Luke Bishop Research Project

download Luke Bishop Research Project

of 40

Transcript of Luke Bishop Research Project

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    1/40

    Luke Bishop

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminatewinning and losing in the knockout stages of

    the 2011 Rugby World Cup!!E-mail: [email protected]

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    2/40

    I have recently finished my BSc Sport and Exercise Science undergraduatedegree at Sheffield Hallam University. This is my final year dissertation,looking into the 2011 Rugby World Cup. I am looking to pursue a career inPerformance Analysis, and have been working as an analyst at Opta for the

    past year.

    If you choose to read my project, then thank you. Any comments or feedbackwould be much appreciated using the address provided on the first page.

    Luke Bishop

    !

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    3/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page ii

    Abstract

    The objectives of the study were to investigate what performance indicators

    discriminated winning and losing teams in the knockout stages of the 2011 Rugby

    World Cup, looking to gain a better understanding of the influence of certain law

    changes (Van Rooyen, Diedrick and Noakes 2010) on the tactical development of the

    game. A review of preceding literature identified several performance indicators that

    were found to be influential on match success in elite rugby; Lineout success,

    turnovers conceded, frequency of kicks out of hand, tackle completion, line breaks,

    penalties conceded and ruck frequency. An independent groups design was used,

    comparing the means of winning and losing teams in the knockout matches (n=8). All

    games were analysed by the primary investigator using a pre-set analysis template,

    which allowed coding of the selected performance variables. Operational definitions

    were assigned to each indicator prior to analysis. Intra- and inter-observer reliability

    tests were completed, with correlation coefficients and percentage errors provided for

    each variable. An effect size calculation was also applied to each variable. Following

    a Mann Whitney U test, winners were found to concede a significantly higher

    percentage of their penalties between halfway and the opposition 22m than losers (P

    = 0.026) and losing teams were found to carry the ball significantly more than winning

    teams (P= 0.035). Effect size calculations also identified meaningful, yet not

    significant findings for several other variables. It was concluded that the ability to

    concede penalties in more attacking positions and concede fewer turnovers, along

    with a kicking, territory-based approach was more beneficial than a possession-

    based one in the knockout stages of the 2011 Rugby World Cup.

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    4/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 1

    1.0 INTRODUCTION

    Since its inception as a professional sport in the 1990s, Rugby Union has

    advanced in terms of the scientific support that teams utilise. Nutritionists, strength

    and conditioning coaches and psychologists have been introduced into modern day

    rugby, all with the aim of enhancing the mental and physical performance of the

    players and team. One of the most recent advances is the introduction of video

    analysis support, aimed at identifying and improving match strategies by reviewing

    video footage to gain a tactical advantage over the opposition.

    Hughes (2004, p104) describes notational analysis as an objective way of

    recording performance so that key elements of that performance can be quantified ina valid and consistent manner. The aim of notational analysis is to provide both

    coaches and players with valuable information regarding sports performance that will

    advance their decision-making (ODonoghue 2006) and understanding of tactical

    issues in their sport, with an overall view to improving future performances (McGarry

    2009). Prior to the introduction of performance analysis into sport, coaches would

    have to make decisions based solely on their observation, which Franks and Miller

    (1991) found to be poor. They reported that international level soccer coaches could

    only recall 30% of what would be considered key events over the duration of a game.In addition to this, Bracewell (2002) suggests coaches struggle to remember rare

    events and are unable to put events they do recall into context. It is suggested that

    this is due to tension, emotion and individual bias a coach experiences during and

    after a game. Jenkins, Morgan and ODonoghue (2007) successfully integrated

    match analysis into the current coaching process of a netball team, highlighting that

    using notational analysis as a way of overcoming the recall limitations of coaches, is

    effective. Reilly and Gilbourne (2003) support these findings, suggesting that using

    notational analysis as a coaching tool can provide detailed and accurate feedback forthe coach regarding positive and negative aspects of performance.

    Lago-Ballesteros and Lago-Peas (2010) state that in order for identified

    performance indictors to be useful, they have to be associated with success.

    Therefore, studies have investigated how winning and losing teams differ with

    regards to these performance indicators. There have been numerous studies across

    several sports looking at the differences between winners and losers (Jones,

    Mellalieu and James 2004, Csataljay et al. 2009, Lago-Peas, Lago-Ballesteros and

    Rey 2011).

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    5/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 2

    To date limited research has been conducted into what performance indicators

    differentiate winning and losing teams in Rugby Union. Indicators that distinguish

    winning and losing found to be common across several studies are lineout success

    (Ortega, Villarejo and Palao 2009, Vaz, Van Rooyen and Sampaio 2010, Jones,

    Mellalieu and James 2004), turnovers conceded (Ortega, Villarejo and Palao 2009,

    Vaz et al. 2011) and number of kicks out of hand (Stanhope and Hughes 1997,

    Ortega, Villarejo and Palao 2009, Vaz et al. 2011). In addition, other secondary

    indicators have been identified between winning and losing teams. These included

    tackle completion percentage (Ortega, Villarejo and Palao 2009, Vaz et al. 2011), line

    breaks (Ortega, Villarejo and Palao 2009), and penalties conceded (Jones, Mellalieu

    and James 2004, Vaz et al. 2011). However, these studies have reported findings

    from a mixture of international and domestic competitions, some of which are league

    based and others are knockout based.

    Van Rooyen, Diedrick and Noakes' (2010) study into ruck frequency is the only

    article to look at knockout matches in the World Cup as a separate entity to the pool

    stage matches. They found contrasting results for ruck frequency and match success

    between the pool stages and knockout stages. This suggests that the format of the

    competition may have an influence on a team's tactical approach and what

    performance indicators are most important for success. Van Rooyen, Diedrick and

    Noakes (2010) suggested that in the 2011 Rugby World Cup, the findings for ruck

    frequency would be different to the 2007 tournament, due to the introduction of the

    Experimental Law Variations (ELVs). Of the thirteen ELVs introduced in 2008, ten

    were used at the 2011 World Cup.It is suggested that the ELVs may have an

    influence on other performance indicators in modern rugby based on Van Rooyen,

    Diedrick and Noakes (2010) findings.

    This study sought to investigate the key performance indicators at the 2011

    Rugby World Cup that distinguish winning and losing in the knockout stages. This

    research will further our understanding of the tactical development of the modern

    game, and how knockout rugby may or may not differ from other competition formats.

    2.0 LITERATURE REVIEW

    2.1. The use of Notational Analysis in Sport

    2.1a. How Notational Analysis is used

    Notational analysis aims to enhance performance by identifying the key

    performance indicators (KPIs) in a particular sport. Performance Indicators are

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    6/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 3

    described as a combination of variables that identify a certain aspect of performance

    and help to achieve success in a certain sport (Hughes and Bartlett 2002, Jones,

    Mellalieu and James 2004). They are used to outline the differences between

    winning and losing performances, thus allowing the coach to recognise which

    elements are the most critical in determining the result (Csataljay et al. 2009).

