Methodological Issues in Interviewing and Using Self ... · INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL...

17
Psychological Assessment 2001, Vol. 13, No. 3,319-335 Copyright 2001 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1040-3590/01/S5.00 DOI: 10.1037//1040-3590.13.3.319 Methodological Issues in Interviewing and Using Self-Report Questionnaires With People With Mental Retardation W. M. L. Finlay and E. Lyons University of Surrey In this article the authors review methodological issues that arise when interviews and self-report questionnaires are used with people with mental retardation and offer suggestions for overcoming some of the difficulties described. Examples are drawn from studies that use qualitative methodology, quantitative studies assessing different question types, and studies reporting on the development of instruments measuring psychiatric symptoms, self-concept, and quality of life. Specific problems that arise with respect to item content (e.g., quantitative judgments, generalizations), question phrasing (e.g., modifiers), response format (e.g., acquiescence, multiple-choice questions), and psychometric properties (factor structure and validity) are discussed. It is argued that because many self-report questionnaires include questions that have been found to be problematic in this population, more attention needs to be paid to establishing the validity of such measures and to clearly defining the population for which the instrument is designed. The importance of obtaining information from people with mental retardation 1 is increasingly stressed by researchers, clini- cians, and those involved in service development. This is a result of the limitations of informant reports for many psychological phenomena (such as subjective states), as well as a change in the philosophy of services toward user consultation and a greater emphasis on people with mental retardation making decisions about their own lives. Structured or semistructured interviews are used in the formulation stage of many psychological therapies, in the assessment of abilities, IQ, and neuropsychology, as well as in forensic, research, and individual planning contexts. Verbal self- report questionnaires are used in clinical assessment (e.g., to assess psychopathology, self-esteem, personality), research, and service evaluation. However, difficulties in conducting interviews and using self-report questionnaires with this population are widely reported. Such difficulties have been described in psychiatric and psychological assessments (e.g., Coelho & Saunders, 1996; Duck- worth, Radhakrishnan, Nolan, & Fraser, 1993; Fraser, Leudar, Gray, & Campbell, 1986; Rojahn, Warren, & Ohringer, 1994; Scott, 1994; Sovner & Hurley, 1986; Zetlin, Heriot, & Turner, 1985), police interviews (Clare & Gudjonsson, 1995; Tully & Cahill, 1984), service evaluations (e.g., Antaki & Rapley, 1996; Dagnan, Dennis, & Wood, 1994; Flynn, 1986; McVilly, 1995; Smyley & Ellsworth, 1997), vocational assessment (Wilgosh & Barry, 1983), and the assessment of service needs and choices (e.g., Gow & Balla, 1994; Lindsey, 1994). The problems of communication associated with mental retar- dation are well documented (e.g., Abbeduto & Hagerman, 1997; Bedrosian, 1993; Beveridge, Conti-Ramsden, & Leudar, 1997; W. M. L. Finlay and E. Lyons, School of Human Sciences, Department of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, England. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to W. M. L. Finlay, School of Human Sciences, Department of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, GU2 5XH, England, United Kingdom. Brinton & Fujiki, 1993, 1994; McLean, Brady, & McLean, 1996; Sabsay & Kernan, 1983). These include difficulties in the produc- tion of symbols, lack of understanding of complex grammatical structures or concepts, and difficulties in designing utterances for the specific, current interactional context (e.g., the inclusion of appropriate content). As a result of these difficulties, interviewers must pay particular attention to providing statements or questions to which the person with mental retardation can respond and to addressing problems in understanding or classifying the responses given. In many cases, the use of questionnaires developed for the general population is inappropriate for people with mental retar- dation because of the respondents' inability to comprehend the question and express an answer clearly and because the psycho- metric properties may not be applicable to this population. For this reason, many new self-report questionnaires for adults with mental retardation have been developed assessing psychiatric symptoms (e.g., Kazdin, Matson, & Senatore, 1983; Lindsay & Michie, 1988; Moss et al., 1993; Reynolds & Baker, 1988; Strohmer, Prout, & Gorsky, 1994), self-concept (e.g., Szivos-Bach, 1993), quality of life (e.g., Barlow & Kirby, 1991; Heal & Chadsey-Rusch, 1985; Schalock, Keith, Hoffman, & Karan, 1989), as well as a range of other constructs. These include community living (Botswick & Foss, 1981), loneliness (Chadsey-Rusch, DeStefano, O'Reilly, Gonzalez, & Collet-Klingenberg, 1992), knowledge of rights 1 The authors recognize that the term people with mental retardation is objectionable to many people and use it here only for reasons of clarity for an international audience. The diagnostic criteria for mental retardation from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition, include IQ scores of below 70 together with some impairment of adaptive functioning (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). In some countries outside the United States, this term is not used, and the same population is referred to as people with learning disabilities (United Kingdom) or people with intellectual disabilities (Australia and New Zealand). 319 This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Transcript of Methodological Issues in Interviewing and Using Self ... · INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL...

Page 1: Methodological Issues in Interviewing and Using Self ... · INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 321 Socially Reflexive Questions Questions asking people how they think others

Psychological Assessment2001, Vol. 13, No. 3,319-335

Copyright 2001 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.1040-3590/01/S5.00 DOI: 10.1037//1040-3590.13.3.319

Methodological Issues in Interviewing and Using Self-ReportQuestionnaires With People With Mental Retardation

W. M. L. Finlay and E. LyonsUniversity of Surrey

In this article the authors review methodological issues that arise when interviews and self-reportquestionnaires are used with people with mental retardation and offer suggestions for overcoming someof the difficulties described. Examples are drawn from studies that use qualitative methodology,quantitative studies assessing different question types, and studies reporting on the development ofinstruments measuring psychiatric symptoms, self-concept, and quality of life. Specific problems thatarise with respect to item content (e.g., quantitative judgments, generalizations), question phrasing (e.g.,modifiers), response format (e.g., acquiescence, multiple-choice questions), and psychometric properties(factor structure and validity) are discussed. It is argued that because many self-report questionnairesinclude questions that have been found to be problematic in this population, more attention needs to bepaid to establishing the validity of such measures and to clearly defining the population for which theinstrument is designed.

The importance of obtaining information from people withmental retardation1 is increasingly stressed by researchers, clini-cians, and those involved in service development. This is a resultof the limitations of informant reports for many psychologicalphenomena (such as subjective states), as well as a change in thephilosophy of services toward user consultation and a greateremphasis on people with mental retardation making decisionsabout their own lives. Structured or semistructured interviews areused in the formulation stage of many psychological therapies, inthe assessment of abilities, IQ, and neuropsychology, as well as inforensic, research, and individual planning contexts. Verbal self-report questionnaires are used in clinical assessment (e.g., to assesspsychopathology, self-esteem, personality), research, and serviceevaluation. However, difficulties in conducting interviews andusing self-report questionnaires with this population are widelyreported. Such difficulties have been described in psychiatric andpsychological assessments (e.g., Coelho & Saunders, 1996; Duck-worth, Radhakrishnan, Nolan, & Fraser, 1993; Fraser, Leudar,Gray, & Campbell, 1986; Rojahn, Warren, & Ohringer, 1994;Scott, 1994; Sovner & Hurley, 1986; Zetlin, Heriot, & Turner,1985), police interviews (Clare & Gudjonsson, 1995; Tully &Cahill, 1984), service evaluations (e.g., Antaki & Rapley, 1996;Dagnan, Dennis, & Wood, 1994; Flynn, 1986; McVilly, 1995;Smyley & Ellsworth, 1997), vocational assessment (Wilgosh &Barry, 1983), and the assessment of service needs and choices(e.g., Gow & Balla, 1994; Lindsey, 1994).

The problems of communication associated with mental retar-dation are well documented (e.g., Abbeduto & Hagerman, 1997;Bedrosian, 1993; Beveridge, Conti-Ramsden, & Leudar, 1997;

W. M. L. Finlay and E. Lyons, School of Human Sciences, Departmentof Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, England.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to W. M. L.Finlay, School of Human Sciences, Department of Psychology, Universityof Surrey, Guildford, GU2 5XH, England, United Kingdom.

Brinton & Fujiki, 1993, 1994; McLean, Brady, & McLean, 1996;Sabsay & Kernan, 1983). These include difficulties in the produc-tion of symbols, lack of understanding of complex grammaticalstructures or concepts, and difficulties in designing utterances forthe specific, current interactional context (e.g., the inclusion ofappropriate content). As a result of these difficulties, interviewersmust pay particular attention to providing statements or questionsto which the person with mental retardation can respond and toaddressing problems in understanding or classifying the responsesgiven.

In many cases, the use of questionnaires developed for thegeneral population is inappropriate for people with mental retar-dation because of the respondents' inability to comprehend thequestion and express an answer clearly and because the psycho-metric properties may not be applicable to this population. For thisreason, many new self-report questionnaires for adults with mentalretardation have been developed assessing psychiatric symptoms(e.g., Kazdin, Matson, & Senatore, 1983; Lindsay & Michie, 1988;Moss et al., 1993; Reynolds & Baker, 1988; Strohmer, Prout, &Gorsky, 1994), self-concept (e.g., Szivos-Bach, 1993), quality oflife (e.g., Barlow & Kirby, 1991; Heal & Chadsey-Rusch, 1985;Schalock, Keith, Hoffman, & Karan, 1989), as well as a range ofother constructs. These include community living (Botswick &Foss, 1981), loneliness (Chadsey-Rusch, DeStefano, O'Reilly,Gonzalez, & Collet-Klingenberg, 1992), knowledge of rights

1 The authors recognize that the term people with mental retardation isobjectionable to many people and use it here only for reasons of clarity foran international audience. The diagnostic criteria for mental retardationfrom the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4thedition, include IQ scores of below 70 together with some impairment ofadaptive functioning (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). In somecountries outside the United States, this term is not used, and the samepopulation is referred to as people with learning disabilities (UnitedKingdom) or people with intellectual disabilities (Australia and NewZealand).

319

This

doc

umen

t is c

opyr

ight

ed b

y th

e A

mer

ican

Psy

chol

ogic

al A

ssoc

iatio

n or

one

of i

ts a

llied

pub

lishe

rs.

This

arti

cle

is in

tend

ed so

lely

for t

he p

erso

nal u

se o

f the

indi

vidu

al u

ser a

nd is

not

to b

e di

ssem

inat

ed b

road

ly.

Page 2: Methodological Issues in Interviewing and Using Self ... · INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 321 Socially Reflexive Questions Questions asking people how they think others

320 FINLAY AND LYONS

(Flynn, Reeves, Whelan, & Speake, 1985), individual choice(Stancliffe, 1995), stigma, and social support (Reiss & Benson,1985).

Despite the widespread use of self-report instruments with thispopulation, however, the validity of many questionnaires has notbeen established. There are a number of difficulties associated withasking questions of people with mental retardation that should leadto particular concerns about validity. The following sections de-scribe problems with question content, phrasing, response format,and the psychometric properties of self-report questionnaires. Be-cause self-report questionnaires are usually administered orallyrather than in written form in this population, issues describedbelow concerning the use of interviews in general are also appli-cable to the design and use of questionnaires. Although peoplewith mental retardation are a heterogeneous population, there are,unfortunately, no adequate data to specify the subgroups for whicheach problem is particularly important. Although techniques forovercoming problems that have been suggested in the literature aredescribed wherever possible, in many cases there are no readysolutions, and more research is needed into strategies to improvequestioning in this population.

Question Content

It is widely acknowledged that vocabulary and meaning shouldbe clear and simple (e.g., Lowe & de Paiva, 1988; Prosser &Bromley, 1998). The person may be unfamiliar with the constructsand vocabulary that the interviewer uses or may be unable orunaccustomed to making the type of judgments requested. Thefollowing sections describe difficulties that arise when questionsconcern quantitative judgments, comparisons, abstract concepts,inferences about the attitudes of others to the self, generalizations,and unfamiliar or sensitive content. Problems of content can befound in questionnaires assessing mental states, frequencies ofevents and symptoms, social judgments, and abstract conceptssuch as autonomy.

Quantitative Judgments and Time Questions

Questions that are about time or that require a judgment offrequency or degree have been found to be problematic for manypeople with mental retardation (Biklen & Moseley, 1988; Booth &Booth, 1994a, 1994b, 1996; Flynn, 1986; Lindsay &Michie, 1988;Malik, Ashton-Shaeffer, & Kleiber, 1991; Matson & Frame, 1986;Moss et al., 1997; Wyngaarden, 1981). For example, Sigelman,Schoenrock, et al. (1981) found that in samples of institutionalizedand community-living adults and children with severe to mildmental retardation, multiple-choice questions requiring a quantita-tive judgment produced low levels of agreement with observersand with alternative question formats. This calls into question theuse of Likert-type questions to assess degree or frequency, such asin the modified versions of the Zung Self-Rating Depression Scale(ZDS) and the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Helsel & Matson,1988; Kazdin et al., 1983; Matson, Kazdin, & Senatore, 1984;Senatore, Matson, & Kazdin, 1985). The modified ZDS, for ex-ample, requires the respondent to make a frequency estimate forcertain emotional states, choosing among the following: none or alittle of the time, some of the time, a good part of the time, andmost of the time. A visual aid (bar graph) is often given to facilitate

such judgments, although the efficacy of this method remains to bedemonstrated. Although it may enable the range of options to beremembered more easily, it is unlikely to overcome the difficultiesthat people may have with making the estimate itself.

