Milinkovitch Et Al, (2013)- Crocodile Head Scales Are Not Developmental Units but Emerge From...

download Milinkovitch Et Al, (2013)- Crocodile Head Scales Are Not Developmental Units but Emerge From Physical Cracking

of 5

Transcript of Milinkovitch Et Al, (2013)- Crocodile Head Scales Are Not Developmental Units but Emerge From...

  • 7/29/2019 Milinkovitch Et Al, (2013)- Crocodile Head Scales Are Not Developmental Units but Emerge From Physical Cracking

    1/5

    DOI: 10.1126/science.1226265, 78 (2013);339Science

    et al.Michel C. Milinkovitchfrom Physical CrackingCrocodile Head Scales Are Not Developmental Units But Emerge

    This copy is for your personal, non-commercial use only.

    clicking here.colleagues, clients, or customers by, you can order high-quality copies for yourIf you wish to distribute this article to others

    here.following the guidelines

    can be obtained byPermission to republish or repurpose articles or portions of articles

    ):January 5, 2013www.sciencemag.org (this information is current as of

    The following resources related to this article are available online at

    http://www.sciencemag.org/content/339/6115/78.full.htmlversion of this article at:

    including high-resolution figures, can be found in the onlineUpdated information and services,

    http://www.sciencemag.org/content/suppl/2012/11/29/science.1226265.DC2.htmlhttp://www.sciencemag.org/content/suppl/2012/11/29/science.1226265.DC1.htmlhttp://www.sciencemag.org/content/suppl/2012/12/07/science.1226265.DC3.html

    can be found at:Supporting Online Material

    http://www.sciencemag.org/content/339/6115/78.full.html#ref-list-1, 2 of which can be accessed free:cites 22 articlesThis article

    registered trademark of AAAS.is aScience2013 by the American Association for the Advancement of Science; all rights reserved. The title

    CopyrighAmerican Association for the Advancement of Science, 1200 New York Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005.(print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published weekly, except the last week in December, by thScience

    http://www.sciencemag.org/about/permissions.dtlhttp://www.sciencemag.org/about/permissions.dtlhttp://www.sciencemag.org/about/permissions.dtlhttp://www.sciencemag.org/about/permissions.dtlhttp://www.sciencemag.org/about/permissions.dtlhttp://www.sciencemag.org/about/permissions.dtlhttp://www.sciencemag.org/content/339/6115/78.full.htmlhttp://www.sciencemag.org/content/339/6115/78.full.htmlhttp://www.sciencemag.org/content/339/6115/78.full.htmlhttp://www.sciencemag.org/content/339/6115/78.full.html#ref-list-1http://www.sciencemag.org/content/339/6115/78.full.html#ref-list-1http://www.sciencemag.org/content/339/6115/78.full.html#ref-list-1http://www.sciencemag.org/content/339/6115/78.full.html#ref-list-1http://www.sciencemag.org/content/339/6115/78.full.html#ref-list-1http://www.sciencemag.org/content/339/6115/78.full.htmlhttp://www.sciencemag.org/about/permissions.dtlhttp://www.sciencemag.org/about/permissions.dtlhttp://oascentral.sciencemag.org/RealMedia/ads/click_lx.ads/sciencemag/cgi/reprint/L22/1580998653/Top1/AAAS/PDF-R-and-D-Systems-Science-121101/RandD-130104.raw/1?x
  • 7/29/2019 Milinkovitch Et Al, (2013)- Crocodile Head Scales Are Not Developmental Units but Emerge From Physical Cracking

    2/5

    Evolution, and Climate. B.G.H. also thanks the Marie CurieActions under the Seventh Framework Programme(PIEF-GA-2009-252888). M.B.A. also thanks theSpanish Research Council (CSIC) for support, and S.A.F.thanks the Landes-Offensive zur EntwicklungWissenschaftlich-konomischer Exzellenz program ofHesses Ministry of Higher Education, Research, and theArts. We thank L. Hansen for help with data and reference

    compilations. We thank the International Union for Conservationof Nature and Natural Resources for making the amphibianand mammal range data available. Data are archived athttp://macroecology.ku.dk/resources/wallace.

