Neural$Dynamics$ofDecision:Making$in$a$Financial$Trading… · 2014. 3. 31. · Subject-level...

1
Occipital pTemporal Parietal Central aTemporal Frontal Time Window (ms) –3.5 3.5 T Values 300 600 900 1200 1500 0 0.05 P Values 300 600 900 1200 1500 ERP Results: Choice Optimality Neural Dynamics of DecisionMaking in a Financial Trading Task Alison Harris 1 , Cary Frydman 2 & Tom Chang 2 1 Department of Psychology, Claremont McKenna College, Claremont, CA USA 2 Department of Finance and Business Economics, Marshall School of Business, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA USA Disposition effect Sell winning stocks more often than losing stocks Deviation from optimal financial decision-making Realization Utility theory Pleasure from sale relative to purchase cost (capital gain) It hurts to sell at a loss, but “locking in” a gain is satisfying When and how does financial decision-making occur in the brain? Value signals related to capital gain Neural correlates of optimal vs. suboptimal trading choice Introduction } Methods N = 60 Investing in stock market with stocks A, B, C Update period: Price change Action period: Buy or sell decision $80 B BUY? Action 2 DOWN $5 --> $80 B Don’t own Update 2 $75 A Cost: $100 SELL? Action 1 Update 1 UP $15 --> $30 C Purchased: $100 Trial str ructure Update 2 s Prep (Fixation) 1-2 s Action RT (max 3 s) ITI (Fixation) 3 * RT Procedure 128-channel EEG Could only hold 0 or 1 units of each stock Informed of stock market properties at start of experiment Good state: p(up) = 0.7, p(down) = 0.3 Bad state: p(up) = 0.3, p(down) = 0.7 20% chance of changing from good to bad state or vice versa Payoff at end of experiment based on stock holdings and sales Predictions Participants will exhibit behavioral disposition effect (DE) Capital gain (CG) at sell decision correlates with neural value signal Ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) From ~400 ms after stimulus onset Neural sensitivity to CG associated with selling “winners” Optimal choice requires overcoming realization utility bias Analogous to regulating behavioral/cognitive conflict Anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) ERP Results: Capital Gain Behavioral Results Average DE = 0.07 significantly greater than zero (p = 0.04) Suboptimal behavior compared to “rational” Bayesian agent -0.7 -0.5 -0.3 -0.1 0.1 0.3 0.5 0.7 Disposition Effect Subject “Rational” DE 0 1250 2500 3750 5000 Held Gains Sold Gains Held Losses Sold Losses Number of Responses Optimal Suboptimal Neutral EEG data time-locked to Action period onset Subject-level linear regression: Capital Gain: y sensor,time = β 0 + β 1 CapitalGain + β 2 BayesianPosterior + ε 100 ms 100 ms + 0.5 μV Lateral Central Optimal Suboptimal Hold Sell -0.4 μV -0.8 μV 0 μV 1.2 μV Gain Loss * EEG data time-locked to Action period onset Paired t test on optimal vs. suboptimal choice Optimal: hold if b i (good) > 0.5 , sell if b i (good) < 0.5 150 ms Frontal 100 ms + 1 μV 1.8 μV 2.1 μV Mean Amplitude <25% Gain 25-50% Gain 50-75% Gain >75% Gain 3 3 T Values 150 to 200 ms post-stimulus 400 to 650 ms post-stimulus 450 ms Central 100 ms + 0.5 μV 0.9 μV 1.3 μV Mean Amplitude 350 to 450 ms post-stimulus -0 μV -0.8 μV 0 μV 1.2 μV Gain Loss Disposition effect exists despite being financially suboptimal Capital gain at sell decision correlates with ERP value signal Emerges 400-650 ms after stimulus onset Localized to vmPFC Neural CG signal correlates with propensity to sell winners Optimal choice requires overcoming realization utility bias Neural signals as early as 100-150 ms after stimulus onset Localized to ACC ERP provides insight into time course of disposition effect Supports role of neural value signals in realization utility bias Conclusions Occipital pTemporal Parietal Central aTemporal Frontal Time Window (ms) –3.5 4 T Values 300 600 900 1200 1500 0 0.05 P Values 300 600 900 1200 1500 100 to 150 ms post-stimulus Correlating ERP with Behavior 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 -1 -0.5 0 0.5 1 Propensity to Sell Winners Capital Gain β 400-650 ms r = 0.28 p = 0.03 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 -1 -0.5 0 0.5 1 Propensity to Sell Losers Capital Gain β 400-650 ms r = 0.05 p = 0.45 ERP Source Reconstruction Distributed source reconstruction in SPM8 (group inversion) Capital Gain, 400-650 ms post-stimulus fMRI Linear ordering of CG quartiles Localized to vmPFC Consistent with fMRI (Frydman et al., 2014) F = 150 y = 39 Optimal vs. Suboptimal, 100-150 ms post-stimulus Optimal > Suboptimal Left dorsal ACC Bilateral precentral gyrus Left anterior insula Suboptimal > Optimal Genual ACC Right anterior insula Bilateral parietal lobe 5 6 –6 –5 T Values vmPFC 100 ms + 0.5 μV Lateral Central 350 ms Mean Amplitude Mean Amplitude