    Performance indicators for invasion games can be split into three groups: Match

    Indicators (e.g. tries scored, number of lineouts), technical indicators (e.g. tackles

    won/lost, number of completed passes) and tactical indicators (e.g. possession, pass

    distribution) (Bartlett 2001, Hughes and Bartlett 2002). Match indicators help to

    describe performance giving the analyst an idea of what the team or individual has

    done successfully or unsuccessfully throughout a game. However, these indicators

    can be misleading if presented on their own, offering no way of determining if

    performance was good. They should be presented in comparison to previous

    performances and/or the oppositions data, which puts the findings in context

    (McGarry 2009). Technical indicators identify which particular skills a team or

    individual are good or bad at. For example, if a rugby team consistently missed 50%

    of their tackles in a game, it could be inferred that their tackling technique is poor,

    meaning this could be addressed in future training sessions. Tactical indicators help

    to identify how a team or individual has played the game, e.g. in soccer, a team may

    focus their attack down the right wing, possibly to target the opposition left-back if

    they have been identified as being poor. In rugby union, a team may kick the ball to

    touch regularly if they feel that their opponents lineout is poor and can put the

    opposition under pressure. Hughes and Bartlett (2002) note that all types of sports

    (i.e. net and wall, striking and fielding) follow the same structure when identifying

    performance indicators, although the indicators are adapted to the rules of each sport

    (e.g. a match indicator in Tennis could be number of aces). However, the provision

    and application of feedback has been suggested to be the most important factor in

    improving performance (Hughes and Bartlett 2002, Liebermann et al. 2002), not

    merely the identification of key performance indicators.

    2.1b. Providing Feedback

    Feedback is often given to the coach using video (ODonoghue 2006). Video

    feedback improves clarity for coaches and players when trying to understand what is

    being presented. This feedback is underpinned by statistics created from match

    analysis. Statistics identify trends in performance indicators and give an unbiased

    and objective record of the game, which can determine certain areas that require

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    7/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 4

    attention (ODonoghue 2007, Vaz et al. 2011). These trends govern what feedback is

    given, providing an objective rationale for showing certain videos. It has been

    suggested that any feedback given on the basis of statistical compilation must be put

    into context (Bracewell 2002, McGarry 2009, Vaz et al. 2011) with regards to the

    opposition, pitch position and previous performances. Vaz et al. (2011) and

    Bracewell (2002) suggest using multivariate statistical techniques, by comparing

    statistics with the opposition, in order to create more powerful results relative to that

    particular match. This is supported by Tenga et al. (2009) who state that performance

    analysis research must reflect the interaction between the two opponents, due to the

    dynamic nature of invasion games, where the actions of each team or player is

    influenced by that of their opposition (Grehaigne, Bouthier and Bernard 1997, Lames

    and McGarry 2007). These statistics, if supported by video highlights, can provide an

    important insight into relative match performance. If not put into context, statistics

    produced are of little value, due to the lack of comparison with another amount. In

    summary, the most effective form of feedback is using video, due to its facility to

    improve clarity for both coaches and players. However, the underpinning of these

    videos should be provided by trends identified in match statistics, which evidence

    suggests should be reported in relation to the opposition and pitch position.

    2.1c. Reliability and Validity of feedback

    The most important methodological issue in performance analysis is ensuring

    valid and reliable results. Valid and reliable results are imperative to making sure that

    the aims and purposes of the study are effectively met (Tenga et al. 2009). Reliability

    is described as the consistency of measurements made using an analysis system

    (Wilson and Batterham 1999). In performance analysis, this would refer to the

    consistency in which performance indicators are measured over time. James, Taylor

    and Stanley (2007) report that reliability gives an indication of the validity of the

    findings of a study. In performance analysis, validity is the extent to which the coded

    events reflect what has happened during the analysed match.

    James, Taylor and Stanley (2007) highlighted that there are three sources of error

    in notational analysis; Operational Error, where the analyst presses the incorrect

    button; Observational Error, where the analyst fails to code an event; Definitional

    Error, where events are labelled incorrectly. It is suggested that to avoid definitional

    errors in the analysis, that operational definitions are given to each variable. Hughes

    (2004) states that these definitions need to be clear and precise, which will help to

    enhance the reliability of the results. However, James, Taylor and Stanley (2007)

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    8/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 5

    suggest that poorly written definitions can lead to uncertainty for the analyst, meaning

    events may be coded incorrectly; highlighting the influence they can have on the

    findings of a study. An inter-rater reliability test can be used to address any errors

    due to the misinterpretation of definitions. Atkinson and Nevill (1998) state that any

    study that involves measurement has to have a measurement tool that is reliable. An

    inter-rater reliability test will identify if the analyst is making regular mistakes.

    However, if the second analyst is also coding these events incorrectly, it suggests

    that there is possibly a problem with the operational definitions used (James, Taylor

    and Stanley 2007). An intra-reliability test will not detect errors due to ambiguous

    definitions, but will indicate how consistently the analyst codes events. Therefore, it is

    suggested that if possible, both types of reliability tests should be completed to

    provide a greater indication of the quality of the results presented.

    Table 2.1. An analysis of different reliability tests carried out in recent performance

    analysis research papers in Rugby Union.

    (Eaves and Hughes 2003, Boddington and Lambert 2004, Jones, Mellalieu and James 2004,

    Eaves, Hughes and Lamb 2005, James, Mellalieu and Jones 2005, Prim, Van Rooyen and

    Lambert 2006, Van Rooyen, Lambert and Noakes 2006, Van Rooyen and Noakes 2006,

    Sasaki et al. 2007, Williams et al. 2007, Ortega, Villarejo and Palao 2009, Van Rooyen,

    Diedrick and Noakes 2010, Vaz, Van Rooyen and Sampaio 2010, Wheeler, Askew and

    Sayers 2010, Williams, Hughes, ODonoghue 2010, Diedrick and Van Rooyen 2011, Vaz et

    al. 2011).

    The use of reliability procedures in previous performance analysis research has

    been varied. Hughes, Cooper and Nevill (2002) considered the reliability of

    procedures across 67 unspecified performance analysis studies. They found that

    Reliability Test Reported Number %

    Inter-analyst 3 17.5

    Intra-analyst 7 41

    Inter and Intra analyst 4 24

    None 3 17.5

    Total 17 100

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    9/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 6

    70% of studies did not mention any reliability procedures and 15% reported the use

    of correlations. However, Bland and Altman (2010) suggest that using solely

    correlations are often an inadequate method for confirming reliability. These studies

    may have completed reliability tests, even though not reported. The minimal

    discussion placed upon the reliability of their results suggests less importance was

    placed upon this part of the research, with more discussion based on the overall

    findings. It is suggested that more time should be spent discussing the reliability of

    the study, as this helps to enhance the overall value of the presented findings

    (Williams et al. 2007). A similar review (Table 2.1) is shown regarding the reported

    reliability tests of recent Rugby Union literature.