Screening questions concerning concrete activities whose fre-quency has been established from informants might be used inpretest sessions to ascertain the ability of individuals to make suchjudgments. In some cases, it may be necessary to use simpleyes-no questions instead. Lindsay and Michie (1988), in a sampleof 29 people with mild and moderate mental retardation, foundyes-no questions to produce greater split-half reliability (r = .69)than 4-point estimates of frequency (r = .12) in the assessment ofanxiety. Because of the importance of frequency estimates in theassessment of many characteristics, further research needs to becarried out to determine the effectiveness of visual aids and todevelop other methods for eliciting such estimates.

To facilitate estimations of time, significant events in the par-ticipants' lives have been used. In a longitudinal, interview-basedstudy of the experiences of 20 parents who had mental retardation,Booth and Booth (1994b) used situational markers rather thanasking for specific dates or time periods. When the dates of certainevents were known (e.g., a child's age, the place the person wasliving at the time), the timing of other events could be determinedwith respect to them. Similarly, the self-report Psychiatric Assess-ment Schedule for Adults with Developmental Disability (PAS-ADD; Moss et al., 1993; Patel, Goldberg, & Moss, 1993) requiresthe interviewer to identify (from informants) a significant event inthe person's life that has occurred about 1 month before theinterview. These events are used in the self-report interview toassess the respondent's concept of time and as a marker to gaugethe recency of any symptoms.

Direct Comparisons

Questions concerning direct comparisons have been found to bedifficult (Biklen & Moseley, 1988; Heal & Sigelman, 1995; Mat-son & Frame, 1986; Smyley & Ellsworth, 1997). Comparisons areoften requested in interviews assessing changes in people's lives,their preferences, symptom severity (e.g., Matson et al., 1984), andin social comparison questionnaires themselves (e.g., Dagnan &Sandhu, 1999). For example, the modified ZDS contains a range ofitems asking the respondent to compare their current emotions orbehavior with the past (e.g., "You can think as good as you usedto think"; Helsel & Matson, 1988).

If comparisons are difficult for respondents, it may be better toask about one element at a time (Biklen & Moseley, 1988; Cat-termole, Jahoda, & Markova, 1990; Heal & Sigelman, 1985). Forexample, Smyley and Ellsworth (1997), in their study of satisfac-tion with day services, asked first about the participants' old daycenter and then about the new one. The results were contentanalyzed and compared to infer positive, negative, or neutralcomparisons. When individuals are able to make direct compari-sons and there are a number of items that are to be ranked,dichotomous choices can be given, in which each item is pairedwith every other item (e.g., Botswick & Foss, 1981; Dagnan et al.,1994). However, respondents may become bored and may ques-tion why they are responding repeatedly to similar.questions.

This

doc

umen

t is c

opyr

ight

ed b

y th

e A

mer

ican

Psy

chol

ogic

al A

ssoc

iatio

n or

one

of i

ts a

llied

pub

lishe

rs.

This

arti

cle

is in

tend

ed so

lely

for t

he p

erso

nal u

se o

f the

indi

vidu

al u

ser a

nd is

not

to b

e di

ssem

inat

ed b

road

ly.

Page 3: Methodological Issues in Interviewing and Using Self ... · INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 321 Socially Reflexive Questions Questions asking people how they think others

INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 321

Socially Reflexive Questions

Questions asking people how they think others view or evaluatethem are difficult for many people (Szivos-Bach, 1993). Suchquestions involve a complex level of social understanding becausethey require the informant to infer the internal emotions, attitudes,or beliefs of another person from their behavior. Informants maybe unaccustomed or unable to make such judgments. Sociallyreflexive items are often found in questionnaires assessing self-esteem and depression (e.g., Matson et al., 1984; Reynolds &Baker, 1988) and perceptions of social situations (e.g., stigma—Reiss & Benson, 1985; quality of life—Schalock et al., 1989).Examples from the Psychopathology Instrument for Mentally Re-tarded Adults (PIMRA—Helsel & Matson, 1988; Kazdin et al.,1983) include, "Do people have trouble understanding what yousay?", "Do people know how you feel?", and "Do people like to bewith you?"2 Simple yes-no answers to such questions may dis-guise a guess or an incorrect understanding of the question.

Abstract Concepts and Generalized Judgments

Researchers often recommend the use of questions that refer tospecific activities or events rather than those referring to abstractconcepts because these are more easily understood and evaluated(e.g., Booth & Booth, 1994; Smyley & Elsworth, 1997). Problemsmay result from difficulties in aggregating specific instances tomake a general evaluation, from a lack of expressive abilities, orfrom a lack of understanding of certain concepts. For example, ina sample of 45 defendants with mental retardation (mean IQ = 61,SD = 6.18), Smith (1993) found that 7 were unable to understandthe term guilty, and 9 did not understand not guilty.

Questions about emotions have been found to be harder toanswer than those about concrete situations for some people withmental retardation (Booth & Booth, 1994b; Lowe & de Paiva,1988; Malik et al., 1991; McVilly, 1995; Sigelman, Winer, &Schoenrock, 1982). For this reason, Voelker et al. (1990) sug-gested that response biases may be less frequent for self-reportedadaptive behavior questionnaires than for those assessing psycho-pathology because the former concerns concrete behaviors ratherthan less easily defined internal states. In a study using a modifiedversion of the Clinical Interview Schedule with 27 hospital resi-dents, Ballinger, Armstrong, Presly, and Reid (1975) found thatconcepts relating to obsessions, compulsions, and depersonaliza-tion were rarely understood by participants. Questions about psy-chiatric symptoms have also been found to be problematic becauseof the inability of some respondents to describe such experiences.For example, in the development of the PAS-ADD, with a sampleof 98 patients who were in contact with a psychiatrist (meanIQ = 37.6, SD = 9.4), Moss, Prosser, and Goldberg (1996) foundthat although auditory hallucinations were the most easily re-corded, few of the participants diagnosed with schizophrenia wereable to give a clear enough account of thought disorder to allowscoring.

Questions using terms relating to mental retardation have alsobeen found to be problematic for some people. Lyall, Holland,Collins, and Styles (1995) reported on the use of a four-questionscreening test for people admitted to custody that was given todetermine whether they had mental retardation. In a sample of 12people with mental retardation taken into custody, 8 did not report

difficulties reading and writing, and 2 did not report having at-tended a special-needs school. Other studies have found that peo-ple may not accept such labels as personally applicable and maydefine it in differing ways (e.g., Biklen & Moseley, 1988; Davies& Jenkins, 1997; Finlay & Lyons, 1998; Simons, 1992; Todd &Shearn, 1997). Any questions using official labels for this popu-lation should therefore be followed with further questions check-ing the person's understanding of the term.

Questions that require the respondent to indicate what 'usually'happens or how they feel 'in general' have also been found to bedifficult for people with mental retardation. In an in-depth analysisof interview transcripts in which two self-concept questionnaireswere administered to 46 adults with mild to severe mental retar-dation (mean IQ = 51, SD = 10) employed in a sheltered work-shop, Zetlin et al. (1985) found that most of the 35% of initialanswers judged to be problematic took the form of specific, singleexamples of when the event occurred or the person felt that way.It was often difficult to establish whether such experiences werefrequent occurrences—the answers never reached the level ofgeneralization required by the question. Antaki (1998), using ex-tracts from quality-of-life interviews, demonstrated how interview-ers anticipated this difficulty and, in their efforts to paraphrase,restricted the meaning of general questions to specific occasions oractivities. Antaki found that word-for-word delivery of questionsas written was rare and that such paraphrasing was the norm.

Concepts are also problematic when the respondent interpretsthe terms with either a more general or a more restricted andcontext-specific definition than the researcher. This is a problemwith any population but may be particularly acute in populations inwhich individuals' life situations are very different from that of theinterviewer. In these cases, even terms that seem to have clearreferents may be interpreted differently. For example, Barlow andKirby (1991), in a study of the life satisfaction of 31 adults withmild mental retardation living either in the community or in aninstitution, found that participants frequently interpreted the termfriends in a more general way than the researchers, includingacquaintances who acted in a friendly manner to them.

Differences in reference might also occur because of differencesin the comparative context in which a question is framed. Stan-cliffe (1995) carried out a study with 46 people with severe toborderline mental retardation who were clients of supported ac-commodation agencies in which the amount of choice that theparticipants reported over various aspects of their lives was com-pared with the amount of choice that the staff reported. Reversewordings were used ("Do you choose . . . ?" and "Does someoneelse choose . . . ?") with 4-point Likert scales. After contradictionswere removed, it was found that clients' self-ratings were signif-icantly greater (i.e., more choice) than staff self-ratings for 3 ofthe 10 items. These concerned spending (Ms = 3.79 and 3.48,respectively), living companions (Ms = 3.40 and 2.60, respect-ively), and employment (Ms = 3.30 and 2.33, respectively). Stan-cliffe explained this as being due to the two groups using adifferent set of social comparison targets and due to the clientshaving a limited experience of the full range of possible choices.

From the Psychopathology Instrument for Mentally Retarded Adults,by J. L. Matson. Copyright 1988, 1994, 1996 by IDS Publishing Corpo-ration, Worthington, OH. All rights reserved. Reprinted with permission.

This

doc

umen

t is c

opyr

ight

ed b

y th

e A

mer

ican

Psy

chol

ogic

al A

ssoc

iatio

n or

one

of i

ts a

llied

pub

lishe

rs.

This

arti

cle

is in

tend

ed so

lely

for t

he p

erso

nal u

se o

f the

indi

vidu

al u

ser a

nd is

not

to b

e di

ssem

inat

ed b

road

ly.

Page 4: Methodological Issues in Interviewing and Using Self ... · INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 321 Socially Reflexive Questions Questions asking people how they think others

322 FINLAY AND LYONS

Questions that are prone to such difficulties are found in ques-tionnaires that assess internal states and emotions and in thoseassessing concepts such as autonomy and responsibility (e.g.,Lindsey, 1994; Reiss & Benson, 1985; Schalock et al, 1989). Forexample, in a study of self-determination, Wehmeyer and Metzler(1995) asked questions such as, "Do you determine which agenciesand organisations provide services and support for you?" and, "Doyou usually choose your friends and acquaintances?" Further ques-tions asked participants how integrated, independent, and produc-tive they were. The extent to which abstract or nonspecific ques-tions are problematic depends on the aims of the interview. If theinterviewer has specific definitions and standards in mind, thensuch questions should be avoided or at least followed by further(scripted) questions to establish the meaning for the participant. If,however, the interviewer is more concerned with the meanings ofthe respondent, then such questions are appropriate, althoughfollow-up questions are also necessary in these cases.

Unfamiliar Content

Through the content of questions or the response options pro-vided, questionnaires might require respondents to consider them-selves or their lives in uncharacteristic ways. This is a problem ofcontent validity and may be found in questionnaires assessingself-concept, psychiatric symptoms, and quality of life. Schurr,Joiner, and Towne (1970) pointed out that instruments used toassess self-concept may often reflect the concerns of the researcherrather than the respondent (see also Gowans & Hulbert, 1983;Nooe, 1977; Rapley, 1995; Zetlin et al., 1985). Relevant content isalso an issue in psychiatric assessment, and some researchers havequestioned whether psychiatric disorders exhibit the same symp-toms in the general population as they do when combined withmental retardation (e.g., Aman, 1991; Moss et al., 1993; Moss,Prosser, & Goldberg, 1996; Sovner & Hurley, 1986). Cautionshould therefore be exercised in modifying questionnaires devel-oped for the general population by simplifying the language butotherwise leaving the item content intact (e.g., Jiranek & Kirby,1990; Kazdin et al., 1983; Lindsay & Michie, 1988; Lindsay,Michie, Baty, Smith, & Miller, 1994; Prout & Schaeffer, 1985;Reynolds & Miller, 1985).

Concern has been expressed in the use of quality-of-life ques-tionnaires that select a number of criteria judged by professionalsto be important indicators of service quality, such as adaptivebehaviors or aspects of the physical environment (Bercovici, 1981;Heal & Chadsey-Rusch, 1985; Taylor & Bogdan, 1981). Whenjudging the success of community services, Cattermole et al.(1990) suggested that it is more important to assess quality of lifethrough finding out what the service users value rather than whatprofessionals assume to be important. Using an open-ended inter-view style and frequency counts, they found in a sample of 15people who had moved to staffed homes in the community thatpersonal autonomy and social life were of more concern to par-ticipants than were learning skills (see also Dudley, Calhoun,Ahlgrim-Delzell, & Conroy, 1997).

A range of procedures suggested in the general literature onpsychological assessment can be used to increase the contentvalidity of new measures (e.g., Haynes, Richard, & Kubany,1995). After initial items are generated, they should be reviewedby multiple judges to ensure that they sample all possible facets of

the construct of interest and that they do not include items thatmeasure correlates rather than the construct itself (Clark &Watson, 1995; Haynes et al., 1995; for an example, see Thorin,Browning, & Irvin, 1988). Initial items, as well as instructions, canbe generated through expert sampling and through pilot work withpeople with mental retardation (e.g., Botswick & Foss, 1981;Smyley & Ellsworth, 1997). For example, Castles and Glass(1986) developed a social self-efficacy questionnaire based onproblem vignettes that were generated from interviews with 39people with mental retardation in a vocational training facilityand 42 professionals. Similarly, Nooe (1977) collected self-relevant statements from a small sample of people with mentalretardation living in a residential facility and used them as a basisfor a card-sorting task to assess self-concept. In such cases, it isimportant that the sample characteristics are well described be-cause content developed from people with mild mental retardation,or from those living in institutions, may not be relevant for thosewith moderate disabilities or those living in the community.

