Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

59
Models of stuttering

Transcript of Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Page 1: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Models of stuttering

Page 2: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Why develop a model of stuttering? What does a model of

stuttering give us In terms of guiding treatment? In terms of guiding research? For one other good reason to

study models of stuttering, stay awake ‘til the end of this presentation!

What must a good model of stuttering account for?

Page 3: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

What is a model of stuttering? In trying to explain stuttering, we must try to account for:

What initially causes stuttering “triggers” vs. causes?

What the basis for fluency breakdown is How stuttering develops over the lifespan (how it is maintained and

perpetuated) Some “models” are not mutually exclusive

(e.g., genetics and learning) Etiology ≠ model, but can help build one

A good model must explain: The phenomenology of stuttering When and how stuttering develops How fluency breakdown fits within known facts about speech

production

Page 4: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

A historical perspective

Stuttering is one of the oldest recorded communication disorders

Theories can be divided into those that view stuttering as:PhysiologicalPsychologicalEnvironmentalMultifactorial

Page 5: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

The nature vs. nurture controversy Models of stuttering tend to divide in their emphasis

on whether stuttering is primarily due to physiological bases, environmental influences, or a mixture of both. Virtually no models ignore some component of learning in

the DEVLOPMENT of the disorder Before discussing models, we will examine studies

that have contrasted PWS and normally fluent speakers on physiological and environmental features.

Page 6: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Nature

Refinement of genetic studies show a clear evidence of genetic contribution to many cases of stuttering; such studies weaken environmental accounts when

affected family members cannot serve as models, or demonstrate patterns of stuttering that differ from that of the affected child.

Conversely, discordant twins show some impact of non-genetic factors on the emergence of stuttering.

Page 7: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Genetic factors in stuttering. At this point, few disagree that stuttering has a genetic

component. This component appears to predict chronicity and recovery better than it predicts severity, and the model of the genetic transmission is still under discussion. Cases in which monozygotes differ in stutter symptoms are problematic, although such situations are found in other disorders having clear genetic roots. Cases in which adopted children who stutter show family histories of stuttering in the adopting family suggest an environmental contribution to stuttering to some researchers (Bloodstein, 1995).

MISSING DATA: Although genetic factors can remove weight from some hypotheses about the nature of stuttering, they do not explain the mechanism by which stuttering physically arises or is best treated. Moreover, because stuttering is at least partially predicted by genetic background, studies of parents of CWS should, but usually don’t, segregate reports of adult parenting styles by fluency status of parent.

Page 8: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Nature Most studies of physiological function in stuttering

divide into the following topic areas:Reaction timeCoordinationTime and visuospatial estimationLaterality and cortical organization for languageTask interference (capacity limitations)

Most find some, but minor and inconsistent differences (see book).

Page 9: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Physiological models

Perhaps the first models of all models (excluding religious explanations): Older models tended to localize stuttering to weaknesses in specific peripheral systems, particularly the tongue, from Greco-Roman times through the last century

Other parts of the vocal tract, e.g., the larynx, were investigated during the 1960’s and 1970’s.

By far, the most common modern physiological models focus on cortical representation of, and control of, speech and language functions.

Page 10: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Challenges to studies of physiological limitations in stuttering Do such limitations exist at stuttering onset or do

they result from adaptation to stuttering? Could transient limitations early in development

yield persistent symptoms? Do observed patterns predict the behavioral

symptoms of stuttering? Are aberrant physiological patterns amenable to

treatment?

Page 11: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Lateralizationmodels (Webster, 1996) Orton-Travis: In normal

speech/language production, a dominant hemisphere takes primary responsibility for linguistic functions. In stuttering, dominance fails to occur, causing competing commands to speech/language functions. From W.

Webster

Page 12: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Orton-Travis (continued)SUPPORTING DATA: Differences between lateralization of language

functions in PWS and fluent speakers, as revealed through numerous tasks: EEG (e.g., Moore & Haynes, 1980), dichotic listening (e.g., Gregory & Curry, 1967), PET, fMRI (current work by teams such as DeNil & Kroll (1995), Fox, Ingham & Ingham (1996), Braun, et al. (1996), more each month.

CONFLICTING DATA: No evidence of changed handedness history in majority of PWS. No evidence that reverting handedness effects fluency.

MISSING DATA: Whether lateralization differences are primary or the result of experience with stuttering; lateralization theories are particularly vulnerable to this challenge, as therapy (particularly those that deal with speech planning and pacing) and emotional reactions are logically presumed to elicit changes in right hemisphere function.

