Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time ...nielsen/soci708/cdocs/jasso_1985.pdf · Marital...

19
http://www.jstor.org Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time: Estimating the Separate Effects of Spouses' Ages and Marital Duration, Birth and Marriage Cohorts, and Period Influences Author(s): Guillermina Jasso Source: American Sociological Review, Vol. 50, No. 2, (Apr., 1985), pp. 224-241 Published by: American Sociological Association Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2095411 Accessed: 07/06/2008 12:00 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=asa. Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. JSTOR is a not-for-profit organization founded in 1995 to build trusted digital archives for scholarship. We enable the scholarly community to preserve their work and the materials they rely upon, and to build a common research platform that promotes the discovery and use of these resources. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

Transcript of Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time ...nielsen/soci708/cdocs/jasso_1985.pdf · Marital...

Page 1: Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time ...nielsen/soci708/cdocs/jasso_1985.pdf · Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time: ... survey, noting especially ... bridewealth

http://www.jstor.org

Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time: Estimating the Separate Effects of Spouses'Ages and Marital Duration, Birth and Marriage Cohorts, and Period InfluencesAuthor(s): Guillermina JassoSource: American Sociological Review, Vol. 50, No. 2, (Apr., 1985), pp. 224-241Published by: American Sociological AssociationStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2095411Accessed: 07/06/2008 12:00

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at

http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless

you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you

may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at

http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=asa.

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed

page of such transmission.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit organization founded in 1995 to build trusted digital archives for scholarship. We enable the

scholarly community to preserve their work and the materials they rely upon, and to build a common research platform that

promotes the discovery and use of these resources. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

Page 2: Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time ...nielsen/soci708/cdocs/jasso_1985.pdf · Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time: ... survey, noting especially ... bridewealth

MARITAL COITAL FREQUENCY AND THE PASSAGE OF TIME: ESTIMATING THE SEPARATE EFFECTS OF SPOUSES' AGES AND MARITAL DURATION, BIRTH AND MARRIAGE COHORTS, AND

PERIOD INFLUENCES*

GUILLERMINA JASSO University of Minnesota

To the extent that marital coital frequency is linked to both within-couple and societalfertility and sex ratio, it may be implicated in a wide range of behavioral and social phenomena. Variation in marital coital frequency over the life course as well as across cohorts may thus affect many aspects of the social life. This paper reports estimates of the ceteris paribus cohort-free effects of spouses' ages, marital duration, and contemporaneous period influences on coital frequency, as well as of the correlations between coital frequency and birth- and marriage-cohort factors. These estimates are obtained by (a) using the properties of the fixed-effects statistical model in order to separate the effects of cohort influences from the agelduration and period effects and to control for the operation of couple-specific unobservables, and (b) using strictly monotonic nonlinear transformations in order to separate the effects of wife's age, husband's age and marital duration.

What are the effects of marital duration and of the spouses' ages on their coital frequency? Have the sexuality-relevant environments faced by American children, adolescents, and newlyweds produced observable differences in levels of sexual activity across birth and mar- riage cohorts? Have advances in contraceptive technology in the last twenty years produced secular increases in sexual activity? These questions are extremely difficult to answer, principally for two reasons: First, unobserva- ble couple-specific factors may be correlated with the observable determinants of coital fre- quency, thus biasing the obtained estimates.' Second, seven factors of substantive interest-the wife's and husband's birthdates and ages, the marriage date and marital dura- tion, and the observation time period-are linearly dependent, so that at most four linear effects may be estimated, leading to composite estimates and, as Ryder (1968:550) puts it, to "the temptation to use age-cum-cohort as if it were age," thereby assuming zero effects for

cohort influences and, in the process, obtaining possibly biased estimates of the effects of age (and, in the present case, of marital duration).2

There is much interest in obtaining estimates of the pure effects of the seven factors just listed, since by their relation to coital fre- quency they may be related as well to the de- termination of a wide range of behavioral and social phenomena. The study of sexual behav- ior derives its importance to sociology from the historical link between coitus and the produc- tion of new members for human society. Mar- ital sexuality is of particular importance be- cause of its dual role as "both an expression of and a conditioner of the general character of the marital relationship" (Rainwater, 1965:61), a relationship central to the institution of the family, which in turn is central to socialization of the young and to major features of social,

*Direct all correspondence to: Guillermina Jasso, Department of Sociology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455.

I am grateful to two anonymous referees and to Henry Braun, Marlaine Lockheed and Jeylan Mor- timer for many valuable comments and suggestions; and to Knut Ekker for kindly helping to prepare the graphs.

I Heterogeneity and failure of the orthogonality assumption have received much attention recently. For an especially useful discussion, see Hausman (1978). Excellent summaries are found in Judge et al. (1980) and Johnston (1984).

2 The problem of identifying the separate effects of age, cohort, and period influences was brought to general scholarly attention by Ryder (1951, 1965, 1968). Illuminating discussion is found in Glenn (1976, 1977). It is well known that additional infor- mation is required in order to achieve identification; Mason et al. (1973) suggest the use of zero restric- tions in cases where it is substantively reasonable to do so. Hobcraft et al. (1982) provide an excellent survey, noting especially the further difficulties posed by cases of double or triple identification problems (such as the present case, in which seven variables are linearly related to each other). Ad- ditional useful discussions are found in Mason et al. (1976), Farkas (1977), Fienberg and Mason (1978), Weiss and Lillard (1978), Rodgers (1982), Smith et al. (1982) and Clogg (1982).

224 American Sociological Review, 1985, Vol. 50 (April:224-241)

Page 3: Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time ...nielsen/soci708/cdocs/jasso_1985.pdf · Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time: ... survey, noting especially ... bridewealth

MARITAL COITAL FREQUENCY 225

economic, and political organization. Ad- ditionally, marital coital frequency is thought to affect fecundability (Keyfitz, 1977:315-17; James, 1979; Marini and Hodsdon, 1981; Rind- fuss and Morgan, 1983) and to affect as well the gender of the conceptus (James, 1983b). Through these effects, coital frequency may operate not only on the biography of an indi- vidual couple, as a determinant of the number of children they have, of the children's gender, of the length of the birth intervals, and hence of the healthiness, productivity, and other char- acteristics of the offspring, but also on the societal fertility rate and sex ratio, both of which may play important parts in virtually all aspects of the social life (Freedman, 1968; Guttentag and Secord, 1983; Keyfitz, 1983).

Other effects of coital frequency may be more subtle. For example, if coital frequency is systematically linked to a couple's productivity-relevant attributes, then there would be generated correlations between fam- ily size and income and wealth and between gender and parental income and wealth. The latter correlation is particularly suggestive, as it would lead not only to gender inequalities in schooling and other wage-relevant opportuni- ties but also to a society in which the three types of families (daughters-only, sons-only, and mixed-sex) are hierarchically arranged along the income continuum, potentially generating distinctive customs of mate selec- tion, dowry and/or bridewealth, reckoning of kinship and descent, and naming practices. To illustrate, if in a village every bride came from a wealthier family than her groom, then a sim- ple behavioral mechanism linking personal well-being (e.g., self-esteem) with social status would swiftly produce the custom of grooms taking their brides' surnames at marriage.

If the direction of the correlation between coital frequency and the wage-relevant attri- butes were itself dependent on exogenous tech- nological factors, then the direction of the cor- relations just discussed would vary sys- tematically across societies and over time, giving rise to great variability in marriage- and family-related customs and laws, a variability we indeed observe (Marshall, 1968; Smith, 1968; Winch, 1968). Moreover, changes which altered the direction of the correlation would alter as well the cultural structures; an example might be the sudden and dramatic shift from a bridewealth to a dowry system which occurred in south India after Independence (Caldwell et al., 1983; see also Goode, 1963; Huber and Spitze, 1983; Huber, 1984).

More immediately pertinent to our present purpose, if coital frequency varies sys- tematically over the life course-for example, if it varies with age-then age at the inception

of noncontracepting married life would affect the family outcomes; put differently, ten years of married life begun at age 25 would produce a set of children of different quantity and quality than ten years of married life begun at age 15. Further, if coital frequency varies nonmono- tonically over the course of the marriage (i.e., with the joint effects of spouses' ages and mar- ital duration), then the coital-frequency "'growth" curve would reveal the time points at which the probabilities of having a child of a given sex are equal (a phenomenon discussed by Aristotle in On The Generation of Animals), as well as the effects on the offspring config- uration of the timing of contraception. Thus, a couple's decision to contracept for, say, the first two years of sexual union would alter the probable sex of the first-born, as would gov- ernment policies which promoted such a childbearing delay.

As the foregoing suggests, although ad- vances in the technologies for contraception and for in vitro fertilization and gestation signal the twilight of the link between coitus and re- production, an understanding of the historical relationships may be central to an understand- ing of the formation of the social institutions and cultural structures we have inherited.3

An additional set of phenomena may be linked to cohort effects on the propensity for sexual activity. It is possible, for example, that systematic intercohort differences, if any, might lead to preferential cross-cohort mating (affecting such observable phenomena as age differences between spouses) and that cross- sex differences in the pace of intercohort changes might lead to accelerated marital dis- ruption and intensified sex and age differentia- tion (visible, for example, in divorce rates and in sex-specific social movements).