Sensitive Content

Questions with sensitive or taboo content have been found to beprone to error and bias in the general population (for a review, seeBarnett, 1998). These problems may be accentuated in people withmental retardation, who often use services in which professionalsexert a large degree of control over their lives. They may, there-fore, be concerned with the possible consequences of their re-sponses, particularly as service use is characterized by the sharingof information among professionals (Biklen & Moseley, 1988;Prosser & Bromley, 1998). Because many people do not havesufficient reading abilities, self-report research with this popula-tion tends to involve face-to-face interviews, which are less anon-ymous and private than written questionnaires and which may leadto underreporting of certain behaviors. Pack, Wallander, andBrowne (1998), in a study involving 82 adolescents with mildmental retardation who were in special education programs, foundthat significantly more participants reported engagement in riskybehaviors (e.g., drug use, carrying weapons) when group admin-istration of an anonymous survey was used compared with aconfidential interview (Ms = 23.8% and 13%, respectively). Inaddition, sensitive questions may lead to different errors or biasesthan in the general population because responding in a sociallydesirable way requires attention to the particular form of thequestion. Shaw and Budd (1982) carried out a study in which 24members of a sheltered workshop (mean IQ = 49.3, SD = 13.2)were asked two reverse-worded questions about a list of behaviors("Is it against the rules to ... ?" and, "Are you allowed to ... ?").They found that participants were more likely to say "no" toquestions concerning prohibited*behavior compared with desirablebehavior, regardless of the form of the question (mean nay-sayingquestions = 13.7 and 2.9, respectively).

Barnett (1998) described a variety of techniques that can be usedto reduce the impact of sensitive questions. Those that may beuseful with people with mental retardation include explicit state-ments that information will not be shared with caregivers orservice workers (see Prosser & Bromley, 1998, for further sug-gestions on explaining research to participants), use of vignettes ornominative techniques (e.g., asking what other people would do),and the use of open-ended questions and conversational styles.

This

doc

umen

t is c

opyr

ight

ed b

y th

e A

mer

ican

Psy

chol

ogic

al A

ssoc

iatio

n or

one

of i

ts a

llied

pub

lishe

rs.

This

arti

cle

is in

tend

ed so

lely

for t

he p

erso

nal u

se o

f the

indi

vidu

al u

ser a

nd is

not

to b

e di

ssem

inat

ed b

road

ly.

Page 5: Methodological Issues in Interviewing and Using Self ... · INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 321 Socially Reflexive Questions Questions asking people how they think others

INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 323

Question Phrasing

The use of complex sentence structures in questions createsdifficulties for many people with mental retardation (Kabzems,1985; McConkey, Morris, & Purcell, 1999; Prosser & Bromley,1998; Wyngaarden, 1981). The following sections describe prob-lems that arise when questions contain negatives and other mod-ifiers, passive tenses, examples, and instances in which the subjectand object can be confused. Many problems with questions andanswers may be due to participants responding to particular wordsin the question rather than to the whole (Matson & Frame, 1986;Zetlin et al., 1985).

Negatively Worded Questions and Modifiers

It has been shown in experimental studies using reaction timesand error rates that negatively phrased items (i.e., when no or notare added to positive phrasings) are more difficult to respond tothan are affirmative items for the general population (Gough,1965; Slobin, 1966). Negatively worded questions have been re-ported as difficult for people with mental retardation (Lowe & dePaiva, 1988; Wehmeyer, 1994) and are an example of the moregeneral problem of modifiers, which are single words or clausesthat change the sense of a question. In these cases, the person mayrespond to the simple form of the item as if the modifier were notpresent. For example, "What things would you like to changeabout yourself?" might be responded to as if it were, "What thingswould you like to change?"

Not only are negatively phrased questions more complicatedconstructions, leading some people with mental retardation toanswer as if the question had been phrased in the positive, butmembers of this population might be less willing to criticize thanto offer positive comments (Lowe & de Paiva, 1988). Negativelyphrased questions can be found in questionnaires assessing affect,self-esteem, and preferences, particularly when there is no com-monly used single word for the negative form of a concept. Theyare often included to counterbalance items phrased positively.Examples include the 1985 assessment by Flynn et al. of people'stolerance of rules, the Eysenck-Withers Personality Inventory forSubnormal Subjects (Eysenck, 1965), and the Self-Report Depres-sion Questionnaire (Reynolds & Baker, 1988). Items in the latterinclude, "I can't fall asleep at night," "I have no energy," and, "Ifeel that people don't like me."

Because of these difficulties, it is better to use affirmative formsof statements. For example, Szivos-Bach (1993), basing her ques-tionnaire on standard self-esteem questionnaires, used statementssuch as, "I cause trouble/I give up easily/I am slow at work/I makea mess of things I try/I forget things." Statements such as these arebetter than simply adding a negative to a positive (e.g., "I am notvery good a t . . . /I can't remember things"). However, it is impor-tant to ensure that the words used are common in the person'severyday environments (e.g., terms such as dislike or unkind mightnot be used).

Subject-Object Confusion and Passive Phrasing

Slobin (1966) found, for the general population, that sentencescontaining the passive tense were more difficult than those usingthe active tense (see also Gough, 1965) and that reversible items,

in which the subject and object can be confused although thesentence still makes sense (e.g., the man chases the dog), wereharder than nonreversible items (e.g., the man waters the flowers).This may be a particular problem for people with mental retarda-tion, who might confuse the subject and object of the question,particularly if they attend to only a few words per question.

Flynn et al. (1985), in a questionnaire to assess people's toler-ance of rules and knowledge of rights, included questions prone tosubject-object difficulties such as, "Do you think you should becareful with things that do not belong to you?" and, "Do you thinkyou should blame other people when you do something wrong?"Questionnaires that attempt to assess people's relationships with,and reactions to, other people are also prone to such difficulties(e.g., Eysenck, 1965; Matson et al., 1984).

Giving Examples

In a study in which 107 children and 42 adults with mild tosevere mental retardation living in both the community and insti-tutions were interviewed about their activities, quality of life, andresidential settings, Sigelman, Budd, et al. (1982) found that whenthey gave examples, people tended to give these back as answers.This particularly may be a problem when respondents have diffi-culties in comprehension, memory retrieval, making generaliza-tions, or with perseveration.

Response Format and Scoring Answers

Self-report questionnaires often involve yes-no or multiple-choice response formats. For example, the most widely usedinstruments that assess self-esteem, psychiatric symptoms, andquality-of-life and service satisfaction use yes-no, either-or, ormultiple-choice or Likert scale formats. The following sectionsaddress issues arising from the use of yes-no, multiple-choice, andopen-ended questions, as well as the problem of how to deal withirrelevant answers.

Yes-No Questions and Acquiescence

Acquiescence in interviews, or yea-saying, is the tendency tosay yes to questions regardless of their content (Block, 1965). It isthe problem that has been described most often in the literature onresponse biases in this population (for a review, see Finlay &Lyons, in press) and is linked to research suggesting that peoplewith mental retardation are more suggestible to leading questions(Kebbell & Hatton, 1999).

In a series of studies using alternative question formats withchildren and adults with severe to mild mental retardation,Sigelman and her colleagues (Sigelman, Budd, Spanhel, &Schoenrock, 1981a, 1981b; Sigelman, Budd, Winer, Schoenrock,& Martin, 1982; Sigelman, Schoenrock, et al., 1981; Sigelman,Winer, et al., 1982; for a review, see Heal & Sigelman, 1995)found that for factual as well as subjective content, yes-no ques-tions were subject to a systematic acquiescence bias, even whenthe answer was absurd (e.g., "Does it usually snow in the summerhere?"). Sigelman, Budd, et al. (1981b), for example, found in asample of 29 people with moderate and severe mental retardation(mean IQ = 31.62, SD = 7.24) that acquiescence rates rangedfrom 20% to 83.3% (depending on the type of question and method

This

doc

umen

t is c

opyr

ight

ed b

y th

e A

mer

ican

Psy

chol

ogic

al A

ssoc

iatio

n or

one

of i

ts a

llied

pub

lishe

rs.

This

arti

cle

is in

tend

ed so

lely

for t

he p

erso

nal u

se o

f the

indi

vidu

al u

ser a

nd is

not

to b

e di

ssem

inat

ed b

road

ly.

Page 6: Methodological Issues in Interviewing and Using Self ... · INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 321 Socially Reflexive Questions Questions asking people how they think others

324 FINLAY AND LYONS

of detection). This tendency was inversely related to IQ scores ontwo of their three composite measures of acquiescence (r = .37and .61; see also Burnett, 1989; Gudjonsson, 1990; Shaw & Budd,1982; cf. Mattika & Vesala, 1997). Either-or questions were foundto be less prone to systematic response bias, as measured byreverse wordings, although there was a tendency in these cases forlast-choice responding (see also Loper & Reeve, 1983). For ex-ample, Sigelman, Budd, et al. (198la) found that in a sample of 42institutionalized adults (mean IQ = 39.8, SD = 13.1), consistencywhen using reverse wordings was greater for either-or questions(86.2% and 75.1%) than for yes-no questions (53.1% and 52%).Sigelman, Winer, et al. (1982), reporting on the same sample, alsofound responsiveness to be highest with yes-no questions (82.2%),followed by either-or questions (68.7%), and to be lowest withopen-ended questions (51%). These papers stressed that the valid-ity of answers given by people with mental retardation must neverbe assumed but must always be demonstrated. Acquiescent re-sponding to yes-no questions has also been found to be a problemby other authors (Burnett, 1989; Gerjuoy & Winters, 1966; Heal &Chadsey-Rusch, 1985; Perlman, Ericson, Esses, & Isaacs, 1994).

A number of studies have suggested that acquiescence may notbe as common in people with mental retardation as suggested bySigelman and colleagues' studies (e.g., Booth & Booth, 1994a;Conroy & Bradley, 1985; Matikka & Vesala, 1997; Rapley &Antaki, 1996; Wehmeyer, 1994). Differences in the rates of ac-quiescence found across studies may be due to differences in themethods used and the samples selected, which unfortunately makethe results noncomparable. Researchers have tended to use threetechniques to identify acquiescence in interviews, nonsense ques-tions to which the answer should be "yes," pairs of questions thatare opposite in meaning, and pairs of questions that ask the samequestion in different formats. Each of these techniques involvesdifficulties in interpretation. Nonsense questions (e.g., Flynn et al.,1985; Sigelman, Budd, et al., 1981b) have been criticized byRapley and Antaki (1996), who questioned whether answers tosuch questions are comparable to those from sensible questions.Such questions may baffle or amuse participants, who may say"yes" for reasons other than a tendency to acquiesce.

Another method used is to include pairs of reverse-wordedquestions. If a respondent affirms both versions, this is taken asevidence of acquiescence (e.g., Flynn et al., 1985; Heal &Chadsey-Rusch, 1985; Sigelman, Budd, et al., 1981a, 1981b).However, Matikka and Vesala (1997) suggested that contradic-tions may arise because different images or occasions are beingelicited, suggesting that using reverse wordings might not be agood way to measure acquiescence. Indeed, Rapley and Antaki(1996) pointed out that inconsistency is a feature of discourse ingeneral. Despite the problems, however, Sigelman, Budd, et al.(1981b) offered some evidence for the validity of these techniques.They found significant correlations between measures based onreverse wordings and nonsense questions (r — .57) and betweenthose based on nonsense questions and total number of affirmativeresponses to questions about involvement in household chores(r = .45).

Yea-saying may be more likely to arise when the answer is notknown or when questions are too long or the structure is toocomplex (Finlay & Lyons, in press). Respondents may then focuson a limited part of the question and miss the subtleties of thephrasing. In these cases, participants may appear to contradict

themselves when questions are reverse worded. In the study de-scribed earlier, Shaw and Budd (1982) found significantly moreacquiescence for socially desirable than undesirable behaviors(Ms = 13.3 and 4.6 questions, respectively), significantly morenay-saying for undesirable than desirable behaviors (Ms = 13.7and 2.9 questions, respectively), and significantly more contradic-tions for people with low compared with higher IQ scores(Ms = 20.4 and 13.4 questions, respectively). The authors con-cluded that cognitive impairments predispose people to responsebiases and that social desirability then determines the direction thiswill take. In reverse wordings, then, people may say "yes" toopposite forms of the same question because they are respondingto the topic rather than to the particular form of the question, andthose who have less ability in receptive language may be partic-ularly vulnerable to this.

In many cases, people are quite willing to answer in the negative(e.g., Booth & Booth, 1994a, 1996; Lowe & de Paiva, 1988;Rapley & Antaki, 1996); therefore, the task is to identify underwhich circumstances the problem is more likely to arise. It isclearly unacceptable to ignore this difficulty, particularly becauseit has been found that people may overestimate the comprehensionof people with mental retardation (Bartlett & Bunning, 1997; Tully& Cahill, 1984) and that service workers may often fail to adjusttheir language accordingly (McConkey, Morris, & Purcell, 1999).

Various steps have been taken to counter the problems ofyea-saying. Seligman and her colleagues recommended thatyes-no questions not be used at all and that either-or questions beused in preference. Many researchers and questionnaire developersnow check for consistency by including reverse wordings of alleither-or or yes-no questions at other points in the interview (e.g.,Shanly & Rose, 1993) or by using screening questions to excludethose participants who are more likely to offer acquiescent re-sponses (e.g., Burnett, 1989; Helsel & Matson, 1988; Jiranek &Kirby, 1990). However, these procedures involve the problems ofnonsense and reverse-worded questions described above and maynot reflect the complexity of the question structures found in thetests themselves. Tests using absurd questions assume that yea-saying is based on a desire to please or agree with the interviewer.However, if it appears in response to questions that are grammat-ically or semantically complex or when the person is uncertain ofthe answer, then such tests would not be a powerful identifier ofpeople who are prone to this.

