous data-driven modifications to the model. However, when the IndSC and InterSC items were factor ana-lyzed separately, two well-fitting one-factor solutions were derived after dropping several items from each scale and allow-ing pairs of error variances among some items to covary. These analyses resulted in an 11-item IndSC scale and a 12-item InterSC scale. Cronbach’s alpha reliabilities were adequate to good, ranging from .89 (United States) to .77 (Kyrgyzstan) on the IndSC scale and from .86 (New Zealand) to .78 (Kyrgyzstan) on the InterSC scale. Moreover, the 11-item IndSC scale dem-onstrated measurement invariance (i.e., invariant factor load-ings) across all three cultures; two items on the InterSC scale were not invariant in the Kyrgyzstan sample. Collapsing across the three samples, both scales demonstrated measurement invariance between genders.

A third Likert-type measure of self-construal was developed by Leung and Kim in 1997 and described in an unpublished manuscript (cited in Levine et al., 2003); not surprisingly, there-fore, it has been used less in the literature than the other two measures. It consists of two 15-item scales, with the majority of items (11 independent and 9 interdependent) appearing on at least one of the other two scales (see Levine et al., 2003, for a list of items appearing on all three measures). Other less fre-quently used measures have been developed by Oyserman (1993), Kağitçibaşi (2007), and E. S. Kashima and Hardie (2000).

Twenty Statements Test as a measure of self-construal. The Twenty Statements Test (TST; Kuhn & McPartland, 1954) has also been used as a self-report measure of self-construal (e.g., Agrawal & Maheswaran, 2005; Somech, 2000). On the TST, participants are asked to complete 20 sentence stems that begin, “I am . . .” Responses to these statements may then be coded by the researcher as either independent (e.g., “I am intelligent”; “I am shy”), relational (e.g., “I am Bob’s girl-friend”; “I am a mother”), or interdependent (e.g., “I am on the soccer team”; “I am African American”). The number of independent, relational, and interdependent statements gener-ated may then be used as self-construal scores. Unfortunately, authors rarely provide detailed descriptions of their coding schemes, and researchers may define RelSC and InterSC quite differently. As a result, it is sometimes difficult to compare the findings from different researchers using the TST. Fur-thermore, the TST does not assess the importance of self-views to the individual. Members of East Asian societies often belong to fewer groups compared to members of Western societies, but these groups are arguably more important, stable, and self-defining for East Asians (Triandis, 1989).

Self-report measures of self-construal: Two or more factors? Given that Markus and Kitayama’s (1991) self-construal theory posits two dimensions of self-construal, IndSC and InterSC, all three Likert-type measures described above were explicitly designed to measure only these two factors. Neither Singelis (1994) nor Gudykunst et al. (1996) considered more

than two factors during development of their self-construal scales. However, there is considerable evidence that this simple two-factor structure does not provide a good fit to the data from any of the scales (Hardin, 2006; Hardin et al., 2004; Levine et al., 2003). In fact, the results of a CFA led Singelis (1994) to note that for the SCS, “Though clearly superior to the one-factor model, the two-factor model seems only ade-quate” (p. 586). The two-factor model was found to be a poor fit to responses on (a) the Leung and Kim (1997) scale in samples of college students from the United States (mainland and Hawaii), Japan, and South Korea (Levine et al., 2003), (b) the Gudykunst et al. (1996) scale among college students in Hawaii (Levine et al., 2003), and (c) Singelis’s (1994) SCS among college students in South Korea, Japan (Levine et al., 2003), and the mainland United States (Hardin, 2006; Hardin et al., 2004; Levine et al., 2003).

More recent research suggests that despite being designed to measure self-construal in terms of independence and inter-dependence, these measures actually have a multidimensional structure. Content analyses of the TST (Somech, 2000) and factor analyses of the Leung and Kim (1997) scale (Levine et al., 2003) and the Singelis (1994) scale (Guo, Schwartz, & McCabe, 2008; Hardin, 2006; Hardin et al., 2004; Sato & McCann, 1998) all show that multidimensional structures fit the data better than a simple two-factor structure. For example, Hardin et al. (2004) identified a higher order factor structure underlying items on the SCS, with four independence factors (Autonomy/Assertiveness, Individualism, Behavioral Consis-tency, and Primacy of Self) and two interdependent factors (Esteem for Group and Relational Interdependence). Note that the two interdependence factors identified in the SCS items map closely onto the RelSC–Coll-InterSC distinction discussed above. Importantly, this higher order multifactor structure rep-licated to samples of Asian American (Hardin et al., 2004), European American (Hardin, 2006; Hardin et al., 2004), African American, and Latino/a (Hardin, 2006) students.

Measuring such specific aspects of IndSC and InterSC does seem to be useful. For example, Somech (2000) found that although kibbutz members were generally more interdependent and less independent than urban residents in Israel, the kibbutzim endorsed some aspects of IndSC more than the urban residents, who endorsed some aspects of InterSC more than the kibbutzim. Cultural differences in in-group favoritism were better explained by the degree to which participants emphasized personal or in-group interests than by broader measures of self-construal or IND-COL (Y.-R. Chen, Brockner, & Katz, 1998). Hardin (2006) also found that more than 50% more variance in social anxiety could be accounted for by aspects of independence and interde-pendence than by the broader dimensions.

Based on this evidence that existing measures of self-construal have a multidimensional structure and that these specific aspects of independence and interdependence are useful predictors of between-group differences and other psy-chological phenomena, there is a clear need for a psychometrically

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

148 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

sound measure of multidimensional self-construal. Hardin, Cross, and Hoang (2009) have begun development of such a measure.

Implicit measures of self-construal. Recently, Kitayama and his colleagues have used several different measures of basic psy-chological tendencies as implicit measures of self-construal. Kitayama, Mesquita, and Karasawa (2006) were apparently the first to suggest that measures of psychological processes thought to reliably be related to IndSC and InterSC might be used as implicit measures of self-construal. They suggested that measures of the tendency to experience socially engaging emotions (e.g., guilt, relatedness) versus socially disengaging emotions (e.g., anger, pride) might be useful as implicit mea-sures of self-construal (see the section below on self-construal and affect for more on the distinction between socially engag-ing and disengaging emotions). Later, Kitayama, Park, Sevincer, Karasawa, and Uskul (2009) used such a measure as one of five implicit measures of self-construal. The others were ten-dency to make situational versus dispositional attributions, use of holistic versus focused attention, relational versus individual correlates of happiness, and symbolic self-inflation. As expected, participants from the United States scored significantly more independently (i.e., reported more socially disengaging emo-tions, made more situational attributions, used more focused attention, demonstrated stronger personal correlates of happi-ness, and demonstrated a larger inflated self) than participants from Japan; moreover, participants from two Western European countries (the United Kingdom and Germany) did not differ from each other and scored between the U.S. and Japanese participants on all five measures. Despite this consistent pattern of results, the five implicit measures were uncorrelated with each other across all four groups, a point to which we return at the end of the article. In addition, scores on an explicit measure of self-construal, Singelis’s (1994) SCS, were also uncorrelated with all five implicit measures, across all four groups. This lack of convergence between the explicit SCS measure and the implicit measures is consistent with other research that shows that implicit and explicit measures sometimes do not correlate (e.g., Bosson, Swann, & Pennebaker, 2000; Greenwald & Farnham, 2000).

Measuring Relational–Interdependent Self-ConstrualSoon after making the argument that gender differences among Western populations could be explained in terms of a RelSC, Cross and her colleagues created a measure of this relational form of self-construal (termed the Relational-Interdependent Self-construal Scale; Cross et al., 2000). This measure focuses on the degree to which the individual defines the self in terms of close relationships. A sample item is “My close relationships are an important reflection of who I am.” Its explicit focus on the individual’s self-definition distinguishes it from more gen-eral measures of communal orientation (e.g., the measure by

Clark, Oullette, Powell, & Milberg, 1987, which focuses on a desire for reciprocity and equity in close relationships), trait-oriented measures of communion or expressivity (Spence, Helmreich, & Stapp, 1974), and a measure of the emotional consequences of investment in close relationships used by Y. Kashima et al. (1995).

Research examining the psychometric properties of the RISC scale among North American college students (N = 4,228; reported in Cross et al., 2000) revealed that it has good internal reliability (Cronbach’s alphas generally range from .85 to .90) and good stability over a 2-month period (test–retest reliability is .76). Among these North American samples, women usually score higher than men (ds range from –.17 to –.57). This measure correlates moderately positively with other measures of relatedness (e.g., the Communal Orientation scale, r = .41, and the Singelis InterSC Scale, r = .41) but does not correlate with measures of independence (e.g., the Singelis IndSC Scale, r = .08). The RISC scale generally is unrelated to well-being measures such as Satisfaction with Life (r = .07; Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985) and the Rosenberg (1965) Self-Esteem Scale (r = .01). Examination of the incre-mental utility of the RISC scale showed that it predicted a significant proportion of variance in scores on broad measures of attitudes toward relationships and group memberships, controlling for other related measures (Cross et al., 2000). Thus, the RISC scale taps self-definition in a unique fashion that is not tapped by other measures of relatedness, expres-sivity, or communalism.

Although Cross and Madson (1997) argued that women in the United States are more likely to construct a RelSC than are men, RelSC is characterized by the degree to which one includes others in the self or defines the self in terms of close relation-ships, not the degree to which individuals endorse gender-stereotyped attributes. In fact, Cross and her colleagues (2000) were wary of making cross-cultural claims about gender dif-ferences in their measure of the RelSC because they recognized that cultures may vary widely in their construction of gender-related norms. Unfortunately, others have at times assumed that culturally based stereotypes of men and women are equivalent to IndSC and RelSC, respectively (e.g., Guimond, Chatard, Martinot, Crisp, & Redersdorff, 2006). For example, Guimond et al. (2006) define IndSC with items that were found to be central to the stereotype of men in France: lazy, coarse, boastful, and selfish. Although these authors argue that this set of attri-butes is consistent with the masculine gender role of dominance and power, their argument is based on the mistaken assumption that IndSC is defined by the male gender role and that RelSC is defined by the female gender role. This confounding of cul-tural gender stereotyping and self-construal impedes progress; cultural stereotypes may vary widely, but definitions of IndSC, InterSC, and RelSC are anchored in a theoretical focus on the relation of self to others. This theoretical focus on self–other relations that defines self-construal is independent of a particular culture’s understanding of gender.

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Cross et al. 149

Measuring Collective–Interdependent Self-Construal

Although the RelSC is defined in terms of significant dyadic relationships, the Coll-InterSC is defined in terms of significant group memberships. Brewer and Chen (2007) demonstrated that although most existing measures of collectivism and of interdependence assess both types of interdependence, relation-ally oriented items are more than twice as common. Brewer and Chen argued that cross-cultural researchers need to clearly delineate between these two types of interdependence, as they have very different implications and predict different outcomes. For example, although past research has noted that men tend to score lower than women on measures of interdependence (see Cross & Madson, 1997), Gabriel and Gardner (1999) demon-strated that distinguishing between RelSC and Coll-InterSC yields more nuanced findings: Men score lower than women in terms of RelSC but higher than women in terms of collective–InterSC.

The measure used in Gabriel and Gardner (1999) study, the Collective Interdependent Self-Construal Scale (CISC), was, until recently, the only measure specifically designed to assess collective or group InterSC. Gabriel and Gardner cre-ated the CISC by modifying the RISC (Cross et al., 2000) to replace references to relationship partners with references to group memberships (e.g., “When I think of myself, I often think of groups I belong to as well” instead of “When I think of myself, I often think of my close friends of family as well”). In a sample of college students, the CISC showed excellent interitem reliability (α = .90), was uncorrelated with scores on the RISC (r = .07), and was correlated with a measure of masculinity (r = .25).

More recently, Harb and Smith (2008) created the Six-Fold Self-Construal Scale that integrates Brewer’s work on the personal, relational, and group selves (Brewer & Chen, 2007; Brewer & Gardner, 1996) with Singelis’s work on vertical and horizontal IND-COL (Singelis, Triandis, Bhawuk, & Gelfand, 1995). The scale assesses the horizontal and vertical collective self-construal as well as the horizontal and vertical RelSC, the personal self-construal, and a humanity-bound self-construal. Each of the six dimensions comprises five items (e.g., “I feel I have a strong relationship with _______”) that are rated on a 7-point Likert-type scale. The horizontal collec-tive dimension referred to “students at my university,” whereas the vertical collective dimension refers to “my social group-ing.” In samples of college students from the United Kingdom, Lebanon, Syria, and Jordan, reliabilities on all six dimensions were adequate to good, ranging from .68 on the personal self dimension (Jordan) to .92 on the vertical collective dimension (the United Kingdom). Most reliabilities were in the mid-.80s. As might be expected, the Arab samples tended to score higher than the U.K. sample on the collective self-construals, whereas the U.K. sample tended to score higher than the other samples on the RelSCs. In light of Brewer and Chen’s (2007) call for researchers to distinguish between the relational and collective

(or group) interdependence, new measures such as this are likely to be of increasing utility.

Between- and Within-Group Differences in Self-ConstrualThese measures have facilitated the explosion in research on self-construal by allowing researchers explicitly to measure IndSC and InterSC and to test their relations to a range of other cognitive, affective, and behavioral variables (which we review more thoroughly below). The development of these measures has also allowed researchers to explore between- and within-group differences in self-construal. To the surprise of many, however, the results from such research tend to be inconsistent, often showing that individuals from different countries do not demonstrate the expected differences in self-construal. After reviewing the literature, Matsumoto (1999) concluded that North Americans do not have higher IndSCs than East Asians, who do not have higher InterSCs than North Americans. A later meta-analysis (Levine et al., 2003) also found that East Asians do not have a higher InterSC than North Americans, nor do East Asians have a higher InterSC than IndSC. Both reviews find examples not only of an absence of difference (e.g., North Americans and Japanese having equally high Ind-SCs; Krull et al., 1999; Sato & Cameron, 1999) but of theoreti-cally incongruent differences, such as North Americans having a higher InterSC than Japanese (Kleinknecht, Dinnel, Kleinknecht, Hiruma, & Hirada, 1997; Sato & Cameron, 1999; for a review, see Oyserman, Coon, & Kemmelmeier, 2002). More recent research continues to find such anomalies using these measures (Kitayama et al., 2009). Matsumoto concludes that the theory of self-construal itself is fundamentally flawed: Culture does not reliably predict self-construal in the ways predicted by theory.

Other authors have been less quick to attribute the mixed results in the literature to flawed theory without first considering other possibilities. One of these is that scores on self-construal measures are affected by situational priming, such that uncon-trolled aspects of the research environment inadvertently acti-vate one or the other self-construal (Levine et al., 2003). Across three studies using the Leung and Kim (1997) scales, however, there was no evidence that situational priming affected either InterSC or InterSC scores among North Americans.

Levine et al. (2003) interpreted these results as supporting another explanation for the mixed results in the literature: that measures of self-construal are fundamentally flawed. These authors argued that because the InterSC is defined as a situation-ally determined, and therefore variable, sense of self, interde-pendence scale scores should be sensitive to priming. In failing to find such priming effects, they concluded that the InterSC scale lacks construct validity. Other authors, however, have argued that although we would expect a high InterSC indi-vidual’s sense of self and concomitant cognitions, emotions, or behaviors to vary based on the situation, we would not expect

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

150 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

his or her fundamental level of InterSC to be variable (Gudykunst & Lee, 2003; M.-S. Kim & Raja, 2003). Thus, these authors contend, the failure of priming to induce changes in self-construal scale scores is actually evidence for the construct validity of these scales.

Other contextual factors may, in fact, explain the failure to find predicted between-group differences, however. Heine, Lehman, Peng, and Greenholtz (2002) demonstrated that so-called reference group effects inherent in the Likert-type scales used by common self-report measures of self-construal can account for much of the data. Just as a 5’9” man would rate himself somewhat tall if he lived in a culture where the average male height was 5’8” but short if in a culture where the average height was 6’, a moderately interdependent individual might rate himself or herself quite differently in terms of interde-pendence depending on his or her reference group. If North Americans use other (presumably highly independent, on aver-age) North Americans as their reference group, they will have a lower IndSC score than if they use Japanese as their referent group, who are presumably perceived to be less independent.

Heine et al. (2002) argued that because measures of self-construal do not specify a reference group, participants are likely to use their own cultural group as their referent. Indeed, across two studies with bicultural Canadian and Japanese par-ticipants, such reference group effects were found in scores on Singelis’s (1994) SCS. When participants were given no explicit instructions regarding reference groups (i.e., the standard self-construal scale instructions were used), expected between-group differences were weak or absent. When participants were explic-itly asked to use their own group as a reference, differences opposite to that predicted by theory were found: Canadians had significantly higher InterSC scores than the Japanese. However, when participants were explicitly asked to use the other group as their reference, expected between-group differences were found: Bicultural Canadians comparing themselves to “most Japanese” perceived themselves as significantly more indepen-dent and less interdependent than bicultural Japanese who were comparing themselves to “most North Americans,” which is exactly what self-construal theory would predict.

Furthermore, because individuals within the same country are likely to share a common cultural standard of reference, reference-group effects should be attenuated or absent when self-construal scale scores are compared for different ethnic groups within the same country (e.g., Asian Americans com-pared to European Americans), allowing expected between-group differences more readily to emerge (Heine et al., 2002). Indeed, Levine et al. (2003) noted that most studies that use multiethnic samples within the same country yielded results consistent with the predictions of self-construal theory. Finally, we would expect such reference-group effects to affect explicit, self-report measures of self-construal, but not more implicit measures. Indeed, Kitayama et al. (2009) found that North Americans, Western Europeans, and Japanese differed in the expected directions on all five of their implicit measures of

self-construal, despite demonstrating unexpected patterns on the explicit measure (Singelis’s, 1994, SCS).

One other methodological issue also accounts for the con-flicting data on between- and within-group differences in self-construal: problems with the content of existing self-report measures (Noguchi, 2007). Hardin et al. (2004) noted that the items on Singelis’s (1994) SCS do not capture several important aspects of IndSC (e.g., the importance of one’s private thoughts and feelings or the role of others in social comparison) or InterSC (e.g., a preference for communicating indirectly). Noguchi argued more specifically that (a) existing measures fail to adequately assess the extent to which indi-viduals rely on and are sensitive to external versus internal information and (b) this is precisely the domain in which cul-tural differences should be most likely to emerge. Thus, researchers have failed to find expected cross-cultural differ-ences in self-construal because they have failed to measure specific aspects of self-construal most likely to differ cross-culturally. To test this hypothesis, Noguchi constructed a new measure with items specifically designed to tap into the extent to which one considers others’ viewpoints (i.e., the extent to which one relies on external versus internal information). Across several studies, Noguchi found that Japanese students did score higher on this other-focused factor whereas American students scored higher on a self-focused factor. Interestingly, Americans consistently scored higher on a helping-others factor, providing further evidence for the importance of both the specific content of items on self-construal scales and mea-suring self-construal multidimensionally.

Manipulations of Self-ConstrualThe development of priming manipulations took research on self-construal into a new era. This development allows research-ers to move from reliance on culture as a proxy for self-con-strual or on explicit self-report measures to experimental manipulations of these constructs. As a result, researchers can more confidently examine causal hypotheses and within-culture consequences of activation of the three different com-ponents of self-construal (Oyserman & Lee, 2008). The premise in this work is that all persons, no matter their cultural back-ground, construct independent, relational, and collective–inter-dependent self-construals. Cultural practices and affordances, however, result in variability in the elaboration and accessibil-ity of independent, relational, or interdependent self-knowl-edge. Priming techniques can make the self-knowledge associated with one or another of these self-construals tem-porarily accessible; thus, priming can be used to experimen-tally examine the effects of IndSC, RelSC, and InterSC on behavior. In this section, we describe the primary ways that self-construal has been manipulated and some of the issues surrounding these manipulations.