Page 13: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Other hemispheric organization models (from Webster):

As illustrated in Figure 1B, the interhemispheric interference model has three major features to it: i) normal left hemisphere lateralization; ii) normal right hemisphere functioning; but iii) interference with the left hemisphere coming from the right hemisphere through a "slop-over" of activity (the large arrow). Within this model, variations in stuttering severity would reflect variations in the amount of overflow from the right to left hemispheres.

Figure 1C illustrates Webster’s Two-Factor Interference Model. Three elements comprise this model: i) we still assume normal left hemisphere specialization for speech and motor sequencing; ii) we see a left hemisphere system for speech and motor sequencing that is inefficient and unusually susceptible to interference (illustrated by the larger number of "pores"), not just from activity in the right hemisphere, but also the left (illustrated with arrows from both hemispheres impinging on the speech mechanisms); and iii) a lack of what we call "left hemisphere activation bias" (illustrated with equal rather than unequal sized arrows coming from the mid-and hind-brain activating system).

Page 14: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Other cerebral lateralization models: Geschwind-Galaburda model (1985): a variety of speech-language

disturbances (dyslexia, stuttering, autism) may arise from delays in left hemisphere development during gestation. Such delays lead to inefficient right hemisphere dominance for language functions. Because of the loading of such disorders in males, the authors hypothesized a role of fetal exposure to high levels of testosterone.

SUPPORTING DATA: Sex-bias in stuttering incidence; other data supporting Orton-Travis; recent anatomical data from Foundas, et al.

CONFLICTING DATA: No obvious conflicts, but theory leaves stuttering in girls somewhat unresolved

MISSING DATA: Mechanisms by which the different disorders arise; evidence of actual testosterone influences during development, lack of known association between stuttering and developmental conditions characterized by hormonal abnormalities.

Page 15: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Data for dominance models

Handedness (inconclusive) Wada test (inconclusive) Dichotic listening and CAP batteries

(variable) Neuroimaging (more promising)

Structural (e.g., CT, MRI)Functional (e.g., fMRI, EEG, EPs)

Page 16: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

More on neuroimaging Previous work found few structural differences between

PWS and NS; however, work in the past year by Foundas, et al. (2001) and Sommer, et al.(2002), have shown anatomical variation between the groups.

(See a short summary of Foundas’ work); Sommer et al. is on your reading list and linked.

Work examining functional differences in processing has been much more promising: rCBF (Watson, et al., 1994; Ingham, Fox & Ingham, 1994) PET (Wu, et al., 1995; Braun, et al., 1997; DeNil, et al., 2000; Fox

et al, 1996 – differences found; Ingham, et al., 1996 – no support for cerebral asymmetries)

(See Fox et al. data here) Some studies suggest that observed differences in

cortical function between PWS and NS may diminish following intensive fluency therapy (DeNil, et al., 2000).

Page 17: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Fig. 2 Brain map illustrating focal rCBF activation during the formulation and expression of language in controls (a–d) and stuttering subjects (e–h) during tasks in which stuttering subjects were dysfluent. Language tasks are contrasted with the oral motor task as a baseline, in order to highlight regions involved in linguistic processing. Data for control subjects are displayed in the top row (a–d), and for

stuttering subjects in the bottom row (e–h).

Example: Braun, et al., 1997

Page 18: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Controls

Pre Post 1 year

L R

Source: L. De Nil, Univ. Toronto

Effects of fluency therapy on brain activity during single word reading

Page 19: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Neuropsycholinguistic models

Neuroimaging data support but do not constitute a model of what deficits underlie stuttering

For a terrific review of the problems in interpreting recent neuroimaging studies, see the recent presentation by Luc De Nil (be patient, it has wonderful graphics and can take a while to load).

Neuropsycholinguistic models are more abstract and provide a more schematic view of the underlying deficit in stuttering; they tend to be grounded in psycholinguistic models of the normal speech/language production process.

Among the more popular are the Covert Repair Hypothesis (Postma & Kolk) and the Neuropsycholinguistic Model (Perkins, Kent & Curlee).

Page 20: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Levelt’s model of speech production and comprehension

Page 21: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Covert Repair Hypothesis Note that Levelt’s model of speech production

contains a pre-articulatory monitoring stage, for which there is considerable evidence. The CRH states that people who stutter have a

temporal impairment in phonological encoding (it is slowed). When the phonetic plan cannot proceed normally, this impairment is adapted to by covert repairs, restarts and postponements, which result in the surface or overt behavior of stuttering. Thus stuttering is seen as a by-product or side-effect of self-repairs.

