McCloskey Wible Cohen (1988)

download McCloskey Wible Cohen (1988)

of 11

Transcript of McCloskey Wible Cohen (1988)

  • 8/10/2019 McCloskey Wible Cohen (1988)

    1/11

    Journal

    o

    Experimental

    Psvchology:

    General

    1988 Vol. 117,No. 2,

    171-'l81

    Copyright 1988hythe Am :anPsychological Association, Inc.

    OD96-3445/88/$00.75

    Is

    There a Special Flashbulb-Memory Mechanism?

    Michael McCloskey, CynthiaG.

    Wible,

    andNealJ.Cohen

    Johns

    Hopkins

    University

    Many people

    reportvivid

    recollections

    of thecircumstancesin which they

    learned

    of

    major

    events, suchas theassassinationofPresident Kennedy,or theassassinationattempton President

    Reagan. Brown and Kulik (1977) argued that

    this

    phenomenon, which they labeledflashbulb

    memory,implies

    the

    existence

    of a

    special memory mechanism that creates

    a

    detailed, permanent

    record of the individual's experience when triggered by an event exceedingcriterial levelsof

    surprise and consequentiality. In this article we evaluate the special-mechanism hypothesis,

    arguingon empirical and logical grounds that the flashbulb-memory phenomenon does not

    motivate the

    postulation

    of a special flashbulb-memory mechanism. We suggest instead that

    flashbulbmemories should be viewed as

    products

    of

    "ordinary"

    memory mechanisms, and hence

    as phenomenathatmayoffer insights into the nature of these mechanisms.

    Where were youwhenyou learned of the assassination of

    John F. Kennedy, the shooting of Ronald Reagan, or the

    explosion of the space shuttle

    Challenged

    How did you hear

    about

    the event? What were you doing at the time? What

    were your first thoughts upon hearing the news? Many people

    report

    that

    they

    can

    remember

    vividlythe

    circumstances

    in

    whichthey learned of certain major public or personal events,

    afeatof memory that has intrigued researchers since at least

    the turn of the century (e.g., Brewer, 1986; Brown & Kulik,

    1977;Colegrove,

    1899; Linton, 1975; Neisser, 1982, 1986a,

    1986b; Pillemer,1984;Rubin & Kozin, 1984; Thompson &

    Cowan, 1986; Winograd&

    Killinger,

    1983;

    Yarmey

    &

    Bull,

    1978).

    Brown

    and Kulik(1977)suggested that this phenomenon,

    whichthey labeledflashbulb memory, implies the existence

    ofaspecial memory mechanism. They argued thatthespecial

    mechanism, when triggered

    by an

    event exceeding criterial

    levels of surprise and consequentiality, creates a permanent

    record of the contents of awareness for the period immediately

    surrounding

    the

    shocking experience.

    The

    special-mechanism

    hypothesis has been the subject of considerable discussion in

    recent years, with some authors endorsing

    the

    hypothesis

    and

    others noting potential problems. Pillemer

    (1984),

    for exam-

    ple, argued

    in

    favor

    of the

    hypothesis

    on the

    basis

    of

    data

    concerning subjects' recollections of the circumstances in

    which they learned about

    the

    assassination attempt

    on

    Presi-

    dent Reagan. In contrast, Neisser(1982)raised doubts about

    the accuracy of flashbulb memories, and also pointed out

    problems with several other aspects of the Brown and Kulik

    hypothesis. Furthermore, Rubin and Kozin(1984)questioned

    whether the

    characteristics

    of flashbulb memoriesclearlyset

    these memories apartfromother autobiographical memories.

    Although

    a number of important issues

    have

    been raised,

    itremains unclear whether the flashbulb-memory phenome-

    We thank Roberta

    Goodman-Schulman,

    Kathi Hirsh, Brenda

    Rapp,Scott Sokol,and Molly Treadway for theirhelpfulcomments.

    Wealsothank the Cognitive Lunch group for an illuminating discus-

    sion.

    Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to

    Michael McCloskey, Cognitive Science Center,

    Johns

    Hopkins Uni-

    versity, Baltimore, Maryland21218.

    non warrants the postulation of a special flashbulb-memory

    mechanism. One reason is that no detailed, coherent

    formu-

    lation

    of a special-mechanism hypothesis has been set forth.