The first studies to manipulate IndSC and InterSC were conducted by Trafimow, Triandis, and Goto (1991). They

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Cross et al. 151

examined the effects of two different manipulations with U.S. college students. In the first technique, participants are asked to think about what makes them different from their family and friends (IndSC prime) or what makes them similar to their family and friends (InterSC prime; we refer to this as the simi-lar/different prime technique). In the second technique, par-ticipants read a story about a ruler selecting a general to send to war based on either individualistic concerns (how it would increase the ruler’s status, prestige) or collective concerns (because he was a member of the ruler’s family). They exam-ined the effects of these manipulations on open-ended responses to the “Who am I?” test (Kuhn & McPartland, 1954). The proportions of two kinds of statements were analyzed: private or idiocentric self-views (deriving from the IndSC) and col-lective self-views (deriving from the InterSC). A third cat-egory, which they termed allocentric responses, referred to aspects of “interdependence, friendship, responsiveness to others, and sensitivity to the viewpoints of others” (p. 650). Because these relational responses were not relevant to their hypotheses, they were eliminated from the analyses. As expected, participants exposed to the IndSC primes described themselves using more individual, private terms than did those exposed to the InterSC primes, whereas those exposed to the InterSC primes reported more collective and group-oriented responses than did those exposed to the IndSC primes (also see Ybarra & Trafimow, 1998, Study 3). Examination of a small group of Asian Americans in their sample revealed a very similar pattern of responses to the primes.

Unfortunately, these studies did not include a neutral or no-prime control condition, which would help disentangle the direction of the effects in these studies. Given that one would expect European American participants to describe themselves using primarily personal, individual descriptors, it is unclear from these results whether the proportions of the two types of self-statements were increased or decreased by the manipula-tions relative to a control condition.

A few years later, Brewer and Gardner (1996) and Gardner, Gabriel, and Lee (1999) introduced a third approach to manipu-lating self-construal. In this technique, participants read a story about going on a trip to the city and circle either singular pronouns (I, me, mine; IndSC prime) or plural pronouns (we, our, us; InterSC prime). Control conditions included a similar task in which third-person pronouns (they, them) or impersonal pronouns (it) were circled. The first series of studies by Brewer and Gardner (1996) focused on aspects of the interdependent (we) self and compared the effects of the we circling task to the control tasks. They hypothesized that when the InterSC (we-us) is primed, others are included in the self, resulting in an increased perception of similarity to others. Using a reaction time task, Brewer and Gardner found that we primed participants made judgments of similarity more quickly than those in the they prime condition. A subsequent study revealed that InterSC-primed participants endorsed collectivist values and obligations to help more than did IndSC-primed participants (Gardner et al., 1999).

These differences were mediated by differences in TST responses; participants primed with InterSC tended to describe themselves in terms of their relationships and group memberships more than did the IndSC-primed participants; TST responses in turn pre-dicted responses to the values and helping measures.

Until recently, it was unclear whether these common InterSC priming techniques made RelSC or group-oriented, Coll-InterSC accessible (or perhaps a combination of the two; Oyserman & Lee, 2008). Brewer and Gardner (1996) attempted to tease apart these two dimensions in studies that contrasted a large group collective to the small group implied in the trip to the city task, but they were only partly successful. In their meta-analysis of IND-COL priming manipulations, Oyserman and Lee (2008) categorize the original Brewer and Gardner (1996) pronoun circling task as a relational collectivism manipulation, but they categorize the Sumerian ruler story manipulation as a mixture of relational and group-level collectiv-ism. Unfortunately, they provide little explanation for the basis of these decisions, and little empirical evidence is available to guide such decisions. In short, there has been much ambiguity remaining about which domain of the InterSC is primed by these commonly used primes—the RelSC or the Coll-InterSC.

Recently, however, Stapel and van der Zee (2006) modified the Brewer and Gardner (1996) manipulation to make these distinctions more clear. For RelSC, they modified the paragraph about the trip to the city to start with sentences that clearly focus on a dyad (“I am with you. We are together, the two of us.”). For the Coll-InterSC, they began the paragraph with sentences that focus on being part of a group (“We are a group of people.”). Manipulation checks documented that the IndSC, RelSC, and Coll-InterSC manipulations had the intended effects on the degree to which participants valued being an independent, unique person, being in close relationships, or being part of a group. Using this new method, researchers will be better able to develop and test theories that distinguish the role and func-tion of RelSC and Coll-InterSC in behavior.

Several other priming tasks have also been used successfully in other studies. For example, Utz (2004) asked participants to write about a typical day in the life of a student from a first-person singular perspective or from a third-person singular perspective. She found this manipulation interacted with chronic social value orientation to predict cooperative behavior. Stapel and Koomen (2001) primed IndSC and InterSC by having par-ticipants write a story about themselves, describing either “Who I am” using the words I, me, myself, and mine in each sentence or “Who we are” using the words we, our, ourselves, and ours (see Wang and Ross, 2005, for a similar task). Others have used a scrambled sentence task with words such as I, me, mine, unique, different, and independence to prime IndSC and we, our, us, cooperative, share, and similar to prime InterSC (Briley & Wyer, 2001; Kühnen & Hannover, 2000; Utz, 2004).

Several cautions are in order for the researcher interested in priming self-construal. First, the degree to which manipula-tions prime the self-construal versus other dimensions of

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

152 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

culture or of individualism and collectivism (IND-COL) is often unclear. For example, some studies use bilingual par-ticipants and manipulate the language used in the study (Kem-melmeier & Cheng, 2004; Ross, Xun, & Wilson, 2002; Trafimow, Silverman, Fan, & Law, 1997) or manipulate cul-tural icons (Y.-Y. Hong, Morris, Chiu, & Benet-Martinez, 2000). These approaches likely activate the associated self-construal (e.g., InterSC when Chinese language or icons are used) but also likely activate many other dimensions of IND-COL that are unrelated to self-construal. For example, Oyser-man and Lee (2008) conclude that languages vary in the degree to which they are focused on abstract versus concrete ways of thinking, which could in turn influence a variety of cognitive tasks. In addition, some manipulations (e.g., language or cul-tural icons) likely activate other aspects of a specific culture that are unrelated to self-construal. Because of these concerns, studies that manipulate the language used by participants as a proxy for variation in self-construal have been excluded from our review of the literature. More importantly, research-ers should be cognizant of the many ways that people from different cultures differ from each other and should not pre-sume that priming different self-construals in members of one cultural group is equivalent to using members of different cultural groups. It is a gross understatement to say that observ-ing the behavior of an American student who has been exposed to one of the InterSC tasks described above is not the same as observing a Japanese or Chinese student. If, however, self-construal is the mechanism hypothesized to explain cultural differences in behavior, this approach would shed light on this causal process in ways that measures do not.

Which of the manipulations described above is most effec-tive in producing differences in self-construal? A meta-analysis of the consequences of priming manipulations by Oyserman and Lee (2008) helps answer this question. They examined the effects of priming tasks on self-concept-related outcomes (e.g., the TST and responses to the Singelis SCS measure). Their results show that the strongest effects were found using the similar/different task (d = .44, n = 4), followed by the Sumerian warrior task (d = .37, n = 6). Other manipulations (e.g., the pronoun circling task) had smaller effects (d < .29) on self-concept-related outcome measures (also see Lalwani & Shavitt, 2009, for a comparison of the effects of different manipulations on responses to the TST). Oyserman and Lee’s meta-analysis also revealed that the different priming approaches differen-tially affected other types of behavior. For example, the similar/different task had the strongest effects on relationality tasks (e.g., liking for another participant), and the pronoun circling task had the strongest effects on cognitive tasks (e.g., perfor-mance on the Embedded Figures task). This suggests that the different priming techniques may activate somewhat different cognitive processes and that researchers should choose their approaches carefully in light of the outcomes in question.

Priming is sometimes a delicate task, and Kühnen and Hannover (2000) caution against the use of very transparent

or obvious priming manipulations. When participants are exposed to a very explicit priming task (Moskowitz & Roman, 1992) or when they are reminded of a previous task meant to prime a construct (Strack, Schwarz, Bless, Kubler, & Wanke, 1993), their judgments contrast with the direction of the expected prime (termed a correction contrast). To examine this effect in the self-construal domain, Kühnen and Hannover created a scrambled sentence task in which the four-word sentences focused on either IndSC (“I like being unique”) or InterSC (“I support my team”). Each scrambled sentence also included one additional word that was related to the type of prime—either a word reflecting independence (assertive) or a word reflecting interdependence (help). The authors varied the salience of the IndSC or InterSC prime by having participants write down either the unscrambled sentence (the “overt” prime) or the unnecessary word (the “subtle” prime). When participants were exposed to the subtle primes, the IndSC prime led to less perceived similarity to a target person than the InterSC prime, as would be expected. But the opposite occurred when they were primed with the overt IndSC prime: Participants viewed themselves as more similar to the target person than when primed with the InterSC prime. The effects in the explicit prime condition may be a consequence of the correction effect sug-gested by others (Strack et al., 1993), or it may be because of a rebalancing of the need for uniqueness and relatedness, as suggested by Brewer’s (1991) optimal distinctiveness theory. Whatever the explanation, this research provides a note of caution to researchers that very strong, explicit manipulations of self-construal may have unintended consequences.

Finally, researchers should ask themselves, “How might chronic individual differences interact with priming manipula-tions to affect outcomes?” Relatively little research has focused on the interaction of individual differences and priming manipu-lations within cultural groups (but see Utz, 2004). Gardner and her colleagues (1999) found that priming the culturally nondomi-nant dimension of self-construal (e.g., InterSC for European Americans and IndSC for East Asians) resulted in greater dif-ferences relative to a no-prime condition than priming the culturally dominant self-construal. The work by Gardner et al. (1999) also points to our final point about priming manipula-tions: Relatively few studies have used these priming techniques with non-Western samples. The Oyserman and Lee (2008) meta-analysis of IND-COL priming studies compared the effects of these oft-used priming techniques for participants from Europe or European American backgrounds and Asian backgrounds. Very few studies have used Asian samples (or samples from Africa or Latin America, for that matter), but Oyserman and Lee’s results indicate that the effects of the Sumerian Warrior and the similar/different priming tasks are similar for East Asians and European Americans for most tasks (see their Table 6). More studies are needed to assess the effectiveness of the pronoun circling task for Asian samples (see, e.g., Oyserman, Sorenson, Reber, & Chen, 2009). As Kitayama, Duffy, and Uchida (2007) suggest, if the self of East Asians is understood to be

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Cross et al. 153

chronically intertwined with others, priming I may activate we as well, resulting in weaker priming effects among East Asians than has been observed among Westerners.

Summary and future directions for measures and manipulations The methods of measuring or manipulating IndSC, RelSC, and Coll-InterSC are continuing to evolve. New measurement approaches are focusing on clarifying specific facets of each of these self-views (Hardin et al., 2009) and on developing implicit measures of IndSC and InterSC (Kitayama et al., 2009). Implicit measures of RelSC are still lacking, however. One starting place for the development of an implicit measure of RelSC is the indi-vidual versus relational version of the IAT used by Cross, Morris, and Gore (2002, Study 1). Undoubtedly, however, creating an implicit measure valid for multiple cultures is a daunting task.

Several new manipulations of self-construal have been reported in the past decade, but researchers frequently fail to include control conditions. Researchers seeking to use these manipulations should attempt to specifically define the dimen-sion of self-construal of interest and think carefully about the implied focus of the priming manipulation (e.g., relational or collective–interdependent self-construal). The newly developed manipulations that differentiate these two dimensions will enhance researchers’ ability to untangle their effects on behavior. Finally, the great majority of these approaches have been devel-oped in the West by Western researchers with Western assump-tions about the person. Although cultural psychology has begun to shine a light on these assumptions and biases, many of these methods may nonetheless reflect cultural assumptions about the person, about ways of thinking and knowing, and about relationships. Focused attempts to consider these constructs from non-Western perspectives and to devise methods to assess or manipulate them that are free from Western biases are the next steps in exploring how variation in self-construal influences behavior. In the next section, we review research that has exam-ined the role of self-construal in cognition, emotion, motivation, and social interaction.

How Do Self-ConstrualsShape Behavior?For more than three decades, researchers have demonstrated the central importance of the self in information processing, affect regulation, motivation, and social behavior (for reviews, see Bau-meister, 1998; Leary & Tangney, 2003; Markus & Wurf, 1987). Most of this work, however, was based on Western samples and assumed an IndSC. If the self is defined interdependently, many of these self-related processes may vary. Although Markus and Kitayama (1991) and Triandis (1989) focused on the role of dif-fering views of the self in explaining cultural differences in behav-ior, researchers soon found that variation in self-construal was also a useful tool for investigating within-culture processes

(particularly in Western societies). Figure 1 depicts the ways that IndSC, Coll-InterSC, and RelSC are thought to influence cogni-tion, affect, motivation, and, through them, social behavior. Although we strive to distinguish Coll-InterSC and RelSC in the figure, many research findings cannot be easily categorized into one or the other of these components of InterSC. In those cases, the figure includes consequences of InterSC with Coll-InterSC. In this section, we review research in each of these domains in which self-construal is measured or manipulated using the most common approaches described above.

Self-Construal Influences on CognitionMarkus and Kitayama (1991) argued that individuals with high InterSC should be especially likely to pay attention to others and the social context of interaction, resulting in very elaborate cogni-tive representations of others and self-representations that include social contexts. Since their formulation, researchers have focused on two primary ways that variation in self-construal affects cogni-tion: differences in attention to the context or relationships and different information processing styles. In the following sections, we review the existing research on each of these processes, describing their influences on self-views, social cognition, and nonsocial cognition.

Context sensitivity versus insensitivity and self-descriptions. Much of the early research that followed the initial publication of Markus and Kitayama’s (1991) and Triandis’s (1989) articles focused on examining the hypothesis that persons with high InterSC were more likely to describe themselves in terms of their social contexts, including close relationships, group memberships, and social identities. As Markus and Kitayama hypothesized, people from collectivist cultures report more social, collective, or group-oriented self-descriptions than do people from individualistic cultures (Bond & Cheung, 1983; Cousins, 1989; Kanagawa, Cross, & Markus, 2001; Rhee, Uleman, Lee, & Roman, 1995; Shweder & Bourne, 1984). In addition, manipulations of IndSC and InterSC generally result in the corresponding differences in self-descriptions on the TST (Oyserman & Lee, 2008).

Perhaps more importantly, IndSC and InterSC are hypoth-esized to influence the variability of self-descriptions across situations. If people with high InterSC are sensitive to situ-ational or relational context, then they should tend to describe themselves differently in different situations. Evidence to sup-port this hypothesis is mixed. Cross-cultural studies that use country as a proxy for self-construal do find that East Asian participants’ self-descriptions depend on situational context more than did those of American participants (Cousins, 1989; Kanagawa et al., 2001; Rhee et al., 1995; Suh, 2002).

The existing evidence, however, does not clearly support the association between self-concept variability across situa-tions and measures of self-construal. The clearest support comes from a study in which self-construal was manipulated using

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

154 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

COGNITIVE CONSEQUENCES

• Low context sensitivity• Differentiation, separation, and

contrast

INDEPENDENTSELF-CONSTRUAL

AFFECTIVE CONSEQUENCES

• Prefer socially disengaging emotions

• Well-being based on emotional experiences and high self-esteem

• Self-consistency promotes well-being

MOTIVATIONAL CONSEQUENCES

• Individualistic values• Self-enhancement • Promotion focused• Value primary control strategies

INTERPERSONAL BEHAVIOR

• Self promoting• Prefer direct communication• Willing to confront or use dominating

strategies

COLLECTIVE-INTERDEPENDENTSELF-CONSTRUAL

COGNITIVE CONSEQUENCES

• High context-sensitivity• Connection and assimilation

AFFECTIVE CONSEQUENCES

• Well-being based on adherence to social norms and harmony with others

• Prefer socially engaging emotions

MOTIVATIONAL CONSEQUENCES

• Value group harmony• Self-criticism• Prevention focused• Value secondary control strategies

SOCIAL BEHAVIOR• Group oriented• Prefer indirect

communication• Avoid confrontation; use

cooperative strategies in interactions

• Likely to imitate and seek proximity to others

RELATIONAL SELF-CONSTRUAL

COGNITIVE CONSEQUENCES

• High relationship sensitivity• Assimilation to others and focus on

similarity with close others

AFFECTIVE CONSEQUENCES

• Well-being based on quality of close relationships

MOTIVATIONAL CONSEQUENCES

• Value warm relationships• Other-enhancement • Pursue relational goals

SOCIAL BEHAVIOR

• Relationship-promoting• Adjust to partner• Focus on joint gains for relationship

Figure 1. Components of self-construal and their influence on cognition, affect, motivation, and social behaviorNote: Research findings that cannot easily be categorized as relational–interdependent or collective–interdependent were included with collective–interdependent self-construal.

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Cross et al. 155

the similar/different manipulation. Participants described them-selves as more context dependent in the InterSC priming condi-tion than did participants in the IndSC priming condition (Kühnen, Hannover, & Schubert, 2001). Studies that measure self-construal are less clear, however. For example, a multi-country study revealed that self-concept consistency was posi-tively related to IndSC but not to InterSC (Church et al., 2008). Another multicountry study discovered that RelSC predicted contextualized self-description differently for Japanese and Korean participants and was not associated with contextualized self-description for participants with European backgrounds (Y. Kashima et al., 2004). In three studies reported by Cross, Gore, and Morris (2003), RelSC (measured by the Cross et al., 2000, RISC scale) generally was not associated with self-concept variability across important relationships (rs ranged from .04 to .14; also see Locke & Christensen, 2007).

Although recent studies have continued to explore cultural differences in self-concept stability within and across relation-ship contexts (see English & Chen, 2007), there is little evidence of self-construal as a mediator of these cultural differences. In part, this may be because of the methods used to assess variation in the self-concept across situations. Most often, these methods request the participants to describe themselves in different situ-ations (e.g., at home, at school, or with friends) or in different relationships (e.g., with family, with specific friends). Partici-pants from Western cultural backgrounds may feel a press to describe themselves consistently across contexts. The Kühnen et al. (2001) study described above, which manipulated self-construal, assessed context-sensitive self-concept with three items from the SCS (e.g., “I act the same no matter who I am with”). Studies that use experience sampling methods, in which participants describe themselves using handheld computers when alerted, may more accurately tap the degree to which self-construal predicts the extent to which people think about themselves differently in different situations.

Context-sensitive social cognition. A variety of social-cognitive tasks have also been linked to variation in self-construal. In studies in which self-construals were primed, Haberstroh, Oyserman, Schwarz, Kühnen, and Ji (2002) showed that InterSC-primed Western participants were more likely than IndSC-primed participants to take a target person’s prior knowl-edge into account and avoided providing redundant informa-tion. Similarly, InterSC-primed participants who prepared to engage in a debate with an opponent listed more of the oppo-nent’s possible arguments in the debate and indicated a greater willingness to listen to the opponent’s position compared to IndSC-primed participants (Gardner & Le, 2000, reported in Gardner & Seeley, 2001). People whose InterSC is chronically activated (or primed in an experiment) tend to give more weight to others’ views or subjective norms about their goals and behaviors than do people with high IndSC, whose per-sonal attitudes are more likely to direct behavior (Torelli, 2006; Trafimow & Finlay, 1996; Ybarra & Trafimow, 1998).

Given that InterSC is associated with context-sensitive cognition, how might it affect memory? In one study, both European American and Asian or Asian American participants were primed with IndSC or InterSC and asked to recall their earliest memories (Wang & Ross, 2005). The IndSC-primed participants tended to describe more individual-focused mem-ories, whereas the InterSC-primed participants tended to describe more group- focused memories and memories that focused on social interaction.