Evidence to support the model is building, but is primarily being gathered by the model’s builders.

It is also unclear at which stage phonological encoding or monitoring is disturbed.

Page 22: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Covert Repair Hypothesis

Page 23: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Self-monitoring loops

Page 24: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Spreading activation and phonological error Connectionist models (e.g., Dell, 1988),

which describe speech production and comprehension in terms of activation of representations and their spread (spreading activation) may account for mis-selections or poorly timed phonetic plans.

Page 25: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Spreading activation: the general concept and an illustration

Page 26: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

A potential model of delayed phonological encoding from Yaruss & Conture, 1996).

Page 27: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Components of the NPL model:

This is basically a temporal dis-synchrony model Disfluencies occur due to a disruption in timing

among various linguistic formulation and motoric execution demands

Stuttering is perceived as a loss of control; the speaker cannot proceed but does not know why; internally imposed time pressure pushes the incomplete phonetic plan forward.

For an excellent review of the Covert Repair and NPL models and their implications, visit Scott Yaruss's supercourse on this topic.

Page 28: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

NPL (continued)

SUPPORTING DATA: most linguistic and motor differences noted between PWS and fluent speakers, including imaging and ERP studies (e.g., Weber-Fox, 2001); linguistic regularities in stutter events.

CONFLICTING DATA: the theory places stuttering on a continuum with normal disfluencies, and thus stutter events should conform to patterns of normal disfluency, which they do not in advanced stuttering (Silverman & Bernstein Ratner, 1997).

MISSING DATA: weak evidence for subjective time pressure and loss of control (although some data do exist: Ezrati-Vinecour & Levin, 2001; Barasch, et al., 2000).

Page 29: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Other “Higher Order” deficit models:

Inverse Modeling Deficit: Neilson & Neilson Some concepts have been lifted into the Demands and

Capacities Model (later), but are based on cybernetic theory. Summary: There is a deficit in the persons ability to make

and use Inverse internal models of the speech production system. the child has a sensory-motor model for speech (it inverts sensory targets into motor commands) Children plan an utterance based on what it should sound like. They generate the motor commands. The motor commands are sent to the muscles which are stored in the brain as models. The actual speech is compared to the model expected. The child has a weakness in making transformations between what they want to say and the motor movements required to say them: the sensory to motor and motor sensory transformation. If the demands are not great, stuttering doesn’t occur because the child can compensate for the weakness.

Page 30: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Stuttering as a timing disorder Van Riper (1982) favored the view that stuttering represented mis-

coordination of the timing gestures needed for speech. Elaborated on by Kent (1984), who also relates earlier theories of cerebral organization to this theory: the left hemisphere is specialized for processing of brief, rapidly changing acoustic/motoric events, and may be impaired in carrying out this function in PWS. Finally, Kent notes the possible interference with effective function by overactivity in the right hemisphere stemming from emotional arousal.

SUPPORTING DATA: Evidence of poorly coordinated gestures during speech production in PWS (e.g., Caruso, Abbs & Gracco, 1988); acoustic measures showing abnormalities in speech timing for VOT and other parameters of speech (e.g., Zimmerman, 1980 and many others). Slowed RT data are plentiful for PWS for a variety of targeted behaviors, both motoric and linguistic (Bosshardt & Frandsen, 1996).

CONFLICTING DATA: Few, although mistimed segments are not well-coordinated with stutter events (see Smith, 1998); variety of stuttered types not well accounted for (by any theory).

MISSING DATA: more data needed from very young children, to show that timing problems are not result of stuttering, or ancillary to stuttering.

Page 31: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Stuttering as an auditory feedback disorder Stromsta (1965) suggested that stutterers have

an built-in “DAF”-like defect in their auditory feedback systems; Tomatis (1963) believed that stutterers suffered from a hearing loss in the dominant ear used to monitor speech. While changes in feedback (DAF, FAF) does produce increased fluency for many persons who stutter, neither theory is highly regarded at this time.

Page 32: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Stuttering as learned behavior

Many of the models in this category can be termed “anticipatory struggle” hypotheses.

Some of the earliest models developed in this century by the first professional speech-language pathologists fit under this rubric.The “Iowa” school of research

Page 33: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Diagnosogenic theory (Johnson, 1959)

A popular reduction of this theory into few words is that stuttering is in the “ear of the parent”

Parents react poorly to normal disfluencies in children; this reaction is perceived and responded to with anxiety and avoidance by the children, setting up a cycle of fluency failure, anxiety, and more fluency failure.