    Supplementary Materialswww.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/339/6115/74/DC1Materials and Methods

    Figs. S1 to S11Tables S1 to S5Appendices S1 and S2References (30729)

    1 August 2012; accepted 15 November 201210.1126/science.1228282

    Crocodile Head Scales Are NotDevelopmental Units But Emergefrom Physical CrackingMichel C. Milinkovitch,1* Liana Manukyan,1 Adrien Debry,1 Nicolas Di-Po,1 Samuel Martin,2

    Daljit Singh,3

    Dominique Lambert,4

    Matthias Zwicker3

    Various lineages of amniotes display keratinized skin appendages (feathers, hairs, and scales) thatdifferentiate in the embryo from genetically controlled developmental units whose spatialorganization is patterned by reaction-diffusion mechanisms (RDMs). We show that, contrary to skinappendages in other amniotes (as well as body scales in crocodiles), face and jaws scales of

    crocodiles are random polygonal domains of highly keratinized skin, rather than geneticallycontrolled elements, and emerge from a physical self-organizing stochastic process distinct fromRDMs: cracking of the developing skin in a stress field. We suggest that the rapid growth of thecrocodile embryonic facial and jaw skeleton, combined with the development of a very keratinizedskin, generates the mechanical stress that causes cracking.

    Amniotes exhibit a keratinized epidermis

    preventing water loss and skin append-

    ages that play major roles in thermoregu-

    lation, photoprotection, camouflage, behavioral

    display, and defense against predators. Whereas

    mammals and birds evolved hairs and feathers,

    respectively, reptiles developed various types of

    scales. Although their developmental processes

    share some signaling pathways, it is unclear

    whether mammalian hairs, avian feathers and

    feet scales, and reptilian scales are homologous

    or if some of them evolved convergently (1). In

    birds and mammals, a reaction-diffusion mech-

    anism (RDM) (2) generates a spatial pattern of

    placodes that develop and differentiate into fol-

    licular organs with a dermal papilla and cycling

    growth of an elongated keratinized epiderm

    structure (hairs or feathers) (3). However, scain reptiles do not form true follicles and mig

    not develop from placodes (4). Instead, reptil

    scales originate in the embryo from regular derm

    epidermal elevations (1). Whereas the regu

    spatial organization of scales on the largest p

    tion of thereptilian body is determined by a RD

    additional positional cues are likely involved

    the development of the scale plates present

    the head of many snakes and lizards. These he

    scales form a predictable symmetrical patte

    (Fig. 1A) and provide mechanical protection

    The face and jaws of crocodilians are cove

    by polygonalscales (hereafter called headscale

    that are strictly adjoining and nonoverlappibut these polygons are irregular and their spa

    distribution seems largely random (Fig. 1

    and C). Using high-resolution three-dimensio

    (3D) geometry and texture reconstructions (5

    1Laboratory of Artificial and Natural Evolution (LANE), Depmentof Genetics and Evolution,University of Geneva, ScieIII, 30, Quai Ernest-Ansermet, 1211 Geneva, Switzerland.Ferme aux Crocodiles, Pierrelatte, France. 3Computer GrapGroup, University of Bern, Switzerland. 4Department of Mematics and Namur Center for Complex Systems, UniversitNamur, Belgium.

    *To whom correspondence should be addressed. [email protected]

    Fig. 1. Spatial distribu-tion of head scales. (A)Head scalesinmost snakes(here, a corn snake) arepolygons (two upper pan-els) with stereotyped spa-tial distribution(twolowerpanels): left (yellow) andright (red) scale edgesoverlap when reflectedacross the sagittal plane(blue).(B) Polygonal headscales in crocodiles havea largely random spatialdistribution without sym-metrical correspondencebetween left and right.(C) Head scales from dif-ferent individuals havedifferent distributions ofscales sizes and localiza-tions (blue and red edgesfromtop andbottomcroc-odiles, respectively).

    4 JANUARY 2013 VOL 339 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org78

    REPORTS

  • 7/29/2019 Milinkovitch Et Al, (2013)- Crocodile Head Scales Are Not Developmental Units but Emerge From Physical Cracking

    3/5

    as well as developmental biology techniques,

    we show that crocodilians head scales are not

    genetically controlled developmental units and

    that their spatial patterning is generated through

    physical cracking of a living tissue in a stress

    field. This phenomenon might not involve any

    specific genetic instruction besides those asso-

    ciated with cell proliferation and general physical

    parameters such as skin stiffness and thickness.

