Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

82
Assessment: An Analysis of 24 Learning and Language Barriers and Possible Intervention Strategies Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA- D (www.marksundberg.com)

description

The VB-MAPP Barriers Assessment: An Analysis of 24 Learning and Language Barriers and Possible Intervention Strategies. Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (www.marksundberg.com). Assessment of a Child’s Needs. - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Transcript of Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Page 1: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

The VB-MAPP Barriers Assessment: An Analysis of 24 Learning and Language

Barriers and Possible Intervention Strategies

Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D

(www.marksundberg.com)

Page 2: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Assessment of a Child’s Needs

• A formal assessment and behavioral analysis is essential for developing an intervention program

• Identify the operant level of the existing skills• Compare those skills to those of a typically developing child• Identify the language, social, behavioral, and learning barriers that

are preventing more efficient learning• The failure to conduct an appropriate assessment results in one of

the biggest problems in programs that serve children with autism: An inappropriate curriculum

Page 3: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Verbal Behavior Milestones Assessment and Placement Program:

The VB-MAPP

• There are five components of the VB-MAPP • The VB-MAPP: Milestones Assessment contains 170 verbal

behavior milestones across 3 developmental levels (0-18 months, 18-30 months, 30-48 months) and 16 different verbal operants and related skills

• The VB MAPP: Barriers Assessment examines 24 common learning and language barriers faced by children with autism or other developmental disabilities

• The VB MAPP: Transition Assessment evaluates a child’s ability to learn in a less restrictive educational environment across 18 different skills

Page 4: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Verbal Behavior Milestones Assessment and Placement Program:

The VB-MAPP

• The VB-MAPP: Skills Task Analysis and Tracking provides a further breakdown of the 16 different skill areas in the form of a checklist containing over 900 specific skills

• The VB-MAPP: Placement and IEP Goals provides recommendations for program development for children based on their VB-MAPP profiles, and their specific scores for each of the 170 milestones and the 24 Barriers. In addition, over 200 IEP goals directly linked to the skills and barriers assessments, and the verbal behavior intervention program are provided

Page 5: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

The VB-MAPP Barriers Assessment

• It is important to find out what a child can do (The VB-MAPP Milestones Assessment), but it is also important to know what he can’t do, and analyze why he can’t do it

• The VB-MAPP Barriers Assessment is a tool that is designed to identify and score 24 different learning and language acquisition barriers that may be affecting an individual child

• Once a specific barrier has been identified, a more detailed descriptive and/or functional analysis of that problem is required

• There are many ways that a verbal repertoire or related skill can become defective or impaired, and an individualized analysis will be necessary to determine what the nature of the problem is for a specific child, and what intervention program might be appropriate

Page 6: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

The VB-MAPP Barriers Assessment

• There are several different types of barriers that can affect learning and language development

• Strong and persistent negative behaviors that impede teaching and learning (e.g., tantrums, aggression, non-compliance)

• Verbal operants or related skills that are absent, weak, or in some way impaired (e.g., echolalia, rote intraverbals, mands that are really tacts)

• Social behavior and the speaker-listener dyad can also become impaired for a variety of reasons (e.g., limited motivation for social interaction, impaired mands, impaired listener skills)

Page 7: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

The VB-MAPP Barriers Assessment

• Fundamental barriers to learning that must be analyzed and ameliorated (e.g., the failure to generalize, weak motivators, prompt dependency)

• Specific behaviors that can compete with learning (e.g., self-stimulation, hyperactive behavior, or sensory defensiveness)

• Problems related to physical or biological barriers that must be overcome or accounted for in some way (e.g., articulation or motor imitation errors may be due to physical limitations, matching errors may be due to visual limitations, or listener errors may be related to hearing problems)

Page 8: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

The VB-MAPP Barriers Assessment

• 24 Common Learning and Language Acquisition Barriers

• Behavior problems• Instructional control (escape/avoidance)• Impaired mand• Impaired tact• Impaired motor imitation• Impaired echoic (e.g., echolalia)• Impaired matching-to-sample • Impaired listener repertoires (e.g., LD, LRFFC)

Page 9: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

The VB-MAPP Barriers Assessment

• Common Learning and Language Acquisition Barriers

• Impaired intraverbal• Impaired social skills• Prompt dependency, long latencies• Scrolling responses• Impaired scanning skills • Failure to make conditional discriminations (CDs)• Failure to generalize• Weak or atypical MOs