Finally, obtaining estimates of the effects of contemporaneous influences is important be- cause such estimates provide clues to structural changes, for example, in the price of privacy (as through housing costs) and in the price of contraception (as through discoveries concerning the safety of contraceptive methods).

It is widely believed that the composite ef- fect of age and marital duration is negative- viz., that coital frequency decreases over the life course. Negative effects of age and/or mar- ital duration have been reported by Trussell and Westoff (1980), Udry et al. (1982), and

3 Note that such understandings would further il- luminate the relationships between biological traits and processes and behavioral and social phenomena. For some recent searching looks, see Barchas et al. (1975), Bennett (1983), Webster and Driskell (1983) and Rossi (1984).

Page 4: Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time ...nielsen/soci708/cdocs/jasso_1985.pdf · Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time: ... survey, noting especially ... bridewealth

226 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

James (1983a). These studies, however, are subject to the two kinds of bias noted above, arising from couple heterogeneity as well as from the confounding of cohort and age/ duration effects. Thus, the findings could as easily be interpreted to support the proposition that American sexuality-relevant environments facing successive birth cohorts have provided increasing encouragement and reinforcement for sexual activity. Given the present state of knowledge, it is difficult to discern whether one or more of the following statements is cor- rect: (1) Coital frequency decreases with age and marital duration. (2) In the last half- century, coital frequency has increased across birth and marriage cohorts. (3) In the last twenty years, coital frequency has increased, net of cohort and age/duration effects.

With respect to the effects of age, it should be noted that Kinsey and his associates (1948, 1953), in their pioneering empirical work on human sexuality, concluded that sexual re- sponsiveness behaves in a nonmonotonic fashion with respect to age, increasing to a maximum and subsequently decreasing. They calculated that among males the maximum is reached at about the age of 17 years and sexual responsiveness immediately begins a very gradual decline, while among females the maximum is reached at an age somewhere between the late twenties and middle forties and sexual responsiveness does not begin to decline until menopause.

This paper uses longitudinal data drawn from the 1970 and 1975 rounds of the National Fer- tility Studies and exploits the properties of the fixed-effects statistical model in order to obtain estimates which are free of the biasing effects of the unobservable factors and which, for the first time, separate the effects of husband's birth cohort, wife's Wirth cohort, and marriage cohort from the effects of husband's and wife's age and their marital duration.

The sample consists of over 2000 con- tinuously married couples, representing the following ranges in the substantive variables of interest: husband's age, 17.25 to 58.58 years; wife's age, 15.5 to 48.08 years; marital dura- tion, one month to 25 years; birth cohorts from 1917 to 1953 for husbands and from 1927 to 1955 for wives; and marriage cohort, 1950 to 1970.

It is well known that the fixed-effects model is the only technique currently available that yields unbiased estimates in the presence of correlation between measured explanatory variables and unobservable individual-specific effects, other techniques achieving consistency at best (Hausman, 1978; Judge et al., 1980; Hausman and Taylor, 1981). However, it ap- pears not to have been recognized that the

fixed-effects model, since it holds constant all permanent attributes, holds constant as well all cohort influences which persist over time, and hence may be quite useful in separating age, cohort, and period effects.

Three sets of estimates are obtained. First, an unbiased estimate of the composite effects of wife's age, husband's age, marital duration, and the contemporaneous period effect is ob- tained. This estimate has the virtue of being completely free of contamination from cohort effects; however, it has the defect of combining four effects, so that at best it indicates that at least one of the four factors must have an effect of the same sign as the composite effect. Sec- ond, nonlinear assumptions are introduced in order to isolate the separate effects of age, marital duration, and observation period. These estimates, carried out within a fixed- effects framework, remain free of cohort ef- fects and of heterogeneity bias but embody the additional albeit mild functional-form assump- tions. Third, the residual from the fixed-effects equation is used to obtain estimates of the re- lationships between the birth and marriage cohorts and the unobservable propensity for sexual activity. These estimates are free of the effects of age, marital duration, and con- temporaneous influences.

The results of this investigation suggest that, other things being equal, in the observed ranges of the variables, marital coital fre- quency decreases with marital duration and possibly with husband's age (the slopes be- coming successively less steep) and increases at a decreasing rate (again, less and less steeply) with wife's age. Since in the sample the youngest husband is 17 years of age and the oldest wife is 48 years of age, these results are completely consistent with Kinsey et al.'s (1948, 1953) findings. Interestingly, the com- bined effects of spouses' ages and marital du- ration are consistent with a curve of coital fre- quency which is nonmonotonic over the course of the marriage, decreasing to a minimum within two or three years of marrying, there- after increasing. Moreover, the time point at which the minimum is reached is itself a func- tion of age at marriage. For example, in the special case in which both spouses are the same age, the minimum is a strictly increasing function of age at marriage; if age at marriage is 24, then the minimum is predicted to occur at about two months before the second wedding anniversary.

With respect to cohort influences, the results (which are completely free of the biasing ef- fects of age or period influences) indicate sub- stantial differences in the propensity for sexual activity across birth and marriage cohorts, with more recent cohorts displaying higher esti-

Page 5: Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time ...nielsen/soci708/cdocs/jasso_1985.pdf · Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time: ... survey, noting especially ... bridewealth

MARITAL COITAL FREQUENCY 227

mated sexuality levels. However, each of the three cohort variables exhibits a different pat- tern, suggesting, among other things, that the sexuality-relevant environments faced by girls changed more rapidly than those faced by boys. Finally, we find also that the 1975 envi- ronment was less conducive to sexual activity than the 1970 environment, suggesting that new information concerning the risks associ- ated with the new contraceptive technology substantially increased the costs of contracep- tion and led to a decline in coital frequency.

Since the birth- and marriage-cohort effects are positive, these results indicate that failure to control for cohort influences would down- wardly bias the estimated effects of age and marital duration. Thus, these results suggest that the negative effect of marital duration is of smaller absolute magnitude than has been re- ported in previous (cohort-confounding) studies, that the effect of husband's age is either zero or negative but of smaller absolute magnitude than has been previously reported, and that the effect of wife's age is positive rather than negative as has been previously reported.

CONJECTURES ABOUT THE DETERMINATION OF MARITAL COITAL FREQUENCY

Concerning the Effects of Age and Marital Duration

The prevailing opinion among scientists and clinicians, as well as novelists, philosophers, theologians, and: others who have thought about the matter, is that marital coital fre- quency decreases over time. This decrease is thought to be related both to the decline of healthiness, hormonal production, and (men- tial and physical) energy levels which are be- lieved to accompany aging (Birren, 1968; Tal- mon, 1968; Udry et al., 1982), and also to the loss of novelty and the reduced need to bond which accompany marital duration (Udry, 1970:377).

Attributing components of this decline with the passage of time to the three factors- spouses' ages and marital duration-is a more complicated matter. With respect to age ef- fects, Kinsey et al. (1953:759-60) found evi- dence to support a conjecture that appears as early as Ovid, viz., that maximum sexual re- sponsiveness is reached very early in the male (at about 17 years of age) and relatively late in the female (perhaps past the fortieth birthday). Thus, Kinsey et al. (1953:353) and others have argued that the observed decrease in marital coital frequency is, as Gebhard (1978:596) puts it in an authoritative article for the lay reader, "more often than not the result of male deteri-

oration." On the other hand, Udry et al. (1982) argue that known facts about differential changes in hormonal production suggest a de- terioration in female responsiveness. Students of aging effects suggest that the sexual drive slackens as age increases (Birren, 1968; Tal- mon, 1968). Shock (1978:308), in an au- thoritative article for the lay reader, noted that "Sexual activity, as reported in interview studies, diminishes progressively between the ages of 20 and 60 in both males and females."

With respect to marital duration, the pre- vailing opinion is that it has a negative effect on coital frequency (Trussell and Westoff, 1980; James, 1983a), due to lack of novelty, reduced need to bond, and psychological fatigue (Udry, 1970). Yet Ovid and others have argued that if practice increases sexual skill and if sexual skill increases sexual pleasure, then marital du- ration should produce an increase in coital fre- quency (an experience effect in marriage).

The literature suggests that, in the ranges of the variables observed in this study, the ceteris paribus effects of spouses' ages and marital duration are monotonic. However, disagree- ment in the literature about the direction of the effects makes it unwise to sign them a priori. Further, the literature suggests that these ef- fects occur very gradually, perhaps becoming more and more gradual as age and marital du- ration increase. Thus, these effects may be modeled by linear functions or by nonlinear functions of the sort which become less steep, viz., increasing-and-concave or decreasing- and-convex functions.

Concerning the Effects of Birth and Marriage Cohorts

All individuals born at the same time (and in the same country) share certain features of a common environment, an environment en- compassing medical beliefs and practices, rec- ommended child-rearing practices, food sup- ply, a range of toys and recreational activities, level of economic development, and a popular culture or folklore concerning love, sex, and marriage. Some of these features have perma- nent consequences for individuals (Ryder, 1951, 1965, 1968; Glenn, 1976, 1977; Elder, 1974, 1979). Thus, it is commonly held that the environment faced by children born during the Depression differs from that faced by chil- dren born during World War II and that both differ from that faced by children born during the baby-boom years (1946-1954), and that some of these differences have led to perma- nent differences between individuals born at those different times.