    The hypothesis as stated by Brown and Kulik

    (1977)

    and

    discussed

    in

    subsequent articles

    is

    underdeveloped

    in

    several

    crucial respects,

    and

    there

    is

    also considerable variation

    be-

    tween

    and

    evenwithinarticles

    in

    what

    is

    claimed about

    the

    mechanism and the memories it produces. A related problem

    isthat discussions

    of the

    special-mechanism hypothesis have

    oftenfailedto engage the critical issue ofwhatwould consti-

    tute

    a

    sufficientbasis

    for

    positing

    a

    special

    flashbulb-memory

    mechanism.

    As

    a consequence of these problems, the implications of

    the evidence and arguments presented in studies of

    flashbulb

    memory have not

    always

    been clear. For example, Neisser

    (1982)

    reported

    two

    examples

    of

    inaccurate flashbulbmem-

    ories in support of his argument that these memories are not

    necessarily veridical. Thompson and Cowan (1986), however,

    presented evidence that for one of Neisser's examples the

    inaccuracy is relatively minor, and concluded that this ex-

    ample does not challenge the

    "basic

    accuracy of flashbulb

    memories"(p.

    200). Neisser

    (1986b), in a

    reply

    to

    Thompson

    and

    Cowan, disagreed with their conclusions.

    Unfortunately,

    the

    exchangefails

    to

    clarifywhat implications

    any

    particular

    degree

    of

    inaccuracy

    has for the

    special-mechanism hypothe-

    sis. Do afewexamples of minor inaccuracy call the hypothesis

    into serious question? Or does the hypothesis remain unchal-

    lengedif the accounts are usually basically accurate ?

    In

    this

    articleweattempttoevaluatethespecial-mechanism

    hypothesis systematically and, more generally, to assess the

    implicationsof theflashbulb-memory phenomenonforthe-

    ories

    of

    memory.

    On the

    basis

    of

    data

    and

    arguments,

    we

    conclude

    that the postulation of a special flashbulb-memory

    mechanism

    is unwarranted. Instead, we suggest, flashbulb

    memories' should be viewed as products of

    "ordinary"

    auto-

    biographical memorymechanisms,

    and

    hence

    as

    phenomena

    that mayofferinsights into the nature of these mechanisms.

    1

    We

    willuse the

    term

    flashbulb memoryin a theoretically neutral

    sense, to refer to individuals' recollections of the circumstances in

    which they

    learned

    about surprising and consequential events. It

    should

    be

    understood that

    our use of

    this term does

    not

    imply

    anythingabout the memory processes underlying the recollections.

  • 8/10/2019 McCloskey Wible Cohen (1988)

    2/11

    172

    M.

    McCLOSKEY, C. WIBLE, AND N.

    COHEN

    The Special-Mechanism Hypothesis

    Underlying

    the

    hypothesis

    of a

    special

    flashbulb-memory

    mechanism is the following implicit argument: Flashbulb

    memories have

    specialcharacteristics,

    that

    is,

    characteristics

    different from thosethememories would haveiftheywere

    producedby ordinary memory mechanisms. These

    special

    characteristics imply

    that the

    memories

    are

    products

    of a

    special memory mechanism.

    Thus, in evaluating the special-mechanism hypothesis, we

    must consider

    the

    followingquestions: What claims does

    the

    hypothesis

    make

    about

    the characteristics of flashbulb mem-

    ories?

    To

    what extent

    are

    these claims warranted

    by the

    available evidence? And, if these claims are accepted, do they

    clearly

    distinguish

    flashbulb

    memoriesfrom"ordinary"mem-

    ories and therefore warrant thepostulationof a special flash-

    bulb-memory mechanism?

    Characteristics of Flashbulb Memories

    Statements

    of the

    special-mechanism hypothesis

    are not

    entirely clear with regard to the characteristics of flashbulb

    memories,

    at

    timespressing strongclaimswhile

    at

    other times

    qualifying

    these claims. As a starting point for discussion, we

    will

    consideroneparticular interpretationof theBrownand

    Kulik (1977) hypothesis, an interpretation that makes strong

    claimsabout thecharacteristics of flashbulbmemories.On

    this rendering the special flashbulb-memory mechanism,

    when triggered by

    criterial

    levels of surprise and consequen-

    tiality,createsinmemoryadetailedandpermanent recordof

    the individual's experience immediately before, during, and

    immediatelyafterlearning of the shocking event.

    (Brown

    and

    Kulik, 1977, p. 87, stated that the mechanism records all

    "brainevents above some leveloforganization, apparently

    meaning something like

    the

    contents

    of

    awareness, including

    such information as where the individual was, what he or she

    wasdoing, etc.)