    By marking and analyzing various features

    directly on 3D models of multiple Nile croco-dile (Crocodylus niloticus) individuals (Fig. 1 and

    movie S1), we show that spatial distribution of

    head scalesis largely random. First, reflection of

    the network of scales edges across the sagittal

    plane indicates high variability between the left

    and right head pattern (Fig. 1B and fig. S1A).

    Second, nonrigid alignment (8) of head geom-

    etries from different individuals indicates a

    similarly large variability in scale patterns in

    terms of polygons sizes and localizations (Fig. 1C

    and fig. S1B).

    This combination of order and chaos in the

    distribution of head scales is reminiscent of the

    topological assemblage of soap foams (9, 10).Recent studies used the 2D foam model for

    studying self-organizing principles and stochas-

    tic processes shaping epithelial topology dur-

    ing growth and homeostasis (1113) because the

    causal cell-surface mechanics is comparable to

    the physics of foam formation (14). Similarly,

    the pattern of crocodile head scales could result

    from energy minimization of contact surfaces

    among genetically determined elements (scales).

    However, two other mechanisms could gener-

    ate random distributions of polygonal elements:

    (i) a RDM patterning the spatial organization of

    genetically determined developmental units, as

    for mammalian hairs or avian feathers, and (ii)

    cracking of a material layer causing its fractureinto adjacent polygonal domains (15).

    Although stochastic patterns generated by

    these processes share some universal mathe-

    matical properties (see supplementary materials),

    foams and crack patterns are generated by very

    different physical phenomena that may be iden-

    tified on the basis of other statistical features.

    First, crocodile head scales do not show a good

    fit to the area distribution function expected for

    foams (fig. S3). Second, a fundamental differ-

    ence between foams and crack patterns is that the

    latter can exhibit incomplete edges (15), of which

    many are observed on the head of crocodiles

    (Fig. 2A).Another key feature is the angle among edges

    at nodes. In foams, edges are circular arcs in-

    tersecting only three at a time with an angle of

    120, as imposed by the three instantaneous

    equal length-tension force vectors acting a

    node. This rule is observed in all types of foam

    including animal epithelia (12, 16), althou

    the distribution of angles can be widened due

    local stress generated by cell division andgrow

    On the other hand, crack patterns can gen

    ate various angle distributions. Nonhierarchi

    cracking arises when fractures propagate sim

    taneously (Fig. 2B), and junctions tend to fo

    at 120 (17, 18). Furthermore, when a crack fr

    splits, or when multiple cracks are nucleatfrom a single point, the junctions among edg

    also tend to be 120. Conversely, crack patte

    can be hierarchical (17, 19); that is, fractures

    formed successively, and propagating cra

    will tend to join previous cracks at a 90 ang

    Indeed, the local stress perpendicular to a cra

    is relaxed and concentrates at the tip of the cra

    (explaining its propagation), but the stress co

    ponent parallel to the crack is not affected. Hen

    as cracks propagate perpendicularly to the

    rection of the maximum stress component

    secondary crack can turn when it approach

    an older one and tends to join it at 90. Sim

    larly, if a crack starts on the side of an oldcrack, it will initially tend to propagate a