    2.2. Notational Analysis in Rugby Union

    2.2a. Development of the Game at the World Cup

    There has been a clear development in several areas of Rugby Union from

    amateur rugby during the 1980's to the modern-day professional era (Eaves and

    Hughes 2003, Eaves, Hughes and Lamb 2005, IRB 2003, 2005, 2007).

    The first development has been the increase in total match time by approximately

    five minutes from 1980's to 2000's. It has been suggested that this is due to an

    advance in the laws in the modern game, with things such as blood replacements,yellow cards and the Television Match Official all adding on to the total match time

    (IRB 2005). Half time is also now 3-4 times longer than during the 1980's (IRB 2005).

    It is suggested that this is required in the modern game due to the increased ball-in-

    play time (IRB 2005). Ball-in-play time has increased from averaging approximately

    21-23 minutes in the 1980's (IRB 2005), compared to 35 minutes in the 2007 World

    Cup (IRB 2007). An increased ball-in-play time leads to an increase in the number of

    game actions, and therefore, the physical effort players exert is greater, meaning half

    time is needed to be increased in order to allow players sufficient recovery time.In terms of game actions, there have been several changes identified over time.

    Eaves and Hughes (2003) report the game has moved towards a quicker, more ruck

    dominated game, with more phases of play in the professional era compared to when

    the game was amateur. IRB (2003, 2007) support this, reporting that the average

    number of rucks per game has increased from 69 per game (at the 1995 World Cup)

    to 144 per game (at the 2007 World Cup). These findings coincide with how often

    teams kick the ball, either in play or in to touch. Eaves, Hughes and Lamb (2005)

    report a 22.5% decrease in the average number of kicks per game from the amateur

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    10/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 7

    to professional era. This is likely to be due to the introduction of laws that meant

    teams gain no territory from kicking the ball directly in to touch from outside their 22m

    area in the modern era. From an average of 80 passes per game in the 1980's (IRB

    2005), numbers of more than 200 passes per game (IRB 2007) were reported at the

    2007 World Cup. These findings relate back to the increased ball-in-play time in the

    modern era, which result in increased game actions. Fewer handling errors per game

    have been reported in the modern era (IRB 2005). It can be inferred that this relates

    to an increase in skill level as the game has become professional. There has been a

    decrease in the number of scrums over time, averaging 31 per game in the 1980's

    compared to the 2000's where there is on average, 19 scrums per game (IRB 2005).

    This could be linked to the reduction in handling errors, which are a common source

    of scrums. In addition, a reduction in the number of lineouts is also apparent. Eaves,

    Hughes and Lamb (2005) report a greater average frequency of lineouts during the

    amateur era. An average of 52 per game was reported in the 1980's (IRB 2005)

    compared to 31 per game at the 2007 World Cup (IRB 2007). This is possibly due to

    the reduction of kicks in the modern era, which reduces the likelihood of a lineout

    occurring. The most recent influence on the development of the game has been the

    introduction of the ELVs in 2008. Of the thirteen laws introduced, three of them

    (Table 2.2) were designed to influence how teams play, with their aim being to

    reward attacking play.

    In summary, it is suggested that due to law changes and an increase in

    professionalism, teams have placed an emphasis on keeping possession of the ball,

    through rucking and recycling the ball through several phase. There has become less

    of a focus on kicking the ball either in play or in touch. These findings look at how the

    game has changed in terms of the overall amount of game actions, but does not

    identify what is the most preferential tactical approach to winning matches. The

    constant development in teams tactical approaches due to increased fitness and skill

    levels, and also due to changing laws, highlights the need for regular analysis into

    what strategies and approaches are most suitable and relevant in elite Rugby Union.

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    11/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 8

    Table 2.2.Experimental Law Variations still in place during 2011 Rugby World Cup.

    (IRB 2008)

    2.3. Research in Rugby Union

    Research into elite Rugby Union has varied. Some literature has looked at

    patterns of play and its development over time (Eaves and Hughes 2003, Williams,

    Hughes and ODonoghue 2005). Others have investigated the physiological

    demands of sport, how this differs between positions and what the ideal physical

    characteristics are at an elite level (Cunniffe et al. 2009, Austin, Gabbett and Jenkins

    2011). More recently, the development and identification of performance indicators

    has become more common (Hughes and Bartlett 2002, James, Mellalieu and Jones

    2005). These indicators represent different aspects of the game, such as possession,

    tackle completion and turnovers conceded. They are generally presented as either

    times, frequency counts or as percentages. Developing from research into identifying

    Law Effect on Game

    If a team puts the ball back into its

    own 22 and the ball is

    subsequently kicked directly into

    touch, there is no gain in ground.

    This ensures that defending teams do not

    have an unfair advantage over attacking

    teams by encouraging tactical kicking and

    counter-attacking skills.

    A quick throw in may be thrown in

    straight or towards the throwing

    teams own goal line.

    This increases the probability of a quick

    throw-in, providing a positive opportunity for

    the team taking the throw-in to run the ball

    instead of choosing a lineout.

    Introduction of an offside line 5

    metres behind the hindmost feet

    of the scrum.

    This increases the space available to the

    team who wins the ball at the scrum. By

    having all the forwards committed at the

    scrum itself and 10 metres between theback lines, more space is available to build

    an attack in.

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    12/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 9

    general performance indicators, current research has begun to investigate what

    performance indicators are related to success in elite Rugby Union (Ortega, Villarejo

    and Palao 2009, Van Rooyen, Diedrick and Noakes 2010, Vaz, Van Rooyen and

    Sampaio 2010). This is largely due to increased professionalism in the sport,

    resulting in the need for improved scientific and analytic support aimed at improving

    performance.

    2.3a. Performance Indicators Associated With Success

    Previous studies into which performance indicators determine winning and losing

    in Rugby Union have found differing results, but some indicators have been found to

    be common across several studies. Success at the lineout has been reported as

    being a determining factor between winning and losing in Rugby Union. Ortega,

    Villarejo and Palaos (2009) (p=

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    13/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 10

    lower percentage pass completion, which is likely to lead to situations where

    turnovers will be conceded. However, both of these studies fail to suggest whether

    pitch location of the turnovers have an influence on the match outcome, with neither

    putting their findings into any context, other than comparing winners and losers. From

    the limited evidence available, it appears that maintaining control of the ball when in

    possession, and forcing errors by the opposition when in defence are related to

    successful performance.

    How teams use possession of the ball can help to identify their game tactics. A

    team who produce a lot of passes and offloads may be typical of a team looking to

    play fast, expansive rugby, whereas a team who kicks the ball a lot, but attempts

    fewer passes, could be indicative of playing a more measured, territorial game.