    The

    representations created

    by the

    special

    mechanism are

    complete, accurate,vivid,

    and

    immune

    to

    forgetting(i.e.,thestored information,in itsoriginal veridical

    form,remains permanently accessible).

    One could certainly formulate weaker interpretations of

    Brown and

    Kulik's

    (1977) position (e.g., flashbulb memories

    are impressively, although not perfectly, complete, accurate,

    vivid,and resistant toforgetting). However,weconsider the

    strong claims

    first

    because these claims,

    if

    supportable, would

    make the most convincing case for the position thatflashbulb

    memoriesare soremarkable that aspecial memory mecha-

    nism must be posited to account for them. If the strong claims

    prove untenable, we can then consider whether weaker claims

    motivate

    the

    postulation

    of a

    special

    flashbulb-memory

    mech-

    anism.

    2

    We begin our evaluation of the strong claims about char-

    acteristicsof flashbulbmemoriesbyreportingastudy aimed

    atevaluating these claims.

    Flashbulb Memories for the Explosion

    ofthe

    Space Shuttle

    With one exception (Pillemer, 1984),studiesof flashbulb

    memories (Brown & Kulik, 1977;Colegrove, 1899; Winograd

    &

    Killinger, 1983; Yarmey&Bull, 1978) have probed sub-

    jects'memories

    for the

    circumstances

    in

    which they learned

    of

    an eventonlyonce, long

    after

    the event. Yet,asNeisser

    (1982)

    has pointed out, this procedure does not permit the

    accuracyof the subjects' reports to be assessed. Moreover, it

    is

    difficult

    to determine whether the amount of remembered

    information

    remains constant over time,

    as it

    should

    if flash-

    bulb

    memories are immune to forgetting, or whether subjects'

    memories instead become less detailed and complete as time

    passes.

    Potentially more informative

    is a

    procedure

    in

    which

    sub-

    jects are questioned twice, once shortly

    after

    a surprising,

    consequential event, and again some time later, as in

    Pille-

    mer's

    (1984) study concerningtheReagan assassination at-

    tempt. The two sets of responses may then be compared with

    regard to amount and consistency of

    information

    reported on

    the two occasions. The explosion of the space shuttleChal-

    lengeron January 28, 1986, tragically provided the opportu-

    nityto carry out a study of this sort. The flight of

    Challenger

    washighlypublicized because the crew included the teacher

    Christa McAuliffe. Furthermore, the explosion was com-

    pletelyunexpected and had important consequences for the

    space program and, more generally, for public confidence in

    government, science,and technology. Thus, with regard to

    surprise and consequentiality, the presumed triggering con-

    ditions for the special flashbulb-memory mechanism, the

    shuttle explosion appears comparable to the events used in

    previous studies of flashbulb memory (e.g., the Reagan assas-

    sination attempt, assassinations

    of

    other political leaders).

    Indeed, the disaster was widely described in the media as one

    of those events for which people remember where they were

    and

    what they were doing when they learned

    of it. In the

    present studyweprobed subjects' memoryforcircumstances

    of

    learning about the shuttle explosion a few days

    after

    the

    explosion,andagain9months later.

    Method

    Questionnaire. The questionnaire presented to subjects included

    four

    questionsabout the circumstances under which the

    respondent

    learned of the explosion of thespaceshuttleChallenger:

    1.

    Where were you when you first learned of the explosion?

    2

    It might be suggested that thestrong

    claims

    we have formulated

    about

    characteristics

    of flashbulb

    memories

    are

    stronger than Brown

    andKulik

    (1977)

    intended. For example, whereas thestrongclaims

    characterizeflashbulbmemoriesascomplete recordsof the

    individ-

    ual's experience around the time of learning about a surprising,

    consequential

    event, Brown and Kulik (1977, pp.74-75,85)

    stated

    thatflashbulbmemories varyin"elaboration,"suggestingthatunder

    some circumstancesthese memories could be less than complete.

    However, Brown

    and

    Kulik also repeatedlyassert(e.g.,

    see

    Brown

    &

    Kulik, 1977,pp. 76,

    95-96)

    thatthe flashbulb-memorymechanism

    recordsallrecent brain events. Thus,potentialconflicts betweenthe

    strong claims we have formulated andstatementsmade by Brown

    and Kulik

    reflect

    ambiguities in their claims. In any event, regardless

    ofwhat Brown

    and

    Kulik intended,

    it is

    important

    to

    consider strong

    claims aboutcharacteristics

    of flashbulb

    memories because,

    as

    stated

    earlier, these claims wouldprovidethe firmestbasisfor assertingthat

    flashbulb

    memories

    arevery

    different from

    ordinary memories.