An alternative when using yes-no questions is to include allparticipants in the interview, to ensure question wording is clearand simple, and to offer a "don't know" option. When an answeris given, follow-up, open-ended questions should be used to elicitexamples to check the interviewee's understanding (e.g., Smyley& Ellsworth, 1997; Szivos-Bach, 1993; Zetlin et al., 1985). Ifexamples are not provided by the interviewee, the answer can befurther checked using preplanned alternative wordings or phras-ings. This allows the detection of acquiescence caused by partic-ipants' focus on only part of the question and, at the same time,allows participation by respondents whose receptive abilities areadequate for yes-no questions, but whose expressive languagedoes not permit them to give examples (Prout & Strohmer, 1994).For interviewees who have greater receptive than expressive abil-ities, Booth and Booth (1996) suggested strategies by which anarrative might be constructed, such as through the elimination ofalternatives and the development of questions by adapting to

This

doc

umen

t is c

opyr

ight

ed b

y th

e A

mer

ican

Psy

chol

ogic

al A

ssoc

iatio

n or

one

of i

ts a

llied

pub

lishe

rs.

This

arti

cle

is in

tend

ed so

lely

for t

he p

erso

nal u

se o

f the

indi

vidu

al u

ser a

nd is

not

to b

e di

ssem

inat

ed b

road

ly.

Page 7: Methodological Issues in Interviewing and Using Self ... · INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 321 Socially Reflexive Questions Questions asking people how they think others

INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 325

previous responses. However, the validity of this method dependson the interviewer being able to check both that the person doesunderstand the questions as intended and that the person is willingto reject false suggestions. This may prove difficult in practice, andfurther research needs to be done on this question.

When using reverse wording for all questions (Stancliffe, 1995),the participant may infer that he or she is giving the wrong answerand thus produce more inconsistency (Rapley, 1995). If reversewording is to be used, it may be helpful to make clear to theinterviewee at the outset that this is going to be done and that itdoes not mean that the original answer was wrong. Alternativephrasings and requests for further detail need to be scripted (e.g.,Lindsay & Michie, 1988; Moss et al., 1997) and developed with asmuch attention as other items. The dangers of spontaneous para-phrasing were illustrated by Antaki (1998), who gave examples ofthe difficulties that interviewers encountered when they tried torephrase questions to elicit scoreable responses on a multiple-choice questionnaire. Because of the need to simplify questions,the threshold at which a positive response was scored was oftenlowered, with the result that overall scores became inflated.

Multiple-Choice Formats

Multiple-choice formats used in questionnaires for the generalpopulation are usually presented in written form. Because manypeople with mental retardation cannot read, multiple-choice for-mats in this population are usually presented orally, althoughvisual aids are also sometimes used. Orally presented multiple-choice formats would present difficulties in the general populationbut may be even more problematic for many people with mentalretardation because of the length of the question, the high memoryload required of the respondent (Kabzems, 1985; Reynolds, 1979),and the difficulties in either fitting the response given into one ofthe categories (Malik et al., 1991; Zetlin et al., 1985) or checkingthat all the options have been understood (Edmonson, McCombs,& Wish, 1979; Smyley & Ellsworth, 1997). These formats alsooften involve judgments of degree or frequency. Antaki and Rap-ley (1996) showed vividly the difficulties of using multiple-choiceformats when interviewing people with mental retardation. Usingtranscripts of quality-of-life interviews, they demonstrated howquestions became transformed through the interviewer's attemptsto paraphrase, the need to introduce items, and the need to offer alist of response alternatives. Answers also became transformedthrough the attempts of the interviewer to fit them into predeter-mined categories. The examples those researchers gave illustratedthat assigning responses to one of the options given in suchverbally administered, multiple-choice questionnaires is often ahaphazard process.

Difficulties in administering multiple-choice formats were alsofound in a small study by Malik et al. (1991) in which yes-no,open-ended, and multiple-choice questions were compared. Sixpeople with mild and moderate mental retardation who wererecruited from special recreation programs participated in a 287-item interview on social integration and recreation participation.These authors found that test-retest reliabilities were lowest on themultiple-choice questions (58.6%) compared with on yes-no andeither-or questions (71.8%). Similarly, in an experimental study inwhich police officers interviewed witnesses with mild and border-line mental retardation, Tully and Cahill (1984) found that al-

though direct, leading questions resulted in 11% of errors, offeringalternative choices accounted for one third of all errors. Theyrecommended that first interviews with witnesses with mentalretardation should use open-ended questions to avoid this problem,with subsequent interviews used for checking the reliabilities ofthese accounts and for probing for details.

Although there is some evidence that multiple-choice formatscan be used successfully in educational tests with people with mildand borderline mental retardation (Reynolds, 1979), questions thatinclude a list of verbally presented alternatives are not appropriatefor many people with mental retardation. When they are used, it isessential to check the answers by asking for examples or by usingother, scripted questions (e.g., Clare & Gudjonsson, 1995; Smith &McCarthy, 1996) and by reporting the proportion of responses thatare deemed valid (e.g., Benson & Ivins, 1992; Smyley & Ells-worth, 1997).

One solution that is possible in some circumstances is to breakthe question into two stages. Rybolt (1969) described the use ofverbally presented semantic differentials with 73 adolescents clas-sified as having educable mental retardation. This involved pre-senting the two extremes as an either-or question (e.g., fast orslow), followed by asking whether the item was "a little" or "veryfast." Test-retest reliabilities (up to 3 weeks) for adjective pairswere found to be between .31 and .82. Rybolt also found asignificant negative correlation (r = —.39) between IQ and levelsof response inconsistency.

When response options can be clearly represented in pictorialform and the options are easy to differentiate, pictures can increaseresponsiveness and understanding (Foxx, Faw, Taylor, Davis, &Fulia, 1993; March, 1992; Sigelman & Budd, 1986; Wadsworth &Harper, 1991). Pictures have been found to be less useful whenthey represent less concrete concepts or when they illustrate quitesubtle distinctions (e.g., Sigelman, Schoenrock et al., 1981; Wil-gosh & Barry, 1983).

Open-Ended Questions

The use of open-ended questions can produce low levels ofresponsiveness (Sigelman, Budd et al., 1981a, 1981b). Dent (1986)carried out a study with 23 children (aged 8-11) with mild mentalretardation who were asked to report on a staged event involvinga man who came into their classroom and demonstrated some toys.She found that more detail was produced with specific questions(e.g., "What color was the man's hair?"), followed by generalquestions (e.g., "What did the man look like?"), and least detailwith free recall (Ms = 18.8, 9.4, and 4.3 points of information,respectively). However, the proportion of errors was also highestwith specific questions and least with general questions (meanaccuracy scores = 64% and 92.7%, respectively)—for similarresults, see Perlman et al. (1994). Recently, cognitive interviewtechniques have been used for witness questioning. These use arange of open-ended questions to enable the interviewee to remem-ber an incident in several different ways (e.g., in reverse order orfrom another person's perspective) and have been found to pro-duce greater recall of details (for a review, see Kebbell & Hatton,1999).

Although open-ended questions might be problematic for somepeople, many people with mental retardation are able to respond tothis type of question, particularly if the questions avoid the types

This

doc

umen

t is c

opyr

ight

ed b

y th

e A

mer

ican

Psy

chol

ogic

al A

ssoc

iatio

n or

one

of i

ts a

llied

pub

lishe

rs.

This

arti

cle

is in

tend

ed so

lely

for t

he p

erso

nal u

se o

f the

indi

vidu

al u

ser a

nd is

not

to b

e di

ssem

inat

ed b

road

ly.

Page 8: Methodological Issues in Interviewing and Using Self ... · INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 321 Socially Reflexive Questions Questions asking people how they think others

326 FINLAY AND LYONS

of phrasings and concepts described above. Open-ended questionshave been used successfully in a number of research projects (e.g.,Booth & Booth, 1994a, 1994b; Dudly et al., 1997; Edmonson &Wish, 1975; Finlay & Lyons, 2000; Neumayer & Bleasdale, 1996;Shanly & Rose, 1993). Smyley and Ellsworth (1997), in a study ofclients' views of service provision, found that problems of contra-dictory responses and checking for understanding, which werefrequent when using closed questions, were minimized when open-ended questions were used. Rodgers (1999) reported the use ofpictures as a prompt in conversational-style interviews to facilitateparticipation by those who had difficulties in responding to verbalquestions.

Quantitative information can also be produced from open-endedquestions. Cattermole et al. (1990), in a study of the success ofpeople's moves to the community from institutions, used a semi-structured interview in which the interviewer had a list of generalthemes to pursue, such as social life, friends, privacy, and choice.The analysis involved counting the evaluative statements in eachcategory and comparing those made before moving from thehospital to those made after moving. Frequency counts from con-tent analysis were also used by Finlay and Lyons (2000) in a studyof social comparisons and by McEvoy (1989), who, investigatingpeople's understandings of death, used judges to rate people'sanswers.

If one is concerned with the point of view of the respondent, inall its complexity, and the researcher does not believe that he orshe can second-guess the possible responses, then open-endedquestions should be used, despite the difficulties of using suchtechniques with those who have more severe language difficulties(Bercovici, 1981; Booth & Booth, 1994b; Schurr et al., 1970;Taylor & Bogdan, 1981). Prout and Strohmer (1994) pointed outthe importance of considering the expressive-receptive languagedichotomy and suggested that a more conversational, open-endedstyle is better for those with good expressive skills, whereas a moredirective style is appropriate for those with more limited expres-sive abilities (e.g., Booth & Booth, 1996). However, it is importantto be aware that the potential for researchers' imposing their owninterpretations and preoccupations is greater when participants areless articulate (Goodley, 1996).

Answers That Appear Irrelevant

If the respondent is preoccupied with certain concerns or prob-lems, he or she may keep returning to these as topics (Biklen &Moseley, 1988; Zetlin et al., 1985). In an attempt to use theCoopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory and the Piers-Harris Self-Concept scale with people with mild to severe mental retardation(mean IQ = 51, SD = 10), Zetlin et al. (1985) found that answerswere often incoherent or contradictory or turned into stories orexamples that did not seem relevant. Answers that do not seemdesigned to answer the question might arise for reasons other thana lack of understanding, such as a lack of inhibitory control overprevious topics (i.e., when the person finds it difficult to changequickly from one topic to the next—Dempster & Corkill, 1999) orbecause the interview is proceeding too quickly. In these cases, thequestion should be repeated later in the interview.

Psychometric Properties and Target Population

Many of the statistics that are reported for new or modifiedself-report questionnaires for people with mental retardation referto the reliability rather than the validity of the questionnaire inquestion (e.g., Dagnan et al., 1994; Eysenck, 1965; Flynn et al.,1985; Heal & Chadsey-Rusch, 1985; Reiss & Benson, 1985;Reynolds & Miller, 1985; Riggen & Ulrich, 1993; Sinnott-Oswald,Gliner, & Spencer, 1991). Because of the problems describedabove, researchers should beware of assuming that reliable ques-tionnaires are also valid in this population. Sigelman, Schoenrock,et al. (1981) pointed out that adequate test-retest reliabilities mightbe due to consistencies in response biases. In addition, many newquestionnaires report test statistics from small or vaguely specifiedsamples (e.g., Dagnan et al., 1994; Flynn et al., 1985; Foxx et al.,1993; Heal & Chadsey-Rusch, 1985; Nooe, 1977), and researchersusing such questionnaires need to investigate their properties fur-ther before using them with confidence.

Studies that have reported acceptable reliability statistics forself-report questionnaires have often found that validity statisticsdo not provide unequivocal support for the instruments (e.g.,Chadsey-Rusch et al., 1992; Harner & Heal, 1993; Kazdin et al.,1983; Lunsky & Benson, 1997; Rojahn et al., 1994). For example,correlations of modified self-report depression scales and thePIMRA depression subscale with each other and with informantscales have been inconsistent (e.g., Helsel & Matson, 1988; Kaz-din et al., 1983; Senatore et al., 1985). Such inconsistencies mightbe explained by the criterion scales lacking validity (particularly ifthese are also self-report), by the use of informants who may havedifferent perspectives (Aman, 1991), or by the invalidity of thescale itself. Examples of the contradictory validity statistics oftenfound in self-report questionnaires in this population are given inTable 1.

A more rigorous approach to questionnaire development wouldovercome some of the problems of interpretation found in suchvalidity studies. After the content validity is addressed (see earlierin this article), researchers should carry out test refinement proce-dures (Clark & Watson, 1995; Smith & McCarthy, 1996; seeThorin et al., 1988, for an example in a questionnaire for peoplewith mental retardation) and explicitly address issues of conver-gent and discriminant validity (Foster & Cone, 1995). Test refine-ment, which involves collecting large enough samples for psycho-metric statistics, is likely to be more time-consuming in thispopulation because instruments will usually require individual,oral administration.

Because the validity of many self-report measures has not beendemonstrated in this population, convergent and ecological valid-ity should be investigated using third-party accounts (althoughthese also entail problems of validity), behavioral measures, directmeasures of the construct, or criterion-related indices. For exam-ple, Schalock et al. (1989), in developing the Quality of LifeQuestionnaire, used validity indices such as staff versions of thequestionnaire, goodness-of-fit ratings (ratio of individuals' behav-ioral capabilities to the performance requirements of the servicesetting), measures of staff involvement, and criteria such as type ofresidential placement. Discriminant validity should be assessed byreporting correlations with measures of response sets, such associal desirability, lying, and yea-saying (Foster & Cone, 1995), aswell as IQ scores (Kazdin, 1995).

This

doc

umen

t is c

opyr

ight

ed b

y th

e A

mer

ican

Psy

chol

ogic

al A

ssoc

iatio

n or

one

of i

ts a

llied

pub

lishe

rs.