    Previous research (Ortega, Villarejo and Palao 2009, Vaz, Van Rooyen and Sampaio

    2010 (p= 0.01)) has identified that winning teams produced more kicks in field and

    kicks to touch than losing teams. Ortega, Villarejo and Palao (2009) also suggest

    losing teams tend to pick and go from rucks, and pass the ball out wide more often

    than winners. These findings suggest that kicking the ball as a form of attack appears

    to be more beneficial than attacking with ball in hand. It could be suggested that if a

    team kicks well, they will create a territorial advantage, thus increasing pressure on

    the opposition. This will increase the likelihood of mistakes being made and point

    scoring opportunities being presented to the team who has kicked the ball. Jones,

    Mellalieu and James (2004) and Vaz et al. (2011) both report successful teams are

    able to gain more penalties in attacking areas of the pitch. They suggest this is due to

    a successful kicking and rucking game, which is used to build pressure. This

    increases the likelihood of penalties being conceded by the defending team, again

    suggesting a kicking oriented game is linked with match success. Van Rooyen and

    Noakes (2006) in a comparison between South Africa, England, Australia and New

    Zealand at the 2003 World Cup, reported that South Africa had more defensive kicks

    than the other three teams, who had a higher number of kicks in attacking positions.

    South Africa were the least successful out of these teams, so it could be inferred that

    pitch position of kicks could also be an indicator of match success, not just the

    frequency of kicks.

    Diedrick and Van Rooyen (2011) suggest that try scoring is strongly linked with

    overall success. This is supported by Jones, Mellalieu and James (2004) (p=

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    14/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 11

    Rooyen, Lambert and Noakes (2006) support the idea that game tactics alter during

    knockout matches, as the most successful team at the 2003 World Cup, England

    scored the most penalties and drop goals, with third placed New Zealand scoring the

    most tries. However, Ortega, Villarejo and Palao (2009) (p= 0.01) report losing teams

    to score more penalty goals than winning teams, perhaps devaluing their importance

    for overall match success. This could possibly be due to the winning team having a

    strong defence, meaning try scoring opportunities would be infrequent, leading to

    goal kicks being their most favourable option to score points.

    Ruck frequency was found to differ between winning and losing knockout matches

    at the 2007 Rugby World Cup (Van Rooyen, Diedrick and Noakes 2010). They

    reported that teams with a lower ruck frequency than their opponents won 100% of

    the knockout matches. This finding was opposite to the pool stage matches, where

    increased ruck frequency was associated with success. This suggests knockout

    rugby requires a different tactical approach to league based matches. It appears that

    the ability to take point scoring opportunities more often and the ability to defend well

    as a team is more beneficial during knockout rugby, rather than having control of the

    ball for longer than the opposition. Stanhope and Hughes (1997) found higher ruck

    frequency led to more success throughout the 1991 World Cup. Eaves, Hughes and

    Lamb (2005) suggest keeping possession and completing more rucks is more

    important now than in previous years in an analysis of 5 Nations and 6 Nations

    games from 1988 to 2002. They suggest a lineout almost guarantees possession to

    the side throwing the ball in, meaning kicking to touch is effectively handing

    possession to the opposition and is not an successful tactic. This suggests there has

    possibly been a development in the importance of rucking and maintaining

    possession of the ball, although there is not enough evidence to come to any definite

    conclusions at present.

    There have been other performance indicators reported to differ between winning

    and losing teams, but have not been reported to be as influential as previously

    mentioned indicators. Ortega, Villarejo and Palao (2009) report winning teams to

    have a higher tackle completion percentage than losing teams, in addition to making

    more tackles than their opponents during Six Nations matches. Vaz, Van Rooyen

    and Sampaio (2010) support these findings, reporting successful teams to make

    more tackles than the opposition during Super 12 fixtures. These findings suggest

    winning teams spend long periods without possession, due to the greater number of

    tackles they have to make. The fact winning teams are able to maintain a tackle

    completion percentage greater than that of their opponents while making a greater

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    15/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 12

    total number of tackles, suggests the ability to defend is key to success in Rugby

    Union. However, it could be that a combination of good attack from one team and

    bad defence from the other is key to success in Rugby Union. Wheeler, Askew and

    Sayers (2010) report top teams in the Super 12 competition are able to break tackles

    with 19% of their carries, compared to 16% for the mid-table teams and 11% for the

    bottom teams. These findings suggest top teams are more proficient in attack in

    comparison to less successful sides. However, it remains unclear whether their

    findings were as a result of good attacking by the successful sides or poor defending

    from opponents.

    Ortega, Villarejo and Palao (2009) report winning teams successfully break the

    defensive line when attacking more often than losing teams do. This is supported by

    Diedrick and Van Rooyen (2011) who reported that at the 2007 World Cup, an

    increase in initial breaks was related to match success. However, Wheeler, Askew

    and Sayers (2010) report that the number of line breaks achieved by a team is not

    associated with success in Rugby Union, but the number of defenders beaten seems

    to be a better predictor of success. However, these two studies looked at different

    competitions (6 Nations and Super 12) where there are clear dissimilarities in how

    teams play, offering an explanation as to why they reported contrasting results.

    2.4. Aims and hypotheses

    The purpose of this study was to examine which of the previously identified

    performance indicators are significant in determining the result of Rugby World Cup

    2011 knockout matches. These findings may provide a basis for possible training

    interventions and suggestions in tactics for knockout rugby. A comparison of the

    chosen performance indicators (Lineout success %, turnovers conceded, kicks out of

    hand, total carries, total passes, ruck frequency, tackle completion %, line breaks and

    penalties conceded) was made with findings from previous studies in order to allow

    inferences to be made as made as to what the influence of the Experimental Law

    Variations has been, and offer any suggestions as to how and why the game has or

    has not changed tactically. Suggestions can then be made as to what was the most

    suitable tactical approach during the 2011 Rugby World Cup knockout stages.

    It is hypothesised that, based on findings and suggestions of previous research,

    winning teams will have a more successful lineout, will concede fewer turnovers, kick

    possession away more often and complete less attacking rucks than losing teams

    during the knockout stages of the 2011 Rugby World Cup. In addition to these

    primary performance indicators, secondary performance indicators will show that

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    16/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 13

    winning teams complete a higher percentage of their attempted tackles, break the

    defensive line more often, and concede fewer penalties than losing teams.

    3.0 METHOD

    3.1. Design

    The study was an independent groups design. Both winning and losing teams

    from the knockout matches of the 2011 Rugby World Cup formed the basis for

    comparison. The identified performance indicators for each group were compared in

    an attempt to identify those factors that distinguish winning and losing teams.