  • 8/10/2019 McCloskey Wible Cohen (1988)

    3/11

    FLASHBULB MEMORY

    7

    2. What were you doingwhenyou first learned of the explosion?

    3.Did you see theeventat thetimeit wasactuallyhappening,or

    did you

    learn

    aboutit

    later?

    If

    later,

    how did you

    learn about

    it?

    4. Whatwereyour first thoughts upon hearing the news?

    We will

    referto

    these

    items

    collectively

    as the

    circumstances ques-

    tions,andindividuallyas thelocation, activity, source,andreaction

    questions, respectively.

    For

    each question subjects were asked

    to

    rate

    theirconfidence in the correctness of their answers on a scalefrom1

    (lowconfidence)

    to 7

    (high confidence).

    The four

    circumstances questions were intended

    to

    probe cate-

    gories

    of

    information that should

    be

    included

    inmemories

    generated

    byamechanism that recordsthecontentsofawarenessfor theperiod

    immediately surroundingthelearning aboutasurprising, consequen-

    tial event. Previous studies have focused

    on six

    canonical

    categories"

    of information: place, ongoing event, information, own

    affect,

    affect

    in others, and aftermath (see, e.g., Brown &

    Kulik,

    1977;

    Pillemer,

    1984).Ourlocation, activity,andsourcequestions closely correspond

    to the first

    three

    of

    these categories, respectively,

    and our

    reaction

    question

    corresponds roughly

    to the ownaffect

    category.

    We did not

    probe affect

    in others or aftermath, because information in these

    categoriesmay notalwaysbeavailableto berecorded,atleast within

    the span

    of

    time over which

    a

    special

    flashbulb-memory

    mechanism

    wouldoperate.For

    example,

    a

    person

    who

    heard

    aradio

    announce-

    ment about the shuttle explosion while alone might have no experi-

    ence

    ofaffect in

    others

    to

    remember.

    The questionnaire alsoincluded

    a

    single question about

    the re-

    spondent's memory for his or her whereabouts at the time of the

    assassination

    attempt

    on President

    Reagan.

    Thesefive

    critical questions (the

    four

    shuttle circumstances ques-

    tions

    and the

    single question about

    the

    Reagan assassination attempt)

    occurred

    in

    identicalform

    on

    both

    theinitialand

    9-month question-

    naires. Instructions

    on

    both questionnaires stressed that answers

    should

    be as

    specific

    as

    possible.

    Both questionnaires also included several additional questions,

    most

    of

    which concerned

    the

    sources

    from

    which

    the

    respondent

    obtained information

    about

    the

    shuttledisaster

    subsequent

    to

    initially

    learning about it, and the details of theshuttleflightitself.The results

    for

    these questions are not relevant for our purposes and therefore

    will

    not be

    discussed.

    Design

    and

    procedure.

    On January 31, 1986, three days

    after

    the

    explosion, questionnaires were distributed

    to 50

    faculty,postdoctoral

    fellows,graduate

    students,

    undergraduate students, and support

    staff

    associatedwith

    the

    Psychology Department

    of

    Johns Hopkins Uni-

    versity.

    Forty-five of these"immediate"questionnaires (90%) were

    returnedwithin 1 week of the shuttle disaster.

    On October 21,1986,approximately 9 months

    after

    the explosion,

    follow-upquestionnaires were distributed

    to the 29

    subjects

    who (a)

    had returned the

    immediate

    questionnaire

    within

    1

    week

    of the

    explosion

    and (b)

    were still available

    at the

    time

    of the

    follow-up.

    Twenty-seven subjects (93%) completed the 9-month questionnaire.

    These subjects

    willbe

    referred

    to as the

    repeated

    testing

    group.

    To

    assess

    effects

    of

    completing

    the

    immediate questionnaire

    on

    responses givenat 9months, the 9-month questionnaire wasalso

    distributed to 35 individuals who had not received the immediate

    questionnaire. Thirty-one (89%) completed

    the

    questionnaire. These

    subjects

    willbe

    referred

    to as the

    9-month-only

    group.