This paper is concerned only with the sexuality-relevant elements (possessing per-

Page 6: Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time ...nielsen/soci708/cdocs/jasso_1985.pdf · Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time: ... survey, noting especially ... bridewealth

228 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

manent effects) of the environments. Did these change over time? Did boys and girls of the same vintage face different environments? Did the rate of change differ for the two sex- specific time series of environments?

Similarly, all couples married at the same time (and in the same place) share certain fea- tures of a common environment, features with long-lasting effects. They have available the same set of marriage manuals, receive similar "advice," are exposed to the same "models" in popular culture (novelistic and cinematic portrayals of marriage). This paper asks: Did the sexuality-relevant environments facing marriage cohorts in the United States change between 1950 and 1970?

Most observers would probably agree that American birth cohorts since 1920 have faced environments increasingly supportive of sexual activity (Reiss, 1960, 1966). Hence, a priori we expect to find substantial differences across cohorts.

Concerning the Effects of the Contemporaneous Time Period

It is widely believed that changes in con- traceptive technology have produced increases in marital coital frequency. Westoff (1974) re- ports a dramatic increase between 1965 and 1970. Trussell and Westoff (1980:247) suggest that the trend continued to 1975, but at "a slower pace." It can be argued, however, that the cost of contraception increased between 1970 and 1975, as information spread about medical risks associated with the two most ef- fective methods. Hence, the sign of the period effect cannot be predicted.

Concerning Interacdions

Following the cogent reasoning of Ryder (1965, 1968) and Glenn (1976, 1977), it can be argued that the spouses' ages and marital duration, as well as their birth and marriage cohorts, con- ditioned the response to the contraceptive rev- olution of the 1960s (Westoff and Ryder, 1977) and that that response had persistent conse- quences for sexual activity in 1970 and 1975. It can also be argued that the response to the contemporaneous influences is conditioned by age and marital duration; for example, infor- mation about the safety of contraceptives will be irrelevant to couples in which the wife is postmenopausal, and increases in the price of housing/privacy will be irrelevant to couples whose children have all grown and moved away. However, these latter interactions are not expected to operate in this study, given the observed ranges of wife's age and marital du- ration.

DESIGN OF THE EMPIRICAL ANALYSES

Specification of the Marital Coital Frequency Equation

Unbiased (or at least consistent) estimation of the causal effects of one factor upon another depends upon correct specification of the equation describing the determination of the dependent variable, "specification" taken in its large meaning, viz., encompassing inclusion of relevant regressors as well as assumptions about the properties of the error term and its relation to included regressors.4

Concerning the set of regressors, if bias is to be avoided, then any factor which affects coital frequency and which is correlated with one or more of the factors of interest in this paper- age and marital duration, birth and marriage cohort, and time period of the observation- must appear in the equation (or be provided for by the estimation procedure). Scientific and clinical accounts (Rainwater, 1965; McCary, 1967; Masters and Johnson, 1970) suggest that any list of the basic determinants of coital fre- quency (CF) must include such factors as whether or not the wife is pregnant and whether or not the couple has a small child, which affect CF through their effects on healthiness and energy levels; both spouses' employment, which affects energy levels; and possibly both spouses' earnings, which provide leisure-time alternatives to sexual activity. Since all of these factors are correlated with age and/or marital duration, they must appear as regressors.

At this point it is clear that the determinants of CF include a set of linearly dependent terms. Wife's age, husband's age, and marital duration are each an exact transformation of time period and wife's birth cohort, husband's birth cohort, and marriage cohort, respectively. This prob- lem is addressed below.

Turning to an examination of the error term and of its relation to the included factors, a priori it would appear reasonable to regard the unobserved term as correlated with the re- gressors. The scholarly and clinical literatures (Kinsey et al., 1953; Rainwater, 1965), as well as the popular personal-development literature (Brody, 1976), suggest the existence and oper- ation of an unobserved couple-specific time- invariant propensity for sexual activity, a propensity which varies greatly across couples and which may affect many of a couple's choices and behaviors. If, ceteris paribus, the greater an individual's propensity for sexual activity, the younger he/she is at marriage and

4 Useful discussions are found in such texts as Judge et al. (1980), Judge et al. (1982) and Johnston (1984). See also Hausman (1978).

Page 7: Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time ...nielsen/soci708/cdocs/jasso_1985.pdf · Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time: ... survey, noting especially ... bridewealth

MARITAL COITAL FREQUENCY 229

the younger is his/her chosen mate, then mari- tal duration and at least one spouse's age (or, alternatively, the signed difference between the two spouses' ages) must be regarded as correlated with the unobserved term. Similar arguments can be made for pregnancy and for employment. Moreover, the couple's propen- sity for sexual activity may be correlated with the spouses' behavioral responses to the aging effects, if any: for example, the couple may reduce the time allocated to certain activities and increase the time allocated to others, may change their diet, etc. Indeed, if allowed to remain statistically uncontrolled, such behav- ioral responses could severely bias the esti- mated aging effects.

The foregoing comments suggest that all the regressors, except the age of one spouse, rep- resent behaviors which are reasonably re- garded as jointly determined with coital fre- quency. Thus, a priori it cannot be assumed that the CF equation satisfies the orthogonality assumption of ordinary least squares regres- sion.

Estimation Procedures

Estimation of the coital frequency equation presents two distinct problems: (a) correlation between error and regressor, and (b) linear de- pendence among regressors. A technique that enables both separation of the cohort effects from the age/duration effects and statistical control of the unit-specific unobservable is the fixed-effects (FE) or "within" estimator. The fixed-effects estimator has many appealing properties (Judge et al., 1980; Judge et al., 1982). The fixed-effects estimator controls for all time-invariant unit-specific factors; it therefore controls for all time-invariant cohort-specific factors. Fixed-effects estimates of the effects of time-varying determinants are unbiased and consistent; they are efficient if and only if orthogonality fails. Since there are strong prior reasons for supposing that ortho- gonality fails and since when orthogonality fails no other estimator is unbiased, the fixed- effects estimator appears ideal for the present research. Moreover, since the age/duration factors vary over time and the cohort factors, with which they are exactly linearly depen- dent, are constant over time, the fixed-effects estimator solves the problem of separating them.5 Further, interactions between cohort or

age/duration and events which took place in the past (such as age when the new contracep- tives were introduced) are also absorbed into the fixed effect, leaving the age/duration fac- tors free of cohort influences.

Although the two age variables, the marital- duration variable, and the observation date vary separately in their raw form, in the deviations-from-unit-means transformation employed by the fixed-effects estimator they become identical. Thus, if the operation of these four factors (the age/duration and period effects) is specified to be linear, then the FE estimator yields only one term for their com- posite effects.

In order to overcome this limitation, it is necessary to specify nonlinear operations for at least three of the four factors. Clearly, there is a trade-off between the number of maintained hypotheses and the number of estimated ef- fects. Fortunately, in the present case it would appear reasonable to specify the three age/ duration terms by nonlinear, strictly increasing transformations. The logarithmic transforma- tion would appear to be particularly appropri- ate, both positive and negative coefficients representing changes that become less and less steep with the passage of time; it has been widely used to model processes of biological growth and decay, as well as social- psychological maturation. Of course, these es- timates of the age/duration effects are also free of cohort effects.6

The fixed-effects estimator also yields an unbiased estimate of the unit-specific fixed ef- fects. Since this estimate of the couple-specific fixed effect contains not only the true couple- specific fixed effect but also cohort-specific effects, it can be used to assess the extent and direction of cohort influences.

Data

The data to be analyzed are drawn from the 1970 and 1975 rounds of the National Fertility Studies (NFS), which contain longitudinal in- formation on the 2361 white married couples who were in the 1970 national probability sam- ple and remained intact in 1975.7 Interviews in

5 Note that if age and cohort effects are both non- zero, then age is correlated with the unobservable. If substantive considerations strongly suggest the op- eration of both age and cohort, then specification tests, such as those proposed by Hausman (1978)-, are unnecessary; the estimation procedure of choice is the fixed-effects estimator, techniques such as

random-effects or pooled time-series/cross-section producing biased estimates.

6 Note that it is "additional information" which permits identifying the coefficients. However, in- stead of assuming zero effects for some of the coeffi- cients, as in the procedure proposed by Mason et al. (1973), we assume that some of the effects are non- linear.

7The National Fertility Studies were designed and directed by Norman B. Ryder and Charles F. West- off. For more information, see Westoff and Ryder (1977). The data sets were deposited with the Data

Page 8: Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time ...nielsen/soci708/cdocs/jasso_1985.pdf · Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time: ... survey, noting especially ... bridewealth

230 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

both rounds were conducted with the wives.8 The NFS are without doubt the best data set one can currently use for studying these ques- tions. The chief advantages are the longitudinal nature of the data (which facilitates statistical control of the couple-specific fixed effect and hence, separation of the cohort effects from the age/duration and period effects) and the pre- cise measurement of coital frequency.