    right angle (17). Multiple examples of 90 co

    nections and incomplete edges reorienting th

    propagation front are visible on the crocodi

    face and jaws (Fig. 2C). We also observe l

    dering patterns (17) of paired parallel prim

    fractures with perpendicular multiple second

    cracks (Fig. 2D) and internal edges connecti

    perpendicularly to the border of the netwo

    (Fig. 2E). The distribution function of edge a

    gles is bimodal in many crocodiles analyz

    (fig. S4A), suggesting either that hierarchi

    and nonhierarchical cracking processes coex

    or that head scale networks undergo a

    marationprocess (2022) (see supplementary te

    Dome pressure receptors (DPRs) are p

    mented round submillimetric sensory orga

    (Fig. 2F), distributed on the crocodile face a

    jaws, that detect surface pressure waves, allo

    ing crocodiles to swiftly orient, even in darkne

    toward a prey perturbing the water-air interfa

    (23). The dome shape of DPRs is due to a mo

    fied epidermis and the presence of a pocket

    various cell types in the outermost portion

    the dermis (Fig. 2F). We marked the localiz

    tion of DPRs on the 3D models of all scann

    individuals (orange dots, Fig. 2, G and H). Ma

    of the cracks that have stopped their course

    so close to a DPR (Fig. 2G and fig. S4C). Giv

    that the most frequent cause for fracture arr

    is when the crack front meets a heterogeneity

    the system (15), it is likely that the modifi

    skin thickness and composition at and arou

    DPRs constitute such heterogeneities. In

    dition, the course of many edges avoids DP

    (Fig. 2H and fig. S4C).

    The overall distribution of DPRs seems rat

    homogeneous except where the density is

    creased near the teeth and decreased at the ba

    of the jaws and on the top of the face (fig. S

    Fig. 2. Signatures of cracking. (A) Many scale edges on crocodiles head areunconnected at one or both ends.(B) Three incomplete cracks interact symmetrically. (C) Edges reorienting (arrows) and connecting with anglesclose to 90. (D) Laddering between parallel primary cracks. (E) 90 network border connections. (F) DPRsare pigmented sensory organs (left) with a modified epidermis (right,section at embryonicstage E70, that is,at70 days of egg incubation) and a pocket of branched nerves (white arrowheads). (G) Incomplete cracksstopping in close proximity to a DPR (orange dots). (H) Crack propagation avoids DPRs.

    www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 339 4 JANUARY 2013

    REP

  • 7/29/2019 Milinkovitch Et Al, (2013)- Crocodile Head Scales Are Not Developmental Units but Emerge From Physical Cracking

    4/5

    Different crocodile individuals differ by as much

    as 21% and 48% in their total number of DPRs

    and crack edges, respectively. Remarkably, these

    two interindividual variations are inversely cor-

    related: Crocodiles with fewer DPRs have more

    crack edges (fig. S4D). Given that the develop-

    ment of DPRs precedes cracking, this correla-

    tion suggests that DPRs constrain cracking, as

    already implied by Fig. 2, G and H. Despite the

    fact that the distributions of cracks and DPRs

    both have a strong stochastic component, theconstraining effect of DPRs on cracking is no-

    ticeable: The edges tend to travel along the zones

    of DPRs lowest local density (fig. S4E).

    The archetypal cracking process in physics

    is due to shrinkage [through removal of a dif-

    fusing quantity, either heat or a liquid (20)] of a

    material layer adherent to a nonshrinking sub-

    strate (15, 17), such that a stress field builds up

    and causes fractures when the stress exceeds a

    threshold characteristic of the material. Croc-

    odiles have a particularly thick and rigid skin

    due to the presence of a highly collagenous

    dermis and an epidermis rich in b-keratins (24).

    The skin covering their head shows a yet thicker(about 2) and more keratinized epidermis. We

    suggest that the rapid growth of the crocodile

    embryonic facial and jaw skeleton (relative to the

    size of the neurocranium), combined with the

    development of a very keratinized skin, gener-

    ates the mechanical stress that causes cracking.

    Here, it is not the cracking layer that shrinks

    but the underlying substrate layer that grows. It

    explains that first-order cracks (fig. S6) tend to

    traverse the width of the face because the head

    is growing longitudinally faster than in other

    directions.

    In snakes and lizards, scales are develop-

    mental units: Each scale differentiates and growsfrom a primordium that can be identified by in

    situ hybridization with probes targeting genes

    belonging to signaling pathways involved in

    early skin appendage development (1, 4). The

    large head scales form a predictable pattern

    following positional cues, such that the identity

    of adult snake head scales can be recognized

    while they develop from primordia in the em-

    bryo (Fig. 3A). In crocodiles, all postcranial

    scales follow that same principle of develop-

    ment (Fig. 3B): Spatial distribution of primordia

    is established, then each primordium differen-

    tiates, first into a symmetrical elevation and sec-

    ond as an oriented asymmetrical scale overlapping

    with more posterior scales (Fig. 3C).