Page 10: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

The VB-MAPP Barriers Assessment

• Common Learning and Language Acquisition Barriers

• Response requirements weakens the MO• Reinforcer dependent• Self-stimulation• Articulation problems• Obsessive-compulsive behavior• Hyperactivity • Failure to make eye contact• Sensory defensiveness

Page 11: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)
Page 12: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

The VB-MAPP Barriers Assessment

• Scoring the VB-MAPP Barriers Form• Rate the child on the VB-MAPP Barriers Assessment Form using a

Likert-type scale of 0 to 4• A score of 0 or 1 would indicate that there are no significant

barriers, and a formal intervention plan may not be required • A score of 2, 3, or 4 would indicate that there is a barrier that

probably should be addressed as part of the intervention program • For some children the immediate focus of the intervention program

should be on removing a particular barrier• Common immediate barriers to remove involve instructional

control, behavior problems, an impaired mand, and prompt dependency

Page 13: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)
Page 14: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)
Page 15: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Impaired Verbal Behavior

• A functional analysis of verbal behavior (Skinner, Chap 1)• A behavioral analysis of words, phrases, and sentences emitted by

children and adults with language delays• Same basic principles of behavior as nonverbal behavior• What is the source of control?• These sources of control will often reveal that what appears to be a

correct response in form is actually incorrect in function• Might not be the same source of control observed in a typically

developing child (e.g., asking “What’s your name”)• Each verbal operant can be susceptible to unwanted sources of

control

Page 16: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Impaired Verbal Behavior

• The behavior analyst must determine what the correct source of control should be, and how that source can be established

• The functional analysis of verbal behavior is on-going • The failure to conduct such an analysis may result in rote or

defective verbal repertoires that can become difficult to change• This is how behavior analysis is different, this is what we do• The primary focus today will be on the verbal operants, but many

of the other barriers are directly related to impaired verbal behavior and will be covered in the workshop (e.g., demand kills the MO, scrolling, prompt dependency, failing to generalize, weak conditional discrimination skills)

Page 17: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Analysis of an Impaired Mand Repertoire

• A substantial number of children with autism have an absent, weak, or impaired mand repertoire

• Many of these same children have extensive tact and listener skills, as well as other elevated scores on the VB-MAPP Milestones Assessment

• Often, under these circumstances it is not uncommon to see the child engage in a tantrum or some other form of negative behavior as a mand

Page 18: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Analysis of an Impaired Mand Repertoire

• A word acquired under SD control may not automatically transfer to MO control

• The distinction between SD and MO antecedent control is not systematically incorporated into many of the popular language assessment and intervention programs designed for children with autism

• There are many potential causes of a defective mand repertoire and a functional analysis is necessary to determine the cause for an individual child

• Here are 50 possible causes of an impaired mand repertoire• Most problems involve a combination of causes

Page 19: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent, Weak, or Impaired Mand Repertoire

• Limited mand training and limited opportunities to mand • Mands are not required to obtain specific reinforcement• Mand training is not part of the child’s early language training

history• Not enough manding trials and opportunities are provided each

day

Page 20: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent, Weak, or Impaired Mand Repertoire

• Response form problems, and failing to try augmentative communication

• The target response form is too difficult for the child• Shaping techniques are not used• There is a limited availability of established imitative or echoic

responses• Can’t establish differential response topographies• Articulation cannot be understood by listeners• When a child has no or limited vocal behavior, sign language or

PECS have not been tried• Push for sentences too soon (e.g., “I want...please”)

Page 21: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent, Weak, or Impaired Mand Repertoire

• Impaired mands are established early • Negative behavior functions as mands• A single response topography functions as the mand (e.g., “more,”

“please,” “this,” “mine,” “yes”)• Inappropriate mands become established and become hard to

eliminate (e.g., “Cup” as the mand for all drinks)• Motivation (MO) does not control the response form

Page 22: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Intervention Strategies forEstablished Impaired Mands

• START OVER• Use a trained professional• Use the strongest MOs• Use sign language• Target 1, then ASAP, 2 specific response topographies• Use standard prompt and fade techniques• Use DRI and extinction for existing negative mand

Page 23: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Impaired Mand Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Mand curriculum problems• Choosing the wrong words (signs or pictures) as the first mands to

teach• The mand training curriculum is poorly sequenced• Mand training does not move beyond tangibles and manipulatives• Mands are not incorporated into daily functional verbal behavior • A small group of mands has a strong history of reinforcement