Sampling. The 1975 round of the NFS rein- terviewed respondents from the 1970 round who were white, less than 25 years of age when they married, in a first marriage for both hus- band and wife, still married, and married less than 25 years in 1975. The sample of married couples is thus censored by marital duration and wife's age at marriage. Sample mortality occurred for a variety of reasons, the major ones being refusals (353) and failure to locate the respondent (81), bringing to 2361 the size of the sample of reinterviewed couples.9

Measurement procedures. Both rounds of the NFS asked the question, "In the past four weeks, how many times have you had inter- course?" The answer was recorded pre- cisely.'0 The number who refused to answer

the coital frequency item was 156 in the 1970 round and 183 in 1975. Of these, 43 respon- dents refused to answer in both years. Thus, a total of 296 cases were deleted due to missing data in either year, reducing the sample to a maximum of 2065 cases.

The time-related variables-wife's date of birth (WIFED9OB), husband's date of birth (HUSBDOB), date of marriage (MARDATE), date of interview in each round (DATINT)- were originally recorded in month and year and are coded for this investigation in decimal year (i.e., century month divided by 12). The ages of the spouses (HUSBAGE and WIFEAGE) and the marital duration variable (MARDUR) are constructed for each of the two survey rounds by subtracting the decimal-year date of birth and marriage from the corresponding interview-date term.

Turning to the other determinants of CF, dummy variables are included for whether or not the wife is pregnant (PREG) and for the wife's and husband's employment (WEMP and HEMP). An additional energy-related factor is likely to be the presence of young children. Ideally, one would want information on child- care arrangements and on the number of hours each child is in school. Such precise informa- tion being unavailable, information in the data set is used to construct a dummy variable (CH6) for presence of a child under six years of age (as of 31 December 1970 and 31 December 1975 for the two NFS rounds, respectively) and, presumably, not yet in school. Since age eligibility requirements vary across state, the last day of the calendar year is used as a con- venient though arbitrary cut-off point.

The NFS measured annual earnings in 1970 in 12 categories, ranging from "Under $2,000" to "$15,000 and over," and in 1975 in 17 categories, ranging from "Under $2,000" to "$35,000 and over." For this investigation, earnings are coded by the midpoint of the bot- tom (c-1) categories and by "$17,000" and "$37,500" for the top categories in 1970 and 1975, respectively. To render comparable the earnings amounts in the two rounds, the 1975 amount is expressed in constant 1970 dollars, with the "all items" version of the Consumer Price Index used to calculate the correction factor.

The Equations To Be Estimated

The substantive considerations and the char- acteristics of the available data lead to the fol-

and Program Library Service of the University of Wisconsin, which makes them available for public use.

8 Unfortunately, the data do not permit assess- ment of the extent or determination of husband-wife differences in coital-frequency reports. Note, how- ever, that, since the same individual (the wife) pro- vided the information at both observation periods, the fixed-effects procedure rids our estimates of any biases that might arise from an individual-specific time-invariant tendency to under- or overestimate coital frequency.

9 If the propensities to marry, to marry at a certain age, to remain married, or to participate in a survey are permanent attributes'of individuals and couples, then their operation is controlled for by the fixed- effects procedure and there is little cause for concern about bias due to selection mechanisms. Absent selectivity bias, general inferences may be drawn concerning the effects of age and of marital duration, as well as concerning the cohort and period effects (for the ranges of these variables observed in the study). Note that, were the data cross-sectional, cor- rection for selectivity bias would require use of Heckman's (1979) procedure.

10 Of course, as several colleagues have pointed out, the answer may or may not be accurate. If the results reported in this paper are interpreted as ef- fects on reported coital frequency, then it is in- teresting that the story they tell is a systematic one. That is, respondents are providing information that is consistent with a somewhat complicated algo- rithm combining the effects of many factors. Such responses could arise from a situation in which respondents, consciously or unconsciously, (a) hold a theory of the determination of coital frequency, and (b) wish their behavior to appear consistent with

the theory. Investigation of the part played by de- scriptive beliefs in the determination of reporting "errors" could prove most useful; however, it lies outside the scope of this paper.

Page 9: Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time ...nielsen/soci708/cdocs/jasso_1985.pdf · Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time: ... survey, noting especially ... bridewealth

MARITAL COITAL FREQUENCY 231

lowing specification of the basic coital frequency equation:

(1) CFt = p1 PREGit + 132 CH6it + 83 WEMPit + 834 HEMPit + Yh HUSBDOB, + yw WIFEDOBi + ym MARDATEi + ah HUSBAGEit + a, WIFEAGEit + am MARDURJt + r DATINTit + wi + eit

(i=1,N; t=1,2),

where all variables are as defined in the previ- ous section, wi denotes the couple-specific time-invariant latent effect, r measures the ef- fects of contemporaneous influences, the yj measure the effects of cohort-speciflic factors, and eit denotes the random disturbance for the ith couple at time t and is assumed to have mean zero and constant variance and to be independently distributed across couples and over time. To this basic equation are added, in different specifications, the spouses' separate and combined earnings.

The fixed-effects estimator of (1) yields un- biased estimates of the ,83j and one unbiased composite estimate, denoted r*, containing the sum of the effects (ah + aw + am + r) of the four variables whose deviations-from-unit- means transformations yield identical values.

Replacing the three age/duration terms in (1) by nonlinear transformations produces a new specification whose FE estimates include esti- mates of the effects a* of the transformed age and marital duration variables.

The fixed-effects estimator also yields an es- timate, denoted wco, of the couple-specific fixed effects, obtained from the formula (Judge et al., 1980:331):

(2) *i = Y1.j- ji.

where Xi. and Yi. denote each couple's own over-time mean. This estimate a; includes not only the truly couple-specific fixed effect but also the effects of all time-invariant variables, which in this research include the effects of the three cohort variables (viz., 'Yh, Ywl 'ym) as well as a new random error.'1 This estimate X will

" The estimated fixed effect thus also includes the effects, if any, of the spouses' ages at marriage, their absolute and signed age difference, and their schooling at marriage. Note that the fixed-effects procedure prevents the operation of these time- invariant factors from biasing the estimates of the age and marital-duration effects.

be referred to as the estimated sexuality pro- pensity. Correlations of the estimated sexuality propensity and the cohort variables reveal the magnitude and direction of cohort influences.

RESULTS Characteristics of the Panel of Married Couples Since the purpose of this research is to esti- mate the effects of age, marital duration, fac- tors associated with birth and marriage cohorts, and factors associated with the con- temporaneous environment, this section begins with a description of these time-related char- acteristics of the panel.

Table 1 reports the frequency distributions of marriage dates and of husband's and wife's birthdates, arranged in five-year cohorts. As Table 1 shows, the birth cohorts represented in the panel span the period 1916 to 1955 for hus- bands and 1926 to 1955 for wives. The presence of the two earliest cohorts (1916-1920 and 1921-1925) among husbands only is due to the fact that the sample was selected by wife's age and to the observed tendency for women to marry men who are older than themselves. The sample thus includes persons born during the "booming" 1920s, during the Depression, dur- ing World War II, and during the baby-boom years.

Table 1. Birth and Marriage Cohorts of the Panel of Married Couples

Husband's Wife's Birth Birth Marriage

Cohort Years Cohort Cohort Cohort 1916-1920 7 -

(0.34)a 1921-1925 38 - -

(1.84) 1926-1930 219 61

(10.62) (2.96) 1931-1935 380 319

(18.43) (15.47) 1936-1940 494 480

(23.96) (23.28) 1941-1945 532 567

(25.80) (27.50) 1946-1950 361 563 3

(17.51) (27.30) (0.15) 1951-1955 31 72 389

(1.50) (3.49) (18.87) 1956-1960 - 494

(23.96) 1961-1965 551

(26.72) 1966-1970 - 625

(30.31) N = 2062 a Percentages shown in parentheses below corre-

sponding frequencies.

Page 10: Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time ...nielsen/soci708/cdocs/jasso_1985.pdf · Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time: ... survey, noting especially ... bridewealth

232 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

Except for three couples married in the five post-World War II years, all the couples were married in the twenty years between 1951 and 1970. Almost twenty percent are potentially parents of baby-boom babies. Over half mar- ried during the decade of the 1960s, a period usually characterized by American involve- ment in the Vietnam War and by social ac- tivism on a range of domestic and foreign pol- icy issues.

The period effects, if any, result from sexuality-relevant features of the environment at the time the interviews were conducted-at the time, that is, during which coital frequency was observed. In the 1970 round of the NFS, interviews were conducted from October 1970 to March 1971, and in the 1975 round, from September 1975 to April 1976, all months in- clusive. The length of the interval between the two interviews (for each respondent) ranged from 54 months to 64 months, with the mean at about 58.82 months. Thus, any changes in coi- tal frequency attributable to period effects are due to changes in sexuality-relevant features of the environment between the respondent's interview dates in the two rounds.

Table 2 reports the means and standard de- viations of the principal characteristics at each of the two survey rounds. All variables except earnings refer to the time of the interview; the earnings terms measure earnings in calendar years 1970 and 1975. The mean wife's age in the 1970 round was 28.96 years and the mean husband's age 31.53 years. The youngest wife in the 1970 round was 15.50 years of age; the oldest wife in the 1975 round was 48.08 years. Among husbands, the youngest age in 1970 was 17.25; the oldest age in 1975 was 58.58 years. The mean marital duration in the 1970 round was 8.98 years. The most recently married couple had been married one month at the time

of the 1970 interview; the maximum marital duration in 1975 was twenty-five years.