    3.2. Sample

    Data were gathered from the knockout matches of the 2011 Rugby World Cup (n=

    8), resulting in eight winning teams and eight losing teams. Only knockout matches

    were chosen for analysis as teams at this stage are generally of a similar standard,

    meaning tactical differences in a team's approach can be crucial in determining the

    result. This differs from games in the pool stages, where a high proportion of games

    are won due to clear differences in playing ability. Thus, meaning a teams tactics are

    less important at this stage of the competition.

    3.3. Procedure

    The eight knockout matches were individually uploaded into the Rugby Union

    DVD 11 (Opta Sports Data, Leeds, United Kingdom) analysis software. The primary

    investigator undertook all of the match analysis. Each match was observed and

    coded using a pre-set analysis template (Appendix 2), which allowed the coding of

    the selected performance variables; Lineout success, Turnovers conceded, Kicks out

    of hand, Passes, Carries (divided into types of carry; Pick and Go, One Out Drive,

    Other Carry, Support Carry and Kick Return), Ruck frequency, Tackle completion,

    Initial breaks, Penalties conceded. Each variable coded was recorded onto a

    timeline; giving the match time it took place as well as the pitch co-ordinates, which

    indicate where on the pitch the event took place.

    After the games had been coded, the data recorded onto the timeline were

    exported from the analysis software into Microsoft Excel, (Microsoft Corporation,

    Washington, USA) where the final set of data for each match was displayed. Data

    analysis was then completed.

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    17/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 14

    3.4. Identification of Performance Indicators

    Performance Indicators were identified following a review of previous Rugby

    Union literature in performance analysis. Primary indicators had been found to differ

    between winning and losing teams common across several studies. Secondary

    indicators were reported to discriminate between winners and losers in some studies,

    but not to the extent of the primary indicators. The chosen indicators were adapted to

    produce a list of the analysis variables for this study. Once these variables had been

    identified, operational definitions (Table 3.1, 3.2) were given to each variable.

    Hughes (2004) notes how it is important these definitions are clear and precise to

    increase the reliability of the analysis.

    3.5. Reliability Procedures

    Both intra-observer (data compared between original analysis and re-analysis for

    three matches) and inter-observer reliability (data compared with an analyst who is

    equally familiar with the software for all matches) tests were undertaken. This was

    done to ensure that the analyst reliably recorded each performance indicator, and

    also to guarantee that the system used for the analysis had test-retest reliability

    (James, Taylor and Stanley 2007). The inter-observer test was used to supplement

    the information provided by the intra-observer test. Both reliability tests were

    undertaken on each performance indicator individually, providing a percentage error

    and correlation coefficient for each variable. Re-analyses for intra-observer reliability

    were completed two weeks after the original analysis.

    3.5a. Percentage Error

    Each performance indicator was compared, and a percentage difference between

    the two analyses calculated. This was measured using a simple test-retest

    agreement method to find the total % error between the original test (A), and re-test

    (B) scores, using the following equation (O'Donoghue 2010):

    ( A - B )

    Total % Error = 100 x

    ( A + B ) / 2

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    18/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 15

    Using this method, the percentage error was calculated by expressing the

    absolute error between two values as a percentage of the mean of the two values.

    Cooper et al. (2007) suggest that there should be an agreement of!90% between

    the compared analyses, i.e. "10% error.

    3.5b. Correlation Coefficient

    Pearson's coefficient of correlation was used for inter-observer reliability, whereas

    an Intraclass Correlation was used for intra-observer reliability. These tests

    determine the association between the sets of data. A correlation of 0.0 represents

    no association between the values, whereas a correlation of 1.0 denotes perfect

    agreement, and therefore good reliability. Values of#0.7 indicate a moderate

    correlation, #0.8 is good and #0.9 denotes a very good association.

    3.6. Data Analysis

    Descriptive statistics (means, medians and standard deviations) were provided to

    present the findings. The Shapiro-Wilk test showed that the majority of dependent

    variables were normally distributed. However, several variables were not normally

    distributed. This, accompanied by the small sample size, suggested a non-parametric

    approach. A Mann Whitney U test was used to identify any statistical differences

    between winning and losing teams at a statistical significance level of 95%.Due to the small sample size, effect sizes were reported for each variable, which

    will help to identify trends in the data. From these trends, suggestions could be made

    as to what variables may be found to be significant had the study been conducted

    using more matches. Effect size (ES) was calculated using Cohens (1969) equation:

    ES = (Mean1 Mean2) / Mean Standard Deviation

    Cohen reports figures of 0.2 (small ES), 0.5 (medium ES) and!0.8 (large ES) as

    guidelines for interpreting effect size.

    All data analysis was completed using Excel 2011 (Microsoft Corporation,

    Washington, USA) and SPSS 20.0.0 (IBM Corporation, New York, USA).

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    19/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 16

    Table 3.1. Operational definitions of Primary match indicators

    Primary Match

    Indicator

    Definition

    Lineout success (%) A successful lineout is determined when the attacking

    team win the ball cleanly, win a free kick or penalty, or

    win the ball in any other way. An unsuccessful lineout is

    when the defending team wins the ball in any of the

    methods identified above.

    Turnovers conceded When the attacking team give possession to the

    opposition through an error on their part (accidental

    offside, bad pass, carried dead, forward pass, unforced

    knock-on, carried in touch) or due to good work from

    the defending side (jackal, forced in touch, forced

    knock-on).

    Kick in play When an attacker kicks the ball in open play, from

    either inside or outside of their 22-metre area.

    Passes When an attacker attempts a pass to a teammate. This

    can be positive, i.e. complete, or negative (incomplete,

    off target, forward, intercepted).

    Carries When an attacker carries the ball towards the defensive

    line. Types include; Pick and go, One out drive, Support

    carry, Kick return and Other carry.

    Pick and Go When an attacker carries the ball from the base of a

    ruck or a maul.

    One Out Drive A carry made following one (usually short) pass from a

    ruck or maul, often to a forward, who takes the ball into

    contact.

    Support Carry A carry made following an offload from a team mate, or

    after a receiving a pass from a team mate who has

    broken the defensive line.

    Kick Return A carry made in the first phase following the reception

    of a kick by the opposition.

    Other Carry Any carry made that does not fit into one of the other

    four types of carry.

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    20/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 17

    Ruck Frequency The number of rucks the attacking team completes

    successfully throughout the match. A ruck is where two

    or more players from each team come together over the

    ball (which is on the ground), often following a tackle.

    Table 3.2 Operational definitions of Secondary match indicators

    Secondary Match

    Indicator

    Definition

    Tackle Completion (%). A completed tackle occurs when the ball carrier is held

    by one or more defenders, causing the ball carrier to go

    to ground, stay on their feet, or offload the ball from thetackle area. This will be recorded as a percentage of

    the total number of attempted tackles.

    Attempted Tackles Frequency of tackles attempted. This includes

    successful tackles where the tackle is completed, and

    unsuccessful ones, i.e. missed tackles.