(e.g., candy, juice, skittles)

Page 24: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Impaired Mand Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• MO Problems• Relevant MOs are not identified• There is no current MO in effect for the targeted item• Satiation effects weaken the MO• MOs are all too similar (e.g., food, toys)• Free or cheap access to reinforcers is available without manding• The child has weak MOs in general• The response requirement is too high and weakens the MO

(specific barrier)

Page 25: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Impaired Mand Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• MO Problems (cont.)• Self-stimulation or obsessive behaviors compete with other MOs • Failing to capture and create MOs• No variation in captured or created MOs• Weak MOs for verbal information• MOs for social and peer manding are weak, and intervention is not

provided• MOs are too strong and the mand repertoire becomes too strong

Page 26: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Impaired Mand Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• SD control (e.g., prompts) problems• The response is prompt bound by physical, echoic, imitative, or

intraverbal discriminative stimuli• A verbal stimulus acquires control and blocks MO control• A nonverbal stimulus acquires control and blocks MO control• A relevant nonverbal stimulus is faded too soon (before solid MO

control is established)• Scrolling gets reinforced (specific barrier)• Spontaneous mands are not fostered and never develop• Manding does not come under the control of natural contingencies• Poor audience control

Page 27: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Intervention Strategies forMand Scrolling

• START OVER• Use a trained professional• Use the strongest MOs• Establish two specific response topographies, then three, etc.• Possibly use one response as a tact • Echoic, imitation, & LDs won’t work as the second topography,

intraverbal responses with signs will (e.g., “sign book”)• Use standard prompt and fade techniques• Don’t fade out the object too soon (multiple control)• Use DRI and extinction for existing negative mand

Page 28: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Impaired Mand Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Consequence problems• Inappropriate manding is reinforced • Specific reinforcement is not used• Reinforcement is delayed• Differential reinforcement with extinction is not systematically

used• Manding is punished• Manding persistence is not established via intermittent

reinforcement

Page 29: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Impaired Mand Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Generalization problems• Mands only required and reinforced in a specific setting, time, or

with specific people• MO generalization training is not provided• Manding response generalization training is not provided• Overgeneralization

Page 30: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

An Analysis of an Impaired Tact Repertoire

• The tact repertoire is less susceptible to becoming defective than the mand or intraverbal, due in part to the nature of the controlling variables for the tact

• Nonverbal stimulus control is more measurable and accessible, and in general, much clearer than motivational control (mand), and verbal stimulus control (intraverbal)

• It is often the case that the wrong nonverbal stimulus acquires control of a tact

• For example, when teaching tacts related to verbs, the goal is that the specific moving nonverbal stimulus evokes a specific response, not the object related to the movement

Page 31: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

An Analysis of an Impaired Tact Repertoire

• Some children learn to emit a word that is a verb in form but not in function, as in the response “drinking juice” when just shown a cup, or “throwing ball” when shown a ball

• Similar problems can be observed in efforts to teach tacts related to other parts of speech such as prepositions and adjectives (e.g., “above” and “below”; “big” and “little”)

• Gone unchecked, these tacting errors can be difficult to change and can become the source of other verbal problems later in training, such as intraverbal rote responding

• There are many potential causes of a defective tact repertoire and a behavioral analysis is necessary

• Here are 30 possible causes of an impaired tact repertoire• Most problems involve a combination of causes

Page 32: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak Tact Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Limited tact training and other barriers• Formal tact training has not been provided, but is necessary• Not enough tact trials are provided each day• Limited tacting in the natural environment• Other barriers such as instructional control and behavior problems

dominate the educational day

Page 33: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak Tact Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Response form problems• The target response form is too difficult for the child• Articulation is unintelligible by listeners• Shaping techniques are not used• There is a limited availability of established imitative or echoic

responses• Can’t establish differential response topographies• Augmentative communication has not been tried• Sentences and carrier phrases are overemphasized too soon

Page 34: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak Tact Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Tact curriculum problems• The general progression of tact development in typically

developing children is ignored, thus the curriculum is poorly sequenced (e.g., adjectives before nouns are firmly established)

• Nonfunctional or irrelevant tacts targeted• Single stimulus and single response tacts have been over

conditioned• Limited training with multiple SDs and multiple response tacting

(e.g., noun-noun, noun-verb)• Tacts are not transferred to other verbal operants (e.g., mands, IVs)• Failure to analyze complexities of tacts involving prepositions,

pronouns, adjectives, private events, social behavior, etc.