Table 3 reports the means and standard de- viations of the changes between the two survey rounds, as well as the proportions of the panel registering an increase, a decrease, or no change on each variable. Coital frequency, while displaying a mean reduced by about .7 times in a four-week period, decreased in less than half of the sample, increasing in about 40 percent and remaining unchanged in about 13 percent. All other characteristics-fewer preg- nant wives, fewer couples with preschool chil- dren, increased wives' labor-force participa- tion, increased earnings-are what one would expect from knowledge of the respondents' ages and of published tabulations from the 1970 Census and the 1976 Survey of Income and Education.

Estimates of Age, Marital Duration, and Period Effects

Table 4 reports estimates of six specifications of the fixed-effects statistical model which, as discussed above, controls for all unmeasured time-invariant characteristics, including those associated with the birth and marriage cohorts. These estimates are thus free of all cohort ef- fects. The six specifications differ in two ways. First, while the first two specifications contain no earnings term, specifications (3) and (4) add total family earnings to the basic set of re- gressors and specifications (5) and (6) add in- stead separate terms for wife's and husband's earnings. Second, specifications (1), (3) and (5) contain one linear term to capture all changes in husband's age, wife's age, and marital dura- tion, as well as in the sexuality-relevant fea- tures of the contemporaneous environment, while specifications (2), (4) and (6) add to the

Table 2. Characteristics of the Panel of Married Couples

1970 round 1975 round

Variable Mean S.D. Mean S.D.

Coital frequency (number in last four weeks) 8.9389 7.1063 8.2304 5.7340 Date of interview (decimal year) 71.0183 .1167 75.9204 .0962 Wife's age (decimal years) 28.9638 5.8668 33.8659 5.8635 Husband's age (decimal years) 31.5265 6.6524 36.4286 6.6510 Marital duration (decimal years) 8.9754 5.6197 13.8775 5.6187 Wife pregnant (I=yes) .0912 .2879 .0330 .1786 Child under six (I =yes) .6004 .4899 .4525 .4979 Wife employed (I=yes) .3618 .4806 .4549 .4981 Husband employed (I = yes) .9534 .2107 .9583 .2000 Wife's earningsa (thousands of 1970 dollars) 1.7198 2.5231 2.1386 3.1530 Husband's earningsa (thousands of 1970 dollars) 9.6916 3.9976 11.5389 5.6253 Combined earnings (thousands of 1970 dollars) 11.2926 4.6546 13.4577 6.4797

N = 2062 a Missing data reduced the number of observations on wife's and husband's earnings to 1988 and on

combined earnings to 2054.

Page 11: Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time ...nielsen/soci708/cdocs/jasso_1985.pdf · Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time: ... survey, noting especially ... bridewealth

MARITAL COITAL FREQUENCY 233

Table 3. Changes Between 1970 and 1975 in the Panel of Married Couplesa

Change Percent Percent Percent Variable Mean S.D. Negative Zero Positive

Coital frequency -0.7085 7.6109 .4651 .1295 .4054 Time 4.9021 .1485 0 0 1.0 Log wife's age .1621 .0314 0 0 1.0 Log husband's age .1502 .0301 0 0 1.0 Log marital duration .6644 .5637 0 0 1.0 Wife pregnant -0.0582 .3348 .0868 .8846 .0286 Child under six -0.1479 .6353 .2866 .5747 .1387 Wife employed .0931 .5711 .1208 .6654 .2139 Husband employed .00485 .2679 .0335 .9282 .0383 Wife's earnings .4187 3.2732 .2993 .2988 .4019 Husband's earnings 1.8473 4.5070 .3189 0 .6811 Combined earnings 2.1651 5.7626 .3276 .0015 .6709

N = 2062 a All variables are measured as reported in Table 2. The number of observations of wife's and husband's

earnings is 1988, and of combined earnings is 2054.

linear term logarithmic transformations of the age and marital duration terms.

Before turning to examine the parameter es- timates, some general comments concerning all six specifications are in order. The fixed- effects statistical model may be estimated in at least two ways (Judge et al., 1980), viz., by performing an OLS regression in which all terms appear in their deviations-from-unit- means transformations and by performing an OLS regression in which all variables appear in

raw form and to the regressors are added a set of N dummies, one for each unit; when the number of time periods represented in the data is two, as in the present case, estimation may also be accomplished by performing an OLS regression in which all variables appear in their first-differenced transformations. While the three procedures lead to the same parameter estimates and to the same residual vector (Judge et al., 1982:478-82), they may lead to different estimates of the error variance, of the

Table 4. Fixed-Effects Estimates of the Determinants of Marital Coital Frequency

Specifications Variable (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) Date of -0.2213 -0.7231 -0.2387 -0.7509 -0.2358 -0.7290

interview (6.24)a (3.06) (6.40) (3.16) (6.01) (3.01) Log wife's 27.6133 28.3692 28.6552

age (2.05) (2.09) (2.08) Log husband's -6.4299 - -6.7987 -7.9204

age (0.49) (0.52) (0.60) Log marital -1.5008 - -1.5361 -1.5101

duration (3.08) (3.15) (2.99) Wife -3.8718 -3.7077 -3.8858 -3.7174 -3.7212 -3.5559

pregnant (7.63) (7.28) (7.65) (7.29) (7.19) (6.84) Child -0.8429 -0.5571 -0.7977 -0.5050 -0.7231 -0.4486

under six (3.06) (1.86) (2.89) (1.68) (2.53) (1.45) Wife .3867 .3720 .2941 .2701 .2724 .2700

employed (1.29) (1.24) (0.96) (0.88) (0.82) (0.81) Husband -1.4287 -1.2812 -1.5451 -1.4029 - 1.6679 -1.5154

employed (2.32) (2.07) (2.47) (2.24) (2.60) (2.35) Wife's - .0472 .0456

earnings (0.81) (0.78) Husband's .0510 .0577

earnings (1.33) (1.51) Combined .0482 .0525 -

earnings (1.62) (1.77) R2 (1) .0427 .0475 .0438 .0488 .0411 .0458 R2 (2) .6656 .6673 .6660 .6678 .6635 .6652 N 2062 2062 2054 2054 1988 1988

a Absolute values of t-ratios appear in parentheses under corresponding parameter estimates.

Page 12: Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time ...nielsen/soci708/cdocs/jasso_1985.pdf · Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time: ... survey, noting especially ... bridewealth

234 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

standard errors, and of R2. With respect to the standard errors, the correct standard errors have in the numerator the ratio of the residual sum of squares to the factor (N(T- 1)-k), where k denotes the number of explanatory variables (excluding, of course, the units' dummies); these correct standard errors can be obtained directly from either the dummy- variable or the first-differences procedures or can be calculated by applying a correction factor to those obtained from the deviations- from-unit-means procedure. The correct stan- dard errors are larger than the incorrect ones by a factor slightly greater than the square root of two (approximately 1.4); hence, obtaining the correct standard errors is essential for cal- culating t-ratios and levels of statistical significance. The t-ratios reported in Table 4 are based on the correct standard errors.

With respect to R2, the deviations-from- unit-means and the first-differences procedures lead to the same value of R2, denoted in Table 4 by the suffix "1": this measure may be inter- preted as the proportion of variation explained in the transformed dependent variable by the transformed regressors. The dummy-variable procedure leads to a different value of R2, de- noted in Table 4 by the suffix "2"; this measure may be interpreted as the proportion of varia- tion explained in the raw dependent variable by all the explanatory variables together with the unit-specific dummies. As Table 4 shows, the explanatory variables explain about 4-5 per- cent of the within-couple variation in coital frequency; the explanatory variables together with the couple dummies explain about two- thirds of the total variation.

Notwithstanding the strong indications given by the values of R2, the hypothesis of no unit- specific effects was formally tested using the F-test of a set of linear restrictions described in Judge et al. (1982:484-85). This test is im- plemented by comparing the residual variance obtained from the fixed-effects equation with the residual variance obtained from an OLS regression on the pooled, raw data. The values of the F-statistic obtained for the six specifi- cations reported in Table 4 range from 1.7840 to 1.8252; at approximately 2000 degrees of freedom in each the numerator and denomi- nator, these values are statistically significant to well beyond the .01 level. Thus, the null hypothesis of no couple effects (i.e., of inter- couple homogeneity) is rejected. It can be con- cluded that there are operating in these data time-invariant couple-specific latent effects. Whether these couple-specific effects are re- lated to the spouses' birth and marriage cohorts will be addressed below.

Turning to the parameter estimates, the re- sults obtained from specifications (1), (3) and

(5) show that a decrement in marital coital fre- quency of about .22 to .24 of one time in a four-week period is associated with each passing year. This estimate, as discussed above, compounds four effects-age, marital duration, and period effects-so that at most it can be concluded that at least one of the four effects is negative. The decrement at the mean interval between interviews (about 4.90 years) thus ranges from slightly over one time to al- most one-and-one-fifth times in a four-week period. That is, the total decrement in coital frequency associated with the passage of time (including advancing age and increasing marital duration) and incorporating changes in the sexuality-relevant features of the contem- poraneous environment between 1970 and 1976 is about three times in a year for each passing year. This decrement is consistent with a wide variety of underlying structures, including the following three: (a) the period effect is negative and of such large magnitude that it offsets pos- itive effects of age and marital duration; (b) there was no change in the sexuality-relevant features of the environment between 1970 and 1976, and all three age and marital duration variables have negative effects; and (c) the pe- riod effect is positive but offset by the negative effects of age and marital duration.