    Results

    Answers

    to the

    location, activity, source,

    and

    reactionques-

    tions

    werefirst

    scored

    simplyas

    substantive

    or"don't

    remem-

    ber responses. Aresponsewasscored assubstantive if it

    provided

    an

    answer

    to the

    question,even

    if the

    response

    was

    nothighly specific.For

    example,

    at

    work

    aswellas

    Lever-

    ingHallnext to the

    news

    stand wasscoredas asubstantive

    answerto thelocationquestion.This scoring criterion appears

    to be consistentwith those

    used

    in previous studies

    (e.g.,

    Brown &

    Kulik,

    1977; Pillemer, 1984)toassesswhethera

    subjectprovidedinformation

    in a

    canonicalcategory.Because

    some subjects reported

    information

    relevantto one of the

    circumstancesquestionsin an

    answer

    to another ofthese

    questions,

    thescoringforeachquestion took accountof any

    relevant

    information the

    subjectprovided

    in answers to

    other

    questions.

    Nine-month responses.Table 1presents,for the repeated

    testing

    subjects

    on the immediateand 9-month question-

    naires, and for the

    9-month-only

    subjectson the9-month

    questionnaire,theproportionof

    subjects

    whoofferedsubstan-

    tiveresponsestoeachof thecircumstances questions. From

    the9-month results,it isclear thattheexplosionofChallenger

    is an

    event

    for which the flashbulb-memory

    phenomenon

    occurs:9months

    after

    the

    shuttleexplosion

    most subjects

    could providereports concerningwhere

    they

    were,

    what they

    weredoing,howtheyheard,and how

    they

    reactedwhenthey

    learnedof thedisaster.

    All

    subjects

    inboth groups

    gave

    substantive answersto at

    leasttwo of thefour

    questions;

    22 of the 27

    repeated-testing

    subjects(81%)and 26 of the 31

    9-month-only subjects(84%)

    gave substantive

    responses

    to all

    fourquestions. Subjects

    were

    also

    highlyconfident

    abouttheirresponses. Acrossthe

    four

    circumstances

    questions,mean confidence forsubstantive

    answers was 6.13 (on a

    scale

    with 7 as the

    highest

    confidence

    level) for the

    repeated-testinggroup,

    and

    6.16

    for the

    9-month-

    only

    group.

    Brown

    and

    Kulik (1977)

    scored a

    subject

    as

    having

    a

    flashbulb

    memory

    for an

    event

    if the

    subject

    (a) answered

    yes

    to the

    question,

    Do yourecall the

    circumstances

    in

    whichyou first heard

    that

    [event]? , and (b)

    provided

    infor-

    mation in at least one of the sixcanonical

    information

    cate-

    gories

    inreporting these circumstances.Assuming that any

    subject

    who

    gave

    asubstantive responseto one ormoreof

    ourcircumstances questions

    would

    have givena yes answer

    to aquestionof theform Do you recall the circumstances in

    which

    you firstheard thatthespace shuttleChallengerhad

    exploded? , thenby the

    Brown

    and

    Kulik

    criteriaall of our

    repeated-testing

    and 9-month-only subjects

    have

    flashbulb

    memories for thespace shuttle explosion.

    Table

    1

    Proportion o Repeated

    Testing

    and

    -Month-Only

    Subjects

    ProvidingSubstantiveResponses to the Circumstances

    Questions on theImmediateand9-Month Questionnaires

    Repeated testing

    Question

    Location

    Activity

    Source

    Reaction

    Immediate

    questionnaire

    1.00

    1.00

    1.00

    .96

    9-month

    questionnaire

    .96

    .89

    1.00

    .89

    9-month

    only

    9-month

    questionnaire

    .94

    .87

    1.00

    .97

    .99

    .94

    .94

  • 8/10/2019 McCloskey Wible Cohen (1988)

    4/11

    74

    M.

    McCLOSKEY,

    C. WIBLE, AND N. COHEN

    Comparison

    of the

    9-monthresults

    for the

    repeated-testing

    group and the

    9-month-only

    group suggests that completing

    the immediate questionnaire had little if anyeffecton the 9-

    month responses of the repeated-testing group. The mean

    number of

    questions answered substantively

    was

    3.74 (out

    of

    4) for the

    repeated-testing group

    and

    3.77

    for the

    9-month-

    only

    group, t < \. Furthermore, confidencedid not

    differ

    across groups (see earlier results on confidencelevels).

    Comparison ofimmediateand 9-monthresponses.The 9-

    month

    results

    paint a seemingly impressive picture of subjects'

    abilityto remember the circumstances in whichtheylearned

    of the

    shuttle explosion. However, comparison

    of the

    9-month

    responses with those given shortly

    after

    the explosion

    reveals

    that

    the

    amount

    of

    stored information subjects could access

    decreased over the retention interval, and further that the

    immediate and 9-month responses were not always consistent.