Page 35: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak Tact Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Stimulus control problems• Tacting is prompt bound by echoic, imitative, or other SDs• Scrolling through targeted tacts gets reinforced• The wrong source of control is established (e.g., tacting verbs or

emotions from pictures)• Metonymical tacts are established and reinforced (e.g., tacting by

function or association)

Page 36: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak Tact Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Stimulus control problems• Verbal plus nonverbal conditional discriminations (CDs) are not

established (e.g., verbal stimuli do not establish a feature of nonverbal stimuli as an SD: as in IV-Tact CDs)

• No spontaneous tacting or tacting in the natural environment due to additional variables present during formal training (e.g., CMO-Reflexive, contextual prompts)

Page 37: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak Tact Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Consequence problems• Tacting is not reinforced• Tacting is punished• Excessive or inappropriate tacting gets reinforced• Tacting not reinforced by natural or automatic contingencies• Intermittent reinforcement is not used to establish persistence

Page 38: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak Tact Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Generalization problems• Generalization training is not provided• Stimulus classes are not established• Response classes are not established

Page 39: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

An Analysis of the VP-MTS Repertoire

• Many children with autism do well on visual discrimination tasks because they are usually easier than verbal tasks

• However, some children do not do well on these tasks, and a more detailed analysis of the child and the task is required

• Visual skills, especially MTS, are often more complex than they might appear

• MTS requires that a child attend to the sample stimulus, scan an array of comparison stimuli, and select a matching item based on some specific criteria (i.e., identical, non identical, arbitrary, association, sequences, patterns, designs, and categories)

• These skills involve a conditional discrimination where the first stimulus (the sample) should establish a second stimulus as a discriminative stimulus (SD)

Page 40: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak VP-MTS Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Physical limitations• Some type of vision impairment• Poor muscle tone, fine motor control, or CP

• Limited VP-MTS training• No formal training on visual discrimination tasks• Instructional control and behavior problems dominate• The child stims with materials

Page 41: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak VP-MTS Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• VP-MTS curriculum problems• The targeted visual tasks are out of developmental sequence• No systematic progression to increasingly complex tasks (e.g.,

steps are too small or too large, or nonexistent)• No functional use of skills outside of teaching sessions (e.g.,

matching socks, arts & crafts)

Page 42: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak VP-MTS Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Conditional discrimination problems• There is a failure to make conditional discriminations (two SDs and

two behaviors)• The sample stimulus does not evoke scanning behavior• The sample stimulus does not establish SD/S-delta relations in the

comparison array

Page 43: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Matching-to-Sample

Nonverbal SD1 + Array (SD

2) Nonverbal Response

Sample Comparison

S SD S Select Ball

Page 44: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Matching-to-Sample:A Conditional Discrimination

SD2/Sr

1

Comparison

S S

S S

Nonverbalarray

Sr2R2 Select ball

R1 scanNonverbal SD

1

(A ball)Sample

Page 45: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak VP-MTS Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Comparison array problems• There is a failure to scan visual arrays and comparisons efficiently• Over conditioning with a small array (limited array variation)• The task is too easy because the items in the comparison array are

very different from each other• Limited training with large arrays and scenes• Limited training with similar stimuli in the array• Limited training with arrays in the natural environment (3D)• Limited training with the combination of large arrays and similar

stimuli in scenes and in the natural environment

Page 46: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Identical Objects: Varied Array Size

1 2 3 4 50

20

40

60

80

100

Object Matching-to-Sample With Varied Array Size

Sets of Trials

Percent Correct Matching

Array of 3 - Neat

Array of 4 - Messy

Array of 6 - Messy

Array of 8 - Messy

Array of 3 - Neat

Participant 4

Page 47: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Identical Pictures: Similar Stimuli

1 2 3 4 50

20

40

60

80

100

Participant 1

Identical Pictures: Array of Similar Stimuli

Percent Correct

Array of 5Array of 3

Array of 7-8

Array of 10

Array of 15

Sessions

Page 48: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Identical Objects: Similar Stimuli-Varied Array Size