In order to separate the period effect from the age and marital-duration effects (and the latter from each other), nonlinear transformations were applied to the three age and marital duration terms. Four sets of transformations were applied in tandem, pro- ducing new specifications which left un- changed all other features of the initial specifi- cation, including the linear passage-of-time term. The four transformations applied were: (a) logarithmic, or In x; (b) square root, or x?; (c) negative-reciprocal, or -x-1; and (d) expo- nential, or ex. These four transformations were chosen because (a) they define strictly in- creasing functions, so that the estimated coef- ficients indicate the effects of increases in age and marital duration, and (b) they differ in con- cavity, the exponential function being concave upwards while the other three are concave downwards. The results (not shown) were re- markably similar-in level of statistical significance and in the direction and magnitude of effects-across the specifications containing the logarithmic, square-root, and negative- reciprocal transformations; in contrast, the ex- ponential terms produced estimates ap- proaching zero (with, for example, 26 zeroes to the right of the decimal point). Thus, these data reject the proposition that coital frequency either increases or decreases more and more steeply with age and marital duration. The ef- fects of these variables appear instead to be

Page 13: Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time ...nielsen/soci708/cdocs/jasso_1985.pdf · Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time: ... survey, noting especially ... bridewealth

MARITAL COITAL FREQUENCY 235

either increasing and concave or decreasing and convex, that is, less and less steep as age and marital duration increase. Put differently, the absolute magnitudes of both increments and decrements decrease over time. Thus, each passing year brings with it a smaller change than the previous year.

Since the results of the three increasing- and-concave transformations are so similar, we report only the results of one set of specifi- cations. We chose those including the logarithmic transformations, since they are probably the most widely used to model pro- cesses of growth and decay and the simplest to manipulate and interpret." These results, re- ported in columns (2), (4) and (6) of Table 4, indicate that the effect of wife's age on coital frequency is positive, statistically significant beyond the .04 level, and of rather large mag- nitude (estimates ranging from 27.61 to 28.66). In contrast, the effects of both husband's age and marital duration are negative, that of hus- band's age unambiguously not statistically sig- nificant but of absolute magnitude ranging from 6.43 to 7.92, that of marital duration highly statistically significant (beyond the .003 level) and of absolute magnitude hovering about 1.5. The linear time-period effect is highly statisti- cally significant (beyond the .003 level), nega- tive, and increases to an absolute magnitude ranging between .72 and .75 per year, or about triple the net negative effect estimated in spe- cifications (1), (3) and (5). Thus, these data suggest that husband's age and marital dura- tion, as well as changes in the environment between 1970 and 1976, are associated with decrements in coital frequency, while wife's age is associated with an increase in coital fre- quency.

The obtained estimates of the period effect suggest that changes in the contemporaneous influences on coital frequency are associated with a decline between the two survey periods of about one time per week. Put differently, the 'environment" appears to have been less con- ducive to, less reinforcing of, marital sexual activity in 1975 than in 1970.

Since the coefficient for log wife's age is positive, these results indicate that CF in- creases at a decreasing rate as the wife grows older-at least for the, age range represented in the sample, 15.50 to 48.08 years. The results show that the ceteris paribus effect of a five- year increase in wife's age is an increase in CF of about 6.16-6.39 times in a four-week period

12 Note that the magnitudes of the estimated ef- fects are sensitive to the particular form of the transformation employed. Thus, while these results indicate the signs of the first and second derivatives, they do not permit precise assessment of the steep- ness of the slopes.

if initial age is 20, about 4.26-4.42 times if initial age is 30, and about 3.25-3.38 times if initial age is 40.

Though the coefficient for log husband's age is not statistically significant, its negative sign suggests that CF decreases at a decreasing rate as the husband grows older-at least for the age range represented in the sample, 17.25 to 58.58 years. The results suggest that the ceteris paribus effect of a five-year increase in hus- band's age is a decrease in CF of about 1.43- 1.77 times in a four-week period if initial age is 20, about .99-1.22 times if initial age is 30, about .76-.93 times if initial age is 40, and about .61-.75 times if initial age is 50. Thus, the predicted decrease in CF attributable to advancing age of the husband, if any, is ex- tremely small.

The decrease in CF attributable to marital duration is very large during the first year of marriage, but subsequently declines less and less steeply. The decrement in CF that occurs in one year, counting from the lowest observed marital duration in the sample-one month-is about 3.85-3.94 times per four-week period. In the next year, however, CF decreases only .98-1.00 times per four-week period. That is, a couple's predicted coital frequency at the 13- month wedding anniversary is once a week less than at the one-month anniversary; at the 25- month wedding anniversary it is once a month less than at the 13-month anniversary. Looking at five-year intervals, the predicted decrement is about 1.04-1.06 times per four-week period between the fifth and tenth wedding anniver- saries, declining to about .33-.34 between the twentieth and twenty-fifty anniversaries.

Figure 1 depicts the graphs of the ceteris paribus effects of the spouses' ages and marital duration, over the ranges observed in the pres- ent study, estimated under the logarithmic- form assumptions.

Because, among continuously married couples, both spouses' ages and their marital duration increase by the same amount over time and because these effects, as shown, dif- fer in direction and magnitude, it would be useful to examine their joint activity with the passage of time. As husband's age, wife's age, and marital duration jointly increase, does coital frequency increase or decrease? In order to answer this question, one can construct a passage-of-time function. From (1), the com- bined effect on CF of age and marital duration can be expressed as a function of the passing of k years. The first and second partial deriva- tives reveal the total effect of the passage of time. It can be shown that this function de- pends on the form of the function assigned to the age and marital-duration variables, as well as on the age at marriage of the two spouses.

Page 14: Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time ...nielsen/soci708/cdocs/jasso_1985.pdf · Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time: ... survey, noting especially ... bridewealth

236 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

120

100 Wife's Age

80

o 60 z

0 40

U_ -J 20

o 0 Oat Marital Duration

-20

Husbands Age -40

15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 60

AGE IN YEARS, MARITAL DURATION IN YEARS PLUS FIFTEEN

Figure 1. Estimated Ceteris Paribus Effects of Spouses' Ages and Marital Duration on Coital Frequency

Notes: Graphs depict the estimates obtained in specification (4) reported in Table 4. Coital frequency is measured as the number of times in the four-week period prior to the interview.

In the case where logarithmic trans- formations are employed (as in the specifi- cations reported in Table 4), the results indi- cate that coital frequency varies nonmonotoni- cally over the course of the marriage, decreas- ing to a minimum with some rapidity, then increasing-first at an increasing rate, then at a decreasing rate-over the ranges of age and marital duration observed in this study. The two critical points-the minimum and the point of inflection-are themselves strictly increas- ing functions of the spouses' ages at marriage. For example, in the special case in which both spouses are the same age, if they marry at 20, then the minimum occurs at about the 18- month wedding anniversary and the point of inflection three months after the 7th wedding

anniversary; if they marry at 25, then the minimum occurs one month before the second wedding anniversary and the point of inflection occurs one month after the 9th wedding an- niversary. Figure 2 depicts graphs of the com- bined effects of spouses' ages and marital du- ration, for four combinations of age at mar- riage, for the logarithmic-transformation case.

Estimates of the Association Between the Propensity for Sexual Activity and Birth and Marriage Cohort

As discussed above, the fixed-effects specifi- cations reported in Table 4 yield estimates of the total couple-specific time-invariant latent effect. The estimate phi contains the combined

Page 15: Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time ...nielsen/soci708/cdocs/jasso_1985.pdf · Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time: ... survey, noting especially ... bridewealth

MARITAL COITAL FREQUENCY 237

85

80

>a F ~~~~W=25 H2,~ z a 75 =HAd w

H =2 Ct =20 H=n=25 I / 00

65

60 L 0 5 10 15 20 25

YEARS AFTER WEDDING Figure 2. Combined Estimated Effects of Spouses' Ages and Marital Duration on Coital Frequency over the

Course of the Marriage

Notes: Graphs depict the combined effects, from the estimates obtained in specification (4) of Table 4, for four combinations of wife's and husband's age at marriage (W and H, respectively). Coital frequency is measured as the number of times in the four-week period prior to the interview.

effects of all unmeasured time-invariant fac- tors, contains, that is, not only the truly couple-specific sexuality propensity co, but also the cohort effects (Yh, Yw' ym, compounding as well a new random error due to sampling var- iability. The question whether individuals of different birth cohorts and couples of different marriage cohorts are in marriages which dis- play a higher or lower sexuality propensity can be answered by inspecting the zero-order cor- relations between the estimated sexuality propensity and the dates of birth and mar- riage.'3 Three sets of correlations were ob-

tained, corresponding to the estimates of coi obtained from the preferred specifications, (2), (4) and (6), reported in Table 4. Table 5 reports these correlations. All are positive, indicating that each successive cohort displays an in- crease in the propensity for sexual activity. The correlation between the estimated sexual- ity propensity and wife's vintage is the strongest, ranging from .6606 to .6713. The correlation of the estimated sexuality propen-

1" While it would be desirable to obtain estimates of the causal effects of each of the birth and marriage cohorts, unfortunately, due to the correlation be-

tween t), and both age at marriage and the age difference between the spouses, unbiased estimates cannot be obtained (see Hausman and Taylor 1981). What are obtainable are estimates of the correlation between the estimated sexuality propensity and each of the three cohort terms.