    However, crocodile head scales do not form

    from scale primordia or further developmental

    stages. Instead, a pattern of DPRs primordia is

    generated on the face and jaws: The dome shape

    of DPRs has already started to form before

    any scale appears (Fig. 3D). Afterward, grooves

    progressively appear, propagate, and intercon-

    nect (while avoiding DPRs) to form a continuous

    network across the developing skin (Fig. 4A).

    The process generates polygonal domains of

    skin, each containing a random number of DPRs.

    Therefore, scales on the face and jaws of croc-

    odiles (i) are not serial homologs of scales else-

    where on thebody and (ii) are not even genetically

    controlleddevelopmental units. Instead, they emerge

    from physical cracking.

    During a typical cracking process, fractures

    are nucleated at the upper surface but quickly

    spread downward and affect the whole thick-

    ness of the material layer (19). The developing

    skin on the crocodiles head similarly reacts to

    the stress field as it develops deep groves that

    can reach the stiff underlying tissues (Fig. 4B).

    Our analyses indicate that cell proliferation in

    the epidermis layer is vastly increased in the

    deepest region of the skin grooves correspond-

    ing to cracks (Fig. 4C), suggesting that a heal-

    ing process allows the skin layer to maintain its

    continuous covering. The local biological pro-

    cess (cell proliferation) might be driven by the

    purely physical parameter (mechanical stress) as

    follows: In zones of highest stress, local bulging

    is nucleated. The local stress component p

    pendicular to the bulge is relaxed and conc

    trates at its tip, explaining the propagation

    both the stress and proliferation maxima (hen

    the corresponding propagation of the bulge).

    a manner entirely analogous to true physi

    cracking, the bulge front would propagate p

    pendicularly to the direction of maximum str

    components, explaining the topology of the

    sulting random polygonal domains of skin. T

    role of proliferation reinforces the above sugg

    tion that crack patterns in crocodiles might exp

    rience maturation(2022), explaining the observ

    mixture of hierarchical and nonhierarchical f

    tures (see supplementary text and fig. S4A).

    We have shown that the irregular polygon

    domains of skin on the crocodile face and ja

    are produced by cracking, a mechanism tha

    distinct from those generating scales on

    postcranial portion of the crocodile body,

    well as on the body and head of all other re

    Fig. 3. Crocodile head scales are not developmental units. (A) In snakes, each body scale (ventrallatero-dorsal, ld) differentiates from a primordium (Shh gene probe for in situ hybridization, cosnake embryo); head scales also develop from primordia, with positional cues determining scidentity (la, labial scales; r, rostral; n, nasal; in, internasal; pf, prefrontal; pro, preocular; so, supraoculpto, postocular). (B) Postcranial scales (zoom on trunk, Ctnnb1 probe) also develop from primordia t

    (C) differentiate into symmetrical, then orientedasymmetrical and overlapping, scales. (D) Crocodile hescales never form scale primordia [nor developmental stages shown in (C)] but, instead, developattern of DPRs (one DPR circled with dotted line; dome shape visible at E45) before any scaappears (probe: Ctnnb1).

    4 JANUARY 2013 VOL 339 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org80

    REPORTS

  • 7/29/2019 Milinkovitch Et Al, (2013)- Crocodile Head Scales Are Not Developmental Units but Emerge From Physical Cracking

    5/5

    tiles. This cracking process is primarily physical.

    However, it does not mean that genetically con-

    trolled parameters are irrelevant. For example,

    although a crack pattern is visible in all croc-

    odilian species, spatial distribution varies consid-

    erably, possibly because of species-specific skull

    geometry and growth but also skin composition

    and thickness. Given that these parameters, as

    well as cell proliferation, are genetically con-

    trolled, the variation of head crack patterns among

    crocodilian species is likely due to an interplay

    between physically and genetically controlled

    param eters . Our stud y suggests that, besides

    RDM, a larger set of physical self-organizational

    processes contribute to the production of the

    enormous diversity of patterns observed in living

    systems.

    References and Notes1. C. Chang et al., Int. J. Dev. Biol. 53, 813 (2009).2. A. M. Turing, Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London Ser. B 237, 37

    (1952).3. M. R. Schneider, R. Schmidt-Ullrich, R. Paus, Curr. Biol.

    19, R132 (2009).4. D. Dhouailly, J. Anat. 214, 587 (2009).5. K. N. Snavely, S. M. Seitz, R. Szeliski, ACM Transactions

    on Graphics (Proc. SIGGRAPH 2006) 25, 835 (2006).