    Initial Breaks. When the ball carrier cleanly breaks the defensive line,

    gaining a better territorial position for their team.

    Penalties conceded This is when the referee awards a penalty against theteam or player who have broken the laws of the game.

    4.0. RESULTS

    4.1. Reliability

    Reliability analyses undertaken on the data were completed in order to provide

    information as to the value of the presented results. Pearsons Correlation Coefficient

    (for inter-observer), Intraclass Correlation Coefficient (for intra-observer) and

    percentage error were provided for the variables. Table 4.1 presents intra-observer

    values, taken from the re-analysis of three matches. Table 4.2 shows inter-observer

    values obtained from a comparison of all matches with another analysts data.

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    21/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 18

    Table 4.1. Correlation Coefficients and % Error for Intra-observer analysis.

    aIntraclass Correlation Coefficient

    4.1a. Intra-observer Reliability

    Results from the Intra-observer analysis in Table 4.1 indicate that the majority of

    variables for winners and losers showed at least a moderate to good relationship.

    Four variables (Initial Breaks, Penalties Conceded, Turnovers Conceded and Lineout

    Success %) showed a perfect correlation (1.000) between the two analyses for

    winners and losers. The lone irregular correlation was found for tackle completion %

    for losers (0.588), which, although is positively correlated, shows a less than

    moderate relationship between the original analysis and re-analysis. Percentage

    Error was found to be within the 10% limit suggested by Cooper et al. (2007) for

    100% of the variables. The smallest percentage error was 0%, found for winners and

    losers in initial breaks, penalties conceded, turnovers Conceded and lineout success

    %. The largest error was reported for total passes (4.92%) for losing teams, which is

    still well within the recommended 10% limit.

    Winners Losers

    Variable Correlation

    Coefficient a% Error Correlation

    Coefficient a%

    Error

    Tackle Completion (%) 0.758 2.56 0.588 2.98

    Total Carries 0.990 2.91 0.879 3.72

    Total Passes 0.998 0.42 0.993 4.92

    Initial Breaks 1.000 0.00 1.000 0.00

    Total Kicks 0.929 2.06 0.969 2.44

    Penalties Conceded 1.000 0.00 1.000 0.00

    Turnovers Conceded 1.000 0.00 1.000 0.00

    Ruck Frequency 0.993 1.45 0.993 0.70

    Lineout Success (%) 1.000 0.00 1.000 0.00

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    22/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 19

    Table 4.2. Correlation Coefficients and % Error for Inter observer analysis.

    Winners Losers

    Variable Correlation

    Coefficient b% Error Correlation

    Coefficient b%

    Error

    Tackle Completion (%) 0.905 1.09 0.789 2.36

    Total Carries 0.994 2.98 0.982 2.00

    Total Passes 1.000 0.00 1.000 0.00

    Initial Breaks 1.000 0.00 1.000 0.00

    Total Kicks 0.992 1.67 0.999 0.53

    Penalties Conceded 1.000 0.00 1.000 0.00

    Turnovers Conceded 1.000 0.00 1.000 0.00

    Ruck Frequency 0.998 1.61 0.995 1.95

    Lineout Success (%) 0.976 1.46 0.981 1.59

    bPearsons Correlation Coefficient

    4.1b. Inter-observer Reliability

    Table 4.2 shows how good to perfect correlation coefficients were reported for all

    variables for the inter-observer analysis. Perfect correlations (1.000) were reported

    for four variables for winners and losers (total passes, initial breaks, penalties

    conceded and turnovers conceded). Percentage error was found to be well within the

    10% error limit, with the largest error being reported for Total Carries (2.98%).

    Overall, there was very good agreement between the two analyses.

    4.2. Key Performance Indicators

    Table 4.3 presents descriptive statistics (Median, Mean and Standard Deviation

    (SD)) for winners and losers. It also shows effect sizes from Cohens dtest and P

    values from the Mann Whitney U test for each of the analysed performance

    indicators. Variables are displayed as either frequency counts or percentages.

    Overall, there were very few significant differences between winners and losers,

    although there did appear to be some trends in the data. Two variables were found to

    be significantly different between winners and losers. Winners conceded a

    significantly higher percentage of their penalties between 50m and the opposition

    22m than losers (P= 0.026) (Figure 4.2c) and losers carried the ball significantly

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    23/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 20

    more than winners (P= 0.035). While the total number of carries was significantly

    higher for losing teams, there was no noticeable difference in the types of carries

    between the groups (Figure 4.1).

    Figure 4.1. Pie chart showing comparison between winners and losers in terms of

    each type of carry as a percentage of total number of carries.

    Figure 4.2. Bar chart showing comparison between winners and losers in distribution

    of a) Turnovers conceded, b) Ruck frequency and c) Penalties conceded, as a

    percentage of total number of each event. (* = Statistical significance, P= 0.026).

    * *

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    24/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 21

    Effect sizes in Table 4.3 showed that some variables differed between winning

    and losing teams, although not statistically significantly. Losers conceded more

    turnovers than winners (ES = 0.89, P= 0.153); Winners conceded a higher

    percentage of their turnovers between their own 22m and 50m than losers (ES =

    0.87, P= 0.205) (Figure 4.2a); Winners kicked the ball out of hand more often than

    losers (ES = 0.82, P= 0.223); Losers attempted more passes (ES = 0.91, P=0.078)

    and had a higher pass completion (ES = 0.85, P= 0.157) than winners; Losers

    completed more rucks than winners (ES = 0.81, P= 0.152), although no difference

    was noted in terms of where on the pitch these rucks took place (Figure 4.2b); Losers

    conceded a higher percentage of their penalties between their own 22m and 50m

    than winners (ES = 0.92, P= 0.088) (Figure 4.2c)despite there being no difference inthe total number of penalties conceded.

    .

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    25/40

    KeyP

    erform

    a

    nceIn

    dic

    ators

    thatdis

    crimin

    atewinning

    an

    dlo

    sin

    gin

    thekn

    ock

    outstagesof

    the2

    011R

    ugbyW

    orld

    Cup.

    Luk

    eBish

    op

    P

    age22

    Table4.3.D

    ifferen

    cesink

    eyperform

    an

    cein

    dicators

    betweenwinn

    ers

    an

    d

    losers

    durin

    gthekn

    ock

    outstag

    esof

    the2

    011R

    ugbyW

    orld

    Cu

    p.

    Winners

    Losers

    Variable

    Median

    Mean

    SD

    Median

    Mean

    SD

    EffectSize

    P1Value

    LineoutSucce

    ss(%)

    89.0

    0

    85.6

    3

    11.3

    9

    87.0

    0

    91.0

    0

    12.