This

arti

cle

is in

tend

ed so

lely

for t

he p

erso

nal u

se o

f the

indi

vidu

al u

ser a

nd is

not

to b

e di

ssem

inat

ed b

road

ly.

Page 9: Methodological Issues in Interviewing and Using Self ... · INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 321 Socially Reflexive Questions Questions asking people how they think others

INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 327

Table 1Validity and Reliability Statistics of Commonly Used Self-Report Depression Questionnaires for People with Mental Retardation

Instrument Reference Sample Validity measures Reliability

PIMRA (depression Kazdin et al.,scale; yes-no 1983format)

Matson et al.,1984

Senatore et al.,1985

Aman et al..1986

Watson, Aman,& Singh,1988

Helsel &Matson,1988

Tymchuk, 1993

110 adults (96% withborderline to moderatemental retardation) —screening test foracquiescence

As above

As above

160 adults (borderline tomoderate mentalretardation) based incommunity (95) orinstitution (65)

Same as Aman et al.(1986)

99 adults (93% withborderline to moderatemental retardation);screened foracquiescence

31 mothers with mentalretardation (mean IQ =68)

Significant correlations with modified BDI,r = .33, and Hamilton RSD, informantscale; r = .2. No relation to modifiedZDS, MMPI depression subscale, andPIMRA depression subscale (informantversion). No relation to IQ or level ofretardation. Women scored significantlyhigher than men, p < .05.

Factor structure different for informant andself-report versions of total PIMRA. Noaffective or depression factor found forself-report version.

Those diagnosed on basis of depressionsubscale had lower adjustment scores (M= 3.2 vs. 2.2) but no differences onother PIMRA subscales, the SocialPerformance Scale (informant scale),modified BDI, or modified ZDS.

No relation to sex or level of retardation.Different factors found compared withthe results of Matson et al. (1984) fortotal PIMRA scale. No affective ordepression factor found.

Different factors found for informantversion of total PIMRA compared withself-report version.

Significant correlations with modified BDI(r = .38) and with ZDS (r = .26). Norelation to receptive vocabulary or levelof retardation

Significant positive relation to neglect as achild (r = .53). No significant relationto a range of demographic variables(e.g., IQ, education, marital status, workhistory).

Item-total correlationsfrom .19 to .60.Test-retestcorrelation = .69(5 months).

Alpha = .66(community )/.62(institution). Meanitem-totalcorrelations = .48(community )/.41(institution). Test-retest correlation =.26, ns (5 months).

Same statistics asAman et al. (1986)

Alpha = .51. Averageitem-totalcorrelation = .50.

PIMRA (depressionscale—Dutch)

Modified ZungDepression Scale(4-point frequencyscales usingvisual-aid bargraph)

van Minnen,Savelsberg,& Hoogduin,1994

Kazdin et al.,1983

79 adults with mild mentalretardation in Holland

110 adults (96% withborderline to moderatemental retardation);screened for use of 4-point scale usingquestions requiringanswer of "never" orother three points(grouped)—see Senatoreet al., 1985

Participants with diagnosedpsychopathology scored significantlyhigher (M = 2.7 vs. 0.8). Significantcorrelation with informant version (r notreported). No relation to age, sex, or IQ.

Those diagnosed with psychopathology hadsignificantly lower scores (p < .01).Significant correlations with modifiedBDI (r = .59), PIMRA totalpsychopathology score (r = 0.25), andIQ (r = - .42). No significantcorrelation with MMPI depression scale,PIMRA depression subscale (self-reportor informant), or Hamilton RSD(informant). High and low scorersdiffered significantly on BDI (M = 21.5vs. 11.4), PIMRA total (M = 21.7vs. 17.4), and Hamilton RSD (M = 18.9vs. 13.9) but not on PIMRA depressionscales (self-report or informant). Thoseidentified as depressed on informantversion of PIMRA scored significantlyhigher (M = 38.3 vs. 30.9).

Alpha = .68.

(table continues)

This

doc

umen

t is c

opyr

ight

ed b

y th

e A

mer

ican

Psy

chol

ogic

al A

ssoc

iatio

n or

one

of i

ts a

llied

pub

lishe

rs.

This

arti

cle

is in

tend

ed so

lely

for t

he p

erso

nal u

se o

f the

indi

vidu

al u

ser a

nd is

not

to b

e di

ssem

inat

ed b

road

ly.

Page 10: Methodological Issues in Interviewing and Using Self ... · INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 321 Socially Reflexive Questions Questions asking people how they think others

328

Table 1 (continued)

FINLAY AND LYONS

Instrument Reference Sample Validity measures Reliability

Modified ZungDepression Scale(yes-no format)

Unmodified ZungDepression Scale

Modified BeckDepressionInventory (4-pointfrequency scalesusing visual-aidbar graph)

Reiss &Benson,1985

Helsel &Matson,1988

Lindsay,Michie,Baty, Smith,& Miller,1994

Dagnan &Sandhu,1999

Prout &Schaefer,1985

Kazdin et al.,1983

Helsel &Matson,1988

45 adults with mild mentalretardation

99 adults (93% withborderline to moderatemental retardation);screening test notdescribed

67 adults with mild ormoderate mentalretardation

43 adults with mentalretardation; BritishPicture Vocabulary Scale(mean score = 16.6,range = 6-28)

21 adults with mild mentalretardation

110 adults (96% withborderline to moderatemental retardation).Screened for use of 4-point scale usingquestions requiringanswer of "never" or"other three points"(grouped)—see Senatoreet al., 1985

99 adults (93% withborderline to moderatemental retardation);screening test notdescribed

Significant correlations with social supportmeasures (self-report, r = -.41 andinformant, r = —.37). No significantrelation to measures of stigma (self-report or informant).

Significant correlations with modified BDI(r = .35) and modified self-report SocialPerformance Survey (r = .38). Norelation to Hamilton RSD, PIMRAdepression scales (self-report orinformant), Social Performance Survey(informant), receptive vocabulary, levelof retardation, or gender.

Significant correlations with modifiedZung Anxiety Scale (r = .59), GHQscales (health, r = .39; anxiety, r = .55;social skills, r = .38; depression, r =.65) and Eysenck-Withers PersonalityScale—Neuroticism (r = .29).

Significant negative correlation withmodified Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale(r = -.39) and modified SocialComparison Scale (r = —.5). Nosignificant correlation with BritishPicture Vocabulary Scale score.

Significant correlations with unmodifiedDepression Adjective Checklist (r =.79) and BDI (r = .73). Agreement forclinical diagnosis with BDI = 91%.Scores significantly higher than controlsample (M = 40.38 vs. 26.0).

Significant correlations with IQ (r =-.47), modified ZDS (r = .59),modified MMPI depression scale (r =.25), PIMRA depression (self-report; r= .33) and total scales (r = .57), andHamilton RSD (informant r = .24). Nosignificant relation to PIMRA depressionor total scales (informant), gender, orlevel of retardation. High and lowscorers differed significantly on ZDS(mean scores = 37.8 vs. 26.1), MMPIdepression scale (M = 26.0 vs. 22.9),PIMRA self-report depression (M = 3.2vs. 1.8) and total (M = 22.8 vs. 12.8)scales, and Hamilton RSD (M = 18.1vs. 12.3) but not on PIMRA informanttotal or depression scales. Thoseidentified as depressed on informantversion of PIMRA scored significantlyhigher (M = 22.1 vs. 15.0).

Significant correlations with modified ZDS(r = .35) and PIMRA depression (self-report; r = .38). No significant relationto Hamilton RSD, PIMRA informantdepression or total scores, PIMRA self-report total scores, Social PerformanceSurvey (self-report or informant),receptive vocabulary, level ofretardation, or gender.

Test-retest = .61(4-10 weeks).

Test-retest = .75.

This

doc

umen

t is c

opyr

ight

ed b

y th

e A

mer

ican

Psy

chol

ogic

al A

ssoc

iatio

n or

one

of i

ts a

llied

pub

lishe

rs.

This

arti

cle

is in

tend

ed so

lely

for t

he p

erso

nal u

se o

f the

indi

vidu

al u

ser a

nd is

not

to b

e di

ssem

inat

ed b

road

ly.

Page 11: Methodological Issues in Interviewing and Using Self ... · INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 321 Socially Reflexive Questions Questions asking people how they think others

INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 329

Table 1 (continued)

Instrument Reference Sample Validity measures Reliability

Unmodified BeckDepressionInventory

Self-ReportDepressionQuestionnaire—most items 3-pointscales

Prout &Schaefer,1985

Reynolds &Baker, 1988

Rojahn et al.,1994

21 adults with mild mentalretardation

89 adults—IQ range, 35-75(mean = 59.11)—screened foracquiescence

29 adults (mild or moderatemental retardation)—screened for use of 3-point scales

Significant correlations with unmodifiedDepression Adjective Checklist (r =.74) and unmodified ZDS (r = .73).Agreement for clinical diagnosis withZDS = 91%. Scores significantly higherthan control sample (M = 19.43vs. 4.97).

No relation to gender, age, IQ, or score onacquiescence test (after those who failedon screening test removed from sample).Significant correlation with Hamiltonclinical interview for depressivesymptomatology (r = .65).

Little agreement in diagnosis with bothReiss Screen for Maladaptive Behaviordepression scale—informant version (K= 0.06) and Diagnostic Interview forChildren and Adolescents (K = 0.07).

Alpha = .90. Averageitem-totalcorrelation = .23.Test-retestcorrelation = .63(11 weeks).

Note. BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; Hamilton RSD = Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression; PIMRA = Psychopathology Instrument for MentallyRetarded Adults; ZDS = Zung Depression Scale.

Instruments developed for the general population are unlikely tobe appropriate for people with mental retardation (Gowans &Hulbert, 1983; Schurr, Joiner, & Towne, 1970). However, studiesfrequently use them without demonstrating their validity for thisgroup. This can be found in the assessment of self-concept (e.g.,Chiu, 1990; Jiranek & Kirby, 1990; Thompson, Lampron, John-son, & Eckstein, 1990; Tymchuk, 1991) and psychiatric symptoms(e.g., Manikam, Matson, Coe, & Hillman, 1995; Prout & Schaefer,1985; Reiss & Benson, 1985). Studies should not report psycho-metric statistics found for the general population because they areunlikely to be applicable to the groups they are studying (e.g.,Calhoun, Whitley, & Ansolabehere, 1978; Chassin, Stager, &Young, 1985; Reynolds & Miller, 1985).

Factor structures found in samples of people with mental retar-dation may differ from those found in the general population. Thisis one reason that existing instruments may be inapplicable andcreates problems for new or modified questionnaires that are basedon factor structures found in the general population (e.g., Riggen& Ulrich, 1993; Szivos-Bach, 1993). For example, Silon andHarter (1985) used the Perceived Competence Scale with a sampleof 126 children with mental retardation (IQ range, 55 to 85) in bothsegregated and mainstream schools and found that instead of theusual four factors, only two factors were present, and there was noglobal self-worth factor (see Rich, Barcikowski, and Witmer,1979, for a similar example using the Piers-Harris Children'sSelf-Concept Scale). Matson et al. (1984) also found differentfactors in the self-report compared with the informant version ofthe PIMRA (see also Aman, Watson, Singh, Turbott, & Wilsher,1986). In their study, with people with severe to borderline mentalretardation who were users of mental health services, the self-report version yielded two factors (labeled anxiety and socialadjustment), whereas the informant version yielded three (affec-tive, somatoform, and psychosis). Because factor structures maybe different for informant compared with self-report versions,psychometric statistics should be reported separately for these two

groups. Factors or reliability statistics found in mixed samples(e.g., Manikam et al., 1995; McGrew, Bruininks, Thurlow, &Lewis, 1992) should be viewed with caution. Given the problemsof validity identified with many self-report questionnaires, how-ever, differences in factor structures that have been found shouldbe treated as only suggestive of differences in underlyingconstructs.

It is possible that people with mental retardation may be tooheterogeneous in terms of personal history and linguistic andcognitive abilities for any single questionnaire to be valid for thewhole population. Reliability and validity statistics reported instudies may not therefore be informative for other samples. Forexample, Prout and Schaefer (1985) used unmodified self-reportmeasures of depression (BDI, ZDS, and the Depression AdjectiveChecklist) and found high intercorrelations (rs = .73 to .79).However, there were only 21 participants in this study, all ofwhom had mild mental retardation. Given the difficulties identifiedwith the questions in such questionnaires, it cannot be assumedthat such correlations would be found with people with moderatemental retardation.

Many studies use screening devices or select samples based onvague criteria, making it difficult to judge the applicability of thequestionnaire for other samples. Studies often select participantsbased on a judgment of their communicative skills. For example,Szivos-Bach (1993), in her study of stigma and self-concept in 50students with mild mental retardation, excluded people "who dem-onstrated that they could not understand the questions asked (forexample by replying 'yes' or 'no' to an open-ended question)" (p.220). Aman et al. (1986), in their exploration of the reliability andfactor structure of the PIMRA (self-report), included people in thestudy only when they were "judged capable of understanding andresponding to questions within the PIMRA" (p. 1072). The criteriafor this judgment were not described in this paper. Similar vaguecriteria for selection are found in a number of studies that usedself-report questionnaires (e.g., Chadsey-Rusch et al., 1992; Moss

This

doc

umen

t is c

opyr

ight

ed b

y th

e A

mer

ican

Psy

chol

ogic

al A

ssoc

iatio

n or

one

of i

ts a

llied

pub

lishe

rs.

This

arti

cle

is in

tend

ed so

lely

for t

he p

erso

nal u

se o

f the

indi

vidu

al u

ser a

nd is

not

to b

e di

ssem

inat

ed b

road

ly.