    For

    each

    of the 107

    circumstances questions answered

    substantively on the immediate questionnaire (27 subjects

    times 4 circumstances questions, minus a single reaction

    question answered "don't remember ),

    two

    independent

    judges compared the immediate response with the corre-

    sponding 9-month response.

    A

    9-month response was scored as the

    same

    as the corre-

    sponding immediate response if it provided basically the same

    information(although not necessarily in the same wording).

    A

    relativelylaxcriterion was applied, so

    that,

    for example,

    theresponses

    "reading"

    and studying to theactivity ques-

    tion

    were scored

    as the

    same.

    A9-month response

    was

    scored

    as

    morespecific than

    the

    immediate response if it was not inconsistent with the im-

    mediate response, but answered the question at afiner level

    of

    detail,

    or

    included particular pieces

    of

    information

    not

    reportedon the immediate questionnaire. Forexample,the

    9-month response eating lunch (actually, drinking coffee)

    with

    S S , S D , and M M "to the

    activity question was scored as morespecificthan the imme-

    diate response eating lunch with some friends.

    Similarly, a 9-month response was scored as

    moregeneral

    than thecorresponding

    immediate response

    if it was not

    inconsistentwiththe immediate response, but was less spe-

    cific,

    or

    omitted particular details.

    For

    example,

    the

    9-month

    response word of mouth was scored as more general than

    the immediate response heardC talking about it.

    A9-month responsewasscoredasinconsistentif itcontra-

    dicted

    the

    corresponding immediate response

    in

    some (al-

    though

    not necessarily all) respects. For example, the 9-month

    response

    at my

    desk

    to the

    location question

    was

    scored

    as

    inconsistent with the immediate response walking out of the

    door toward

    B 's

    office.

    Finally,

    a 9-month response was scored asdon'tremember

    if

    the question was answered substantively on the immediate

    questionnairebutgivena"don'tremember responseon the

    9-month questionnaire.

    The two judges agreed on 86% of the initial classification

    decisions. Disagreements were resolved through discussion.

    For some ambiguous responses, the subject was consulted for

    clarification.

    For example, one subject indicated that the 9-

    month response examining somedata for an experiment

    did not

    refer

    to thesame activity as theimmediate response

    Table 2

    ComparisonofImmediateand 9-MonthResponsesfor

    Repeated-Testing Subjects

    Relationship of 9-month response to

    immediate

    response

    Question

    Location

    Activity

    Source

    Reaction

    Total

    Same

    21

    15

    16

    13

    65

    More

    specific

    3

    2

    7

    More

    general

    2

    4

    7

    7

    20

    Inconsistent

    2

    4

    2

    9

    Don't

    remember

    3

    0

    2

    6

    designing

    an

    experiment. Table

    2

    presents

    for

    each

    of the

    fourcircumstances questionsthenumberofresponsesfalling

    into each

    of the

    scoring categories.

    Although there

    is

    considerable consistency over

    the 9-

    month retention interval, there is also substantial evidence of

    forgetting

    and inaccuracy.First,the data for"don'tremem-

    ber responses clearly reveal forgetting over the retention

    interval. On the

    immediate questionnaire,

    26 of the 27 re-

    peated-testing subjects provided a substantive answer to all

    fourcircumstances questions,

    and the

    remaining subject gave

    asubstantive response

    to

    three

    of the

    fourquestions.

    On the

    9-month questionnaire, however,4 of the 27 subjects(15.4%)

    gave

    "don't

    remember responses to a total of 6 questions

    theyhad answered substantively on the immediate question-

    naire.

    Further evidence

    of

    forgetting

    may be

    found

    by

    comparing

    the

    frequency

    ofmorespecificandmore general responses.If

    the accessible

    information

    remains constant over time, there

    should be no

    systematic

    difference

    between

    the

    immediate

    and9-month questionnaires in

    specificity

    of responses. That

    is,

    although

    corresponding immediate

    and

    9-month responses

    mayoccasionally varyinspecificitydue tovariationinsub-

    jects' decisionsabout

    how

    much

    of the

    accessible information

    to report, we would expect the number of responses that

    became more specificat 9monthsto beapproximatelythe

    same as the number that became more general. On the other

    hand,if theamountofaccessible

    information

    decreased over

    the interval between questionnaires, we might expect a sys-

    tematic

    shift

    toward more general responses on the 9-month

    questionnaire.This

    is

    just what

    we

    found. Whereas only

    7

    responses became more specific,

    20

    became more general

    (p