Transcript of Neural$Dynamics$ofDecision:Making$in$a$Financial$Trading… · 2014. 3. 31. · Subject-level...

Page 1: Neural$Dynamics$ofDecision:Making$in$a$Financial$Trading… · 2014. 3. 31. · Subject-level linear regression: Capital Gain: y sensor,time = β 0 + β 1CapitalGain + β 2BayesianPosterior

Occipital

pTemporal

Parietal

Central

aTemporal

Frontal

Time Window (ms)

–3.5

3.5

T Values

300 600 900 1200 1500

0

0.05

P Values

300 600 900 1200 1500

ERP  Results:  Choice  Optimality

Neural  Dynamics  of  Decision-­‐Making  in  a  Financial  Trading  TaskAlison Harris1, Cary Frydman2 & Tom Chang2

1 Department of Psychology, Claremont McKenna College, Claremont, CA USA2 Department of Finance and Business Economics, Marshall School of Business, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA USA

❖ Disposition effect❖ Sell winning stocks more often than losing stocks❖ Deviation from optimal financial decision-making

❖ Realization Utility theory❖ Pleasure from sale relative to purchase cost (capital gain)❖ It hurts to sell at a loss, but “locking in” a gain is satisfying

❖ When and how does financial decision-making occur in the brain?❖ Value signals related to capital gain❖ Neural correlates of optimal vs. suboptimal trading choice

Introduction

}

Methods❖ N = 60❖ Investing in stock market with stocks A, B, C

❖ Update period: Price change❖ Action period: Buy or sell decision

$80B

BUY?

Action 2

DOWN $5 --> $80B

Don’t own

Update 2

$75A

Cost: $100 SELL?

Action 1

Update 1

UP $15 --> $30C

Purchased: $100

Trial structureTrial structureTrial structureTrial structure

Update2 s

Prep (Fixation)1-2 s

ActionRT (max 3 s)

ITI (Fixation)3 * RT

❖ Procedure❖ 128-channel EEG❖ Could only hold 0 or 1 units of each stock❖ Informed of stock market properties at start of experiment

❖ Good state: p(up) = 0.7, p(down) = 0.3❖ Bad state: p(up) = 0.3, p(down) = 0.7❖ 20% chance of changing from good to bad state or vice versa

❖ Payoff at end of experiment based on stock holdings and sales

Predictions❖ Participants will exhibit behavioral disposition effect (DE)❖ Capital gain (CG) at sell decision correlates with neural value

signal❖ Ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC)❖ From ~400 ms after stimulus onset❖ Neural sensitivity to CG associated with selling “winners”