Page 12: Methodological Issues in Interviewing and Using Self ... · INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 321 Socially Reflexive Questions Questions asking people how they think others

330 FINLAY AND LYONS

et al., 1997; Smith & McCarthy, 1996; Stancliffe, 1995; Voelker etal., 1990). The danger is that instruments might have adequatereliability and validity statistics because they are based on a highlyselective sample of those with the greatest communicative abili-ties. When selection criteria are not given or are vague, it isdifficult for others to judge whether the obtained scores can beinterpreted correctly. Notable exceptions to this are the EmotionalProblems Scale (Strohmer et al., 1994), which is for people withmild and borderline mental retardation, and the Eysenck-WithersPersonality Inventory (Eysenck, 1965), which was developed forpeople whose IQ is in the range of 50 to 80.

The degree of verbal ability needed to answer certain types ofquestions and participate in particular questionnaires needs to bemore clearly addressed (Dagnan et al., 1994; Smith & McCarthy,1996; Smyley & Ellsworth, 1997). A test of verbal competence,such as morphosyntactic ability (Abbeduto & Hagerman, 1997),would be useful for determining the receptive language necessaryfor certain questionnaires. One example of a questionnaire thatassesses auditory comprehension is given by Beaumont, Marjori-banks, Flury, and Lintern (1999). This test includes a range ofyes-no questions at different levels of sentence complexity. Morespecific tests of conceptual understanding, vocabulary, or theability to use particular response formats (for examples, seeProsser & Bromley, 1998) might be needed for specificinstruments.

Finally, some questionnaires can be completed as either self-reports or by informants, such as the PIMRA, the PAS-ADD(Moss et al., 1993, 1997), and the Interview of Community Ad-justment (McGrew et al., 1992). It is important in these cases tospecify clearly when the self-report version can be used. Forexample, the PAS-ADD involves a three-tier interview (Moss etal., 1993), each stage involving an assessment of whether theparticipant can move to the next stage. In a study of elderly peoplewith moderate and severe mental retardation, 40 of 105 peoplewere excluded at the background information stage, and only 14people completed the interview (Patel et al., 1993). A positivediagnosis is given if either the informant or the self-report versionindicates one, but the agreement in diagnosis between the twoversions has been found to be rather low. Patel et al. (1993) foundthat for those who obtained a diagnosis, agreement between thetwo methods occurred in only one quarter of cases (see also Moss,Prosser, Ibbotson, & Goldberg, 1996; Moss et al., 1997). Theauthors stated that they have no framework for judging the relativevalidity of either source, and so validity statistics were based onthe combined diagnosis. Despite this problem, the technique mightincrease the detection rate of psychiatric disorders because it canovercome some of the problems of either respondents with mentalretardation who are unable to answer the questions or informantswho lack the required information (e.g., about subjectivephenomena).

Suggestions for Interviews and QuestionnaireDevelopment

A summary of the difficulties in conducting interviews andusing self-report questionnaires with people with mental retarda-tion is presented in Table 2. It should be noted that the difficultiesdescribed in this paper do not apply to all people with mentalretardation, many of whom have few problems with interview

questions. For people with borderline and mild mental retardationin particular, many questionnaires developed for the general pop-ulation may be valid, although this must always be demonstratedrather than assumed. Although it would be wrong to oversimplifyquestions where it is not necessary, standardized questionnairesdeveloped for widespread use should, however, take note of thesedifficulties. It should also be acknowledged that certain constructsmay be too conceptually complex to be assessed with many peoplewith mental retardation. The suggestions given below, therefore,are aimed at increasing the inclusiveness of interviews and thevalidity of self-report questionnaires. It is not proposed that allsuggestions should be used with all interviewees.

The minimum number of words should be used, and ambiguousor complex phrasings should be avoided. These include structureswith modifiers, passive phrasings, and questions that are revers-ible. Interviewers should always be aware that respondents mightbe responding to the topic rather than to the particular form of thequestion. Content such as direct comparisons, judgments of fre-quency and quantity, and the perceptions of third parties shouldalso be avoided. Comparisons can be split into two parts, andsignificant events can be used as markers to judge time periods.Rather than asking people to generalize or answer in the abstract,questions may be more successful when situated in specific con-texts or events from the person's own life. It will often be neces-sary to check how the question was understood by asking forexamples or using alternative phrasings (scripted), particularly foryes-no and multiple-choice questions. When probes or rephrasingsare to be used, interviewees should be informed at the start of theinterview that these are included in the interview as checks and donot mean that their original answer was wrong.

Plenty of time should be allowed for interviews. The adminis-tration of self-report questionnaires should not follow the sameprocedure as with the general population, in which each itemelicits a single response and the next item is then addressed.Because of the need for rephrasing and probing, several question-and-answer turns may be necessary for each item to check how thequestion was understood, to find a phrasing the person under-stands, and to allow for perseveration and unclear responses. It isbetter to build such options into the questionnaire rather thanrequire the interviewer to come up with them spontaneously. Whenprobing produces conflicting information, the interviewer shouldrecord both responses and discuss them with colleagues after theinterview, rather than attempt to make an arbitrary judgment on thespot. In some cases, a consistent, scoreable answer may not bepossible.

The difference between expressive and receptive abilities shouldbe recognized because professionals may often overestimate thecomprehension of people who appear to have good expressivelanguage abilities. Further research is also needed on techniques ofobtaining information from those who can understand questionsbut have limited expressive abilities. For this group, the problemsof checking yes-no responses for understanding are particularlyacute.

Tully and Cahill (1984), in their report on police interviewing,suggested an introductory period during which responses to ques-tions for which the answer is already known by the interviewer canbe elicited. The interviewer can check how easily the intervieweewill agree to suggestions, how he or she exhibits uncertainty, andassure him or her that it is all right to say that he or she does not

This

doc

umen

t is c

opyr

ight

ed b

y th

e A

mer

ican

Psy

chol

ogic

al A

ssoc

iatio

n or

one

of i

ts a

llied

pub

lishe

rs.

This

arti

cle

is in

tend

ed so

lely

for t

he p

erso

nal u

se o

f the

indi

vidu

al u

ser a

nd is

not

to b

e di

ssem

inat

ed b

road

ly.

Page 13: Methodological Issues in Interviewing and Using Self ... · INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 321 Socially Reflexive Questions Questions asking people how they think others

INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 331

Table 2Summary of Difficulties and Possible Actions

Problem area Specific problems Possible action

Question content Quantitative judgments

Time questionsComparisonsSocially reflexive questionsAbstract or general

concepts

Psychiatric symptomsIrrelevant content

LabelsSensitive content

Question phrasing Negative wordings

Modifiers

Subject-object confusionand passive phrasings

Question not understood

Response format Yes-no questions

Multiple-choice format

Understanding orclassifying responses

Psychometricproperties

Factor structure

Target population

Avoid Likert scales and questions of degree and frequency.Use pretest screening questions about concrete events for

which frequency is known.Use significant events as markers.Ask about each element separately.Check meaning of answer.Check meaning of answer.Use concrete situations or events.Allow that people may not be able to make generalized

judgments.Avoid symptoms difficult to understand or describe.Elicit item content from appropriate populations. Beware of

basing scales on those for general population.Check understanding.Be aware of the difficulties. Ask about specifics rather than

generalities. Stress that information will not be sharedwith carers.

Avoid adding no and not to positive phrasings. Usenegative form of words.

Avoid modifiers, particularly at end of sentences. Checkmeaning.

Be aware of questions where this is possible—checkmeaning.

Keep question structure simple, avoid technical vocabulary.Write alternative phrasings and probes into questionnaires.

Avoid modifiers and complex question structures.Include "don't know" option.Check meaning by asking for examples and probing further

(use scripted probes).Have preinterview to check how person exhibits

uncertainty or responds to false suggestions.Break down into two either-or stages.Use pictures only if meaning is clear.Avoid Likert scales and offering multiple options.Tape record interviews.Allow scorer to record "other/uncodeable" responses in

questionnaires.Return to question later.

Do not assume that this is the same as for the generalpopulation.

New questionnaires must clearly report sample selectioncriteria. Use tests of verbal ability.

know. They also suggested that two officers be involved so thatone can check that the other is not driving the interview and thatthe importance of accuracy be repeatedly stressed. Introductoryperiods can also be used to check whether interviewees can makeestimates of frequency or degree.

Scale developers need to pay more attention to the validation ofscales in terms of both content development and convergence withalternative indicators. The dangers of using questionnaires devel-oped for the general population, or modifications of these, need tobe recognized because they may be based on inappropriate contentor factor structures. Selection criteria should be given in detail sothat clinicians and researchers can decide whether the question-naire is appropriate for their respondents (Aman, 1991). A ques-tionnaire or interview that excludes a lot of people is at least betterthan none, but only if this is made clear. Scale developers should

identify tests of verbal ability that can be used to specify thereceptive language necessary for participation. To date, the issueof how to select who can and cannot take a test has rarely beendirectly addressed, and more research and discussion is needed onthis topic. In certain cases, such as psychiatric diagnoses, it mightbe worthwhile to use both self-report and informant versions andaccept whichever provides a positive identification (Moss et al.,1997; Patel et al., 1993).

Finally, questionnaires should allow for missing or uncodeableanswers, rather than requiring the interviewer to fill in every item,and the proportion of responses that are deemed valid should bereported. Yes-no questions should also include "don't know"options. Tape recording is useful, both to piece together narratives thatdo not closely follow the questions asked and because participantsmay be difficult to understand at the time because of unclear speech.

This

doc

umen

t is c

opyr

ight

ed b

y th

e A

mer

ican

Psy

chol

ogic

al A

ssoc

iatio

n or

one

of i

ts a

llied

pub

lishe

rs.

This

arti

cle

is in

tend

ed so

lely

for t

he p

erso

nal u

se o

f the

indi

vidu

al u

ser a

nd is

not

to b

e di

ssem

inat

ed b

road

ly.

Page 14: Methodological Issues in Interviewing and Using Self ... · INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 321 Socially Reflexive Questions Questions asking people how they think others

332 FINLAY AND LYONS

Conclusions

This article has reviewed methodological issues in using inter-views and self-report questionnaires with people with mental re-tardation and outlined a number of question structures and contentsthat have been found to be problematic. We have argued thatbecause many self-report questionnaires include such questions,more attention needs to be paid to establishing the validity of suchmeasures and to clearly defining the population for which theinstrument is designed. A range of strategies to enhance the effec-tiveness of interviewing and using self-report questionnaires in thispopulation was also reviewed. Although the review necessarilyfocused on problems with interviewing in this population, its

purpose was to allow the voices of people with mental retardationto be heard more clearly. It should be stressed that the difficultiesdescribed above do not apply to the population as a whole. Thecategory of people with mental retardation is heterogeneous, andmany people can participate in interviews and answer self-reportquestionnaires without these problems. However, researchers andclinicians must be aware of the sources of potential difficulties topromote the inclusion of as many people as possible and toenhance the validity of interviewing techniques.

References

Abbeduto, L., & Hagerman, R. J. (1997). Language and communication inFragile X syndrome. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabili-ties Research Reviews, 3, 313-322.

Aman, M. G. (1991). Assessing psychopathology and behavior problems inpersons with mental retardation: A review of available instruments.Rockville, MD: National Institute of Mental Health.

Aman, M. G., Watson, J. E., Singh, N. N., Turbott, S. H., & Wilsher, C. P.(1986). Psychometric and demographic characteristics of the Psychopa-thology Instrument for Mentally Retarded Adults. PsychopharmacologyBulletin, 22, 1072-1076.

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical man-ual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

Antaki, C. (1998). Interviewing persons with a learning disability: Howsetting lower standards may inflate well-being scores. Qualitative HealthResearch, 9, 437-454.

Antaki, C., & Rapley, M. (1996). Questions and answers to psychologicalassessment schedules: Hidden troubles in "quality of life" interviews.Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 40, 421-437.

Ballinger, B. R., Armstrong, J., Presly, A. S., & Reid, A. H. (1975). Use ofa standardized psychiatric interview in mentally handicapped patients.British Journal of Psychiatry, 127, 540-544.

Barlow, J., & Kirby, N. (1991). Residential satisfaction of persons withintellectual disability living in an institution or in the community.Australia and New Zealand Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 17,7-23.

Barnett, J. (1998). Sensitive questions and response effects: An evaluation.Journal of Managerial Psychology, 13, 63-76.

Bartlett, C., & Bunning, K. (1997). The importance of communicationpartnerships: A study to investigate the communicative interactionsbetween staff and adults with learning disabilities. British Journal ofLearning Disabilities, 25, 148-152.

Beaumont, J. G., Marjoribanks, J., Flury, S., & Lintern, T. (1999). Ascreening test of auditory comprehension for individuals with severephysical disability. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38, 1-4.

Bedrosian, J. L. (1993). Making minds meet: Assessment of conversationaltopic in adults with mild to moderate mental retardation. Topics inLanguage Disorders, 13, 36-46.

Benson, B. A., & Ivins, J. (1992). Anger, depression and self-concept in

adults with mental retardation. Journal of Intellectual Disability Re-search, 36, 169-175.

Bercovici, S. (1981). Qualitative methods and cultural perspectives in thestudy of deinstitutionalization. In R. H. Bruininks, C. E. Meyers, B. B.Sigford, & K. C. Lakin (Eds.), Deinstitutionalization and communityadjustment of mentally retarded people. Washington, DC: AmericanAssociation of Mental Deficiency.

Beveridge, M., Conti-Ramsden, G., & Leudar, I. (1997). Language andcommunication in people with learning disabilities. London: Routledge.