If people with high InterSC are especially likely to attend to the context when thinking about themselves and others, then they should also tend to consider situational factors when mak-ing attributions for others’ behavior. In fact, cultural variation in situational versus dispositional attributions was the focus of some of the earliest research by cultural social psychologists (e.g., Miller, 1984; Morris & Peng, 1994). In general, these studies revealed that East Asian participants are more likely to attribute others’ behavior to situations than are European Americans. Singelis (1994), in his original publication describ-ing the SCS, reported that measures on the InterSC subscale were positively associated with situational attributions, control-ling for participants’ cultural background. Surprisingly, how-ever, other researchers have seldom attempted to link attribution processes to differences in self-construal. One exception is the work by Oishi, Wyer, and Colcombe (2000), who found that subliminal priming with interdependence-related concepts led to lower levels of dispositional attributions for another person’s negative behaviors compared to priming with independence-related concepts (but see Krull et al., 1999).

Finally, if persons with a high InterSC pay attention to and remember information about social contexts, then they should have a rich and easily accessible store of interpersonal knowl-edge (as suggested by Markus & Kitayama, 1991). For exam-ple, Markus and Kitayama suggest that the asymmetric results of self-to-other versus other-to-self comparisons (as originally shown in Holyoak & Gordon, 1983) are because of IndSC, in which unique aspects of the self are privileged over other information. They provided evidence that this asymmetry was absent among Japanese participants, presumably because of the greater knowledge of others accessible in the InterSC. Kühnen and Haberstroh (2004) tested this hypothesis in a study in which self-construal was manipulated using the pro-noun search task (Gardner et al., 1999). They found the typical asymmetry among German participants in the IndSC-prime condition but no asymmetry in the InterSC-prime condition.

Relational self-construal and relationship sensitivity. In research that focused specifically on RelSC, Cross and her colleagues (2002) argued that if the RelSC is a central framework for defining the self, then it should influence information process-ing without conscious control. In a series of studies using North American participants, they examined the role of RelSC in a variety of implicit cognitive processes that centered on relationship-oriented material. For example, they found that

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

156 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

participants who scored high on the RISC scale remembered more relationship-related information about a target person (Study 3) and organized information about others in terms of their relationships (Study 4). In addition, persons with high RelSC were more likely to respond positively to relationship-oriented terms in an IAT task (Greenwald, McGee, & Schwartz, 1998) and were more likely to have dense associative networks for relationship–oriented terms, compared to lows on their RISC scale (Study 2). In short, persons with chronically high RelSC are “tuned” to pay attention to and organize their worlds in terms of relationships.

When others are connected to the self and viewed as self-defining, a person will tend to consider the other person’s interests and take the other’s perspective in social interaction and decision making. Cross and her colleagues (2000) found that students with high RelSC were more likely to consider the needs and wishes of friends and family members when making decisions about how to spend their summer than were low RelSC students. Likewise, Gore and Cross (2006) found that North Americans with high RelSC tended to include other people in their rationale for pursuing important goals. Among new college roommates, those with high RelSC were better able to predict their roommate’s responses to a set of questions about values and beliefs than were low relationals, indicating better memory for the roommate’s revelations about themselves (Cross & Morris, 2003). Thus, RelSC is associated with atten-tion to, consideration of, and memory for relational contexts, especially important or close relationships.

Context-sensitive nonsocial cognition. Researchers have paid sub-stantial attention to the roles of self-construal and context-sensitivity for nonsocial events. Kühnen et al. (2001) found when IndSC was primed, German and American participants were quicker to find geometric figures that had been drawn within more complex geometric designs (the Embedded Figures Test; Witkin & Berry 1969). In contrast, InterSC-primed par-ticipants performed better than IndSC-primed participants on a task that was especially sensitive to context-dependent think-ing (also see Konrath, Bushman, & Grove, 2009). Others have demonstrated similar effects among Western participants using a modified Stroop task and a cognitive switching task (Han-nover, Pohlman, & Springer, 2005). Likewise, InterSC-primed North American participants were more sensitive to contextual information presented in a causal reasoning induction task than were IndSC-primed participants (K. Kim, Grimm, & Markman, 2007; also see Haberstroh et al., 2002).

Much of the early work linking self-construal to context-dependent thinking was limited to Western European or North American participants. More recently, Oyserman and her col-leagues showed that priming the InterSC promotes better memory for the location of objects in an array among both European American participants (Kühnen & Oyserman, 2002) and East Asian participants (Oyserman et al., 2009). Further-more, when the demands of a task matched the self-construal

prime (i.e., a task that requires one to ignore the context per-formed by a IndSC-primed participant), performance on the task was enhanced among both Western and East Asian par-ticipants (Oyserman et al., 2009).

Finally, considerable research has demonstrated significant cultural differences in attention to the context versus attention to the focal objects in a display (Masuda & Nisbett, 2001; Morris & Peng, 1994; Nisbett, Peng, Choi, & Norenzayan, 2001). Can these differences be accounted for by differences in self-construal? This question was addressed in an innovative study that examined neural responses to target objects or to novel contextual stimuli among European American and Asian American participants (Lewis, Goto, & Kong, 2008). The Event Related Potentials (ERP) data revealed the expected cultural differences in attention to the focal object or the con-text; this, in turn, was mediated by scores on the Singelis (1994) InterSC scale. In another study, priming IndSC and InterSC in Chinese participants resulted in neural activity that reflected cultural differences in cognition. Z. Lin, Lin, and Han (2008) found that priming IndSC resulted in increased focus on local targets in a compound letter task (i.e., the small letters that make up larger letters), whereas an InterSC prime resulted in increased focus on the global target (i.e., the larger letter), as evidenced by neural activity in the extrastriate cor-tex (which is related to visual perceptual processing; also see Z. Lin & Han, 2009). These and other recent studies of neural activity (e.g., Hedden, Ketay, Aron, Markus, & Gabrieli, 2008) provide provocative evidence that self-construal underlies important cultural differences in cognition.

Connecting and assimilating versus differentiating and contrasting. Markus and Kitayama (1991) argued that people with a high IndSC will strive to differentiate themselves from others, whereas people with a high InterSC will seek to connect to others. Since then, considerable research has focused on this effect for the self and social judgments; research on nonsocial judgments is just beginning to emerge.

Connecting versus differentiating in self- and social judgments. Social comparison processes have been thoroughly explored in social psychology research, but it entered a new era with the finding that self-construal moderated social comparisons processes. As Stapel and Koomen (2001) demonstrated, when the IndSC is activated, contrast or differentiation from the comparison target occurs. When the InterSC is activated, people tend to assimilate or view themselves as similar to the comparison target (also see Kemmelmeier & Oyserman, 2001; Kühnen & Haberstroh, 2004; Kühnen & Hannover, 2000; Stapel & van der Zee, 2006)

Behavioral evidence for assimilation versus contrast effects of self-construal on behavior was demonstrated in a study of the effects of priming a negatively stereotyped group on test performance (Bry, Follenfant, & Meyer, 2008). When exposed to the stereotype of dumb blondes, French students performed

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Cross et al. 157

worse on a general knowledge test if the InterSC was salient than if it was not (and worse than participants in a control condition), suggesting that the InterSC-primed participants had connected themselves with the dumb blonde stereotype. A contrast effect among IndSC-primed participants was dem-onstrated in Study 2: Participants exposed to the dumb blonde stereotype performed better than participants in the control condition when the IndSC was made salient (for related research, see Blanton & Stapel, 2008; Keller & Molix, 2008; Sui & Han, 2007).

Cross and her colleagues (2002) hypothesized that persons with high RelSC will also strive for connections with relation-ship partners and so will tend to see similarities between them-selves and close others. North American participants rated the descriptiveness of multiple traits, values, and abilities for them-selves and a same-sex friend. An indirect measure of similarity was calculated by computing intraclass correlations for each person’s pair of ratings in each domain (traits, values, and abilities). In regression analyses that controlled for self-esteem, RISC scale scores significantly predicted each type of simi-larity. In short, both RelSC and InterSC promote ways of think-ing that enhance connections, similarity or assimilation in social comparison. IndSC promotes differentiation, contrast, and cognitive separation from others.

Connecting versus differentiating in nonsocial cognition. The tendency to engage in differentiation versus assimilation may also apply to other domains of cognition. To date, however, few people have investigated this hypothesis in nonsocial domains. One study extended this line of reasoning to the generation of novel ideas. When IndSC was primed, people tended to generate more unusual examples of categories (e.g., clothing or furniture) compared to control conditions or InterSC-priming conditions (Wiekens & Stapel, 2008). In another study, Oyserman and her colleagues (2009) argued that the completion of multiple-choice tests, such as GRE tests that require students to select the correct antonym for a word or to complete analogies, requires a differentiation mind-set (i.e., the ability to separate the correct answer from the rest). They found that InterSC-primed participants performed worse on these tasks than IndSC-primed or control participants. These studies suggest that many of the academic tasks valued in Western cultural contexts, such as creativity, finding novel solutions to a problem, and completion of multiple-choice-type tasks, are related to IndSC and that exploration of other learning- and education-related topics may profit from con-sideration of the role of self-construal.

Summary and recommendations for future research. As Markus and Kitayama (1991) predicted, variation in self-construal is related to a variety of cognitive processes. In particular, researchers have focused on the role of self-construal in context-dependent or –independent cognition and on cognitive tendencies to differentiate or to connect information. Most of the research on cognitive

processes has understandably focused on self and social judg-ments, but work exploring the effects of self-construal on non-social cognitive processes is expanding and suggests promising avenues for new research. For example, spatial judgments (Krishna, Zhou, & Zhang, 2008) and aesthetic preferences (Zhang, Feick, & Price, 2006) are influenced by self-construal priming. As suggested above, researchers interested in learning styles, creativity, and other dimensions of education may find consideration of self-construals useful in their work.

Some processes linked to the self-construal by Markus and Kitayama (1991), however, have not received much attention by self-construal researchers. For example, they suggest that differences in self-construal may be the source of observed cultural differences in counterfactual thinking and categoriza-tion. To our knowledge, researchers have not (successfully) investigated these hypotheses with measures or manipulations of self-construal (but see interesting cross-cultural findings for categorization by Ji, Zhang, & Nisbett, 2004). Furthermore, although scant research exists on the relations between self-construal and attribution, these studies suggest that manipula-tions of self-construal may provide important insights into attribution-related processes.

New evidence linking self-construal to variation in neural activity is exciting and provocative. Although examination of neural activity cannot replace studies of cognitive behavior, they provide support for the contention that culture’s effects on the individual are more than skin deep.

Self-Construal Influences AffectDespite strong interest in cultural differences in emotion, little research has actually measured self-construal to investigate directly how independence and interdependence relate to vari-ous affective consequences. The few studies that have directly measured self-construal tend to find that, in a variety of sam-ples, IndSC is associated with greater subjective happiness (Elliott & Coker, 2008) as well as decreased depression (e.g., B. T. Lam, 2005; Okazaki, 1997; Sato & McCann, 1998), unhappiness (Y. Kim, Kasser, & Lee, 2003), general anxiety (e.g., Hardin, Varghese, Tran, & Carlson, 2006; Y. Kim, Kasser, & Lee, 2003; Xie, Leong, & Feng, 2008), and social anxiety (Hardin et al., 2006; J. J. Hong & Woody, 2007; Okazaki, 1997; Xie et al., 2008), whereas InterSC is often associated with increased levels of these negative affects (Hardin et al., 2006; Okazaki, 1997; Sato & McCann, 1998).

Such results, of course, beg the question of why self-construal is related to affect in these ways. Kitayama, Karasawa, and Mesquita (2004) laid out a dual process model of emotion in which cultural presses to be either independent or interdepen-dent result in distal and proximal cultural affordances that encourage the development of culturally consistent affective experience and expression. More specifically, they argue that IndSC is related to a tendency to experience and express socially disengaging emotions such as anger or pride, whereas

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

158 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

InterSC is related to a tendency to experience socially engag-ing emotions, such as guilt or relatedness. Although not specifi-cally mentioned by Kitayama et al., social anxiety would be another socially engaging emotion because it refers to feelings of nervousness that occur in the context of social relationships. It is not surprising that individuals high in InterSC, for whom interpersonal relationships are central to definitions of the self, would express more concern about appropriate behavior in social contexts. Interestingly, IndSC is often found to be a better predictor of social anxiety than InterSC; this appears to be particularly true for European Americans as opposed to East Asians (Xie et al., 2008).

But what of the relation of self-construal to other types of negative affect, such as unhappiness and depression? Kita-yama, Karasawa, and Mesquita (2004) explicitly identify these emotions as neither engaging nor disengaging, and thus we might expect them to be unrelated to self-construal. It is likely that the observed relations are largely artifactual, explained by the high correlations between social anxiety and other types of negative affect. Okazaki (1997) demon-strated that once social anxiety is controlled, neither IndSC nor InterSC is related to depression. Furthermore, ethnic group differences in depression disappeared when social anxi-ety was controlled. Thus, the common finding that Asian Americans self-report higher levels of depression than Euro-pean Americans (e.g., Aldwin & Greenberger, 1987; Kuo, 1984) is likely, at least in part, because of the fact that Asian Americans self-report higher levels of social anxiety than European Americans, which in turn is at least in part because of the greater interdependence and lower independence com-mon among Asian Americans (Okazaki, 1997).

Such failure to control for social anxiety may account for other findings that, on the surface, appear to contradict predic-tions of self-construal theory. For example, given that interde-pendence is culturally consistent with collectivist values, we might expect InterSC to be associated with positive outcomes in collectivist cultures. To be sure, some evidence does support this hypothesis: InterSC was associated with greater life sat-isfaction among college students in Hong Kong but was unre-lated to life satisfaction among college students in the United States (Kwan et al., 1997). Surprisingly, then, greater relative InterSC predicted greater unhappiness and less happiness in South Koreans but was unrelated to unhappiness among stu-dents in the United States (Y. Kim, Kasset, & Lee, 2003). If the measures of happiness and unhappiness used in this study are highly correlated with social anxiety, the apparently incon-sistent finding that InterSC predicts unhappiness in South Korea may simply be an artifact of InterSC predicting social anxiety. These results, however, must be interpreted with caution. First, InterSC scores were subtracted from IndSC scores to create a unidimensional self-construal score that reflected an individ-ual’s relative level of interdependence. As discussed previously, such unidimensional scores are inconsistent with both theory and intended use of most self-construal scales. In addition,

although the correlation between self-construal and unhappi-ness was statistically significant among the South Korean sample (r = –.15) but not in the U.S. sample (r = –.10), the relative magnitude of the correlations was not compared sta-tistically to determine if the relation is actually stronger for the South Koreans than for the Americans.

In addition to these relations being at least partly artifactual, it is likely that other variables mediate these relations. For example, InterSC is also associated with maladaptive perfec-tionism, probably because of the emphasis on being a good group member; however, after controlling for maladaptive perfectionism, InterSC no longer predicted depressive symp-toms (Yoon & Lau, 2008). Thus, it is not that being interde-pendent is inherently depressing but rather that being interdependent is associated with other variables, some of which may in turn be associated with negative affect. Just as independence may manifest in some individuals as an unhealthy disconnection from others, interdependence may manifest in some individuals as an unhealthy overdependence on others that leads to maladaptive perfectionism, among other things. Perhaps more likely, existing measures of self-construal may be incapable of adequately distinguishing independence from social isolation and interdependence from dependence.

Given the importance of interpersonal relationships to those with an InterSC and the importance of internal, private self-evaluations to those with an IndSC, several researchers have argued—and demonstrated—that life satisfaction is predicted by different variables, depending on self-construal. Large multinational studies have found that life satisfaction is pre-dicted by emotions (an internal, private experience) in individu-alistic cultures but by adherence to social norms in collectivist cultures (Schimmack, Radhakrishnan, Oishi, Dzokoto, & Ahadi, 2002; Suh, Diener, Oishi, & Triandis, 1998). However, because neither of these studies directly measured self-construal, the hypothesis that self-construal accounted for this difference remained speculative until a recent study provided direct sup-port (Suh, Diener, & Updegraff, 2008). Suh et al. showed that for individuals who were relatively higher in independence than interdependence, life satisfaction was predicted only by emotions (i.e., the balance of positive and negative affect) and not by the extent to which the individuals felt significant others would evaluate the participants’ lives positively. For those with relatively stronger InterSC, however, both these social appraisals and emotion predicted life satisfaction.

Such findings are consistent with those of Kwan et al. (1997), who found that in both Hong Kong and the United States relationship harmony fully mediates the relation between interdependence and life satisfaction, whereas global self-esteem fully mediates the relation between independence and life satisfaction. In a multiethnic U.S. sample, self-esteem also mediated the relation between independence and life satisfac-tion (measured in terms of positive and negative affect), although relationship harmony did not mediate the relation between interdependence and life satisfaction (Reid, 2004).

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Cross et al. 159

Similarly, B. T. Lam (2005) found that self-esteem fully medi-ates the relation between independence and depression in a sample of Vietnamese American adolescents: Those with greater independence tended to have more positive self-appraisals, which resulted in fewer depressive symptoms. Contrary to past research (e.g., Kwan et al., 1997; Okazaki, 1997; Sato & McCann, 1998), however, interdependence was also associated with greater global self-esteem and less depression and self-esteem also fully medi-ated the relation between interdependence and depression. This is likely because of the more bicultural nature of Lam’s adoles-cent sample. Unlike the other samples, which used college stu-dents (Kwan et al., 1997; Okazaki, 1997; Sato & McCann, 1998), Lam’s participants were adolescents in junior high and high school who were still living at home with their parents. Given that a majority of Lam’s participants (87%) reported Vietnamese was the primary language spoken at home, it appears these par-ticipants largely were living with ethnically enculturated families; thus, Lam argued that higher levels of interdependence reflected an important cultural consistency with the participants’ home environments, which in turn was associated with greater self-esteem and less depression.

Indeed, B. T. Lam (2005) found that different factors pre-dicted self-esteem for adolescents high in IndSC versus high in InterSC. Based on arguments similar to those of Kwan et al. (1997), Lam argued that family cohesion should be more important to the self-esteem of high InterSC adolescents. Given that these adolescents’ peer groups are more likely to reflect dominant cultural values, peer support should be more important to the self-esteem of high IndSC adolescents. As predicted, family cohesion fully mediated the relation between interdependence and global self-esteem, whereas peer support partially mediated the relation between independence and self-esteem.

Together, these studies (Kwan et al., 1997; B. T. Lam, 2005; Suh et al., 2008) demonstrate that IndSC and, in some samples, InterSC predict life satisfaction at least in part through their relation with self-appraisals such as global self-esteem and affect. Furthermore, interdependence predicts life satisfaction through its relation with interpersonal constructs such as rela-tionship harmony, family cohesion, and social appraisals.

By simply measuring self-construal, however, none of these studies demonstrates that self-construal causes these differences in the bases of life satisfaction; however, Suh et al. (2008) con-ducted a second study that does. They primed either an IndSC or an InterSC and obtained the same results: Life satisfaction was predicted by both social appraisal and emotion for those primed with InterSC, but life satisfaction was predicted only by emotion for those primed with IndSC. Moreover, the same pattern emerged whether self-construal was manipulated in the United States or in Korea, providing strong evidence that self-construal causes differences in the bases of life satisfaction.

Although most of the studies cited above examined inde-pendence and interdependence as orthogonal constructs, none of the research considered interactions between IndSC and

InterSC. One study that has done so examined the roles of self-construal and family cohesion in predicting mental distress among Asian American high school students (Liu & Goto, 2007). The authors found that increasing interdependence was associated more strongly with increasing distress among those adolescents low in independence; although increasing inter-dependence was also associated with increasing distress for those high in independence, the relationship was weaker. Impor-tantly, family cohesion mediated this effect, with adolescents high in both independence and interdependence reporting more family cohesion than adolescents high in interdependence but low in independence. For these latter adolescents low in inde-pendence, increasing interdependence was actually associated with decreasing family cohesion. Although acculturation of adolescents and their parents was not measured, these findings were attributed to higher independence representing a more bicultural identity on the part of adolescents, which buffered the negative effects of acculturative conflict between parents and adolescents. These results highlight the need for researchers not only to consider IndSC and InterSC as orthogonal constructs but also to consider interactions between them.