    6. Y. Furukawa, J. Ponce, Int. J. Comput. Vis. 84, 257(2009).

    7. M. Kazhdan, M. Bolitho, H. Hoppe, EurographicsSymposium on Geometry Processing 2006, 61(2006).

    8. H. Li, B. Adams, L. J. Guibas, M. Pauly, ACM Transactionson Graphics (Proc. SIGGRAPH Asia 2009) 28,175 (2009).

    9. D. Weaire, S. Hutzler, The Physics of Foams (Oxford Univ.Press, Oxford, 1999).

    10. D. Weaire, N. Rivier, Contemp. Phys. 25, 59 (1984).11. T. Lecuit, P. F. Lenne, Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 8, 633

    (2007).12. M. C. Gibson, A. B. Patel, R. Nagpal, N. Perrimon, Nature

    442, 1038 (2006).13. D. A. W. Thompson, On Growth and Form (Cambridge

    Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1917).14. R. Farhadifar, J. C. Rper, B. Aigouy, S. Eaton, F. Jlicher,

    Curr. Biol. 17, 2095 (2007).15. H. J. Herrmann, S. P. Roux, Eds. Statistical Models for the

    Fracture of Disordered Media: Random Materials and

    Processes (North-Holland, Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1990).16. T. Hayashi, R. W. Carthew, Nature 431, 647 (2004).17. K. A. Shorlin, J. R. de Bruyn, M. Graham, S. W. Morris,

    Phys. Rev. E Stat. Phys. Plasmas Fluids Relat. Interdiscip.

    Topics 61, 6950 (2000).18. E. A. Jagla, A. G. Rojo, Phys. Rev. E Stat. Nonlin. Soft

    Matter Phys. 65, 026203 (2002).19. S. Bohn, J. Platkiewicz, B. Andreotti, M. Adda-Bedia,

    Y. Couder, Phys. Rev. E Stat. Nonlin. Soft Matter Phys.71, 046215 (2005).

    20. L. Goehring, L. Mahadevan, S. W. Morris, Proc. Natl.Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 387 (2009).

    21. L. Goehring, S. W. Morris, Europhys. Lett. 69, 739(2005).

    22. L. Goehring, R. Conroy, A. Akhter, W. J. Clegg,A. F. Routh, Soft Matter 6, 3562 (2010).

    23. D. Soares, Nature 417, 241 (2002).24. L. Alibardi, L. W. Knapp, R. H. Sawyer, J. Submicrosc

    Cytol. Pathol. 38, 175 (2006).

    Acknowledgments: This work was supported by the Univeof Geneva, the Swiss National Science Foundation, and thSchmidheiny Foundation. A. Tzika helped with in situs. H.assisted with nonrigid registration. We thank R. Pellet forassistance in mechanics design and A. Roux, M. Gonzalez-GaiB. Chopard, U. Schibler, and anonymous reviewers foruseful comments and suggestions.

    Supplementary Materialswww.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/science.1226265/DC1Material and MethodsSupplementary TextFigs. S1 to S6Table S1Movie S1References (2533)

    18 June 2012; accepted 29 October 2012Published online 29 November 2012;10.1126/science.1226265

    Fig. 4. Crocodile headskin cracks during devel-opment. (A) There is nosign of cracking at E45(but DPRs primordia arealready developed, Fig.3D), then primary cracks(arrowheads) appear onthe sides of the upperjaws and progress toward

    the top of the face (dottedline).AtE65, primary cracksreached the top of thehead and are followedby secondary cracks inother orientations (ar-rows). (B) Three sequen-tial skin sections alongprimary (pc) and second-ary (sc) cracks (ep, epi-dermis; de, dermis; bo,bone tissue). (C) Antibodyto pan cadherin stains thewholeepidermis, antibodyto proliferating cell nucle-

    arantigen(PCNA)indicatesincreased proliferation (ar-rows), and terminal deox-ynucleotidyl transferasemediated deoxyuridinetriphosphate nick end la-beling (TUNEL) assay indi-cates absence of apoptosisin cracks.

    www sciencemag org SCIENCE VOL 339 4 JANUARY 2013

    REP