    80

    0.11

    0.6

    6

    4

    TurnoversCon

    ceded

    13.5

    0

    13.7

    5

    3.11

    16.5

    0

    17.1

    3

    4.4

    5

    0.89*

    0.1

    5

    3

    Own22m(%)

    3.0

    0

    5.2

    5

    7.0

    9

    8.5

    0

    8.2

    5

    5.04

    0.4

    9

    0.24

    5

    22m-50m

    (%)

    34.5

    0

    35.3

    8

    9.5

    9

    30.5

    0

    25.8

    8

    12.14

    0.87*

    0.2

    0

    5

    50m-Opposition22m(%)

    33.0

    0

    37.3

    8

    18.21

    46.0

    0

    45.7

    5

    10.

    59

    0.5

    8

    0.27

    7

    Opposition

    22m(%)

    23.0

    0

    21.8

    8

    13.62

    20.0

    0

    20.12

    8.92

    0.1

    6

    0.6

    9

    7

    TotalKicksou

    tofhand

    28.5

    0

    29.6

    3

    5.21

    26.0

    0

    24.4

    0

    7.3

    3

    0.82*

    0.22

    3

    Kicksin22

    m(%)

    30.0

    0

    30.8

    8

    12.2

    9

    35.0

    0

    35.7

    5

    10.48

    0.4

    3

    0.3

    6

    8

    KicksinPlay(%)

    70.0

    0

    69.1

    3

    12.2

    9

    65.0

    0

    64.2

    5

    10.48

    0.44

    0.3

    6

    8

    Passes

    97.0

    0

    100.3

    8

    39.81

    154.5

    0

    139.0

    0

    45.

    01

    0.91*

    0.07

    8

    PassComp

    letion(%)

    96.0

    0

    96.0

    0

    2.62

    98.0

    0

    97.5

    0

    0.9

    3

    0.85*

    0.1

    5

    7

    Carries

    92.0

    0

    91.0

    0

    26.27

    114.0

    0

    11

    6.8

    8

    32.

    30

    0.88*

    0.035

    **

    PickandG

    o(%)

    16.0

    0

    15.6

    3

    7.87

    12.5

    0

    14.1

    3

    6.62

    0.21

    0.64

    4

    OneOutDrive(%)

    16.5

    0

    19.0

    0

    7.4

    3

    19.0

    0

    21.0

    0

    9.32

    0.24

    0.52

    1

    SupportCa

    rry(%)

    4.0

    0

    2.5

    9

    4.1

    3

    6.5

    0

    5.2

    5

    2.87

    0.41

    0.3

    9

    3

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    26/40

    KeyPerforma

    nceIndicatorsthatdiscriminatewinningandlosingintheknockoutstagesofthe2011RugbyWorldCup.

    LukeBishop

    P

    age23

    KickReturn

    (%)

    11.50

    13.00

    6.97

    9.00

    10.00

    4.14

    0.54

    0.51

    4

    OtherCarry

    (%)

    48.50

    47.38

    7.37

    48.00

    49.50

    8.00

    0.28

    1.00

    0

    RuckFrequency

    79.00

    76.00

    24.75

    100.50

    98.00

    29.49

    0.81*

    0.15

    2

    Own22m(%

    )

    4.50

    7.00

    6.46

    5.00

    6.75

    6.32

    0.04

    0.90

    4

    22m-50m(%)

    22.50

    27.38

    13.76

    22.00

    25.13

    6.24

    0.23

    0.89

    8

    50m-Oppo

    sition22m(%)

    47.00

    43.63

    13.55

    45.00

    47.00

    14.59

    0.24

    0.62

    7

    Opposition

    22m(%)

    27.00

    22.00

    11.16

    17.50

    21.25

    13.59

    0.06

    0.62

    5

    TackleComple

    tion(%)

    92.00

    91.75

    4.37

    91.00

    90.50

    2.45

    0.37

    0.52

    2

    AttemptedTac

    kles

    124.5

    122.75

    32.87

    103.00

    98.13

    39.75

    0.68

    0.16

    9

    InitialBreaks

    2.00

    2.50

    1.77

    2.50

    2.63

    1.06

    0.09

    0.62

    1

    PenaltiesConceded

    7.00

    7.75

    2.19

    8.50

    8.38

    2.67

    0.26

    0.55

    5

    Own22m(%

    )

    12.00

    16.00

    16.66

    15.50

    14.63

    11.41

    0.10

    0.81

    3

    22m-50m(%)

    27.00

    29.38

    20.87

    43.50

    45.44

    13.99

    0.92*

    0.08

    8

    50m-Oppo

    sition22m(%)

    35.50

    39.13

    18.31

    19.50

    22.00

    10.59

    1.09*

    0.026

    **

    Opposition

    22m(%)

    13.50

    15.50

    13.62

    10.50

    17.19

    8.92

    0.10

    0.89

    7

    1MannWhitneyU.

    *LargeEffectS

    ize(>0.80).

    **SignificantP

    value(

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    27/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 24

    5.0 DISCUSSION

    5.1. Summary

    The purpose of the study was to identify performance indicators that discriminate

    between winners and losers in the knockout matches at the 2011 Rugby World Cup.

    This would then allow suggestions to be made as to the tactical development of the

    game, and recommendations to be made about what was the most appropriate

    tactical approach during the 2011 Rugby World Cup knockout stages.

    The findings from the study indicate that there were very few clear differences

    between winning and losing teams. The only significant differences were that winners

    conceded a higher percentage of their penalties between the halfway line and the

    opposition 22m than losers, and losing teams carried the ball more often than

    winners. In addition to these significant differences, it was found that following an

    effect size calculation, other variables may have been significantly different between

    winners and losers had there been a larger sample size (winning teams kicked the

    ball out of hand more often than losing teams; winners conceded a higher

    percentage of their turnovers between their 22m and the halfway line than losing

    teams; losing teams conceded more turnovers, attempted more passes, had a higher

    pass completion, had a higher ruck frequency and conceded a higher percentage of

    their penalties between their 22m and the halfway line than winning teams).

    5.2. Differences between winners and losers

    Winning teams conceded a significantly higher percentage of their penalties

    between halfway and the opposition 22m (mean = 39.13%) than losing teams (mean

    = 22%). When comparing this to the finding that losing teams conceded a higher

    percentage of their penalties between their 22m and halfway (mean = 45.44%)

    compared to winning teams (mean = 29.38%), it could be suggested that pitch

    location of penalties conceded had an influence on the match outcome. Conceding

    penalties in the defending half increases the likelihood of conceding points through

    subsequent goal kicks, or from a possible territorial loss. This supports the view that

    winning teams are able to gain more penalties in attacking positions than losing

    teams (Jones, Mellalieu and James 2004, Vaz et al. 2011). Therefore, it is suggested

    that a teams ability to concede penalties further up the field, outside the point scoring

    range of the opposition, is indicative of success during the knockout stages of the

    2011 Rugby World Cup. The pitch position seems to be of more importance than

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    28/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 25

    simply the frequency of penalties conceded, where there was a negligible difference

    between winning and losing teams.