Relational self-construal and affect. RelSC and well-being are associated under some conditions. One of these is the type of well-being considered. Although RelSC is not associated with general psychological well-being (defined in terms of life sat-isfaction, depressive symptoms, perceived stress, positive and negative affect) among predominantly European American college students (Cross, Gore, & Morris, 2003, Studies 1 and 3), RelSC is associated with greater relational well-being (defined in terms of positive relations with others and aspects of collective self-esteem; Cross et al., 2003, Study 2). Sample also affects whether relations between well-being and RelSC are found. Berkel and Constantine (2005) argued that a need for affiliation may be stronger and more beneficial to women of color in predominantly White environments; therefore, they hypothesized that RelSC would predict life satisfaction among these women of color. Indeed, in their sample of African American and Asian American women recruited from a pre-dominantly White university, greater RelSC predicted greater life satisfaction, even after controlling for relationship har-mony and family conflict.

Some researchers are also examining whether different factors predict well-being for those with a more RelSC. Unfortunately, much of this research relies on gender as a proxy for RelSC rather than measuring it directly. However, this research does tend to support the hypothesis that positive interpersonal rela-tionships are more predictive of life satisfaction for those with a more relational InterSC. For example, women show greater decrements in daily life satisfaction than men when they feel misunderstood in interpersonal interactions (Lun, Kesebir, & Oishi, 2008), and women’s life satisfaction (measured in terms of positive and negative affect) was predicted by relationship harmony, whereas men’s life satisfaction was not (Reid, 2004).

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

160 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

Summary and recommendations for future research. As evi-denced by the relative brevity of the preceding section, sur-prisingly little research has been conducted that has actually measured self-construal in investigating the relations between self-construal and affect. The little research that has been done has tended to focus on whether one type of self-construal is more strongly associated with particular types of affect than another type of self-construal and whether different variables mediate the relation between one type of self-construal and affect as compared to another type of self-construal. Markus and Kitayama’s (1991) specific predictions about self-construal and affect (i.e., that the same emotion is elicited by different conditions depending on self-construal, that different emotions are experienced by independent and interdependent people and to different degrees) have received support. For example, the same construct of life satisfaction is elicited by different con-ditions depending on self-construal: by internal experiences, such as emotion or positive self-esteem, for those with a more IndSC, and by relationship variables, such as relationship harmony or social appraisal, for those with a more InterSC (Kwan et al., 1997; B. T. Lam, 2005; Suh et al., 2008).

Apart from the research reviewed here, however, most of the research relevant to these predictions has relied on either proxies of self-construal (e.g., culture of origin; Cole, Bruschi, & Tamang, 2002; Kitayama et al., 2006) or measures of IND-COL (e.g., Fischer, Manstead, & Rodriguez Mosquera, 1999). Although generally consistent with predictions, such research fails to explicitly link observed differences to self-construal. In addition, more research like that of Suh et al. (2008) that actually manipulates self-construal is needed. Demonstrating, for example, that priming an IndSC leads participants to expe-rience a socially disengaging emotion (e.g., anger) whereas priming an InterSC leads participants to experience a socially engaging emotion (e.g., shame) in response to the same stimu-lus would provide compelling evidence that self-construal causes differences in emotional experience.

In addition, work such as that by Liu and Goto (2007) raises important questions about how IndSC and InterSC interact in predicting affective experiences. Are such interactions found only among bicultural participants, such as Liu and Goto’s Asian American high school students, for whom being high in both independence and interdependence reflects an impor-tant adaptation to living in two cultures, or are similar interac-tions found among monocultural individuals? We know that individuals high in independence and relatively low in inter-dependence experience life satisfaction as a result of internal experiences such as affect or self-esteem; is the same true for individuals high in both independence and interdependence? Moreover, who is more likely to chronically experience high IndSC and InterSC, and under what conditions might others demonstrate both types of self-construal?

Finally, recent work on specific aspects of IndSC and InterSC raises new questions. For example, Hardin (2006) found that although InterSC was uncorrelated with social anxiety among

Asian Americans, a specific aspect of InterSC, relational inter-dependence (as captured by Singelis’s, 1994, SCS), was associ-ated with social anxiety. Moreover, some aspects of IndSC (autonomy/assertiveness and behavioral consistency), but not others (individualism and primacy of self), were associated with social anxiety. For European Americans, only autonomy/assertiveness predicted social anxiety. Thus, Markus and Kitayama’s (1991) prediction that the same emotion is elicited by different conditions depending on self-construal may be more nuanced. Does social appraisal predict life satisfaction equally well for those high in the relational interdependence component of InterSC as for those high in the esteem for group component? Or do different types of social appraisal predict life satisfaction, with positive appraisals from significant others being more relevant for those high in the relational interdepen-dence component whereas positive appraisals from one’s group are more relevant for those high in the collective interdepen-dence or esteem for group components? Turning to another of Markus and Kitayama’s predictions, that high IndSC and high InterSC individuals experience different emotions, are certain emotions more tied to some aspects of self-construal than oth-ers? As noted above, research that examines the consequences of experimentally manipulating these aspects of self-construal would provide the most compelling evidence for the role of dimensions of self-construal in affective experience.

Self-Construal Influences Motivation and Self-RegulationAccording to Markus and Kitayama’s (1991) conceptualization, people with high InterSC are expected to have a greater number of social motives compared to those with high IndSC. Indeed, these social motives are an integral part of the definition of InterSC. The independent self is motivated to be autonomous and agentic. In contrast, being a part of social groups and maintaining harmonious relationships with important others are of great importance to the interdependent self. Recently, Wiekens and Stapel (2008) provided relevant evidence. Using Brewer and Gardner’s (1996) method, they primed either the IndSC or the InterSC of their participants. Participants whose IndSC was primed reported higher levels of motivation to be independent, different, and alone compared to participants whose InterSC was primed. Those who received the InterSC prime reported higher levels of motivation to be accepted, to conform, and to be together than those who received the IndSC prime (also see Verplanken, Trafimow, Khusid, Holland, & Steentjes, 2009). Other studies have shown that measures of IndSC are positively related to the importance of individual goals and to status and achievement motivations and are nega-tively related to affiliation motivation (Brutus & Greguras, 2008; van Horen, Pöhlmann, Koeppen, & Hannover, 2008). In contrast, InterSC is positively related to the importance of social goals (van Horen et al., 2008). Likewise, when facing a serious illness, people with high interdependent self-views

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Cross et al. 161

(as measured by the E. S. Kashima & Hardie, 2000, scale) had more social concerns (e.g., being a burden on people close to me), whereas people with high independent self-views expressed more individual concerns (e.g., not being able to rely on myself; Uskul & Hynie, 2007).

Second, agency for IndSC and InterSC results from differ-ent motivational sources. For IndSC, personal goals, desires, and abilities become the fuel for action, whereas for InterSC coordination of the goals, desires, and needs of relational oth-ers with those of the self are the primary sources of motivation (Kitayama & Uchida, 2005; Markus & Kitayama, 2004). In Markus and Kitayama’s (2004) conceptualization, what differs is not the content of the needs, goals, and motivations but the processes that put these into action. For instance, two students who have the goal of achieving in school might have different motivational processes underlying this goal. One of these students, who has high IndSC, might see school success as a way of actualizing himself or herself, and the other student, who has high InterSC, might be motivated to be successful to better realize his or her obligations to her family and to the society. Here, self-actualization can be considered a personal reason for goal pursuit, whereas fulfilling the wishes of one’s family can be considered a relational reason. It is important to note, however, that personal and relational aspects of a goal are different than its autonomous and controlled aspects (Deci & Ryan, 2000). That is, a person may be intrinsically motivated to pursue a relational goal if the goal is autonomous (“I am pursuing this because it is important to someone who is impor-tant to me”) rather than controlled (“I am pursuing this goal because other people expect me to”). Likewise, someone may be unmotivated to pursue a personal goal if the goal is con-trolled (“I am pursuing this because the situation demands it”) rather than autonomous (“I am pursuing this because I really believe it is an important goal to have”).

In two longitudinal studies conducted with North American participants, Gore and Cross (2006) examined the relation between self-construal and relational versus personal reasons for pursuing goals. Their results showed that people with higher RelSC had more relational-autonomous reasons for their goals compared to those with lower RelSC (as assessed by the RISC scale; Cross et al., 2000). They found that both personal-autonomous and relational-autonomous reasons for goals affected perceived progress and effort toward achieving the goal. A cross-cultural replication of this study obtained similar results among American and Japanese samples (Gore, Cross, & Kanagawa, 2009). These studies demonstrate the link between RelSC and forms of interdependent agency, as proposed by Markus and Kitayama (2004).

A third and very important implication of different self-construal concerns self-related motives. Motives that are directly related to the self (i.e., self-motives) are expected to be experienced very differently by people with differing self-construals. These self-motives can be described as directed at establishing a particular self-representation (see Leary, 2007).

Markus and Kitayama (1991) speculated on the implications of different self-systems for self-enhancement and self-consistency self-motives. Although considerable research has examined differences in consistency motives for Westerners and East Asians (with a focus on cognitive dissonance pro-cesses; see Heine & Lehman, 1997; Hoshino-Browne et al., 2005; Kitayama, Snibbe, Markus, & Suzuki, 2004), to our knowledge no studies have used measures or manipulations of self-construal. Below, we focus on self-enhancement because it has been the focus of significant cross-cultural and self-construal research and has prompted a lively debate.

Self-enhancement. Self-enhancement, which is motivated by the need to see oneself with positive regard and to protect the self from negative information, has long been considered to be one of the basic tendencies of the self. Biases related to self-enhancement are viewed as conducive to having a healthy and well-adjusted self (Taylor & Brown, 1988).

According to Heine, Lehman, Markus, and Kitayama (1999), however, self-enhancement motivation is mostly a result of the IndSC. From the point of view of IndSC, behavior is directed by the internal characteristics of the unique indi-vidual. Thus, enhancing positive internal attributes and seeing oneself with positive regard are beneficial. The InterSC, on the other hand, is not as separated from the surrounding group as the IndSC. Maintaining personal relationships, pursuing group harmony, and being willing to sacrifice one’s own needs are expected. Given the InterSC, self-enhancement may elicit jealousy or envy from others and so could be detrimental for basic social needs such as maintaining harmonious relation-ships. Thus, persons with a high InterSC would not be highly motivated to see themselves in a positive light. Much research on self-enhancement has relied on group comparisons using IND-COL of the given culture as a proxy measure for the prevalent self-construal of the cultural group under investiga-tion. This line of research has provided valuable evidence for this perspective with a wide range self-enhancing behaviors (e.g., Chang & Asakawa, 2003; Heine, Kitayama, & Lehman, 2001; Heine & Lehman, 1995; Heine, Takata, & Lehman, 2000; Kitayama, Markus, Matsumoto, & Norasakkunkit, 1997).

Others, however, have claimed that self-enhancement is a universal motive, and they have argued the self-enhancement will occur in different domains depending on the salient dimen-sion of self-construal. For example, Sedikides, Gaertner, and Toguchi (2003, Study 2) conducted a study where they employed the better-than-average method to observe group differences. Participants rated themselves relative to a hypothetical group member on two sets of behaviors and traits that were differen-tially meaningful for IndSC and InterSC. The two groups did not differ from each other on overall self-enhancement, but par-ticipants high on IndSC (as measured by Singelis’s, 1994, scale) self-enhanced on the independence-related measure, whereas participants high on InterSC self-enhanced on the interdepen-dence-related measure. The measure of self-enhancement used

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

162 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

in the Sedikides et al. studies and in similar research by Kurman (2001), the better-than-average method, has been criticized on multiple grounds (Heine & Hamamura, 2007). In this approach, participants are asked to compare themselves to an average per-son on specific characteristics, traits, and abilities on which most people consider themselves better than most others. In a given population, if most people consider themselves better than most others, this would be a statistical impossibility, and so it is con-sidered a bias (Alicke, 1985). Any given individual, on the other hand, can be objectively better than most other people. Correlat-ing self-construal scores of individual participants with their standing on the better-than-average measure is therefore not appropriate for assessing self-enhancement bias at the individual level.

A second problem with this measure concerns the process of making comparisons with an average other. It has been argued that when people are asked to compare themselves to an average other, they engage in absolute rather than relative evaluations of themselves (Klar & Giladi, 1999). In fact, when people are evaluating any randomly selected target, they tend to evaluate it as better than average. Because most people have a favorable absolute evaluation of themselves, it appears that most people have a bias in comparative measures (for further criticism of this method, see Klar & Giladi, 1997).

Other studies have approached the role of self-construal in self-enhancement from different theoretical perspectives. For example, Gardner, Gabriel, and Hochschild (2002) examined this issue from the perspective of Tesser’s (1980) self-evaluation maintenance (SEM) model. SEM predicts that people reflect on the success of close others when the success is in a self-irrelevant domain, and this in turn boosts self-esteem. When the success of the close other is in a self-relevant domain, this results in comparison and thus a reduction in self-esteem. These effects are reversed when the social other is not a close person. Gardner et al. (2002) manipulated the self-construal of their participants to see if the above predictions hold true when InterSC is activated. Consistent with the predictions of SEM theory, participants in the IndSC prime condition evalu-ated their friend as more successful than a stranger in a self-irrelevant domain and as less successful in a self-relevant domain. Participants in the InterSC prime condition evaluated their friend as more successful than a stranger in both the self-relevant and self-irrelevant domains. This shows that for people with an InterSC, comparing oneself to a close other is not a source of threat but instead is an opportunity to bask in the reflected glory of the relational other (also see Cheng & Lam, 2007). Similarly, people exposed to upward and downward social comparisons respond differently depending on their primed self-construal. When InterSC is primed (compared to IndSC), people experience less positive affect after exposure to a down-ward comparison target and less negative affect after exposure to an upward comparison target (White, Lehman, & Cohen, 2006). In short, priming the InterSC seems to create a sense of having a bond with the target, resulting in stronger empathic

responses rather than contrasting (i.e., self-enhancing) affec-tive responses.

Self-regulation. Self-regulation can be defined broadly as the exercise of control over oneself to attain desired end states (Schmeichel & Baumeister, 2004). It is an essential element in any goal-directed behavior, and as such it is extremely relevant for the dynamic relations between motivation and self-construal. Most self-construal research in this area has centered on the topic of regulatory focus. Yet there has been some research attention to differences of the direction of con-trol and self-regulatory strength across the two primary self-construals. In this section, we briefly summarize this line of research.

According to Higgins (1996, 1997) there are two basic self-regulatory foci: a promotion focus, which is characterized by a motivation to approach desired end states, and a prevention focus, which is characterized by a motivation to avoid undesired end states. Regulatory focus has important consequences for behavior. For instance, prevention focus is shown to increase speed of braking in an accident-prone simulated traffic condi-tion, which points to risk avoidance (Werth & Forster, 2007). Although these regulatory foci can be situationally induced by various priming techniques (e.g., Shah, Higgins, & Friedman, 1998), individuals differ in their chronic levels of prevention and promotion focus. Chronic self-regulatory foci have roots in early socialization practices, where parental emphasis on positive outcomes by rewarding desired behavior fosters pro-motion focus and parental emphasis on negative outcomes by punishing undesired behavior fosters prevention focus in the child (Higgins & Silberman, 1998).

Parental practices are one antecedent of chronic regulatory focus. Another antecedent could be self-construal. Maintaining relational harmony, fitting into one’s proper roles in the society, and living up to the standards of significant others are essential for people with high InterSC. This requires self-monitoring and sensitivity to self-relevant information that could signal success or failure in the fulfillment of obligations. Hence, InterSC may support a prevention self-regulatory focus. People with high IndSC, on the other hand, are socialized to be self-actualized individuals who pursue personal aspirations. Unlike people with InterSC, who act with a concern for not harming significant relationships, those with IndSC are motivated to be positively distinct persons with achievements. Hence, IndSC may support a promotion focus.

Lee, Aaker, and Gardner (2000) tested these assumptions using a sample of participants with chronic IndSC versus InterSC (as measured by the Singelis SCS) as well as a sample of participants whose self-construal was experimentally primed. They utilized specific scenarios that provided the same informa-tion, except for a difference in framing. Half of the scenarios were presented in terms of gains (gain-framed information), and the other half were presented in terms of losses (loss-framed information). Results showed that participants with high InterSC

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Cross et al. 163

evaluated loss-framed information as more important than the gain-framed information, and participants with high IndSC evaluated gain-framed information as more important than the loss-framed information (also see Aaker & Lee, 2001). More recently, Hamilton and Biehal (2005) provided additional sup-port for these effects of self-construal on financial risk taking. Participants were primed with either IndSC or InterSC and asked to allocate a hypothetical budget across several funds that varied in their gain and risk levels. They found that the IndSC-primed participants allocated the budget so as to maximize gain and took greater risks, whereas the InterSC-primed participants allocated the budget so as to minimize losses and risk. Moreover, the relation between self-construal prime and risk taking was completely mediated by a measure of promotion and prevention orientation.

Zhang and Mittal (2007) tested a similar hypothesis about regulatory focus using a different experimental paradigm. They reasoned that people with a promotion focus would find enriched options more attractive but that people with a prevention focus would prefer impoverished options. Enriched options have high attribute scatter with extreme values. Impoverished options have values clustered toward the average. Suppose that you are a home buyer and the four important criteria are the size of the rooms and the deck and the condition of the kitchen and bathrooms. Suppose further that you have two options from which to choose. The first house, which represents the enriched option, has huge rooms and a fabulous kitchen with state-of-the-art appliances. But the deck is very small and the bathrooms are not in good shape. The second house, which represents the impoverished option, is average on all four criteria: average-sized rooms and deck and kitchen and bathrooms in acceptable condition.

In a series of cross-cultural studies with Chinese and American samples, Zhang and Mittal (2007) found that par-ticipants with high InterSC (as measured by Trafimow et al.’s, 1991, scale) evaluated impoverished options as more attractive than enriched options. Participants with high IndSC, however, evaluated enriched options as more attractive than impover-ished options. Moreover, self-construal completely mediated the relation between culture and option evaluations. In addi-tion, they employed a questionnaire measure of regulatory focus in a separate study and successfully showed that the relation of self-construal (as measured by Singelis’s, 1994, scale) with option evaluations was mediated by regulatory focus. This lends strong support to the argument that different self-systems afford different regulatory foci.

Regulatory focus is only one aspect of the motivational components of self-regulation. Another important aspect is the direction of control. Challenged by a stressful situation, people can cope with it in two different ways. They can either direct their control efforts to the environment to change it to fit their personal needs (primary control) or direct control toward them-selves through altering feelings and cognitions to adjust to the objective environment (secondary control; Weisz, Rothbaum, & Blackburn, 1984).

People who construe themselves in relation to others are hypothesized to prefer secondary control over primary control (Morling, Kitayama, & Miyamoto, 2002; Weisz et al., 1984). Therefore, one would expect people with high InterSC to engage in secondary control when faced with stressors. Indeed, A. G. Lam and Zane (2004) showed in a cross-cultural study that this assumption was correct. At the group level, Asian participants scored higher than Americans on a self-report measure of secondary control and American participants in turn, scored higher than Asians on primary control. At the indi-vidual level, IndSC was positively related to primary control, whereas InterSC was related to secondary control. Moreover, culture’s influence on direction of control was mediated by self-construals.