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 140

20

40

60

80

100

Identical Objects: Similar StimuliDifferent Array Sizes

Sets of Trials

Percent Correct

3-N

4-M 10-M 7-M

3-N

6 - M 4 - N

4 -N3 - N 4-M

6 -M

3 - N

6 - M

3 - N

N = Near ArrayM = Messy Array

Participant 1

Page 49: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak VP-MTS Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Impaired VP-MTS established early• The child is prompt bound by position, body movement, eye or

pointing prompts, etc.• Scrolling behavior gets reinforced • There is a reinforcement history for position preference, or

specific response patterns• A verbal consequence like “No” becomes an SD to pick another

item• If reinforcement is not provided for first selection, the child

quickly selects another item

Page 50: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak VP-MTS Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Generalization problems• Generalization training is not provided• Stimulus classes are not established• Response classes are not established

Page 51: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

An Analysis of the Listener Repertoire

• The complexities of verbal stimulus control• An early indication of impending language problems is that a child

does not appear to attend to others when they speak, or “understand” what is said

• The behavior of the listener involves several repertoires • 1) Necessary for a verbal episode• “The behaviors of the speaker and listener taken together compose

what may be called the total verbal episode” (Skinner, 1957, p. 2)• 2) The listener consequates the speaker’s behavior• Mediates reinforcement (the definition of VB, p. 2)• “The verbal community maintains the behavior of the speaker with

generalized reinforcement” (p. 151)

Page 52: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

An Analysis of the Listener Repertoire

• 3) The listener functions as an SD and MO for verbal behavior (The Audience, Chapter 7 in VB)

• “The listener, as an essential part of the situation in which verbal behavior is observed, is... a discriminative stimulus” (p. 172)

• “This function is to be distinguished from the action of the listener in reinforcing behavior” (p. 172)

• 4) The listener “takes additional action”• “Verbal behavior would be pointless if a listener did nothing more

than reinforce the speaker for emitting it” (p. 151)• “The action which a listener takes with respect to the verbal

response is often more important to the speaker than generalized reinforcement” (p. 151)

Page 53: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

An Analysis of the Listener Repertoire

• There are three types of action• Nonverbal respondent behavior• Nonverbal operant behavior• Verbal behavior

Page 54: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

The Different Action Taken by the Listener

• Nonverbal respondent behavior• “Among the special effects of verbal behavior are the emotional

reactions of the listener” (p. 154)• “If a verbal stimulus accompanies some state of affairs which is

the unconditioned or previously conditioned stimulus for an emotional reaction the verbal stimulus eventually evokes this reaction” (p. 154) (e.g., “snake” literary works, anger, passio

Page 55: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

An Analysis of the Listener Repertoire

• Nonverbal operant behavior (“Receptive language”)• Listener compliance (e.g., “jump”)• Listener discriminations (LDs) (e.g., “Touch the car.” “Where is

the number 5?”) • Listener Responding by Function, Feature, and Class (LRFFC)

(e.g., “Can you find an animal?” “Which one do you eat with?”)• “These examples remind us of the fact that the behavior of the

listener is not essentially verbal. The listener reacts to a verbal stimulus whether with conditioned reflexes or discriminated operant behavior, as he reacts to any feature of the environment” (p. 170)

Page 56: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

An Analysis of the Listener Repertoire

• Verbal operant behavior• “In many important instances the listener is also behaving at the

same time as a speaker” (p. 34)• “An important fact about verbal behavior is that the speaker and

listener may reside within the same skin” (p. 163)• “Some of the behavior of listening resembles the behavior of

speaking, particularly when the speaker ‘understands’ what is said” (p. 11)

• Much of what is traditionally called “listening” is covert verbal behavior, consisting of all the verbal operants (e.g., we can covertly emit echoics, mands, tacts, intraverbals, autoclitics, etc.)

Page 57: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak Listener Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Physical limitations• Some type of hearing impairment

• Limited listener training• No formal training on listener skills• Instructional control and behavior problems dominate• The child stims with the materials

Page 58: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak Listener Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Fails to participate in the verbal episode• The speech of others does not function as an SD for attending• The speech of others does not function as a conditioned reinforcer• People are conditioned punishers (avoidance, loner)• Social barriers present (separate barrier)

• Fails to consequate the speaker’s behavior• Does not mediate reinforcement for the verbal behavior of others

(e.g., get things manded)• Does not reinforce the verbal behavior of others (e.g., head nods,

attending)

Page 59: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak Listener Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Fails to function as an SD and MO for verbal behavior• The child has not become an SD for certain verbal behavior from

others (e.g., “Did you bring your game boy?”)• The child’s presence and behavior does not establish MOs for

others

Page 60: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak Listener Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Listener curriculum problems• The general progression of listener development in typically

developing children is ignored, thus the curriculum is poorly sequenced (e.g., teaching adjectives before nouns are firmly established)