Biklen, S. K., & Moseley, C. R. (1988). "Are you retarded?" "No, I'mCatholic": Qualitative methods in the study of people with severehandicaps. Journal for the Association of People with Severe Handi-caps, 13, 155-162.

Block, J. (1965). The challenge of response sets. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Booth, T., & Booth, W. (1994a). The use of depth interviewing withvulnerable subjects: Lessons from a research study of parents withlearning difficulties. Social Science and Medicine, 39, 415-424.

Booth, T., & Booth, W. (1994b). Parenting under pressure: Mothers andfathers with learning difficulties. Buckingham, England: Open Univer-sity Press.

Booth, T., & Booth, W. (1996). Sounds of silence: Narrative research withinarticulate subjects. Disability and Society, 11, 55-69.

Botswick, D. H., & Foss, G. (1981). Obtaining consumer input: Twostrategies for identifying and ranking the problems of mentally retardedyoung adults. Education and Training of the Mentally Retarded, 16,207-212.

Brinton, B., & Fujiki, M. (1993). Communication skills and communityintegration in adults with mild to moderate mental retardation. Topics inLanguage Disorders, 13, 9-19.

Brinton, B., & Fujiki, M. (1994). Ability of institutionalized andcommunity-based adults with retardation to respond to questions in aninterview context. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 369-377.

Burnett, P. C. (1989). Assessing satisfaction in people with mental retar-dation living in community-based residential facilities. Australian Dis-ability Review, 1, 14-19.

Calhoun, G., Whitley, J. D., & Ansolabehere, E. M. (1978). An investi-gation of the Goodenough-Harris Drawing Test and the (Coopersmith)Self-Esteem Inventory. Educational and Psychological Measure-ment, 38, 1229-1232.

Castles, E. E., & Glass, C. R. (1986). Training in social and interpersonalproblem solving skills for mildly and moderately mentally retardedadults. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 91, 35-42.

Cattermoie, M., Jahoda, A., & Markova, I. (1990). Quality of life forpeople with learning difficulties moving to community homes. Disabil-ity, Handicap and Society, 5, 137-152.

Chadsey-Rusch, J., DeStefano, L., O'Reilly, M., Gonzalez, P., & Collet-Klingenberg, C. (1992). Assessing the loneliness of workers with mentalretardation. Mental Retardation, 30, 85-92.

Chassin, L., Stager, S. F., & Young, R. D. (1985). Self-labeling byeducably mentally retarded high school students in their mainstream andspecial education classes. American Journal of Community Psychol-ogy, 13, 449-465.

Chiu, L. (1990). Self-esteem of gifted, normal, and mildly mentally hand-icapped children. Psychology in the Schools, 27, 263-268.

Clare, I. C. H., & Gudjonsson, G. H. (1995). The vulnerability of suspectswith mental retardation during police interviews: A review and experi-mental study of decision-making. Mental Handicap Research, 8, 110—128.

Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity: Basis issues inobjective scale development. Psychological Assessment, 7, 309-319.

Coelho, R. J., & Saunders, J. L. (1996). Diagnostic implications of duaV

This

doc

umen

t is c

opyr

ight

ed b

y th

e A

mer

ican

Psy

chol

ogic

al A

ssoc

iatio

n or

one

of i

ts a

llied

pub

lishe

rs.

This

arti

cle

is in

tend

ed so

lely

for t

he p

erso

nal u

se o

f the

indi

vidu

al u

ser a

nd is

not

to b

e di

ssem

inat

ed b

road

ly.

Page 15: Methodological Issues in Interviewing and Using Self ... · INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 321 Socially Reflexive Questions Questions asking people how they think others

INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 333

diagnosis: Mental retardation and mental illness. Journal of AppliedRehabilitation Counselling, 27, 19-24.

Conroy, J. W., & Bradley, V. J. (1985). The Pennhurst Longitudinal Study:A report of five years of research and analysis. Philadelphia: TempleUniversity Developmental Disabilities Center.

Dagnan, D., Dennis, S., & Wood, H. (1994). A pilot study of the satisfac-tion of people with learning disabilities with the services they receivefrom community psychology services. British Journal of DevelopmentalDisabilities, XL, 38-44.

Dagnan, D., & Sandhu, S. (1999). Social comparison, self-esteem anddepression in people with intellectual disability. Journal of IntellectualDisability Research, 43, 372-379.

Davies, C. A., & Jenkins, R. (1997). "She has different fits to me": Howpeople with learning difficulties see themselves. Disability and Soci-ety, 12, 95-109.

Dempster, F. N., & Corkill, A. J. (1999). Interference and inhibition incognition and behavior: Unifying themes for educational psychology.Educational Psychology Review, 11, 1-88.

Dent, H. R. (1986). An experimental study of the effectiveness of differenttechniques of questioning mentally handicapped child witnesses. BritishJournal of Clinical Psychology, 25, 13-17.

Duckworth, M. S., Radhakrishnan, M. E., Nolan, M. E., & Fraser, W. I.(1993). Initial encounters between people with a mild mental handicapand psychiatrists: An investigation of a method of evaluating interviewskills. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 37, 263-276.

Dudley, J. R., Calhoun, M. L., Ahlgrim-Delzell, L., & Conroy, J. (1997).A consumer satisfaction survey of people with mental retardation andmental illness. Psychiatric Services, 48, 1075-1077.

Edmonson, B., McCombs, K., & Wish, J. (1979). What retarded adultsbelieve about sex. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 84, 11-18.

Edmonson, B., & Wish, J. (1975). Sex knowledge and attitudes of mod-erately retarded males. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 80,172-179.

Eysenck, S. B. G. (1965). Manual of the Eysenck-Withers PersonalityInventory for Subnormal Subjects (50-80 I.Q.). London: University ofLondon Press.

Finlay, M., & Lyons, E. (1998). Social identity and people with learningdifficulties: Implications for self-advocacy groups. Disability and Soci-ety, 13, 37-51.

Finlay, W. M. L., & Lyons, E. (2000). Social categorizations, socialcomparisons and stigma: Presentations of self in people with learningdifficulties. British Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 129-146.

Finlay, W. M. L., & Lyons, E. (in press). Acquiescence in interviews withpeople with mental retardation. Mental Retardation.

Flynn, M. C. (1986). Adults who are mentally handicapped as consumers:Issues and guidelines for interviewing. Journal of Mental DeficiencyResearch, 30, 369-377.

Flynn, M. C., Reeves, D., Whelan, E., & Speake, B. (1985). The devel-opment of a measure for determining the mentally handicapped adult'stolerance of rules and recognitions of status. Journal of Practical Ap-proaches to Developmental Handicap, 9, 18-24.

Foster, S. L., & Cone, J. D. (1995). Validity issues in clinical assessment.Psychological Assessment, 7, 248-260.

Foxx, R. M., Faw, G. D., Taylor, S., Davis, P. K., & Fulia, R. (1993)."Would I be able to ... ?" Teaching clients to assess the availability oftheir community living life style preferences. American Journal onMental Retardation, 98, 235-248.

Fraser, W. I., Leudar, I., Gray, J., & Campbell, I. (1986). Psychiatric andbehavior disturbance in mental handicap. Journal of Mental DeficiencyResearch, 30, 49-57.

Gerjuoy, I. R., & Winters, J. J. (1966). Lateral preference for identicalgeometric forms: Retardates. Perceptions and Psychophysics, 1, 104-106.

Goodley, D. (1996). Tales of hidden lives: A critical examination of life

history research with people who have learning difficulties. Disabilityand Society, 11, 333-348.

Gough, P. B. (1965). Grammatical transformations and speed of under-standing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4, 107-111.

Gow, L., & Balla, J. (1994). Methodological difficulties encountered indetermining the service needs of a "hidden population." Journal ofIntellectual Disability Research, 38, 329-339.

Gowans, F., & Hulbert, C. (1983). Self-concept assessment of mentallyhandicapped adults: A review. Mental Handicap, 11, 121-123.

Gudjonsson, G. H. (1990). The relationship of intellectual skills to sug-gestibility, compliance and acquiescence. Personality and IndividualDifferences, 11, 227-231.

Harner, C. J., & Heal, L. W. (1993). The Multifaceted Lifestyle Satisfac-tion Scale (MLSS): Psychometric properties of an interview schedule forassessing personal satisfaction of adults with limited intelligence. Re-search in Developmental Disabilities, 14, 221-236.

Haynes, S. N., Richard, D. C. S., & Kubany, E. S. (1995). Content validityin psychological assessment: A functional approach to concepts andmethods. Psychological Assessment, 7, 238-247.

Heal, L. W., & Chadsey-Rusch, J. (1985). The Lifestyle Satisfaction Scale(LSS): Assessing individual's satisfaction with residence, communitysetting, and associated services. Applied Research in Mental Retarda-tion, 6, 475-490.

Heal, L. W., & Sigelman, C. K. (1995). Response biases in interviews ofindividuals with limited mental ability. Journal of Intellectual DisabilityResearch, 39, 331-340.

Helsel, W. J., & Matson, J. L. (1988). The relationship of depression tosocial skills and intellectual functioning in mentally retarded adults.Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 32, 411-418.

Jiranek, D., & Kirby, N. (1990). The job satisfaction and/or psychologicalwell being of young adults with mental retardation and nondisabledyoung adults in either sheltered employment, competitive employmentor unemployment. Australia and New Zealand Journal of Developmen-tal Disabilities, 16, 133-148.

Kabzems, V. (1985). The use of self-report measures with mentally re-tarded individuals. Mental Retardation and Learning Disability Bulle-tin, 13, 106-114.

Kazdin, A. E. (1995). Preparing and evaluating research reports. Psycho-logical Assessment, 7, 228 -237.

Kazdin, A. E., Matson, J. L., & Senatore, V. (1983). Assessment ofdepression in mentally retarded adults. American Journal of Psychiatry,140, 1040-1043.

Kebbell, M. R., & Hatton, C. (1999). People with mental retardation aswitnesses in court: A review. Mental Retardation, 37, 179-187.

Lindsay, W. R., & Michie, A. M. (1988). Adaptation of the Zung self-rating anxiety scale for people with a mental handicap. Journal ofMental Deficiency Research, 32, 485-490.

Lindsay, W. R., Michie, A. M., Baty, F. J., Smith, A. H. W., & Miller, S.(1994). The consistency of reports about feelings and emotions frompeople with intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual DisabilityResearch, 38, 61-66.

Lindsey, P. (1994). Assessing the ability of adults with mental retardationto give direct consent for residential placements: A follow-up study forthe Consent Screening Interview. Education and Training in MentalRetardation and Developmental Disabilities, 29, 155-164.

Loper, A. B., & Reeve, R. E. (1983). Response biases on a locus of controlmeasure by learning disabled children. Journal of Abnormal ChildPsychology, 11, 537-548.

Lowe, K., & de Paiva, S. (1988). Canvassing the views of people with amental handicap. Irish Journal of Psychology, 9, 220-234.

Lunsky, Y., & Benson, B. (1997). Reliability of ratings on consumers withmental retardation and their staff on multiple measures of social support.American Journal on Mental Retardation, 102, 280-284.

Lyall, I., Holland, A. J., Collins, S., & Styles, P. (1995). Incidence of

This

doc

umen

t is c

opyr

ight

ed b

y th

e A

mer

ican

Psy

chol

ogic

al A

ssoc

iatio

n or

one

of i

ts a

llied

pub

lishe

rs.

This

arti

cle

is in

tend

ed so

lely

for t

he p

erso

nal u

se o

f the

indi

vidu

al u

ser a

nd is

not

to b

e di

ssem

inat

ed b

road

ly.

Page 16: Methodological Issues in Interviewing and Using Self ... · INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 321 Socially Reflexive Questions Questions asking people how they think others

334 FINLAY AND LYONS

persons with a learning disability detained in police custody. A needsassessment for service development. Medicine, Science and the Law, 35,61-71.

Malik, P. B., Ashton-Schaeffer, C., & Kleiber, D. A. (1991). Interviewingyoung adults with mental retardation: A seldom used research method.Therapeutic Recreation Journal, 25, 60-73.

Manikam, R., Matson, J. L., Coe, D. A., & Hillman, N. (1995). Adolescentdepression: Relationships of self-report to intellectual and adaptivefunctioning. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 16, 349-364.

March, P. (1992). Do photographs help adults with severe mental handi-caps to make choices. British Journal of Mental Subnormality, XXXVII,122-128.

Matikka, L. M., & Vesala, H. T. (1997). Acquiescence in quality-of-lifeinterviews with adults who have mental retardation. Mental Retarda-tion, 35, 75-82.

Matson, J. L., & Frame, C. L. (1986). Psychopathology among mentallyretarded children and adolescents. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Matson, J. L., Kazdin, A. E., & Senatore, V. (1984). Psychometric prop-erties of the Psychopathology Instrument for Mentally Retarded Adults.Applied Research in Mental Retardation, 5, 81-89.

McConkey, R., Morris, I., & Purcell, M. (1999). Communications betweenstaff and adults with intellectual disabilities in naturally occurring set-tings. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 43, 194-205.

McEvoy, J. (1989). Investigating the concept of death in adults who arementally handicapped. British Journal of Mental Subnormality, XXXV,115-121.

McGrew, K. S., Bruininks, R. H., Thurlow, M. L., & Lewis, D. R. (1992).Empirical analysis of multidimensional measures of community adjust-ment for young adults with mental retardation. American Journal ofMental Retardation, 96, 475-487.

McLean, L. K., Brady, N. C., & McLean, J. E. (1996). Reported commu-nication abilities of individuals with severe mental retardation. AmericanJournal of Mental Retardation, 100, 580-591.

McVilly, K. R. (1995). Interviewing people with a learning disability abouttheir residential service. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23,138-142.