These results point to the willingness of persons with high InterSC to adjust themselves to fit into a situation or harmonize with others. As a result, people with chronically high InterSC may develop strong self-control strategies over time. This idea of increasing one’s ability to exert self-control is consistent with recent research on self-regulation. According to Baumeister and his colleagues, self-regulatory strength is comparable to the strength of a muscle: Like the use of a muscle, use of one’s store of self-regulatory resources will eventual lead to temporary exhaustion, termed “ego-depletion” (Baumeister & Vohs, 2003; Schmeichel & Baumeister, 2004). Although it may be exhausted, the strength of this “muscle” can also be improved by chronic use. For this reason, one would expect people with high InterSC, who are accustomed to adjusting to situations, to experience less ego-depletion after a self-regulatory task, compared to others.

Seeley and Gardner (2003) tested this hypothesis in a study in which participants engaged in a self-regulation task (suppressing any thoughts of a “white bear” for 5 min) followed by another task that also required considerable self-control (squeezing a handgrip exerciser). In line with the hypothesis, participants with high InterSC persisted on the second task longer than participants with high IndSC, indicating lower levels of regulatory depletion among the former group. Simi-larly, regulatory depletion was more pronounced for Americans compared to Asians. This study provides initial behavioral evidence that self-construal is related to differences in self-regulation, but much more research is needed to uncover the particular types of situations and specific self-regulation tasks that may be influenced by variation in self-construal.

Summary and recommendations for future research. Most of Markus and Kitayama’s (1991) predictions concerning the relations between motivation and self-construal have found empirical support. First, self-construal research showed that social motives are an integral part of the definition for InterSC. Evidence for the importance of social motives for people with InterSC comes both from studies that manipulated self-construals (e.g., Wiekens & Stapel, 2008) and from studies that measured participants’ existing self-construals (e.g., Brutus & Greguras, 2008; van Horen et al., 2008).

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

164 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

Markus and Kitayama’s (1991) second proposal—that agency differs for IndSC and InterSC—also has received empirical support. Specifically, for people with high IndSC, personal concerns such as self-actualization are important in goal pursuit. For people with high InterSC, however, relational concerns are important sources of motivation. For people with high RelSC, relational reasons are very influential in goal pursuits in the long run, and they serve as intrinsic sources of motivation (Gore & Cross, 2006; Gore et al., 2009). These initial findings point to the importance of considering prevail-ing theories of motivation and agency from the perspective of variation in self-construal. For example, although self-determination theory posits that relatedness is one of three pri-mary motives for all people (the others being competence and autonomy), it has received far less attention than the other two. This may in part be because of the assumptions of the self as independent and separate from others that pervade the environ-ment of Western researchers and the populations they study. Other forms of social motivation, such as motivations that focus on one’s reputation in the community (as are represented in the concepts of face or honor), also may be linked to variation in self-construal.

Among all of the proposals of Markus and Kitayama (1991), the one that has prompted the liveliest debate was that self-motives such as self-enhancement would take on different meanings given different self-systems. In their view, self-enhancement motivation is mostly a result of the IndSC, which promotes evaluating oneself positively. For the InterSC, how-ever, the need to be a good group member requires self-improvement and a self-critical attitude. Although considerable cross-cultural research has lent support for these predictions (e.g., Heine et al., 2000; Heine, Kitayama, & Lehman, 2001; Heine, Kitayama, Lehman, et al., 2001; Kitayama et al., 1997), some researchers have argued that people with high InterSC also engage in self-enhancement, but only in ways mean-ingfully related to their self-construal (e.g., Kurman, 2001; Sedikides et al., 2003). Others, in contrast, have proposed that self-enhancement motivation is not related to self-construal at all. In a series of studies by Thomsen, Sidanius, and Fiske’s (2007), self-enhancement was not related to self-construal but to the endorsement of interpersonal leveling beliefs. In sum, although there is controversy, the existing evidence mostly supports the proposal of Markus and Kitayama (1991) that self-enhancement takes on different meanings for differ-ent self-systems.

Self-regulation has been another topic that has attracted considerable research attention. In general, self-construal stud-ies concerning regulatory focus, direction of control, and self-regulatory strength are generally in line with the predictions derived from Markus and Kitayama’s (1991) theorizing. InterSC supports prevention focus, whereas IndSC supports promotion focus (Lee et al., 2000; Zhang & Mittal, 2007). Concerning direction of control, IndSC is positively related to primary control and InterSC is related to secondary control

(A. G. Lam & Zane, 2004). And finally, research on self-regulatory strength points out that people with high InterSC experience less regulatory depletion compared to people with high IndSC in some situations (Seeley & Gardner, 2003). This research on the role of self-construal in self-regulation is pre-liminary but promising, and it may be beneficial in numerous domains.

To date, although considerable attention has been paid to cognitive approaches to the issue of consistency (as described above), no one has examined the role of self-construal in dis-sonance processes. Cross-cultural research has made great strides in showing how dissonance processes vary for people from East Asian versus Western backgrounds (Hoshino-Browne et al., 2005; Kitayama, Snibbe, et al., 2004). To be brief, West-ern participants tend to seek consistency within themselves, whereas East Asians tend to seek consistency with others’ views and expectations. Unfortunately, most research in this area uses culture as a proxy for self-construal and has not used explicit measures or manipulations of self-construal (but see Pöhlmann, Carranza, Hannover, & Iyengar, 2007, for a closely related study). Considering the importance of self-consistency as a self-motive and the range of cross-cultural findings on this topic, examina-tion of the hypothesized role of (measured or manipulated) self-construal in this process will be an informative next step.

Because of its influence on motivation and self-regulation, the possible applications of self-construal theory are almost countless. Researchers in applied disciplines have begun to examine its importance in a variety of domains. Links have been made to the fields of leisure studies (Walker, Deng, & Dieser, 2005), eating disorders (Green, Scott, DeVilder, Zeiger, & Darr, 2006), health (Lun et al., 2008; Uskul & Hynie, 2007), counseling (Yeh & Arora, 2003), consumer behavior (Zhang & Shrum, 2009), and business management (Gelfand, Major, Raver, Nishii, & O’Brien, 2006; see next section). We suspect that further applications in education, vocational psychology, and marital and family therapy—to name just a few—are not far behind.

Self-Construal Shapes Interpersonal BehaviorHow does variation in self-construal shape interaction with other people? Although Markus and Kitayama (1991) did not address social behavior specifically, there are many obvious applications of their thesis to social interaction. People with high InterSC should seek to maintain connectedness and har-mony in relationships, whereas those with high IndSC should seek to maintain individuality and separateness from others (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). Pursuit of these goals may be relatively automatic or nonconscious when the associated self-construal is chronically or temporarily activated. For example, people who have chronically high InterSC (or who are exposed to an InterSC prime) tend to sit closer to another person in a lab situation than do those with a primed IndSC or chronically high IndSC (Holland, Roeder, van Baaren,

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Cross et al. 165

Brandt, & Hannover, 2004). Similarly, priming the InterSC results in a greater likelihood of imitating the behavior of another person, compared to priming the IndSC or a control condition (van Baaren, Maddux, Chartrand, de Bouter, & van Knippen-berg, 2003). Why might imitation be important in the develop-ment or maintenance of social relationships? Imitation may create a sense of interpersonal similarity or synchrony, which then greases the wheels of interpersonal interaction. In general, people who are mimicked by an interaction partner tend to like that partner, to have greater rapport with the partner, and to engage in more prosocial behavior (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999; van Baaren, Holland, Steenaert, & van Knippenberg, 2003; see Chartrand, Maddox, & Lakin, 2005, for a review). The association between self-construal and mimicry is bidirec-tional: People who are imitated by others also come to describe themselves more interdependently than do people who are not mimicked (as measured by responses to the TST; Ashton-James, van Baaren, Chartrand, Decety, & Karremans, 2007). Furthermore, when participants were unobtrusively imitated by another person, they were more likely to describe themselves as interdependent with others (relative to non-mimicked participants), which in turn predicted their willing-ness to help a third party (Ashton-James et al., 2007). Self-construal also influences self-presentation; IndSC prim-ing increases self-enhancing forms of self-presentation, whereas InterSC priming increases self-presentations of social appropriateness (Lalwani & Shavitt, 2009).

People with chronically high InterSC also behave in a more other-oriented fashion, compared to those with a high IndSC. For example, when exposed to scenarios that pitted the indi-vidual’s self-interest against the interest of an important in-group, InterSC-primed European American and Asian American participants were more likely than IndSC-primed participants to choose the group’s welfare over their own (Gardner, Gabriel, & Dean, 2004). Similarly, when given power to distribute tasks to themselves and to another participant, highly relational participants distributed the tasks less selfishly than did lows on this dimension (S. Chen, Lee-Chai, & Bargh, 2001).

Self-construal and close relationships. These studies of proxim-ity, mimicry, and other-oriented behavior utilized laboratory procedures in which participants interacted with strangers. Thus, they provide compelling confirmation that the InterSC promotes positive relational behavior even when there is no prior relationship between the partners. When there is an ongoing relationship or the possibility of continuing interac-tion, however, individuals with high RelSC should be espe-cially likely to engage in a variety of behaviors that promote closeness and harmony in the relationship. Cross and her colleagues (Cross et al., 2003; Cross & Morris, 2003; Gore, Cross, & Morris, 2006) have investigated how North Ameri-cans with varying levels of RelSC (as measured by the RISC scale, Cross et al., 2000) interact with strangers who were assigned to be their roommates. The roommate situation

provides an excellent opportunity to examine relationship processes among previously unacquainted persons who have not self-selected to live together.

Cross and her colleagues hypothesized that persons with high RelSC will seek to make the most of the situation and will strive to develop a close relationship with the partner. If a close friendship is not possible with the new roommate, the very relational person will nonetheless seek to have a harmonious, relatively conflict-free relationship. One way to promote a rela-tionship with a new partner is to share openly one’s own interests, goals, values, and beliefs and to attend closely to and respond sensitively to the self-disclosures of one’s partner. Gore et al. (2006, Study 2) found that participants’ degree of chronically activated RelSC had both direct and indirect effects on their own and their roommates’ closeness and commitment to the relationship. Both high RelSC individuals and their roommates reported enhanced relationship quality after 1 month. This effect was mediated by perceptions of one’s own and one’s partner’s emotional disclosure and perceptions that one’s partner responded sensitively to one’s own disclosures. In short, because high RelSC participants disclosed openly and responded sensitively to their roommates, they created a supportive environment for the development of a close relationship (for other studies of self-construal and self-disclosure, see Cross et al., 2000, Study 3; Morry, 2005; Terzino & Cross, 2009).

When a person pays close attention to his or her relationship partner’s self-revelations, that person should be able to predict the partner’s responses to a variety of situations. Cross and Morris (2003) found that participants with high RelSC were better able to predict their roommate’s responses to a values measure. This association was strongest in relationships that were relatively distant and weaker in relationship in which the two roommates were very close. Cross and Morris (2003) sug-gested that in very close relationships, the relationship itself motivates the participants to attend to each others’ cares and concerns and thereby enhances agreement on this task. In more distant relationships, the person with high RelSC may pay close attention to the partner as a strategy for avoiding conflict or in hopes that a deeper relationship is still possible. In their second study, Cross and Morris found evidence for the latter goal: Participants with high RelSC had overly optimistic views of their roommate’s feelings of closeness in the relationship. Such optimism can foster continued efforts to work at the relationship in hopes of greater intimacy eventually. Indeed, other studies suggest that persons with high RelSC or InterSC report closer relationships and more social support than lows (Cross et al., 2000), and they are more likely to include others in the self (Cross et al., 2000; Vorauer & Cameron, 2002).

Furthermore, persons with high RelSC are less likely than others to view conflict with friends and close others as a zero-sum game, in which one person wins and the other loses. Gore and Cross (in press) asked participants to imagine themselves in a variety of situations in which they were resolving a conflict with a friend, roommate, or another close other. The results

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

166 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

revealed zero-sum thinking among those with low RelSC but not among those with high RelSC. For the low RelSC partici-pants, decisions that benefitted the partner were perceived as costly to the self. For the participants with high RelSC, there was typically no relation between evaluations of the outcome of a decision for oneself and for the relationship partner. In a third study, where participants were asked to recall a conflict situation, high RelSC participants indicated regret over making a self-serving decision. In other words, persons with high RelSC do not perceive a trade-off between acting in their own interests and acting in the interests of close others. Instead, consistent with the notion that the other is part of the self, they perceive decisions that benefit the relationship as ultimately benefitting the self as well.

Friendships and roommates are important parts of people’s lives, but romantic relationships receive much more attention from researchers. To date, however, researchers have paid little attention to the role of variation in self-construal in romantic relationships. Given the strong association of gender with RelSC (Cross et al., 2000), one might expect that self-construal plays a significant role in many aspects of relationship devel-opment and maintenance. Sinclair and Fehr (2005) examined the association between self-construal and responses to dissatisfaction in romantic relationships. When measured (Study 1) or primed (Study 2), IndSC was positively associ-ated with the preference to use the active, constructive strat-egy of voice when one is dissatisfied with the relationship. Measured (Study 1) and primed (Study 2) InterSC was posi-tively associated with the passive, constructive strategy of loyalty, in which the person waits for things to improve (also see Yum, 2004). These findings are consistent with other research that suggests that IndSC is associated with a promo-tion focus and InterSC is associated with a prevention focus (Lee et al., 2000) and with other studies showing that indi-viduals with high InterSC avoid dominating forms of conflict resolution (see below).

In what other ways might self-construal influence relation-ship development and maintenance? Persons with high RelSC may be more likely than others to make situational (rather than dispositional) attributions for a partner’s bad behavior, which are associated with relationship satisfaction (Bradbury & Fincham, 1990). High RelSC persons tend to perceive them-selves as more similar to their partner than low relationals, which can also help maintain relationships (Byrne & Blaylock, 1963; Cross et al., 2002). When a relationship is threatened, highly relational participants may be more likely to engage in prorelationship behavior rather than withdraw (Lydon, Menzies-Toman, Burton, & Bell, 2008). If InterSC is associated with perceiving the self as malleable (as cross-cultural research suggests; Heine, Kitayama, Lehman, et al., 2001), then highly interdependent persons may tend to adjust to their relationship partners to maintain harmony in the relationship (Morling et al., 2002). In short, a focus on variation in self-construal may help researchers better understand a wide variety of relationship

processes. Perhaps Sinclair and Fehr (2005) said it best when they closed their article with this statement: “[I]t may even be the case that self-construal is the fundamental, distal factor that drives many of the other self-related processes that have been examined in the close relationships literature” (p. 303).

Self-construal and communication processes. When people’s self-construals focus them on interdependence versus independence, their characteristic ways of communicating will reflect these orientations. As Singelis and Brown (1995) describe, the focus on harmonious relationships and the definition of the self in terms of close relationships and in-groups among those with high RelSC or Coll-InterSC should result in a preference for indirect communication, sensitivity to the context in social interaction, attention to others’ thoughts and feelings, and non-confrontational conflict resolution styles. In short, the goal of communication, given RelSC or Coll-InterSC, is the mainte-nance of harmonious relationships with close others and in-group members. In contrast, for individuals with high IndSC, the goal of communication is to express the person’s unique goals, wishes, thoughts, feelings, and abilities. As a result, IndSC should be associated with direct communication styles, little attention to contextual aspects of social interaction, attention to one’s own thoughts and feelings during social interaction, and willingness to engage in confrontational dispute resolu-tion styles.

Researchers have made several inroads into investigating these theoretical consequences of self-construal for commu-nication processes. For example, InterSC is positively related to concern for a conversation partners’ feelings and possible negative evaluation of the self (Gudykunst et al., 1996; M.-S. Kim, Sharkey, & Singelis, 1994; Mandel, 2003), a desire to avoid arguments (M. Kim, Aune, Hunter, Kim, & Kim, 2001), and the use of cooperative strategies in group discussions (Oetzel, 1998). IndSC is positively related to a concern for clarity or directness in communication (Gudykunst et al., 1996; M.-S. Kim et al., 1994); open and expressive communication (Gudykunst et al., 1996); and the use of assertive or dominat-ing strategies in group discussions (Oetzel, 1998; also see Bresnahan, Ohashi, Liu, Nebashi, & Liao, 1999). In addition, IndSC is associated with approval of others’ positive self-presentations, but InterSC is associated with approval of oth-ers’ negative self-presentations and approval of others when they attribute their own success to the help of others (Ellis & Wittenbaum, 2000; J. Kim, Kim, Kam, & Shin, 2003).

Differing self-construals should also engender different reactions to conflict, especially conflict with meaningful others (e.g., friends, relatives, or coworkers). One conceptualization of conflict strategies draws on conceptions of face and facework (Ting-Toomey, 1988). Facework is the strategies one uses to protect, challenge, or defend one’s own or another’s “claimed sense of positive image in the context of social interaction” (or face; Oetzel et al., 2001, p. 235). In a study that included participants from China, Germany, Japan, and the United

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Cross et al. 167

States, Oetzel and his colleagues (2001) asked participants to recall a conflict and to rate their experience in that conflict with respect to several facework strategies. IndSC (as measured by the Gudykunst et al., 1996, measure) was strongly associ-ated with facework strategies that defended one’s own face. InterSC was strongly associated with strategies that reflected concern for the other’s face and mutual face concerns, such as problem solving, apologizing, and giving in to the other person. InterSC was negatively related to dominating strategies in the interaction (also see Oetzel & Ting-Toomey, 2003; Ting-Toomey, Oetzel, & Yee-Jung, 2001).

Unfortunately, most of these studies of self-construal and communication employ cross-sectional designs and use only self-report data. New strides in understanding how variation in self-construal relates to communication processes may be made by using experimental paradigms that manipulate self-construal and employ behavioral measures of direct or indirect communication strategies. A good example of an experimental approach is found in a recent study by Seeley Howard, Gardner, and Thompson (2007). Three studies showed that the behavior of InterSC-primed participants who had high power in a nego-tiation depended on the situational context. When negotiating with a lower power individual, high-power and InterSC-primed participants were more generous and more likely to reach a settlement with their lower power opponent than were IndSC-primed participants (Study 1). In a second study, power and self-construal were again manipulated but participants engaged in either a negotiation with one other person or a team negotia-tion, in which teams of three participants negotiated with each other. Interestingly, when negotiating with a group, high power InterSC-primed participants were less generous than the high- power IndSC-primed participants. Negotiating individually or as a group did not affect the final settlement amounts decided by high-power IndSC-primed participants. These studies sug-gest that persons with high InterSC have a sense of responsi-bility or attachment to other persons in the negotiation but that the attachment depends on the context. When negotiating individually with a lower status target, the high-power and high-InterSC negotiator will behave benevolently. But when negotiating as part of a team, their loyalties and attachments lie with the other team members. Their commitment is to pursue the interests of the team, resulting in less generous settlements with the opposing party.

This work highlights the potential importance of self-construal to organizational psychology and leadership (van Knippenberg, van Knippenberg, De Cremer, & Hogg, 2004). A few researchers have begun to consider the role of self-construal in topics of organizational justice. For example, Brockner, De Cremer, van den Bos, and Chen (2005) argued that because procedural fairness communicates that individu-als are respected and valued in the organization, it therefore reflects the importance of relational values. Consequently, people who tend to define themselves in terms of their relation-ships may be especially sensitive to procedural fairness in

organizations. Across three studies, Brockner and his colleagues found that perceptions of procedural fairness (e.g., the degree to which one has a voice in decisions or the fairness of inter-personal treatment) were more strongly related to a variety of outcomes (e.g., cooperation, positive affect, and desire to inter-act with the other party) for people who had high InterSC than for lows on this dimension (also see Brockner, Chen, Mannix, Leung, & Skarlicki, 2000; Gollwitzer & Bücklein, 2007; Holmvall & Bobocel, 2008). Research focused on equity in social exchange revealed that responses to inequitable treatment depended on the participants’ RelSC and their relationship with the other person: Persons with high RelSC were less likely than lows to respond negatively when a close other had treated them unfairly (Y. Chen, Chen, & Portnoy, 2009, Study 2) but were more likely to respond negatively when a person who was not close treated them badly (Holmvall & Sidhu, 2007). Others have found that the degree to which the three dimen-sions of self-construal (IndSC, RelSC, and InterSC) moderate the association between procedural justice and work-related outcomes differs depending on the specific forms of procedural justice under investigation (Johnson, Selenta, & Lord, 2006).