• Single stimulus and single response listener discriminations (LDs) have been over conditioned

• Limited training with multiple SDs and multiple listener responses• No systematic progression to increasingly complex tasks (e.g.,

steps are too small or too large, or nonexistent)

Page 61: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak Listener Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Listener curriculum problems• No functional use of skills outside of teaching sessions (e.g., all

table top LDs)• Failure to analyze complexities of LDs related to prepositions,

pronouns, adjectives, social behavior, etc.• Listener skills are not related to other verbal operants (e.g., tacts,

mands, intraverbals)• No LRFFC training has been provided

Page 62: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak Listener Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Conditional discrimination problems• Failure to emit conditional discriminations across modalities (a

verbal and nonverbal SDs and two behaviors)• Verbal stimuli do not evoke scanning behavior• The verbal stimulus does not establish SD/S-delta relations in the

comparison array

Page 63: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Listener Discriminations

Verbal SD1 + Array (SD

2) Nonverbal Response

“Touch ball” + S SD S Select Ball

Page 64: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Listener Discriminations (LD): A Conditional Discrimination

SD2/Sr

1

S S

S S

Nonverbalarray

Sr2R2 Select ball

R1 scanVerbal SD1

(“Touch ball”)

Page 65: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak Listener Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Comparison array problems• There is a failure to scan the comparisons arrays efficiently• Over conditioning with a small array (limited array variation)• The task is too easy because the items in the comparison array are

very different from each other• Limited training with large arrays and scenes,• Limited training with similar stimuli in the array• Limited training with arrays in the natural environment (3D)• Limited training with the combination of large arrays and similar

stimuli in scenes and in the natural environment

Page 66: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak Listener Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Impaired listener behaviors established early• The child is prompt bound by position, body movement, eye or

pointing prompts, etc.• Scrolling behavior gets reinforced • There is a reinforcement history for position preference, or

specific response patterns• A verbal consequence like “No” becomes an SD to pick another

item• If reinforcement is not provided for first selection, the child

quickly selects another item

Page 67: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak Listener Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Generalization problems • Generalization training is not provided• Stimulus classes are not established• Response classes are not established

Page 68: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

An Analysis of the Intraverbal Repertoire

• Verbal SDs are usually much more complicated than the nonverbal SDs

• Verbal SDs usually contain multiple components, occurring in a brief time frame

• Multiple words as SDs almost always involve verbal conditional discriminations

• Vocal verbal stimuli are transitory, nonverbal stimuli tend to be more static

• Attending to verbal SDs is often more laborious than attending to nonverbal SDs

Page 69: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

An Analysis of the Intraverbal Repertoire

• Many words that are not clearly evoked by a corresponding nonverbal stimulus (e.g., the, a, can’t, usually, if, its, for, of, anyway, whatever) but form the VCDs

• IV responses are typically more complex than responses associated with tacts

• The MLU of a tact tends to be much shorter than the MLU for an intraverbal

• There is often only so much that can be said about a specific nonverbal stimulus (e.g., the tact bike vs. IV story about a bike)

• The tact response is often shaped to include only the salient information

Page 70: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

An Analysis of the Intraverbal Repertoire

• Intraverbal behavior is most prone to becoming rote for children with autism

• The task of directly teaching intraverbal behavior is complicated and endless

• Early intraverbal training is pretty straight forward, but by 3-4 years of age, a typical child acquires 1000s of different intraverbal relations

• Most adults have hundreds of thousands of different intraverbal relations as a part of their verbal repertoires (e.g., newspaper, books, the internet)

Page 71: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

An Analysis of the Intraverbal Repertoire

• Contact with these verbal stimuli can evoke numerous intraverbal responses, such as discussions of the facts (e.g., global warming, autism, SD vs. the MO)

• The number of different intraverbal relations far outnumbers the number of different mands and tacts. The frequency of mands may be greater than intraverbals, but often the mands are related to a relatively small set of MOs

• Language would be simple “if a verbal repertoire was like a passenger list on a ship or plane” (Skinner, 1957 p. 91)

Page 72: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

An Analysis of the Intraverbal Repertoire

• Many children with autism have a great deal of difficulty acquiring meaningful intraverbal behavior. Some have acquired 100s of tacts and LDs, but fail to acquire more than a few simple intraverbal relations