What types of evidence would be required to validate this theory?

Still a popular theory with broad implications for advisement to parents, even though most of its principal tenets have been disproven.

Page 34: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Diagnosogenic theory (continued) SUPPORTING DATA: Early studies showing

high levels of demand in parents of CWS; high sensitivity to normal disfluencies as pathological.

CONFLICTING DATA: Almost all studies done in recent years. In general, there are few differences between parents of CWS and their fluent peers (see Bernstein Ratner & Silverman, 2000); it is virtually impossible to tell, when attitudes differ, which came first: the attitude or the stuttering? Finally, effectiveness of some operant, parent-

administered programs, such as Lidcombe, are in direct conflict with predictions flowing from the diagnosogenic theory.

Page 35: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Ethics and stuttering research: the “Monster Study” What was the “Monster Study”? Read more about the “Monster Study” Discussion:

What are the ethical obligations of researchers in communication disorders?

What data are required to support a model of stuttering?

Page 36: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Approach-avoidance

General principles: Conflict occurs when a person experiences demands

or desires that are incompatible with each other. In approach-approach conflict we are attracted to two equally desirable goals. In avoidance-avoidance conflict we must choose between two equally undesirable demands. In approach-avoidance conflict we have one goal that has positive and negative aspects. And in double approach-avoidance conflict we experience two or more goals, both of which have positive and negative aspects.

How would this apply to speaking and stuttering? Read Joe Sheehan's "Message to a stutterer"

Page 37: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Approach-Avoidance (Sheehan, 1953, 1958) in more detail

Stuttering results from the conflict between the opposed drives to speak and to avoid speaking. When the desire to speak is stronger than the desire not to speak, speakers can be fluent. When the desires to speak and not to speak are equal, stuttering results. Speech avoidance might result from: 1) reactions to specific words, stemming from conditioned responses to phonetic factors, 2) reactions to threatening speaking situations, 3) guilt or anxiety about message content, 3) anxiety about relationship between the speaker and listener, and 5) the need of the ego to avoid competitive situations that might result in failure. This theory is a hybrid of learning theory and personality factors as primary components in stuttering development.

Approach-avoidance conflict may have its roots in generalized emotions about speaking, but quickly develops strong ties to feelings about fluency and stuttering itself.

Page 38: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

The approach-avoidance gradient

Page 39: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

The Iceberg of Stuttering

Page 40: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

More recent views of learning and stuttering In the 1960’s more formal accounts of how stuttering

could be described in terms of classical (instrumental) and operant (respondant) conditioning were advanced. In classical conditioning, the child learns to associate speaking

with an emotional response In operant conditioning, fluency failures are shaped by the

responses they elicit. This account explains development better than onset

Example of such a model: Brutten & Shoemaker (1967) two-factor model:

Stuttering is the involuntary disruption of speech resulting from negative emotional responses that are classically conditioned, while secondary behaviors are instrumentally conditioned

Page 41: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?
Page 42: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?
Page 43: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?
Page 44: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?
Page 45: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Learning theory (continued)SUPPORTING DATA: Interventions in which the techniques of operant conditioning are

effective suggest some support for learning theory in stuttering (e.g., punishment, negative reinforcement, time-out, positive reinforcement of fluency, etc.) Ingham notes that no intervention has managed to increase stuttering by rewarding it, an imbalance for a full-fledged model of stuttering as a conditioned behavior.

CONFLICTING DATA: generally low level of success of such interventions with adults who stutter; genetic factors, motor factors, linguistic factors that unite many PWS.

MISSING DATA: Plots of events that might have conditioned onsets; mechanisms by which operant techniques appear to achieve results (Bonelli, Dixon, Ratner & Onslow, 1998).

Page 46: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Related model:Bloodstein: the continuity hypothesis Stuttering evolves from normal disfluency,

and the child’s responses that lead to tension and fragmentation.These responses are self-generated and are

not caused by parental reactions.

Page 47: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Multi-factorial models

Multi-factorial models attempt to integrate physiology, learning and the environment in the etiology and development of stuttering

Because they involve multiple factors, they are hard to test and validate, although some are more empirically testable than others (e.g., Smith’s Multifactorial-Dynamic model vs. Demands and Capacities).