Summary and recommendations for future research. Surprisingly little research has focused on the effects of differing self-construals for social behavior, close relationships, and other phenomena related to interactions with others or with groups. Consistent with expectations, InterSC is associated with a vari-ety of processes that serve to enhance or maintain relationships, including mimicry, prosocial behavior, and indirect, noncon-frontational communication strategies. Cross and her col-leagues have examined the role of RelSC in a variety of relationship-related processes, but many more processes may be fruitfully explored from the perspective of variation in self-construal. For example, many gender differences in behavior and relationship processes have been observed, and these dif-ferences may be in part a function of gender differences in self-construal (Cross & Madson, 1997; Kiecolt-Glaser & Newton, 2001). When the mechanisms that explain gender differences in behavior are identified (e.g., differences in self-construal), researchers are better able to stipulate the conditions under which those behaviors are most likely to be expressed, ways that the behaviors may be modified, and other factors that may maintain or bolster the behaviors. Consider the finding that many couples differ in their expectation of closeness or intimacy in the marriage, with women desiring more close-ness and communication of love and affection than men (see Eldridge & Christensen, 2002, for a review). If these differ-ences derive from different self-construals, then both research-ers and therapists have a theoretically rich starting point for further investigation into the cognitive, affective, and moti-vational foundations of differences in communication, conflict, and marital satisfaction.

Cross-cultural studies have observed cultural differences in a variety of social phenomena, such as conformity (H. Kim

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

168 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

& Markus, 1999), trust (Yamagishi & Yamagishi, 1994), and social loafing (Earley, 1993). To date, these studies have not been followed up by additional work aimed at identifying the mechanisms responsible for these differences. For example, one would expect that participants with high InterSC would be more likely to conform to the behavior of in-group members than would those with high IndSC.

There are also promising avenues for further research into the consequences of different self-construals for social behav-ior in other settings, such as the workplace. Many aspects of organizational behavior, such as citizenship behaviors, job withdrawal, and leadership styles, may be linked to differences in self-construals (e.g., Gahan & Abeysekera, 2009; Gelfand et al., 2006). Ultimately, whether a person considers himself or herself to be independent of others or fundamentally inter-dependent with others in a situation can importantly affect other social behaviors, such as conformity, compliance, aggres-sion, altruism, cooperation, and persuasion.

Where to From Here?Self-construal was initially proposed as a means of understand-ing cultural differences in behavior (Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Triandis, 1989). Today, however, researchers also recognize the potential role of within-culture variation in self-construal for explaining many psychological phenomena (e.g., Cross & Madson, 1997). We suspect that these dual goals—understanding cultural differences and within-culture processes—will continue to motivate considerable research in the near term. To further these goals, we next turn to theoretical, conceptual, and meth-odological issues that sometimes thwart this work.

As we mentioned in the section on Between- and Within-Group Differences in Self-Construal, there are times when measures of self-construal fail to demonstrate the expected cultural patterns of differences (Oyserman et al., 2002). There are also many times measures of self-construal fail to mediate cultural differences in specific behaviors (Kitayama et al., 2009). We are confident that a great many studies have been relegated to the file drawer because manipulations of self-construal fail to produce the expected effects on behavior. In an innovative study, Kitayama and his colleagues presented an argument for why explicit measures of self-construal may fail to predict cultural differences in behaviors that have been linked theoretically to the self. They observed the expected differences in American, Western European, and Japanese participants’ performance on several tasks (e.g., dispositional attribution, focused vs. holistic attention, and preference for disengaging or socially engaging emotions), but there was no systematic association between performance on these tasks and Singelis’s (1994) measure of IndSC and InterSC. Moreover, there was little to no association of the tasks with each other. In other words, people who described themselves as high in IndSC were no more likely than those low on this dimension to engage in dispositional attribution, to focus on the target

rather than the context in an attention task, or to prefer disen-gaging to socially engaging emotions. Moreover, individuals who tended to engage in one of these IndSC-related tasks were not necessarily likely to engage in other IndSC-related tasks.

Kitayama and his colleagues (2009), along with Y. Kashima (2009), offer helpful insight into these sorts of apparent anoma-lies in cross-cultural data. First, Kitayama et al. remind us of Simpson’s paradox: the observation that constructs that are related at the societal level (e.g., InterSC and context-sensitive thinking) may not necessarily be related at the individual level (also see Oyserman & Uskul, 2008; van de Vijver, van Hemert, & Poortinga, 2008). They also contend that cultural mandates to be independent or interdependent may be enacted through any of several different cultural tasks. For example, two Americans, who both strive to be independent, may go about that by focusing on different dimensions of independence. Ed demonstrates his independence by focusing on his uniqueness and differentiating himself from others, whereas Valerie dem-onstrates her independence by aggressively pursuing her personal goals. Moreover, each cultural task is associated with specific psychological tendencies (e.g., using focused vs. holis-tic attention or making dispositional vs. situational attribu-tions). Just as different individuals use different cultural tasks to fulfill their culture’s mandate, different cultural tasks are associated with different sets of psychological tendencies. Thus, within an individual, there may be relatively little association among different culture-related processes.

Second, Kitayama et al. (2009) observe that these cultural mandates begin to influence children from birth and the vari-ous psychological tendencies and cultural tasks used to comply with the cultural mandate become habitual, automatic, or rote. Explicit views of the self, as tapped by measures of self-construal, develop in adolescence, and they may not be linked conceptually to these automatic, habitual, or overlearned tasks. Thus, Americans and East Asians may perform very differently on a variety of culture-linked tasks (as they did in Kitayama et al.’s, 2009, study), but performance on these tasks may not require the activation of the associated self-construal. Kashima (2009) also notes that culturally based processes are typically learned and become automatized in specific situations and activities; some behaviors are strongly driven by the norms of particular contexts rather than by personal beliefs or self-views (Zou et al., 2009). Thus, attempts to elicit cultural tasks and behaviors (i.e., through priming) may not be effective if the attempt does not tap the appropriate context or if the behavior is strongly driven by situational factors.

Third, explicit measures of self-construal appear to assess Kitayama et al.’s cultural tasks (e.g., being unique or self-effacing) rather than the accompanying psychological tendencies (e.g., preferring engaged or disengaged emotions). Indeed, many of the specific dimensions of self-construal identified by Hardin et al. (2004) as underlying items on Singelis’s (1994) SCS (e.g., primacy of self) map directly onto Kitayama et al.’s cultural tasks. Given that Kitayama et al. (2009) argue explicitly that different

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Cross et al. 169

cultural tasks are associated with different psychological tenden-cies, we would expect weak correlations, at best, between broad measures of cultural tasks (e.g., explicit measures such as the Singelis SCS scales) and the specific measures of psychological tendencies. Instead, specific psychological tendencies (e.g., cul-tural differences in dispositional attribution) may be better explained by domain- specific measures (e.g., beliefs in attitude–behavior consistency; see Y. Kashima, Siegal, Tanaka, & Kashima, 1992). Likewise, Kashima (2009) argues that the commonly used priming tasks induce specific ways of thinking or feeling that are associated with particular behaviors but not with others. The reason that the pronoun circling priming task promotes attention to the target rather than the context (as in the Kühnen et al., 2001, studies), he contends, is that thinking about oneself as an indi-vidual prompts analytical processing of information among Eng-lish speakers. Thinking about oneself as “we,” in contrast, may hinder analytical thinking. Cultural differences in processes that are unrelated to analytical thinking, however, may not be influ-enced by “I” versus “we” primes (see Oyserman & Lee’s, 2008, review for the effects of different types of primes on cognitive and other outcomes). In short, when researchers seek to demon-strate that a particular cultural difference in behavior is associated with self-construals, they would be wise to focus on the specific dimension of IndSC, RelSC, or InterSC that accounts for the behavior and to consider the specific contexts in which the behav-ior develops.

These issues raise the question of whether self-construal is best understood as the cause of a variety of behaviors or is better viewed as a way to interpret cultural differences in behavior. Kashima (2009) offers a compelling argument for self-construal as an interpretive tool rather than a causal con-struct (and he acknowledges as well that an interpretive tool can be a causal mechanism). In our view, this is similar to saying that extraversion does not cause talkativeness so much as it is a way to interpret or describe that behavior. Yet just as measures of extraversion have been productively used to predict a wide variety of behaviors, so also many of the studies reviewed here suggest that measures of self-construal are use-ful tools in the exploration of within-culture patterns of behav-ior. Likewise, priming techniques help researchers begin to approximate the ways that self-construal influences thinking and feeling. As better measures of self-construal are developed and as priming techniques become more specific, some of these apparent anomalies may be resolved.

Researchers may best avoid dead ends and detours along their routes if they are careful about how they describe their work and recall how they got where they are now. Authors have at times referred to the pronoun circling task or other common priming tasks as “cultural priming.” This term is most appro-priately applied to the cultural icons task devised by Y.-Y. Hong and her colleagues (2000) and perhaps to studies that use lan-guage as a prime with bicultural participants. For other primes, however, particular perceptions of the self (as an individual or as a member of a group, or as similar to or different from others)

or ways of thinking (or mind-sets; Oyserman et al., 2009) are primed, not cultures. The danger in calling these latter proce-dures cultural priming is that it equates culture with self-con-strual or mind-set and ignores the fact that these are just two of the many ways culture shapes individual behavior. Moreover, the distinction made by Kitayama et al. (2009) among very basic cognitive and affective processes (their psychological tendencies), complex behavioral goals (their cultural tasks), and the cultural mandates of individualistic and collectivist societies reminds us that the global concept of culture cannot be adequately represented by a very specific way of thinking. In our view, equating culture with self-construal or mind-set (or equating self-construal with a specific psychological ten-dency) can lead outsiders to view social or personality psycholo-gists engaged in cultural psychology as naïve, uninformed, and overly narrow in our interests.

In addition, cross-cultural researchers should remember that our understanding of self-construal has developed out of com-parisons between Europeans or Americans and East Asians. Con-sequently, they may not apply as well to Africans or Latin Americans. Research in these understudied regions will help us identify the culture-based assumptions that are largely implicit in this work. For example, we wonder whether InterSC and holistic thinking are associated as strongly in Africa as in East Asia. Latin Americans are typically more expressive and emotive than East Asians; thus, the associations between InterSC and emotional expression may be very different in Latin American cultures than in East Asian societies. Similarly, InterSC is positively associated with the self-criticism and social isolation dimensions of the self-compassion scale in Taiwan (which has a Confucian heritage) but negatively associated with these dimensions in Thailand (which has a strong Buddhist heritage; Neff, Pisitsungkagarn, & Hsieh, 2008). Just as English is spoken and written differently in the United States and the United Kingdom, so also people in Africa, Latin America, or Middle Eastern societies may express IndSC, RelSC, and Coll-InterSC with somewhat different “dialects.”

Self-construals represent how people view themselves with respect to other people—as separate, independent, and autono-mous; as related to individual friends, family, and coworkers; or as part of larger social groups. This conceptualization has generated considerable research and fruitful theories. Unfor-tunately, not all that research could be reviewed here, but we have attempted to provide an overview of those efforts and a snapshot of the current status of these constructs. We hope this serves as a useful tour book for researchers exploring this new and promising terrain of the self.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Kari Terzino, Tsui-Feng Wu, and Meng Zhang, who offered helpful comments on this article.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The authors declared no potential conflicts of interests with respect to the authorship and/or publication of this article.

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

170 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

Financial Disclosure/FundingThe authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the authorship, and/or publication of this article: This article was completed while the first and third authors were supported by National Science Foundation Grant BCS0646360.

References

Aaker, J. L. (2000). Accessibility or diagnosticity? Disentangling the influence of culture on persuasion processes and attitudes. Journal of Consumer Research, 26, 340-357. doi:10.1086/209567

Aaker, J. L., & Lee, A. Y. (2001). “I” seek pleasures and “we” avoid pains: The role of self-regulatory goals in information process-ing and persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, 28, 33-49. doi:10.1086/321946

Aaker, J., & Schmitt, B. (2001). Culture-dependent assimilation and differentiation of the self—Preferences for consumption symbols in the United States and China. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psy-chology, 32, 561-576. doi:10.1177/0022022101032005003

Agrawal, N., & Maheswaran, D. (2005). The effects of self-construal and commitment on persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, 31, 841-849. doi:10.1086/426620

Aldwin, C., & Greenberger, E. (1987). Cultural differences in the pre-dictors of depression. American Journal of Community Psychology, 15, 789-813. doi:10.1007/BF00919803

Alicke, M. D. (1985). Global self-evaluation as determined by the desirability and controllability of trait adjectives. Jour-nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1621-1630. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.49.6.1621

Andersen, S. M., & Chen, S. (2002). The relational self: An interper-sonal social-cognitive theory. Psychological Review, 109, 619-645 doi:10.1037/0033-295X.109.4.619

Aron, A., Aron, E. N., Tudor, M., & Nelson, G. (1991). Close relationships as including other in the self. Jour-nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 241-253. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.241

Ashton-James, C., van Baaren, R. B., Chartrand, T. L., Decety, J., & Karremans, J. (2007). Mimicry and me: The impact of mimicry on self-construal. Social Cognition, 25, 518-535. doi:10.1521/soco.2007.25.4.518

Baumeister, R. F. (1986). Public self and private self. New York, NY: Springer.

Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The self. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Baumeister, R. F., & Sommer, K. L. (1997). What do men want?

Gender differences and two spheres of belongingness: Comment on Cross and Madson (1997). Psychological Bulletin, 122, 38-44. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.122.1.38

Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2003). Self-regulation and the executive function of the self. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self and identity (pp. 197-217). New York, NY: Guilford.

Berkel, L. A., & Constantine, M. G. (2005). Relational variables and life satisfaction in African American and Asian American college women. Journal of College Counseling, 8, 5-13.

Blanton, H., & Stapel, D. A. (2008). Unconscious and spontaneous and . . . complex: The three selves model of social comparison

assimilation and contrast. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-chology, 94, 1018-1032. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.6.1018

Bond, M. H., & Cheung, T. (1983). College students’ spontaneous self-concept: The effect of culture among respondents in Hong Kong, Japan, and the United States. Jou rnal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 14, 153-171. doi:10.1177/0022002183014002002

Bosson, J. K., Swann, W. B., Jr., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2000). Stalking the perfect measure of implicit self-esteem: The blind men and the elephant revisited? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 631-643. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.631

Bradbury, T. N., & Fincham, F. D. (1990). Attributions in mar-riage: Review and critique. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 3-33. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.107.1.3

Bresnahan, M. J., Levine, T. R., Shearman, S. M., Lee, S. Y., Park, C., & Kiyomiya, T. (2005). A multimethod multitrait validity assessment of self-construal in Japan, Korea, and the United States. Human Communication Research, 31, 33-59. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2005.tb00864.x

Bresnahan, M. J., Ohashi, R., Liu, W. Y., Nebashi, R., & Liao, C. (1999). A comparison of response styles in Singapore and Taiwan. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30, 342-358. doi:10.1177/0022022199030003004

Brewer, M. B. (1991). The social self: On being the same and differ-ent at the same time. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 475-482. doi:10.1177/0146167291175001

Brewer, M. B., & Chen, Y. (2007). Where (who) are collectives in collectivism? Toward conceptual clarification of individu-alism and collectivism. Psychological Review, 114, 133-151. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.133

Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. (1996). Who is this “we”? Levels of collective identity and self representations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 83-93. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.1.83

Briley, D. A., & Wyer, R. S., Jr. (2001). Transitory determinants of values and decisions: The utility (or nonutility) of individualism and collectivism in understanding cultural differences. Social Cognition, 19, 197-227. doi:10.1521/soco.19.3.197.21474

Brockner, J., Chen, Y., Mannix, E. A., Leung, K., & Skarlicki, D. P. (2000). Culture and procedural fairness: When the effects of what you do depend on how you do it. Administrative Science Quarterly, 45, 138-159. doi:10.2307/2666982

Brockner, J., De Cremer, D., van den Bos, K., & Chen, Y. R. (2005). The influence of interdependent self-construal on procedural fairness effects. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 96, 155-167. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2004.11.001

Brown, P., & Brown, S. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in lan-guage usage. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Brutus, S., & Greguras, G. J. (2008). Self-construals, motivation, and feedback-seeking behaviors. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 16, 282-291. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2389.2008.00434.x

Bry, C., Follenfant, A., & Meyer, T. (2008). Blonde like me: When self-construals moderate stereotype priming effects on intellec-tual performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 751-757. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2007.06.005

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Cross et al. 171

Byrne, D., & Blaylock, B. (1963). Similarity and assumed similarity of attitudes between husbands and wives. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 636-640. doi:10.1037/h0045531

Chang, E. C., & Asakawa, K. (2003). Cultural variations on optimis-tic and pessimistic bias for self versus a sibling: Is there evidence for self-enhancement in the West and self-criticism in the East when the referent group is specified? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 569-581. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.3.569

Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Jour-nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893-910. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893

Chartrand, T. L., Maddux, W. W., & Lakin, J. L. (2005). Beyond the perception-behavior link: The ubiquitous utility and motivational moderators of nonconscious mimicry. In R. Hassin, J. Uleman, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought 2: The new unconscious (pp. 334-361). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Chen, S., Lee-Chai, A. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). Relationship orientation as a moderator of the effects of social power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 173-187. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.173

Chen, Y., Chen, X., & Portnoy, R. (2009). To whom do positive norm and negative norm of reciprocity apply? Effects of ineq-uitable offer, relationship, and relational-self orientation. Jour-nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 24-34. doi:10.1016/j .jesp.2008.07.024

Chen, Y.-R., Brockner, J., & Katz, T. (1998). Toward an explanation of cultural differences in in-group favoritism: The role of individ-ual versus collective primacy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1490-1502. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.6.1490

Cheng, R. W., & Lam, S. (2007). Self-construal and social com-parison effects. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 197-211. doi:10.1348/000709905X72795

Church, T. A., Anderson-Harumi, C. A., del Prado, A. M., Curtis, G. J., Tanaka-Matsumi, J., Valdez Medina, J., . . . Katigbak, M. S. (2008). Culture, cross-role consistency, and adjustment: Testing trait and cultural psychology perspectives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 739-755. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.95.3.739

Clark, M. S., Oullette, R., Powell, M. C., & Milberg, S. (1987). Recipi-ent’s mood, relationship type, and helping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 94-103. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.53.1.94

Cole, P. M., Bruschi, C. J., & Tamang, B. L. (2002). Cultural dif-ferences in children’s emotional reactions to difficult situations. Child Development, 73, 983-996. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00451

Cousins, S. D. (1989). Culture and self-perception in Japan and the United States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 124-131. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.124

Cross, S. E., Bacon, P. L., & Morris, M. L. (2000). The relational– interdependent self-construal and relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 791-808. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.4.791

Cross, S. E., Gore, J. S., & Morris, M. L. (2003). The relational–interdependent self-construal, self-concept consistency, and

well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 933-944. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.5.933

Cross, S. E., & Madson, L. (1997). Models of the self: Self-construals and gender. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 5-37. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.122.1.5

Cross, S. E., & Morris, M. L. (2003). Getting to know you: The relational self-construal, relational cognition, and well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 512-523. doi:10.1177/0146167202250920

Cross, S. E., Morris, M. L., & Gore, J. S. (2002). Thinking about one-self and others: The relational–interdependent self-construal and social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social psychology, 82, 399-418. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.3.399

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pur-suits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psycho-logical Inquiry, 11, 227-268. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

Deiner, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction with Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71-75. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13

Devos, T., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Implicit self and identity. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self and identity (pp. 153-175). New York, NY: Guilford.