Page 16: Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time ...nielsen/soci708/cdocs/jasso_1985.pdf · Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time: ... survey, noting especially ... bridewealth

238 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

Table 5. Correlations Between Birth and Marriage Cohorts and Estimates of the Couple's Sexuality Propensitya

Estimated Sexuality from Specification

Cohort Variable (2) (4) (6)

Wife's Birth Date .6606 .6713 .6607 Controlling for .6585 .6689 .6582

Wife's Education Husband's Birth Date .5754 .5834 .5692

Controlling for .5762 .5847 .5706 Husband's Education

Marriage Date .5886 .5966 .5855 Controlling for Both .6069 .6179 .6073

Spouses' Education a Zero-order correlations are reported on the first,

third, and fifth rows. Partial correlations are reported on the second, fourth, and sixth rows.

sity with husband's birth vintage, though less strong than that with wife's vintage by almost 9-10 percentage points, is nevertheless strong, with estimates ranging from .5692 to .5834. The correlation with marriage date lies between the other two, with estimates ranging from .5855 to .5966. These strong correlations suggest the operation of a positive trend in the sexuality- relevant features of the environments facing American children born between 1917 and 1955 and by American couples married between 1950 and 1970.

It is important to examine as well the partial correlations, holding constant schooling at marriage since the observed differences in the propensity for sexual activity across cohort could be due to differences in schooling. Table 5 also reports these partial correlations, which show that the correlations with birth cohort change by less than half of a percentage point and that those with marriage vintage change by two percentage points. Thus, net of differences in schooling, the sexuality-relevant features of the environments faced by American children, adolescents, and newlyweds appear to have been moving in the direction of supporting and reinforcing a greater quantity of marital sexual activity. 14

In order to capture more precisely the dif- ferences across birth and marriage cohort, es- pecially any nonlinearities which would not be captured in the correlations, the estimated sexuality propensity was regressed on a set of cohort dummy variables, each representing a five-year interval, separately for husband's birth cohort, wife's birth cohort, and marriage cohort, and both with and without the school-

ing terms. As before, the results were almost the same, whether the estimated sexuality propensity was obtained from specification (2), (4) or (6). Table 6 reports one set of results, those obtained using co* estimated from spe- cification (4) of Table 4. These results, almost all of whose estimates reach high levels of statistical significance and whose values of R2 are, as expected from the results reported in Table 5, in the range of .32 to .43, indicate an unambiguous departure from linearity in the association with husband's birth cohort and some tendencies to depart from linearity in the other two relations.

Looking first at the differences across hus- band's birth cohort, the figures of Table 6 show that, while the estimated sexuality propensity increases across cohort, the rate of increase is not constant. Instead, it would appear that the estimated sexuality propensity increases at an increasing rate through the 1941-1945 cohort and subsequently increases at a decreasing rate, so that the difference between men born in the 1946-1950 period and men born in the 1951-1955 period is even smaller than the dif- ference between men born in the period 1917- 1925 and men born in the period 1926-1930. The largest increase occurs between the 1936-1940 and the 1941-1945 cohorts, at a magnitude somewhat less than 4. The smallest increase occurs between the 1946-1950 and the 1951-1955 cohorts, at a magnitude of approx- imately 1.1 to 1.3.

Turning to the differences across wife's birth cohort, the increases in the sexuality propen- sity display a zigzag pattern, which due to its slightness over most of the range could be due entirely to random fluctuation. However, the unexpectedly large increase between the 1946-1950 and the 1951-1955 cohorts (of mag- nitude 4.9 to 5.1) suggests the possibility of a true departure at that point from linearity.

Notice that the smallest increase between adjacent wife's birth cohorts, of magnitude of approximately 3.4, occurring between the 1931-1935 and the 1936-1940 cohorts, is greater than all but one of the increases be- tween adjacent husbands birth cohorts. This suggests that the environment faced by Ameri- can girls was changing much more rapidly than the environment faced by boys. Put differently, these results suggest that the sexuality-relevant rearing practices and other social influences were changing differentially for boys and girls, perhaps laying the foundation for the sex-role conflicts and movements which would emerge in the late 1960s.

The marriage-cohort regressions suggest a constant increase in the sexuality propensity between the couples who married in 1951-1955 and those who married in 1956-1960 and be-

14 For a probing look at trends in other features of the environments faced by children and adolescents, see Alwin (1984).

Page 17: Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time ...nielsen/soci708/cdocs/jasso_1985.pdf · Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time: ... survey, noting especially ... bridewealth

MARITAL COITAL FREQUENCY 239

Table 6. Differences in Estimated Sexuality Propensity Across Birth and Marriage Cohorts

Husband's Wife's Variable Birth Cohort Birth Cohort Marriage Cohort

1926-1930 1.7444 1.7072 - (1.89)a (1.85)

1931-1935 4.0871 4.0075 3.7437 3.7200 - (4.60) (4.52) (5.14) (5.1 1)l

1936-1940 6.8712 6.8194 7.1138 7.0860 - (7.83) (7.79) (10.04) (10.01)

1941-1945 10.5049 10.4816 10.9452 10.9232 - - (12.01) (12.01) (15.59) (15.56)

1946-1950 13.1033 13.0581 14.6395 14.6029 (14.71) (14.70) (20.85) (20.80)

1951-1955 14.4018 14.1578 19.7129 19.5079 (10.96) (10.78) (21.81) (21.42)

1956-1960 - 3.8364 3.9999 (10.09) (10.74)

1961-1965 7.8284 8.1293 (21.11) (22.30)

1966-1970 10.7975 11.2593 (29.88) (31.51)

Wife's -0.1073 -0.4140 Education (1.75) (5.20)

Husband's -0.1740 -0.2211 Education (3.26) (3.41)

Constant -12.9596 -10.7292 -15.0857 -13.7306 -11.4395 -3.8252 (15.44) (9.92) (22.60) (13.45) (40.36) (4.51)

R2 .3281 .3316 .4334 .4343 .3354 .3637 a Absolute values of t-ratios appear in parentheses under corresponding parameter estimates.

tween the latter and those who married in 1961-1965, an increase of about 3.8-4.1. Be- tween the 1961-1965 and the 1966-1970 cohorts, however, the increase was smaller, of magnitude about 3.0-3.1. While this decline in the rate of increase could be due to sampling variability, it also suggests that the environ- ment faced by couples who married in 1966- 1970 might have substituted away from ad- ditional reinforcement of sexual activity toward reinforcement of other activities, such as those usually denoted by the term "social activism."

SUMMARY

This paper reported estimates of the ceteris paribus effects of spouses' ages, marital dura- tion, and contemporaneous period influences on coital frequency, as well as of the correla- tions between coital frequency and birth- and marriage-cohort factors, based on data drawn from a panel of over 2000 married couples interviewed in the 1970 and 1975 rounds of the National Fertility Studies. These estimates were obtained by (a) using the properties of the fixed-effects statistical model in order to sepa- rate the effects of cohort influences from the age/duration and period effects and to control for the operation of couple-specific unobserva- bles, and (b) using strictly monotonic nonlinear transformations in order to separate the effects

of wife's age, husband's age, and marital dura- tion.

Our main findings are as follows: Among women between the ages of 15 and 48, coital frequency increases at a decreasing rate as the wife grows older. Though we cannot reject the hypothesis that, among men between the ages of 17 and 58, age has no effect on coital fre- quency, the estimated coefficients suggest that there may be a slight negative effect, such that coital frequency decreases, the absolute mag- nitude of the decrement declining. For marital durations between one month and twenty-five years, coital frequency decreases, also less and less steeply, as marital duration increases. The combined effects of spouses' ages and marital duration reported in this paper are consistent with a curve of coital frequency which is non- monotonic over the course of the marriage, decreasing to a minimum within two or three years of the wedding, thereafter increasing- first at an increasing rate, then shifting to a decreasing rate at about the sixth to tenth wed- ding anniversary.

The sexuality-relevant environments faced by successive birth and marriage cohorts in the United States appear to have changed in the direction of supporting and reinforcing a greater amount of marital sexual activity. In the birth cohorts observed (1917-1955) the en- vironments faced by females changed at a con- siderably faster rate than those faced by males.

Page 18: Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time ...nielsen/soci708/cdocs/jasso_1985.pdf · Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time: ... survey, noting especially ... bridewealth

240 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

Moreover, while the rate of change of the envi- ronments faced by female birth cohorts was constant if not increasing, the rate of change of the environments faced by male birth cohorts shifted from increasing to decreasing after World War II.

With respect to period effects, we found the 1975 environment less conducive to sexual ac- tivity than the 1970 environment.

Thus, our work suggests that, when the strongly positive cohort effects are controlled by the estimation procedure, the effect of wife's age, at least prior to menopause, is positive, as almost universally believed from Ovid to Kinsey. With respect to husband's age, there is a tendency in the data to support the classical view that male sexual responsiveness begins to decline at the early age of 17 years: however, even if correct, the magnitude of the decline is very slight. Our analyses do not sup- port the strongly negative effects of aging re- ported in recent years and, indeed, imply that such findings result from the combination of (a) strongly positive cohort effects, and (b) the use of estimation procedures that do not take ac- count of these cohort effects.