Moss, S., Ibbotson, B., Prosser, H., Goldberg, D., Patel, P., & Simpson, N.(1997). Validity of the PAS-ADD for detecting psychiatric symptoms inadults with learning disability (mental retardation). Social Psychiatryand Psychiatric Epidemiology, 32, 344-354.

Moss, S., Patel, P., Prosser, H., Goldberg, D., Simpson, N., Rowe, S., &Lucchino, R. (1993). Psychiatric morbidity in older people with mod-erate and severe learning disability I: Development and reliability of thepatient interview (PAS-ADD). British Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 471-480.

Moss, S., Prosser, H., & Goldberg, D. (1996). Validity of the schizophreniadiagnosis of the Psychiatric Assessment Schedule for Adults with De-velopmental Disability (PAS-ADD). British Journal of Psychiatry, 168,359-367.

Moss, S., Prosser, H., Ibbotson, B., & Goldberg, D. (1996). Respondentand informant accounts of psychiatric symptoms in a sample of patientswith learning disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 40,457-465.

Neumayer, R., & Bleasdale, M. (1996). Personal lifestyle preferences ofpeople with an intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual and Devel-opmental Disability, 21, 91-114.

Nooe, R. M. (1977). Measuring self-concepts of mentally retarded adults.Social Work, 22, 320-322.

Pack, R. P., Wallander, J. L., & Browne, D. (1998). Health risk behaviorsof African American adolescents with mild mental retardation: Preva-lence depends on measurement method. American Journal on MentalRetardation, 102, 409-420.

PateJ, P., Goldberg, D., & Moss, S. (1993). Psychiatric morbidity in older

people with moderate and severe learning disability II: The prevalencestudy. British Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 481-491.

Perlman, N. B., Ericson, K. I., Esses, V. B., & Isaacs, B. J. (1994). Thedevelopmentally handicapped witness: Competency as a function ofquestion format. Law and Human Behavior, 18, 171-187.

Prosser, H., & Bromley, J. (1998). Interviewing people with intellectualdisabilities. In E. Emerson, C. Hatton, J. Bromley, & A. Caine (Eds.),Clinical psychology and people with intellectual disabilities. Chichester,England: Wiley.

Prout, H. T., & Schaefer, B. M. (1985). Self-reports of depression bycommunity-based mildly mentally retarded adults. American Journal ofMental Deficiency, 90, 220-222.

Prout, H. T., & Strohmer, D. C. (1994). Assessment in counseling andpsychotherapy. In D. C. Strohmer & H. T. Prout (Eds.), Counseling andpsychotherapy with persons with mental retardation and borderlineintelligence. Brandon, VT: Clinical Psychology.

Rapley, M. (1995). Black swans: Conversation analysis of interviews withpeople with learning disabilities. Clinical Psychology Forum, 84, 17-23.

Rapley, M., & Antaki, C. (1996). A conversation analysis of the "acqui-escence" of people with learning disabilities. Journal of Community andApplied Social Psychology, 6, 207-227.

Reiss, S., & Benson, B. A. (1985). Psychosocial correlates of depression inmentally retarded adults: I. Minimal social support and stigmatization.American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 89, 331-337.

Reynolds, W. M. (1979). The utility of multiple-choice test formats withmildly retarded adolescents. Educational and Psychological Measure-ment, 39, 325-332.

Reynolds, W. M., & Baker, J. A. (1988). Assessment of depression inpersons with mental retardation. American Journal on Mental Retarda-tion, 93, 93-103.

Reynolds, W. M., & Miller, K. L. (1985). Depression and learned help-lessness in mentally retarded and nonmentally retarded adolescents: Aninitial investigation. Applied Research in Mental Retardation, 6, 295-306.

Rich, C. E., Barcikowski, R. S., & Witner, J. M. (1979). The factorialvalidity of the Piers-Harris children's self-concept scale for a sample ofintermediate-level EMR students enrolled in elementary school. Educa-tional and Psychological Measurement, 39, 485-490.

Riggen, K., & Ulrich, D. (1993). The effects of sport participation onindividuals with mental retardation. Adapted Physical Quarterly, 10,42-51.

Rodgers, J. (1999). Trying to get it right: Undertaking research involvingpeople with learning difficulties. Disability and Society, 14, 421-433.

Rojahn, J., Warren, V. J., & Ohringer, S. (1994). A comparison of assess-ment methods for depression in mental retardation. Journal of Autismand Developmental Disorders, 24, 305-313.

Rybolt, G. A. (1969). Stability characteristics of the semantic differentialresponses of the mentally retarded. Psychological Reports, 24, 103-108.

Sabsay, S. L., & Kernan, K. T. (1983). Communicative design in thespeech of mildly retarded adults. In K. T. Kernan, M. J. Begab, & R. B.Edgerton (Eds.), Environments and behavior: The adaptation of men-tally retarded persons. Baltimore: University Park Press.

Schalock, R. L., Keith, K. D., Hoffman, K., & Karan, O. C. (1989). Qualityof life: Its measurement and use. Mental Retardation, 27, 25—31.

Schurr, K. T., Joiner, L. M., & Towne, R. C. (1970). Self-concept researchon the mentally retarded: A review of empirical studies. Mental Retar-dation, 8, 39-43.

Scott, S. (1994). Mental retardation. In M. Rutter, E. Taylor, & L. Hersov(Eds.), Child and adolescent psychiatry: Modem approaches. Oxford,England: Blackwell.

Senatore., V., Matson, J. L., & Kazdin, A. E. (1985). An inventory to assesspsychopathology of mentally retarded adults. American Journal of Men-tal Deficiency, 89, 459-466.

Shanly, A., & Rose, J. (1993). A consumer survey of adults with learning

This

doc

umen

t is c

opyr

ight

ed b

y th

e A

mer

ican

Psy

chol

ogic

al A

ssoc

iatio

n or

one

of i

ts a

llied

pub

lishe

rs.

This

arti

cle

is in

tend

ed so

lely

for t

he p

erso

nal u

se o

f the

indi

vidu

al u

ser a

nd is

not

to b

e di

ssem

inat

ed b

road

ly.

Page 17: Methodological Issues in Interviewing and Using Self ... · INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 321 Socially Reflexive Questions Questions asking people how they think others

INTERVIEWING PEOPLE WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 335

disabilities currently doing work experience: Their satisfaction withwork and wishes for the future. Mental Handicap Research, 6, 250-262.

Shaw, J. A., & Budd, E. C. (1982). Determinants of acquiescence andnaysaying of mentally retarded persons. American Journal of MentalDeficiency, 87, 108-110.

Sigelman, C. K., & Budd, E. C. (1986). Pictures as an aid in questioningmentally retarded persons. Rehabilitation Counselling Bulletin, 29, 173—181.

Sigelman, C. K., Budd, E. C., Spanhel, C. L., & Schoenrock, C. J. (1981a).Asking questions of retarded persons: A comparison of yes-no andeither-or formats. Applied Research in Mental Retardation, 2, 347-357.

Sigelman, C. K., Budd, E. C., Spanhel, C. L., & Schoenrock, C. J. (1981b).When in doubt say yes: Acquiescence in interviews with mentallyretarded persons. Mental Retardation, 19, 53-58.

Sigelman, C. K., Budd, E. C., Winer, J. L., Schoenrock, C. J., & Martin,P. W. (1982). Evaluating alternative techniques of questioning mentallyretarded persons. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 86, 511-518.

Sigelman, C. K., Schoenrock, C. J., Winer, J. L., Spanhel, C. L., Hromes,S. G., Martin, R. W., Budd, E. C., & Bensberg, G. J. (1981). Issues ininterviewing mentally retarded persons: An empirical study. In R. H.Bruininks, C. E. Meyers, B. B. Sigford, & K. C. Lakin (Eds.), Deinsti-tutionalization and community adjustment of mentally retarded people.Washington, DC: American Association of Mental Deficiency.

Sigelman, C. K., Winer, J. L., & Schoenrock, C. J. (1982). The respon-siveness of mentally retarded persons to questions. Education and Train-ing of the Mentally Retarded, 17, 120-124.

Silon, E. L., & Harter, S. (1985). Assessment of perceived competence,motivational orientation, and anxiety in segregated and mainstreamededucable mentally retarded children. Journal of Educational Psychol-ogy, 77, 217-230.

Simons, K. (1992). Sticking up for yourself. York, England: Joseph Rown-tree Foundation.

Sinnott-Oswald, M., Gliner, J. A., & Spencer, K. C. (1991, December).Supported and sheltered employment: Quality of life issues amongworkers with disabilities. Education and Training in Mental Retarda-tion, 388-397.

Slobin, D. I. (1966). Grammatical transformations and sentence compre-hension in childhood and adulthood. Journal of Verbal Learning andVerbal Behavior, 5, 219-227.

Smith, P., & McCarthy, G. (1996). The development of a semi-structuredinterview to investigate the attachment-related experiences of adultswith learning disabilities. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 24,154-160.

Smith, S. A. (1993). Confusing the terms "guilty" and "not guilty":Implications for alleged offenders with mental retardation. Psychologi-cal Reports, 73, 675-678.

Smyley, S. R., & Ellsworth, K. (1997). Interviewing clients: A project toexplore client views about a change in service provision. British Journalof Learning Disabilities, 25, 64—67.

Sovner, R., & Hurley, A. D. (1986). Four factors affecting the diagnosis ofpsychiatric disorders in mentally retarded persons. Psychiatric Aspectsof Mental Retardation Reviews, 5, 45-49.

Stancliffe, R. J. (1995). Assessing opportunities for choice-making: Acomparison of self- and staff-reports. American Journal on MentalRetardation, 99, 418-429.

Strohmer, D. C., Prout, H. T., & Gorsky, J. (1994). The development of apersonality inventory for adolescents and adults with mild mental retar-dation and borderline intelligence. Assessment in Rehabilitation andExceptionality, 1, 78-89.

Szivos-Bach, S. E. (1993). Social comparisons, stigma and mainstreaming:The self-esteem of young adults with a mild mental handicap. MentalHandicap Research, 6, 217-236.

Taylor, S. J., & Bogdan, R. (1981). A qualitative approach to the study ofcommunity adjustment. In R. H. Bruininks, C. E. Meyers, B. B. Sigford,& K. C. Lakin (Eds.), Deinstitutionalization and community adjustmentof mentally retarded people. Washington, DC: American Association ofMental Deficiency.

Thompson, R. J., Lampron, L. B., Johnson, D. F., & Eckstein, T. L. (1990).Behavior problems in children with the presenting problem of poorschool performance. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 15, 3-20.

Thorin, E., Browning, P., & Irvin, L. (1988). Measuring knowledge ofcitizenship rights and responsibilities. Research in Developmental Dis-abilities, 9, 85-92.

Todd, S., & Shearn, J. (1997). Family secrets and dilemmas of status:Parental management of the disclosure of "learning disability." Disabil-ity and Society, 12, 341-366.

Tully, B., & Cahill, D. (1984). Police interviewing of the mentally hand-icapped: An experimental study. London: Police Foundation.

Tymchuk, A. J. (1991). Self-concepts of mothers who show mental retar-dation. Psychological Reports, 68, 503-510.

Tymchuk, A. J. (1993). Symptoms of psychopathology in mothers withmental handicap. Mental Handicap Research, 6, 18-35.

van Minnen, A., Savelsberg, P. M., & Hoogduin, K. A. L. (1994). A Dutchversion of the Psychopathology Inventory for Mentally Retarded Adults(PIMRA). Research in Developmental Disabilities, 15, 269-278.

Voelker, S. L., Shore, D. L., Brownmore, C., Hill, L. C., Miller, L. T., &Perry, J. (1990). Validity of self-reports of adaptive-behavior skills byadults with mental retardation. Mental Retardation, 28, 305-309.

Wadsworth, J. S., & Harper, D. C. (1991). Increasing the reliability ofself-report by adults with moderate mental retardation. Journal of theAssociation for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 16, 228-232.

Watson, J. E., Aman, M. G., & Singh, N. N. (1988). The PsychopathologyInstrument for Mentally Retarded Adults: Psychometric characteristics,factor structure, and relationship to subject characteristics. Research inDevelopmental Disabilities, 9, 277-290.

Wehmeyer, M. L. (1994). Reliability and acquiescence in the measurementof locus of control with adolescents and adults with mental retardation.Psychological Reports, 75, 527-537.

Wehmeyer, M. L., & Metzler, C. A. (1995). How self-determined arepeople with mental retardation? The National Consumer Survey. MentalRetardation, 33, 111-119.

Wilgosh, L., & Barry, M. (1983). Assessment of vocational interests ofTMH students. The Mental Retardation and Learning Disability Bulle-tin, 11, 79-84.

Wyngaarden, M. (1981). Interviewing mentally retarded persons: Issuesand strategies. In R. H. Bruininks, C. E. Meyers, B. B. Sigford, & K. C.Lakin (Eds.), Deinstitutionalization and community adjustment of men-tally retarded people. Washington, DC: American Association of MentalDeficiency.

Zetlin, A. G., Heriot, M. J., & Turner, J. L. (1985). Self-concept measure-ment in retarded adults: A micro-analysis of responses. Applied Re-search in Mental Retardation, 6, 113-125.

Received May 27, 1999Revision received January 30, 2001

Accepted March 19, 2001

This

doc

umen

t is c

opyr

ight

ed b

y th

e A

mer

ican

Psy

chol

ogic

al A

ssoc

iatio

n or

one

of i

ts a

llied

pub

lishe

rs.

This

arti

cle

is in

tend

ed so

lely

for t

he p

erso

nal u

se o

f the

indi

vidu

al u

ser a

nd is

not

to b

e di

ssem

inat

ed b

road

ly.