Earley, P. C. (1993). East meets West meets Mideast: Further explo-rations of collectivistic and individualistic work groups. Academy of Management Journal, 36, 319-348. doi:10.2307/256525

Eldridge, K. A., & Christensen, A. (2002). Demand-withdraw commu-nication during couple conflict: A review and analysis. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Elliott, I., & Coker, S. (2008). Independent self-construal, self-reflection, and self-rumination: A path model for predicting happiness. Australian Journal of Psychology, 60, 127-134. doi:10.1080/00049530701447368

Ellis, J. B., & Wittenbaum, G. M. (2000). Relationships between self-construal and verbal promotion. Communication Research, 27, 704-722. doi:10.1177/009365000027006002

English, T., & Chen, S. (2007). Culture and self-concept stability: Consistency across and within contexts among Asian Americans and European Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-chology, 93, 478-490. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.3.478

Fischer, A., Manstead, A. S. R., & Rodriguez Mosquera, P. M. (1999). The role of honor-related vs. individualistic values in conceptualizing pride, shame, and anger: Spanish and Dutch cultural prototypes. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 149-179. doi:10.1080/026999399379311

Gabriel, S., & Gardner, W. L. (1999). Are there “his” and “hers” types of interdependence? The implications of gender differences in collective versus relational interdependence for affect, behav-ior, and cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 642-655. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.3.642

Gahan, P., & Abeysekera, L. (2009). What shapes an individual’s work values? An integrated model of the relationship between work values, national culture, and self-construal. Interna-tional Journal of Human Resource Management, 20, 126-147. doi:10.1080/09585190802528524

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

172 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S., & Dean, K. K. (2004). The individual as “melting pot”: The flexibility of bicultural self-construals. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 22, 181-201.

Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S., & Hochschild, L. (2002). When you and I are “we,” you are not threatening: The role of self-expansion in social comparison. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 239-251. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.2.239

Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S., & Lee, A. Y. (1999). “I” value freedom, but “we” value relationships: Self-construal priming mirrors cul-tural differences in judgment. Psychological Science, 10, 321-326. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00162

Gardner, W. L., & Seeley, E. A. (2001). Confucius, “Jen,” and the benevolent use of power. In A. Y. Lee-Chai & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The use and abuse of power (pp. 263-280). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.

Geertz, C. (1975). On the nature of anthropological understanding. American Scientist, 63, 47-53.

Gelfand, M. J., Major, V. S., Raver, J. L., Nishii, L. H., & O’Brien, K. (2006). Negotiating relationally: The dynamics of the relational self in negotiations. Academy of Management Review, 31, 427-451.

Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women’s development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Gollwitzer, M., & Bücklein, K. (2007). Are “we” more punitive than “me”? Self-construal styles, justice-related attitudes, and punitive judgments. Social Justice Research, 20, 457-478. doi:10.1007/s11211-007-0051-y

Gore, J. S., & Cross, S. E. (2006). Pursuing goals for us: Rela-tionally autonomous reasons in long-term goal pursuit. Jour-nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 848-861. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.848

Gore, J. S., & Cross, S. E. (in press). Conflicts of interest: Relational self-construal and decision-making in interpersonal contexts. Self and Identity.

Gore, J. S., Cross, S. E., & Kanagawa, C. (2009). Acting in our interests: Relational self-construal and goal motivation across cultures. Moti-vation and Emotion, 33, 75-87. doi:10.1007/s11031-008-9113-1

Gore, J. S., Cross, S. E., & Morris, M. L. (2006). Let’s be friends: Rela-tional self-construal and the development of intimacy. Personal Relationships, 13, 83-102. doi:10.1111/j.1475-6811.2006.00106.x

Green, M. A., Scott, N. A., DeVilder, E. L., Zeiger, A., & Darr, S. (2006). Relational–interdependent self-construal as a function of bulimic symptomatology. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, 943-951. doi:10.1002/jclp.20270

Greenwald, A. G., & Farnham, S. D. (2000). Using the Implicit Association Test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 1022-1038. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.1022

Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Mea-suring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464-1480. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464

Greenwald, A. G., & Pratkanis, A. R. (1984). The self. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 3, pp. 129-178). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Gudykunst, W. B., & Lee, C. M. (2003). Assessing the validity of self-construal scales: A response to Levine et al. Human Com-munication Research, 29, 253-274. doi:10.1093/hcr/29.2.253

Gudykunst, W. B., Matsumoto, Y., Ting-Toomey, S., Nishida, T., Kim, K., & Heyman, H. (1996). The influence of cultural indi-vidualism-collectivism, self-construals, and individual values on communication styles across cultures. Human Communication Research, 22, 510-543. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.1996.tb00377.x

Guimond, S., Chatard, A., Martinot, D., Crisp, R. J., & Redersdorff, S. (2006). Social comparison, self-stereotyping, and gender differ-ences in self-construals. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-chology, 90, 221-242. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.221

Guo, X., Schwartz, S. J., & McCabe, B. E. (2008). Aging, gender, and self: Dimensionality and measurement invariance analysis on self-construal. Self and Identity, 7, 1-24.

Haberstroh, S., Oyserman, D., Schwarz, N., Kühnen, U., & Ji, L. (2002). Is the interdependent self more sensitive to question con-text than the independent self? Self-construal and the observa-tion of conversational norms. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 323-329. doi:10.1006/jesp.2001.1513

Hackman, M. Z., Ellis, K., Johnson, C. E., & Staley, C. (1999). Self-construal orientation: Validation of an instrument and a study of the relationship to leadership communication style. Communica-tion Quarterly, 47, 183-195.

Hamaguchi, E. (1985). A contextual model of the Japanese: Toward a methodological innovation in Japan studies. Journal of Japanese Studies, 11, 289-321.

Hamilton, R. W., & Biehal, G. J. (2005). Achieving your goals or protecting their future? The effects of self-view on goals and choices. Journal of Consumer Research, 32, 277-283. doi:10.1086/432237

Hannover, B., Birkner, N., & Pöhlmann, C. (2006). Ideal selves and self-esteem in people with independent or interdependent self-construal. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 119-133. doi:10.1002/ejsp.289

Hannover, B., & Kühnen, U. (2005). Culture, context, and cognition: The semantic procedural interface model of the self. European Review of Social Psychology, 15, 297-333.

Hannover, B., Pöhlmann, C., Springer, A., & Roeder, U. (2005). Implications of independent versus interdependent self-knowledge for motivated social cognition: The semantic pro-cedural interface model of the self. Self and Identity, 4, 159-175. doi:10.1080/13576500444000245

Harb, C., & Smith, P. B. (2008). Self-Construals across cultures: Beyond independence–interdependence. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39, 178-197. doi:10.1177/0022022107313861

Hardin, E. E. (2006). Convergent evidence for the multidimension-ality of self-construal. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 37, 516-521. doi:10.1177/0022022106290475

Hardin, E. E., Cross, S. E., & Hoang, U. (2009). [Development of the Multidimensional Self-Construal Scale (MDSCS)]. Unpub-lished raw data.

Hardin, E. E., Leong, F. T. L., & Bhagwat, A. A. (2004). Factor structure of the Self-Construal Scale revisited: Implications for

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Cross et al. 173

the multidimensionality of self-construal. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 327-345. doi:10.1177/0022022104 264125

Hardin, E. E., Leong, F. T. L., & Osipow, S. H. (2001). Cultural relativity in the conceptualization of career maturity. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 58, 36-52. doi:10.1006/jvbe.2000.1762

Hardin, E. E., Varghese, F. V., Tran, U. V., & Carlson, A. Z. (2006). Anxiety and career exploration: Gender differences in the role of self-construal. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 69, 346-358. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2006.05.002

Hedden, T., Ketay, S., Aron, A., Markus, H. R., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2008). Cultural influences on neural substrates of attentional control. Psycho-logical Science, 19, 12-17. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02038.x

Heine, S. J., & Hamamura, T. (2007). In search of East Asian self-enhancement. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 4-27. doi:10.1177/1088868306294587

Heine, S. J., Kitayama, S., & Lehman, D. R. (2001). Cultural differ-ences in self-evaluation: Japanese readily accept negative self-relevant information. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 434-443. doi:10.1177/0022022101032004004

Heine, S. J., Kitayama, S., Lehman, D. R., Takata, T., Ide, E., Leung, C., & Matsumoto, H. (2001). Divergent consequences of success and failure in Japan and North America: An inves-tigation of self-improving motivations and malleable selves. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 599-615. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.4.599

Heine, S. J., & Lehman, D. R. (1995). Cultural variation in unre-alistic optimism: Does the West feel more vulnerable than the East? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 595-607. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.4.595

Heine, S. J., & Lehman, D. R. (1997). Culture, dissonance, and self-affirmation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 389-400. doi:10.1177/0146167297234005

Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999). Is there a universal need for positive self-regard? Psychological Review, 106, 766-794. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.106.4.766

Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Peng, K., & Greenholtz, J. (2002). What’s wrong with cross-cultural comparisons of subjective Lik-ert scales? The reference-group effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 903-918. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.903

Heine, S. J., Takata, T., & Lehman, D. R. (2000). Beyond self-presentation: Evidence for self-criticism among Japanese. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 71-78. doi:10.1177/0146167200261007

Higgins, E. T. (1996). The “self-digest”: Self-knowledge serving self-regulatory functions. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-chology, 71, 1062-1083. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.6.1062

Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychol-ogist, 52, 1280-1300. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.12.1280

Higgins, E. T., & Silberman, I. (1998). Development of regula-tory focus: Promotion and prevention as ways of living. In J. Heckhausen & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Motivation and self-regulation across the life span (pp. 79-113). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Holland, R. W., Roeder, U., van Baaren, R. B., Brandt, A. C., & Hannover, B. (2004). Don’t stand so close to me: The effects of self-construal on interpersonal closeness. Psychological Science, 15, 237-242. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00658.x

Holmvall, C. M., & Bobocel, D. R. (2008). What fair procedures say about me: Self-construals and reactions to procedural fairness. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 105, 147-168. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2007.09.001

Holmvall, C. M., & Sidhu, J. (2007). Predicting customer ser-vice employees’ job satisfaction and turnover intentions: The roles of customer interactional justice and interdependent self-construal. Social Justice Research, 20, 479-496. doi:10.1007/s11211-007-0049-5

Holyoak, K. J., & Gordon, P. C. (1983). Social reference points. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 881-887. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.5.881

Hong, J. J., & Woody, S. R. (2007). Cultural mediators of self-reported social anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 1779-1789. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.01.011

Hong, Y.-Y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C.-Y., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2000). Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55, 709-720. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.7.709

Hoshino-Browne, E., Zanna, A. S., Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., Kitayama, S., & Lackenbauer, S. (2005). On the cultural guises of cognitive dissonance: The case of Easterners and Western-ers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 294-310. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.294

James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press (reprinted 1983).

Ji, L., Zhang, Z., & Nisbett, R. E. (2004). Is it culture or is it language? Examination of language effects in cross-cultural research on categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 57-65. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.1.57

Johnson, R. E., Selenta, C., & Lord, R. G. (2006). When organizational justice and the self-concept meet: Consequences for the organi-zation and its members. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 99, 175-201. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.07.005

Jordan, J. V., & Surrey, J. L. (1986). The self-in-relation: Empathy and the mother-daughter relationship. In T. Bernay & D. W. Cantor (Eds.), The psychology of today’s women (pp. 81-104). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Kağitçibaşi, C. (2005). Autonomy and relatedness in cultural con-text: Implications for self and family. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 36, 403-422. doi:10.1177/0022022105275959

Kağitçibaşi, C. (2007). Family, self, and human development across cultures: Theory and applications (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Kanagawa, C., Cross, S. E., & Markus, H. R. (2001). “Who am I?” The cultural psychology of the conceptual self. Personal-ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 90-103. doi:10.1177/ 0146167201271008

Kashima, E. S., & Hardie, E. A. (2000). The development and vali-dation of the Relational, Individual, and Collective Self-Aspects

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

174 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

(RIC) Scale. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 3, 19-48. doi:10.1111/1467-839X.00053

Kashima, Y. (2009). Culture comparison and culture priming: A crit-ical analysis. In R. S. Wyer, C. Chiu, & Y. Hong (Eds.), Under-standing culture: Theory, research, and application (pp. 53-77). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

Kashima, Y., Kashima, E., Farsides, T., Kim, U., Strack, F., Werth, L., & Yuki, M. (2004). Culture and context-sensitive self: The amount and meaning of context-sensitivity of phe-nomenal self differ across cultures. Self and Identity, 3, 125-141. doi:10.1080/13576500342000095

Kashima, Y., Siegal, M., Tanaka, K., & Kashima, E. S. (1992). Do people believe behaviours are consistent with attitudes? Towards a cultural psychology of attribution processes. British Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 111-124.

Kashima, Y., Yamaguchi, S., Kim, U., Choi, S., Gelfand, M. J., & Yuki, M. (1995). Culture, gender, and self: A perspective from individualism-collectivism research. Journal of Per-sonality and Social Psychology, 69, 925-937. doi:10.1037/ 0022-3514.69.5.925

Keller, J., & Molix, L. (2008). When women can’t do math: The inter-play of self-construal, group identification, and stereotypic perfor-mance standards. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 437-444. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2007.01.007

Kemmelmeier, M., & Cheng, B. Y. (2004). Language and self-construal priming: A replication and extension in a Hong Kong sample. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 705-712. doi:10.1177/0022022104270112

Kemmelmeier, M., & Oyserman, D. (2001). The ups and downs of thinking about a successful other: Self-construals and the con-sequences of social comparisons. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31(3), 311-320. doi:10.1002/ejsp.47

Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Newton, T. L. (2001). Marriage and health: His and hers. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 472-503. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.4.472

Kim, H., & Markus, H. R. (1999). Deviance or uniqueness, harmony or conformity? A cultural analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 785-800. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.4.785

Kim, J., Kim, M., Kam, K. Y., & Shin, H. (2003). Influence of self-construals on the perception of different self-presentation styles in Korea. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 6, 89-101. doi:10.1111/1467-839X.t01-1-00013

Kim, K., Grimm, L. R., & Markman, A. B. (2007). Self-construal and the processing of covariation information in causal reasoning. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1337-1343.

Kim, M., Aune, K. S., Hunter, J. E., Kim, H., & Kim, J. (2001). The effect of culture and self-construals on predispositions toward verbal communication. Human Communication Research, 27, 382-408. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2001.tb00786.x

Kim, M.-S., & Raja, N. S. (2003). When validity testing lacks validity: Comment on Levine et al. Human Communication Research, 29, 275-290. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2003.tb00839.x

Kim, M.-S., Sharkey, W. F., & Singelis, T. M. (1994). The relationships between individuals’ self-construals and perceived importance of

interactive constraints. International Journal of Intercultural Rela-tions, 18, 117-140. doi:10.1016/0147-1767(94)90008-6

Kim, Y., Kasser, T., & Lee, H. (2003). Self-concept, aspirations, and well-being in South Korea and the United States. Journal of Social Psychology, 143, 277-290.

Kitayama, S., Duffy, S., & Uchida, Y. (2007). Self as cultural mode of being. In S. Kitayama & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (pp. 136-174). New York, NY: Guilford.

Kitayama, S., Karasawa, M., & Mesquita, B. (2004). Collective and personal processes in regulating emotions: Emotion and self in Japan and the U.S. In P. Philippot & R. S. Feldman (Eds.), The regulation of emotion (pp. 251-273). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit, V. (1997). Individual and collective processes in the construction of the self: Self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in Japan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1245-1267. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.6.1245

Kitayama, S., Mesquita, B., & Karasawa, M. (2006). Cultural affordances and emotional experience: Socially engaging and disengaging emotions in Japan and the United States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 890-903. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.5.890

Kitayama, S., Park, H., Sevincer, A. T., Karasawa, M., & Uskul, A. K. (2009). A cultural task analysis of implicit independence: Com-paring North America, Western Europe, and East Asia. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 236-255. doi:10.1037/a0015999

Kitayama, S., Snibbe, A. C., Markus, H. R., & Suzuki, T. (2004). Is there any “free” choice? Self and dissonance in two cultures. Psychologi-cal Science, 15, 527-533. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00714.x

Kitayama, S., & Uchida, Y. (2005). Interdependent agency: An alternative system for action. In R. M. Sorrentino, D. Cohen, J. M. Olson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Cultural and social behavior: The Ontario Symposium (Vol. 10, pp. 137-164). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Klar, Y., & Giladi, E. E. (1997). No one in my group can be below the group’s average: A robust positivity bias in favor of anony-mous peers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 885-901. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.5.885

Klar, Y., & Giladi, E. E. (1999). Are most people happier than they peers, or are they just happy? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 585-594. doi:10.1177/0146167299025005004

Kleinknecht, R. A., Dinnel, D. L., Kleinknecht, E. E., Hiruma, N., & Hirada, B. A. (1997). Cultural factors in social anxiety: A comparison of social phobia symptoms and Taijin Kyo-fusho. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 11, 157-177. doi:10.1016/S0887-6185(97)00004-2

Konrath, S., Bushman, B. J., & Grove, T. (2009). Seeing my world in a million little pieces: Narcissism, self-construal, and cog-nitive-perceptual style. Journal of Personality, 77, 1197-1228. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00579.x

Krishna, A., Zhou, R., & Zhang, S. (2008). The effect of self-construal on spatial judgments. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 337-348. doi:10.1086/588686

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Cross et al. 175

Krull, D. S., Loy, M. H., Lin, J., Wang, C., Chen, S., & Zhao, X. (1999). The fundamental fundamental attribution error: Cor-respondence bias in individualist and collectivist cultures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1208-1219. doi:10.1177/0146167299258003

Kuhn, M. H., & McPartland, T. (1954). An empirical investiga-tion of self-attitudes. American Sociological Review, 19, 58-76. doi:10.2307/2088175

Kühnen, U., & Haberstroh, S. (2004). Self-construal activation and focus of comparison as determinants of assimilation and contrast in social comparisons. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 22, 289-310.

Kühnen, U., & Hannover, B. (2000). Assimilation and contrast in social comparisons as a consequence of self-construal activation. Euro-pean Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 799-811. doi:10.1002/1099-0992(200011/12)30:6<799::AID-EJSP16>3.0.CO;2-2

Kühnen, U., Hannover, B., & Schubert, B. (2001). The semantic-procedural interface model of the self: The role of self-knowledge for context-dependent versus context-independent modes of think-ing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 397-409. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.3.397

Kühnen, U., & Oyserman, D. (2002). Thinking about the self influ-ences thinking in general: Cognitive consequences of salient self-concept. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 492-499. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00011-2

Kuo, W. H. (1984). Prevalence of depression among Asian-Americans. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 172, 449-457. doi:10.1097/00005053-198408000-00002

Kurman, J. (2001). Self-enhancement: Is it restricted to individualistic cultures? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1705-1716. doi:10.1177/01461672012712013

Kwan, V. S. Y., Bond, M. H., & Singelis, T. M. (1997). Pancultural explanations for life satisfaction: Adding relationship harmony to self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1038-1051. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.5.1038

Lalwani, A. K., & Shavitt, S. (2009). The “me” I claim to be: Cultural self-construal elicits self-presentational goal pursuit. Unpublished manuscript, University of Texas at San Antonio.

Lam, A. G., & Zane, N. W. S. (2004). Ethnic differences in coping with interpersonal stressors: A test of self-construals as cultural mediators. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 446-459. doi:10.1177/0022022104266108

Lam, B. T. (2005). Self-construal and depression among Vietnamese-American adolescents. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 29, 239-250. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2005.05.007

Leary, M. R. (2007). Motivational and emotional aspects of the self. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 317-344. doi:10.1146/annurev .psych.58.110405.085658

Leary, M. R., & Tangney, J. P. (Eds.). (2003). Handbook of self and identity. New York, NY: Guilford.