REFERENCES

Alwin, Duane F. 1984 "Trends in parental socialization values:

Detroit, 1958-1983." American Journal of Sociology 90:359-82.

Barchas, Patricia, B. Ecker, W. Jose, B. Kopell and W. T. Roth

1975 "Brain activity (CVN) as affected by social comparison process (status)." Technical Report No. 52, Laboratory for Social Re- search, Stanford University.

Bennett, Neil G. (ed.) 1983 Sex Selection of Children. New York: Aca-

demic. Birren, James E.

1968 "Aging: psychological aspects." Pp. 176-96 in David L. Sills (ed.), International Ency- clopedia of the Social Sciences. Volume 1. New York: Macmillan.

Brody, Jane E. 1976 Jane Brody's The New York Times Guide

to Personal Health. New York: Times Books.

Caldwell, J. C., P. H. Reddy and Pat Caldwell 1983 "The causes of marriage change in south

India." Population Studies 37:343-61. Clogg, Clifford

1982 "Cohort analysis of recent trends in labor force participation." Demography 19:459-79.

Elder, Glen H., Jr. 1974 Children of the Great Depression. Chicago:

University of Chicago Press. 1979 "Historical change in life patterns and per-

sonality." Pp. 118-61 in Paul B. Baltes and Orville G. Brim (eds.), Life-Span Develop-

ment and Behavior. Volume 2. New York: Academic.

Farkas, George 1977 "Cohort, age, and period effects upon the

employment of white females: evidence for 1957-1968." Demography 14:33-42.

Fienberg, Stephen E. and William M. Mason 1978 "Identification and estimation of age-

period-cohort models in the analysis of dis- crete archival data." Pp. 1-67 in Karl F. Schuessler (ed.), Sociological Methodology 1979. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Freedman, Ronald 1968 "Fertility." Pp. 371-82 in David L. Sills

(ed.), International Encyclopedia of the So- cial Sciences. Volume 5. New York: Mac- millan.

Gebhard, Paul H. 1978 "Sexual behaviour, human." Pp. 593-601 in

The New Encyclopaedia Britannica, Mac- ropaedia 16. Fifteenth edition. Chicago: Britannica.

Glenn, Norval 1976 "Cohort analysts' futile quest: statistical

attempts to separate age, period and cohort effects." American Sociological Review 41:900-904.

1977 Cohort Analysis. Beverly Hills: Sage. Goode, William J.

1963 World Revolution and Family Patterns. New York: Free Press.

Guttentag, Marcia and Paul Secord 1983 Too Many Women? The Sex Ratio Ques-

tion. Beverly Hills: Sage. Hausman, Jerry A.

1978 "Specification tests in econometrics." Econometrica 46:1251-72.

Hausman, Jerry A. and William E. Taylor 1981 "Panel data and unobservable individual

effects." Econometrica 49:1377-98. Heckman, James

1979 "Sample selection bias as a specification error." Econometrica 47:153-62.

Hobcraft, John, Jane Menken and Samuel Preston 1982 "Age, period and cohort effects in demog-

raphy: a review." Population Index 48:4-43.

Huber, Joan 1984 "Sex ratio effects on the value of women: a

review of Guttentag and Secord." Contem- porary Sociology 13:154-55.

Huber, Joan, and Glenna Spitze 1983 Sex Stratification: Children, Housework,

and Jobs. New York: Academic. James, William H.

1979 "The causes of the decline in fecundability with age." Social Biology 26:330-34.

1983a "Decline in coital rates with spouses' ages and duration of marriage." Journal of Biosocial Science 15:83-87.

1983b "Timing of fertilization and the sex ratio of offspring." Pp. 73-99 in Neil G. Bennett (ed.), Sex Selection of Children. New York: Academic.

Johnston, John 1984 Econometric Methods. Third edition. New

York: McGraw-Hill.

Page 19: Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time ...nielsen/soci708/cdocs/jasso_1985.pdf · Marital Coital Frequency and the Passage of Time: ... survey, noting especially ... bridewealth

MARITAL COITAL FREQUENCY 241

Judge, George C., William E. Griffiths, R. Carter Hill and Tsoung-Chao Lee

1980 The Theory and Practice of Econometrics. New York: Wiley.

Judge, George G., R. Carter Hill, William E. Grif- fiths, Helmut Lutkepohl and Tsoung-Chao Lee

1982 An Introduction to the Theory and Practice of Econometrics. New York Wiley.

Keyfitz, Nathan 1977 Applied Mathematical Demography. New

York: Wiley. 1983 "Foreword." Pp. xi-xiii in Neil G. Bennett

(ed.), Sex Selection of Children. New York: Academic.

Kinsey, Alfred C., Wardell B. Pomeroy and Clyde E. Martin

1948 Sexual Behavior in the Human Male. Philadelphia: Saunders.

Kinsey, Alfred C., Wardell B. Pomeroy, Clyde E. Martin and Paul H. Gebhard

1953 Sexual Behavior in the Human Female. Philadelphia: Saunders.

Marini, Margaret Mooney and Peter J. Hodsdon 1981 "Effects of the timing of marriage and first

birth on the spacing of subsequent births." Demography 18:529-48.

Marshall, Gloria A. 1968 "Marriage: comparative analysis." Pp.

8-19 in David L. Sills (ed.), International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences. Vol- ume 10. New York: Macmillan.

Mason, Karen O., William M. Mason, Hallinan H. Winsborough and William K. Poole

1973 "Some methodological issues in cohort analysis of. archival data." American Sociological Review 38:242-58.

Mason, William M., Karen 0. Mason and Hallinan H. Winsborough

1976 "Reply to Glenn." American Sociological Review 41:904-905.

Masters, William H. and Virginia E. Johnson 1970 The Pleasure Bond: A New Look at Sexu-

ality and Commitment. Boston: Little, Brown.

McCary, James Leslie 1967 Human Sexuality. New York: Van Nos-

trand Reinhold. Rainwater, Lee

1965 Family Design: Marital Sexuality, Family Size, and Contraception. Chicago: Aldine.

Reiss, Ira L. 1960 Premarital Sexual Standards in America.

New York: Free Press. 1966 "The sexual renaissance in America."

Journal of Social Issues 22:1-37. Rindfuss, Ronald R. and S. Philip Morgan

1983 "Marriage, sex, and the first birth interval: the quiet revolution in Asia." Population and Development Review 9:259-78.

Rodgers, Willard L. 1982 "Estimable functions of age, period, and

cohort effects." American Sociological Re- view 47:774-87.

Rossi, Alice S. 1984 "Gender and parenthood." American

Sociological Review 49:1-19.

Ryder, Norman B. 1951 The Cohort Approach: Essays in the Mea-

surement of Temporal Variations in Demo- graphic Behavior. Ph.D. dissertation, De- partment of Sociology, Princeton Univer- sity. Reprinted in 1980 by Arno Press.

1965 "The cohort as a concept of the study of social change." American Sociological Re- view 30:843-61.

1968 "Cohort analysis." Pp. 546-50 in David L. Sills (ed.), International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences. Volume 2. New York: Macmillan.

Shock, Nathan W. 1978 "Aging, human." Pp. 305-311 in The New

Encyclopaedia Britannica, Macropaedia 1. Fifteenth edition. Chicago: Britannica.

Smith, Herbert L., William M. Mason and Stephen E. Fienberg

1982 "More chimeras of the age-period-cohort accounting framework: comment on Rod- gers." American Sociological Review 47: 787-96.

Smith, Raymond T. 1968 "Family: comparative structure." Pp.

301-313 in David L. Sills (ed.), Interna- tional Encyclopedia of the Social Sci- ences. Volume 5. New York: Macmillan.

Talmon, Yonina 1968 "Aging: social aspects." Pp. 186-96 in

David L. Sills (ed.), International Encyclo- pedia of the Social Sciences. Volume 1. New York: Macmillan.

Trussell, James and Charles F. Westoff 1980 "Contraceptive practice and trends in coital

frequency." Family Planning Perspectives 12:246-49.

Udry, J. Richard 1970 The Social Context of Marriage. Second

edition. Philadelphia: Lippincott. Udry, J. R., F. R. Deven and S. J. Coleman

1982 "A cross-national comparison of the rela- tive influence of male and female age on the frequency of marital intercourse." Journal of Biosocial Science 14:1-6.

Webster, Murray and James E. Driskell, Jr. 1983 "Beauty as status." American Journal of

Sociology 89:140-65. Weiss, Yoram and Lee A. Lillard

1978 "Experience, vintage, and time effects in the growth of earnings: American scien- tists." Journal of Political Economy 86:427-47.

Westoff, Charles F. 1974 "Coital frequency and contraception."

Family Planning Perspectives 6:136. Westoff, Charles F. and Norman Ryder

1977 The Contraceptive Revolution. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

Winch, Robert F. 1968 "Marriage: family formation." Pp. 1-8 in

David L. Sills (ed.), International Encyclo- pedia of the Social Sciences. Volume 10. New York: Macmillan.