• Tacting, imitation, echoic, matching, LDs, textual, and transcriptive have a degree of sameness that may come easier for children with autism than intraverbal behavior

• Not only are the antecedents for these repertoires more consistent and clearer, but also the response is frequently the same (e.g., a spoon is usually “spoon,” 2 is always “two”)

Page 73: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

An Analysis of the Intraverbal Repertoire

• Intraverbal relations, by their very nature, involve constantly changing SDs and responses

• For example, a tree is always a tree for echoic, tacting, matching, etc., but the discussion about trees can be comprised of hundreds, if not thousands of different intraverbal relations

• Furthermore, the discussion about trees may never occur exactly the same way each time

• However, this type of defective intraverbal behavior is not uncommon for some high functioning individuals with autism, and especially those with Aspergers

Page 74: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Absent or Weak Intraverbal Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Limited intraverbal training• No formal training on intraverbal skills• Instructional control and behavior problems dominate

Page 75: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Impaired Intraverbal Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Intraverbal curriculum problems• Training is provided, but it’s to early in the intervention program

to focus on intraverbals• The general progression of intraverbal development in typically

developing children is ignored, thus the curriculum is poorly sequenced

• The specific target responses are not in the child’s repertoire as tacts, mands, LDs, or LRFFCs

• No systematic progression to increasingly complex tasks (e.g., steps are too small or too large, or random)

• Failure to analyze complexities of intraverbals related to prepositions, pronouns, adjectives, adverbs, social behavior, etc.

Page 76: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Impaired Intraverbal Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Intraverbal curriculum problems• The IV responses are broken from MOs and nonverbal SDs too soon• Intraverbal chains are not established (self as a listener)• Continued intraverbal training is provided in the absence of the

relevant nonverbal and MO context (e.g., listing words)• No functional use of skills outside of teaching sessions • No formal multiple tact plus intraverbal training (e.g., “What color

is the ball” vs. “What shape is the ball?”)• Limited training with multiple intraverbal responses• Intraverbal skills are not related to other verbal operants (e.g., tacts,

mands, LDs, LRFFC)• No LRFFC training has been provided

Page 77: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Impaired Intraverbal Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Verbal conditional discrimination problems• One verbal stimulus does not alter the evocative effect of a second

verbal stimulus• The problem becomes worse with more than two critical stimuli,

however training continues on without fixing the problem • Insufficient training on basic verbal conditional discriminations • Nonverbal and cross-modalities CDs may be absent, weak, or

impaired• The individual stimuli in the verbal antecedent do not have stimulus

control over any verbal responses (e.g., “not”)• Child does not attend to multiple verbal stimuli (early words are S-

deltas)

Page 78: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Impaired Intraverbal Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Stimulus and motivational control problems• The child’s echoic repertoire is too strong (echolalia)• Specific verbal stimuli in the antecedent are too salient and block the

establishment of stimulus control by other words• Nonverbal stimuli control the response form (tact prompt bound)• MOs control response forms (strong IVs on favorite topics)• Rote intraverbal response have been over conditioned• Poor audience control• The speaker is his own listener (self-talk)

Page 79: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Impaired Intraverbal Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Impaired intraverbal behaviors established early• Rote intraverbal responses have been firmly established due to a

conditioning history• Out of context or irrelevant intraverbal training may establish odd

forms of IV behavior• Automatic reinforcement may establish odd forms of intraverbal

behavior (“delayed echolalia”)• Simple verbal stimuli and single verbal responses have been over

conditioned (e.g., opposites)• Intraverbal scrolling behavior gets reinforced

Page 80: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Impaired Intraverbal Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Consequence problems• No automatic reinforcement for IV behavior• Intermittent reinforcement procedures have not been used to

established persistence• A punishment history for intraverbal behavior• An extinction history for intraverbal behavior• A reinforcement history for odd, inappropriate, or impaired IV

behavior• Failure to self-edit verbal behavior

Page 81: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Potential Causes of an Impaired Intraverbal Repertoire and Intervention Strategies

• Generalization problems • Generalization training is not provided• Sameness has been over conditioned in the tact, LD, and MTS,

and the inherent variation of verbal stimuli cause problems• Verbal stimulus classes are not established• Verbal response classes are not established

Page 82: Mark L. Sundberg, Ph.D., BCBA-D (marksundberg)

Thank You!

For an electronic version of this presentation visit:

marksundberg.com/ABAI