Lee, A. Y., Aaker, J. L., & Gardner, W. L. (2000). The pleasures and pains of distinct self-construals: The role of interdependence in regulatory focus. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1122-1134. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.6.1122

Levine, T. R., Bresnahan, M. J., Park, H. S., Lapinski, M. K., Wittenbaum, G. M., Shearman, S. M., . . . Ohashi, R. (2003). Self-construal scales lack validity. Human Communication Research, 29, 210-252. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2003.tb00837.x

Lewis, R. S., Goto, S. G., & Kong, L. L. (2008). Culture and context: East Asian American and European American differ-ences in P3 event-related potentials and self-construal. Person-ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 623-634. doi:10.1177/ 0146167207313731

Lin, Z., & Han, S. (2009). Self-construal priming modulates the scope of visual attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 802-813. doi:10.1080/17470210802271650

Lin, Z., Lin, Y., & Han, S. (2008). Self-construal priming modu-lates visual activity underlying global/local perception. Biologi-cal Psychology, 77, 93-97. doi:10.1016/j.bio psycho.2007.08.002

Liu, F. F., & Goto, S. G. (2007). Self-construal, mental distress, and family relations: A mediated moderation analysis with Asian American adolescents. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 13, 134-142. doi:10.1037/1099-9809.13.2.134

Locke, K. D., & Christensen, L. (2007). Re-construing the relational–interdependent self-construal and its relationship with self-consistency. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 389-402. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2006.04.005

Lun, J., Kesebir, S., & Oishi, S. (2008). On feeling understood and feeling well: The role of interdependence. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 1623-1628. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2008.06.009

Lydon, J. E., Menzies-Toman, D., Burton, K., & Bell, C. (2008). If-then contingencies and the differential effects of the availability of an attractive alternative on relationship maintenance for men and women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 50-65. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.50

Mandel, N. (2003). Shifting selves and decision making: The effects of self-construal priming on consumer risk-taking. Journal of Consumer Research, 30, 30-40. doi:10.1086/374700

Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Impli-cations for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224-253. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224

Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2004). Models of agency: Socio-cultural diversity in the construction of action. In V. Murphy-Berman & J. J. Berman (Eds.), Cross-cultural differences in perspectives on the self: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 49, pp. 1-57). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

Markus, H., & Wurf, E. (1987). The dynamic self-concept: A social psychological perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 299-337. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.38.020187.001503

Masuda, T., & Nisbett, R. E. (2001). Attending holistically versus analytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 922-934. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.922

Matsumoto, D. (1999). Culture and self: An empirical assessment of Markus and Kitayama’s theory of independent and interde-pendent self-construal. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 2, 289-310. doi:10.1111/1467-839X.00042

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

176 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

Miller, J. G. (1984). Culture and the development of everyday social explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 961-978. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.46.5.961

Morling, B., Kitayama, S., & Miyamoto, Y. (2002). Cultural prac-tices emphasize influence in the United States and adjustment in Japan. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 311-323. doi:10.1177/0146167202286003

Morris, M. W., & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 949-971. doi:10.1037/ 0022-3514.67.6.949

Morry, M. M. (2005). Allocentrism and friendship satisfaction: The mediating roles of disclosure and closeness. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 37, 211-222. doi:10.1037/h0087258

Moskowitz, G. B., & Roman, R. J. (1992). Spontaneous trait infer-ences as self-generated primes: Implications for conscious social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 728-738. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.62.5.728

Neff, K. D., Pisitsungkagarn, K., & Hsieh, Y.-P. (2008). Self-compassion and self-construal in the United States, Thailand, and Taiwan. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39, 267-285. doi:10.1177/0022022108314544

Nezlek, J. B., Kafetsios, K., & Smith, C. V. (2008). Emotions in everyday social encounters: Correspondence between culture and self-construal. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39, 366-372. doi:10.1177/0022022108318114

Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108, 291-310. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.291

Noguchi, K. (2007). Examination of the content of individualism/collectivism scales in cultural comparisons of the USA and Japan. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 10, 131-144.

Oetzel, J. G. (1998). Explaining individual communication pro-cesses in homogeneous and heterogeneous groups through individualism-collectivism and self-construal. Human Commu-nication Research, 25, 202-224. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.1998.tb00443.x

Oetzel, J. G., & Ting-Toomey, S. (2003). Face concerns in inter-personal conflict: A cross-cultural empirical test of face negation theory. Communication Research, 30, 599-624. doi:10.1177/0093650203257841

Oetzel, J., Ting-Toomey, S., Masumoto, T., Yokochi, Y., Pan, X., Takai, J., & Wilcox, R. (2001). Face and facework in conflict: A cross-cultural comparison of China, German, Japan, and the United States. Communication Monographs, 68, 235-258. doi:10.1080/03637750128061

Oishi, S., Wyer, R., & Colcombe, S. (2000). Cultural variation in the use of current life satisfaction to predict the future. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 434-445. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.3.434

Okazaki, S. (1997). Sources of ethnic differences between Asian American and White American college students on measures of depression and social anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 52-60. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.106.1.52

Oyserman, D. (1993). The lens of personhood: Viewing the self and others in a multicultural society. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 993-1009. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.5.993

Oyserman, D. (2006). High power, lower power and equality: Cul-ture beyond individualism and collectivism. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 16, 352-356. doi:10.1207/s15327663jcp1604_6

Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethink-ing individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 3-72. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.128.1.3

Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. W. S. (2008). Does culture influ-ence what and how we think? Effects of priming individual-ism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 311-342. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.311

Oyserman, D., Sorensen, N., Reber, R., & Chen, S. X. (2009). Con-necting and separating mind-sets: Culture as situated cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 217-235. doi:10.1037/a0015850

Oyserman, D., & Uskul, A. K. (2008). Individualism and collectiv-ism: societal-level processes with implications for individual-level and society-level outcomes. In F. J. R. van de Vijver, D. A. van Hemert, & Y. H. Poortinga (Eds.), Multilevel analysis of individuals and cultures (pp. 145-174). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Ozawa, K., Crosby, M., & Crosby, F. (1996). Individualism and resistance to affirmative action: A comparison of Japanese and American samples. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26, 1138-1152. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1996.tb02289.x

Pöhlmann, C., Carranza, E., Hannover, B., & Iyengar, S. S. (2007). Repercussions of self-construal for self-relevant and other-relevant choice. Social Cognition, 25, 284-305. doi:10.1521/soco.2007.25.2.284

Pöhlmann, C., & Hannover, B. (2006). Who shapes the self of inde-pendents and interdependents? Explicit and implicit measures of the self’s relatedness to family, friends and partner. European Journal of Personality, 20, 525-547. doi:10.1002/per.599

Polyorat, K., & Alden, D. L. (2005). Self-construal and need-for-cognition effects on brand attitudes and purchase intentions in response to comparative advertising in Thailand and the United States. Journal of Advertising, 34, 37-48.

Random House Dictionary. (1980). New York: Ballantine Books.Reid, A. (2004). Gender and sources of subjective well-being. Sex

Roles, 51, 617-629. doi:10.1007/s11199-004-0714-1Rhee, E., Uleman, J. S., Lee, H. K., & Roman, R. J. (1995). Spon-

taneous self-descriptions and ethnic identities in individualistic and collectivistic cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 142-152. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.1.142

Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Cross et al. 177

Ross, M., Xun, W. Q. E., & Wilson, A. E. (2002). Language and the bicultural self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1040-1050. doi:10.1177/01461672022811003

Sato, T., & Cameron, J. E. (1999). The relationship between collec-tive self-esteem and self-construal in Japan and Canada. Journal of Social Psychology, 139, 426-435.

Sato, T., & McCann, D. (1998). Individual differences in related-ness and individuality: An exploration of two constructs. Per-sonality and Individual Differences, 24, 847-859. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00020-8

Schimmack, U., Radhakrishnan, P., Oishi, S., Dzokoto, V., & Ahadi, S. (2002). Culture, personality, and subjective well-being: Integrating process models of life satisfaction. Jour-nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 582-593. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.4.582

Schmeichel, B. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (2004). Self-regulatory strength. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 84-98). New York, NY: Guilford.

Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 1-62). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

Sedikides, C., & Brewer, M. B. (Eds.). (2001). Individual self, rela-tional self, collective self. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., & Toguchi, Y. (2003). Pancultural self-enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 60-79. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.60

Seeley, E. A., & Gardner, W. L. (2003). The “selfless” and self-regulation: The role of chronic other-orientation in avert-ing self-regulatory depletion. Self and Identity, 2, 103-117. doi:10.1080/15298860309034

Seeley Howard, E. A., Gardner, W. L., & Thompson, L. (2007). The role of the self-concept and the social context in determin-ing the behavior of power holders: Self-construal in intergroup versus dyadic dispute resolution negotiations. Journal of Per-sonality and Social Psychology, 93, 614-631. doi:10.1037/ 0022-3514.93.4.614

Shah, J., Higgins, E. T., & Friedman, R. S. (1998). Performance incentives and means: How regulatory focus influences goal attainment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 285-293. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.285

Shweder, R. A., & Bourne, E. J. (1984). Does the concept of the person vary cross-culturally? In R. A. Shweder & R. A. LeVine (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion (pp. 158-199). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Sinclair, L., & Fehr, B. (2005). Voice vs. loyalty: Self-construals and responses to dissatisfaction in romantic relationships. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 298-304. doi:10.1016/j .jesp.2004.06.004

Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and inter-dependent self-construals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 580-591. doi:10.1177/0146167294205014

Singelis, T. M., Bond, M. H., Sharkey, W. F., & Lai, C. S. Y. (1999). Unpackaging culture’s influence on self-esteem and embarrass-ability: The role of self-construals. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30, 315-341. doi:10.1177/0022022199030003003

Singelis, T. M., & Brown, W. J. (1995). Culture, self, and col-lectivist communication: Linking culture to individual behavior. Human Communication Research, 21, 354-389. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.1995.tb00351.x

Singelis, T. M., Triandis, H. C., Bhawuk, D., & Gelfand, M. J. (1995). Horizontal and vertical dimensions of individualism and collectivism: A theoretical and measurement refinement. Cross-Cultural Research: Journal of Comparative Social Science, 29, 240-275. doi:10.1177/106939719502900302

Somech, A. (2000). The independent and the interdependent selves: Different meanings in different cultures. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 24, 161-172. doi:10.1016/S0147-1767(99)00030-9

Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Stapp, J. (1974). The Personal Attributes Questionnaire: A measure of sex-role stereotypes and masculinity-femininity. Journal Supplement Abstract Service Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 4, 43-44.

Stapel, D. A., & Koomen, W. (2001). I, we, and the effects of oth-ers on me: How self-construal level moderates social comparison effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 766-781. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.5.766

Stapel, D. A., & van der Zee, K. I. (2006). The self-salience model of other-to-self effects: Integrating principles of self-enhancement, complementarity, and imitation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 258-271. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.258

Stewart, A. J., & Lykes, M. B. (1985). Conceptualizing gender in personality theory and research. In A. J. Stewart & M. B. Lykes (Eds.), Gender and personality: Current perspectives on theory and research (pp. 2-13). Durham, NC: Duke University Press.

Strack, F., Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Kubler, A., & Wanke, M. (1993). Awareness of the influence as a determinant of assimilation ver-sus contrast. European Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 53-62. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420230105

Suh, E. M. (2002). Culture, identity consistency, and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1378-1391. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1378

Suh, E., Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Triandis, H. C. (1998). The shifting basis of life satisfaction judgments across cultures: Emotions versus norms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 494-512. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.482

Suh, E. M., Diener, E., & Updegraff, J. A. (2008). From culture to priming conditions: Self-construal influences on life satisfac-tion judgments. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39, 3-15. doi:10.1177/0022022107311769

Sui, J., & Han, S. (2007). Self-construal priming modulates neural substrates of self-awareness. Psychological Science, 18, 861-866. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01992.x

Takano, Y., & Osaka, E. (1999). An unsupported common view: Comparing Japan and the U.S. on individualism/collectivism.

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

178 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 2, 311-341. doi:10.1111/ 1467-839X.00043

Tangney, J. P., & Leary, M. R. (2003). The next generation of self-research. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self and identity (pp. 667-674). New York, NY: Guilford.

Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193-210. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.103.2.193

Terzino, K. A., & Cross, S. E. (2009). Predicting commitment in new relationships: Interactive effects of relational self-construal and power. Self and Identity, 8, 321-341.

Tesser, A. (1980). Self-esteem maintenance in family dynamics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 77-91. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.39.1.77

Tesser, A., Crepaz, N., Beach, S. R. H., Cornell, D., & Collins, J. C. (2000). Confluence of self-esteem regulation mechanisms: On integrating the self-zoo. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1476-1489. doi:10.1177/01461672002612003

Thomsen, L., Sidanius, J., & Fiske, A. P. (2007). Interpersonal level-ing, independence, and self-enhancement: A comparison between Denmark and the US, and a relational practice framework for cultural psychology. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 445-469. doi:10.1002/ejsp.366

Ting-Toomey, S. (1988). Intercultural conflict styles: A face-negotiation theory. In Y. Y. Kim & W. Gudykunst (Eds.), Theories in intercul-tural communication (pp. 213-235). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Ting-Toomey, S., Oetzel, J. G., & Yee-Jung, K. (2001). Self-construal types and conflict management styles. Communication Reports, 14, 87-104.

Torelli, C. J. (2006). Individuality or conformity? The effect of inde-pendent and interdependent self-concepts on public judgments. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 16, 240-248. doi:10.1207/s15327663jcp1603_6

Trafimow, D., & Finlay, K. A. (1996). The importance of subjective norms for a minority of people: Between-subjects and within-subjects analyses. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 820-828. doi:10.1177/0146167296228005

Trafimow, D., Silverman, E. S., Fan, R. M., & Law, J. S. F. (1997). The effects of language and priming on the relative accessibility of the private self and the collective self. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 28, 107-123. doi:10.1177/0022022197281007

Trafimow, D., & Smith, M. D. (1998). An extension of the “two-baskets” theory to Native Americans. European Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 1015-1019. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(1998110)28:6<1015::AID-EJSP900>3.0.CO;2-S

Trafimow, D., Triandis, H. C., & Goto, S. G. (1991). Some tests of the distinction between the private self and the collective self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 649-655. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.5.649

Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differ-ing cultural contexts. Psychological Review, 96, 506-520. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.96.3.506

Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview.

Uskul, A. K., & Hynie, M. (2007). Self-construal and concerns elic-ited by imagines and real health problems. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 37, 2156-2189. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816 .2007.00283.x

Utz, S. (2004). Self-construal and cooperation: Is the interdepen-dent self more cooperative than the independent self? Self and Identity, 3, 177-190. doi:10.1080/13576500444000001

van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., Steenaert, B., & van Knippenberg, A. (2003). Mimicry for money: Behavioral consequences of imita-tion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(4), 393-398. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00014-3

van Baaren, R. B., Maddux, W. W., Chartrand, T. L., de Bouter, C., & van Knippenberg, A. (2003). It takes two to mimic: Behavioral consequences of self-construals. Jour-nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1093-1102. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.1093

van de Vijver, F. J. R., van Hemert, D. A., & Poortinga, Y. H. (2008). Conceptual issues in multilevel models. In F. J. R. van de Vijver, D. A. van Hemert, & Y. H. Poortinga (Eds.), Multilevel analysis of individuals and cultures (pp. 3-26). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

van Horen, F., Pöhlmann, C., Koeppen, K., & Hannover, B. (2008). Importance of personal goals in people with independent ver-sus interdependent selves. Social Psychology, 39, 213-221. doi:10.1027/1864-9335.39.4.213

van Knippenberg, D., van Knippenberg, B., De Cremer, D., & Hogg, M. A. (2004). Leadership, self, and identity: A review and research agenda. Leadership Quarterly, 15, 825-856. doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2004.09.002

Verplanken, B., Trafimow, D., Khusid, I. K., Holland, R. W., & Steentjes, G. M. (2009). Different selves, different values: Effects of self-construals on value activation and use. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 909-919. doi:10.1002/ejsp.587

Vorauer, J. D., & Cameron, J. J. (2002). So close, and yet so far: Does collectivism foster transparency overestimation? Jour-nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1344-1352. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1344

Walker, G. J., Deng, J., & Dieser, R. B. (2005). Culture, self-construal, and leisure theory and practice. Journal of Leisure Research, 37, 77-99.

Wang, Q., & Ross, M. (2005). What we remember and what we tell: The effects of culture and self-priming on memory representations and narratives. Memory, 13, 594-606. doi:10.1080/09658210444000223

Weisz, J. R., Rothbaum, F. M., & Blackburn, T. C. (1984). Standing out and standing in: The psychology of control in America and Japan. American Psychologist, 39, 955-969. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.39.9.955

Werth, L., & Forster, J. (2007). The effects of regulatory focus on brak-ing speed. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 37, 2764-2787. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2007.00280.x

White, K., Lehman, D. R., & Cohen, D. (2006). Culture, self-construal, and affective reactions to successful and unsuccessful others. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 582-592. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.10.001

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Cross et al. 179

Wiekens, C. J., & Stapel, D. A. (2008). I versus we: The effects of self-construal level on diversity. Social Cognition, 26, 368-377. doi:10.1521/soco.2008.26.3.368

Witkin, H. A., & Berry, J. W. (1975). Psychological differentiation in cross-cultural perspective. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychol-ogy, 6, 4-87.

Xie, D., Leong, F. T. L., & Feng, S. (2008). Culture-specific per-sonality correlates of anxiety among Chinese and Caucasian col-lege students. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 11, 163-174. doi:10.1111/j.1467-839X.2008.00253.x

Yamada, A. M., & Singelis, T. M. (1999). Biculturalism and self-con-strual. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 23, 697-709.

Yamagishi, T., & Yamagishi, M. (1994). Trust and commitment in the United States and Japan. Motivation and Emotion, 18, 129-166. doi:10.1007/BF02249397

Ybarra, O., & Trafimow, D. (1998). How priming the private self or collective self affects the relative weights of attitudes and sub-jective norms. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 362-370. doi:10.1177/0146167298244003

Yeh, C. J., & Arora, A. K. (2003). Multicultural training and interdependent and independent self-construal as predictors of universal-diverse orientation among school counselors. Journal of Counseling & Development, 81, 78-83.

Yoon, J., & Lau, A. (2008). Maladaptive perfectionism and depressive symptoms among Asian American college stu-dents: Contributions of interdependence and parental relations.

Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 14, 92-101. doi:10.1037/1099-9809.14.2.92

Yuki, M., Maddux, W. W., Brewer, M. B., & Takemura, K. (2005). Cross-cultural differences in relationship- and group-based trust. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 48-62. doi:10.1177/0146167204271305

Yum, Y. (2004). Culture and self-construal as predictors of responses to accommodative dilemmas in dating relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 21, 817-835. doi:10.1177/0265407504047839

Zhang, Y., Feick, L., & Price, L. J. (2006). The impact of self-construal on aesthetic preference for angular versus rounded shapes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 794-805. doi:10.1177/0146167206286626

Zhang, Y., & Mittal, V. (2007). The attractiveness of enriched and impoverished options: Culture, self-construal, and regulatory focus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 588-598. doi:10.1177/0146167206296954

Zhang, Y., & Shrum, L. J. (2009). The influence of self-construal on impulsive consumption. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 838-850. doi:10.1086/593687

Zou, X., Tam, K., Morris, M. W., Lee, S., Lau, I. Y., & Chiu, C. (2009). Culture as common sense: Perceived consensus versus personal beliefs as mechanisms of cultural influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(4), 579-597. doi:10.1037/a0016399

at University of Sussex Library on June 17, 2011psr.sagepub.comDownloaded from