❖ Optimal choice requires overcoming realization utility bias❖ Analogous to regulating behavioral/cognitive conflict❖ Anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)

ERP  Results:  Capital  Gain

Behavioral  Results❖ Average DE = 0.07 significantly greater than zero (p = 0.04)❖ Suboptimal behavior compared to “rational” Bayesian agent

-0.7

-0.5

-0.3

-0.1

0.1

0.3

0.5

0.7

Dis

po

siti

on

Eff

ect

Subject

“Rational” DE

0

1250

2500

3750

5000

Held Gains Sold Gains Held Losses Sold Losses

Num

ber

of R

esp

ons

es

OptimalSuboptimalNeutral

❖ EEG data time-locked to Action period onset❖ Subject-level linear regression:

Capital Gain: ysensor,time = β0 + β1CapitalGain + β2BayesianPosterior + ε

99 ms

100 ms

100 ms

+0.5 µV

Lateral

Central

OptimalSuboptimal

HoldSell

-0.4 µV

-0.8 µV

0 µV

1.2 µV

Gain Loss

*

❖ EEG data time-locked to Action period onset❖ Paired t test on optimal vs. suboptimal choice

❖ Optimal: hold if bi(good) > 0.5 , sell if bi(good) < 0.5

150 ms

150 msFrontal

100 ms

+1 µV

–1.8 µV

2.1 µVMean Amplitude

<25% Gain 25-50% Gain 50-75% Gain >75% Gain

–3

3

T V

alue

s

150 to 200 ms post-stimulus

400 to 650 ms post-stimulus

450 ms

450 ms

Central

100 ms

+0.5 µV

–0.9 µV

1.3 µVMean Amplitude

350 to 450 ms post-stimulus

-0 µV

-0.8 µV

0 µV

1.2 µV

Gain Loss

❖ Disposition effect exists despite being financially suboptimal❖ Capital gain at sell decision correlates with ERP value signal

❖ Emerges 400-650 ms after stimulus onset❖ Localized to vmPFC❖ Neural CG signal correlates with propensity to sell winners

❖ Optimal choice requires overcoming realization utility bias❖ Neural signals as early as 100-150 ms after stimulus onset❖ Localized to ACC

➡ ERP provides insight into time course of disposition effect➡ Supports role of neural value signals in realization utility bias

Conclusions

Occipital

pTemporal

Parietal

Central

aTemporal

Frontal

Time Window (ms)

–3.5

4

T Values

300 600 900 1200 1500

0

0.05

P Values

300 600 900 1200 1500

100 to 150 ms post-stimulus

Correlating  ERP  with  Behavior

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1

-1 -0.5 0 0.5 1

Pro

pen

sity

to S

ell W

inne

rs

Capital Gain β 400-650 ms

r = 0.28p = 0.03

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1

-1 -0.5 0 0.5 1

Pro

pen

sity

to S

ell L

ose

rs

Capital Gain β 400-650 ms

r = 0.05p = 0.45

ERP  Source  Reconstruction❖ Distributed source reconstruction in SPM8 (group inversion)

Capital Gain, 400-650 ms post-stimulus

fMRI ❖ Linear ordering of CG quartiles

❖ Localized to vmPFC❖ Consistent with fMRI

(Frydman et al., 2014)F = 150

y = 39

Optimal vs. Suboptimal, 100-150 ms post-stimulus

❖ Optimal > Suboptimal❖ Left dorsal ACC❖ Bilateral precentral gyrus❖ Left anterior insula

❖ Suboptimal > Optimal❖ Genual ACC❖ Right anterior insula❖ Bilateral parietal lobe

5 6–6 –5

T Values

vmPFC

100 ms

+0.5 µV

Lateral

Central

Latency 350 ms from TVals

−3.1

−1.6

0

1.6

3.1

350 ms

Mean Amplitude

Mean Amplitude