Page 48: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Demands and Capacities (DCM)

Model popularized by Starkweather, Gottwald & Halfond, 1990. Based on earlier proposals by Andrews & Neilson (1981) Genetically conditioned weaknesses in systems that support

fluency (language, motor, emotional, cognitive, etc.) interact with environmental factors (external and internal demands) to precipitate and maintain fluency failure

Consistent with recent advances in the genotype/phenotype, nature/nurture controversy

The model has intuitive appeal to many clinicians, but has been criticized as circular and untestable (see recent issue of JFD (2000) devoted entirely to this debate).

However, there is clear evidence of a series of “trading relationships” among language, motor coordination and fluency (Smith & colleagues; Ratner & colleagues) that provide some level of support for the model.

These will be explored more fully in a later lecture.

Page 49: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

DCM (continued) SUPPORTING DATA: Stuttering arises during the most dynamic

period of speech, language and fluency development, a time in which trade-offs are readily noted in children’s speech (Crystal, 1987, Ratner, 1997). Some environmental modifications to reduce parental speech rate and demand on children appear to facilitate fluency (but complicated by normal patterns of remission to be expected in 80% of CWS).

CONFLICTING DATA: No evidence that parents of CWS are more demanding than other parents (Bernstein Ratner & Silverman, 2000; Miles & Bernstein Ratner, 2001)

MISSING DATA: The notion that individual children may have capacity limitations that can be stressed by normal levels of demand essentially make the model untestable. Ingham & Cordes (1997) rightfully critique the current model as circular. Data to suggest that given parental behaviors affect fluency patterns in a systematic fashion might address this problem.

Page 50: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Multifactorial-Dynamic Model The symptoms of stuttering are like smoke coming out of

a volcano; they tell us little about the underlying processes that produce the phenomenon.

Stuttering evolves from essentially normal systems that interact poorly; Neural pathways are dynamic and self-learning; abnormal

patterns can self-perpetuate and become stabile. Problems integrating systems are apparent in PWS; linguistic

and cognitive demands impair their motor functioning more obviously than in PWDNS.

Similar to DeNil and colleagues’ Neurophysiological Model, in which learning of new skills is seen to change physiological markers of speech/language processing

Page 51: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Stuttering as an emotional or psychological problem Still a popular lay account, especially in other cultures Stuttering is still classified as a neurosis in the DSM-IV

(Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of the APA) Ex: repressed need hypothesis: stuttering is the result of a

repressed neurotic conflict Psycho-sexual (Glauber, Freud) or Inadequate interpersonal relationships (Barbara, Wyatt) Most thoroughly summarized by Travis (1957)

No evidence to support the theory (e.g., Yairi, 1997); no evidence to suggest that psychotherapy is effective in managing or curing stuttering (Andrews, et al., 1983).

Distinguishing cause from effect in psychological involvement in stuttering

What data would be required to support such models? What data exist to weaken them?

Page 52: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Specific theories: Stuttering as manifestation of repressed need: oral gratification (Coriat

(1931) Repressed desires to remain silent (Fenichel, 1945), or withhold

unspeakable thoughts (Barbara, 1954) Response to childhood stress and trauma (e.g., Wyatt, 1958)

SUPPORTING DATA: Early studies showing levels of personality disorder and maladjustment in PWS.

CONFLICTING DATA: Most, if not all, recent research. Genetics, motor data, linguistic data. Lack of effectiveness of psychotherapy in treating stuttering symptoms. Obvious difficulty in determining the directionality of any differences between personality attributes of people who stutter and comparison groups.

MISSING DATA: Genetic contributions of some personality traits. FOR BEST DISCUSSIONS, SEE BLOODSTEIN’S HANDBOOK, 1995.

Page 53: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Freud’s iceberg (compare to Sheehan’s)

Page 54: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Why teach about theories and models?

Discussion: How relevant are they

to treatment? Can good treatment

occur in the absence of a good model or theory?

What will you say when a client or parent asks (or tells you) what causes stuttering?

Page 55: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Besides…

It might be on the ASHA exam!

Page 56: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Evaluating research in stuttering

Using the internet to do research PubMed searches

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgiTry the tutorial:

http://www.nlm.nih.gov/bsd/pubmed_tutorial/m1001.html

Consider your terms carefully! Other good resources: EBSCO

Page 57: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?
Page 58: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?
Page 59: Models of stuttering Why develop a model of stuttering?

Department research on stuttering

Work in the department can be used to support neurospycholinguistic and multifactorial models of stuttering. Read some recent work, including our published articles, athttp://www.bsos.umd.edu/hesp/reRatner.htmlDo you have a research question you’d like to

ask? Come by, and let’s talk!