    Losing teams (mean = 17.13) were found to concede more turnovers than winning

    teams (mean = 13.75), which supports findings from previous research (Ortega,

    Villarejo and Palao 2009, Jones, Mellalieu and James 2004). There was found to be

    minimal difference in terms of pitch position of turnovers conceded between winners

    and losers apart from between 22m and halfway in the defending half. Winning

    teams (mean = 35.38) conceded more turnovers here than losing teams (mean =

    25.88). This finding was not anticipated as tries most frequently originate from this

    area of the pitch (Van Rooyen, Lambert and Noakes 2000, Boddington and Lambert

    2004). This would suggest that a higher number of turnovers conceded here is likely

    to be related to unsuccessful performance. However, it appeared not to be the case

    in the 2011 Rugby World Cup.

    Overall, findings from the current study suggest that the number of penalties

    conceded did not discriminate between winning and losing teams at the 2011 Rugby

    World Cup. However, pitch position of penalties conceded did appear to be influential

    on the match outcome. The capability of teams to concede a high percentage of their

    penalties in more attacking positions in comparison to their opponents was shown to

    differentiate winning and losing teams. Contrary to this, the frequency of turnovers

    conceded seems to help discriminate winners and losers more so than the pitch

    position that these turnovers take place. A lower frequency of turnovers conceded

    was apparent in winning teams.

    5.2a. Style of Play

    Losing teams (mean = 116.88) carried the ball significantly more than winning

    teams (mean = 91). However, there was found to be no differences in the types of

    carries by either group (Figure 4.2), which would give an indication of their tactical

    approach, i.e. more Pick and Go carries would indicate a more narrow, less

    adventurous approach, whereas more Other Carries would indicate a more

    expansive approach. It is suggested that winning and losing teams adopted a similar

    approach in terms of their distribution of carry types. These findings do not support

    Ortega, Villarejo and Palao (2009), who suggested that losing teams pick and go

    more often than winning teams. This difference is possibly due to the analysis being

    completed on different competition formats (6 Nations and World Cup) where it has

    appeared different tactical approaches are needed. Losing teams (mean = 139) were

    also found to pass the ball more often than winning teams (mean = 100.38), which

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    29/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 26

    supports Ortega, Villarejo and Palao's (2009) findings that losing teams pass the ball

    more frequently than winning teams. Losing teams percentage pass completion was

    higher than winning teams, which contradicts the findings of Ortega, Villarejo and

    Palao (2009), who reported a lower percentage pass completion for losing teams.

    This was an unanticipated finding, as pass completion and turnovers conceded are

    somewhat related (Ortega, Villarejo and Palao 2009), so it was expected that a

    superior pass completion would result in an inferior number of turnovers conceded.

    Winners (mean = 29.63) were found to kick the ball more often than losers (mean =

    24.40), which supports the previous findings of Ortega, Villarejo and Palao (2009)

    and Vaz, Van Rooyen and Sampaiao (2010) who all found winning teams to kick the

    ball as a form of attack more frequently than losing teams. It does however disagree

    with Eaves, Hughes and Lamb (2005) who suggested that keeping possession was

    more related to match success than kicking the ball. Although it was found that

    winning teams kicked more often in attacking positions than losing teams, this

    difference was minor. While this agrees with Van Rooyen and Noakes' (2006)

    findings that pitch position of kicks is linked to success, the findings from the current

    study suggest that the frequency of kicks was more influential on match outcome

    than the pitch position of these kicks. The findings suggest that kicking as a form of

    attack is more associated with success than choosing to run with the ball. This

    suggests that the impact that the Experimental Law Variations (Table 2.2) were

    designed to have, has maybe not worked. The laws introduced were designed to

    reward teams who look to counter-attack and run with the ball as opposed to kicking

    it. Based on the results of this study, it is suggested that running and passing the ball

    is not linked with success, signifying that the new laws have not favoured teams who

    play more expansively. Contrary to this, perhaps winning teams have simply adapted

    better to the law change with the use of better tactical kicking than losing teams and

    as a result are being rewarded for this.

    Losing teams (mean = 98) completed more rucks than winning teams (mean =

    76). These findings are in agreement with what was found by Van Rooyen, Diedrick

    and Noakes (2010) who reported that lower ruck frequency was linked to success in

    the knockout stages of a World Cup. However, the findings in the current study do

    not agree with the suggestion made by these authors that an increase in ruck

    frequency will be related to success in the 2011 Rugby World Cup due to the

    influence of the Experimental Law Variations. This again suggests that the

    Experimental Law Variations have not had the influence the IRB had intended, with

    more attacking play not appearing to relate to match success. It does seem that

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    30/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 27

    these laws are having an influence on the overall development of the game though.

    There was an average of 174 rucks per game in the 2011 knockout stages compared

    to 121 rucks per game in the 2007 knockout stages. Thus, suggesting that while the

    laws have had an impact on the number of rucks completed during a game, perhaps

    making the game a better spectacle for supporters, it appears that a rucking,

    possession based approach is not linked with match success.

    In terms of the style of play required for success in the knockout stages of the

    2011 Rugby World Cup, it seems a game based around good tactical kicking and

    territory was more effective than a possession dominated game plan, built around

    rucking and carrying the ball.

    5.3. Similarities between winners and losers

    There were some variables that were found to be similar between winning and

    losing teams, despite what previous research had suggested. These variables were;

    Lineout success %, tackle completion % and initial breaks.

    Firstly, a somewhat unexpected result was that the difference in lineout success %

    between winners (mean = 85.63%) and losers (mean = 91%) was statistically

    insignificant. In fact, the findings show that losing teams actually had a slightly better

    average lineout success %. This suggests that success in the lineout was not

    essential for success in the knockout stages of the 2011 Rugby World Cup. These

    findings contrast to several studies, which all reported that success at the lineout was

    indicative of match success (Jones, Mellalieu and James 2004, Ortega, Villaerjo and

    Palao 2009, Vaz, Van Rooyen and Sampaio 2010, Vaz et al. 2011). A possible

    reason for the findings of the current study opposing previous research is that the

    previous studies looked at different competition formats, namely 6 Nations and Super

    12, rather than specifically knockout matches. This further supports the idea that

    different match indicators may be important in different competition formats.

    There was also found to be no clear difference in tackle completion % between

    winning (mean = 91.75%) and losing teams (mean = 90.50%), which differs from

    what was reported by Ortega, Villarejo and Palao (2009). Despite, the tackle

    completion % being similar, it was noted how winning teams (122.75) attempted a

    greater average number of tackles per game than losing teams (98.13), although this

    was not reported to be a significant difference. This agrees with Vaz, Van Rooyen

    and Sampaio (2010) who report that winning teams make a greater number of

    tackles than losing teams. From these results it could be suggested that during the

    knockout stages of the 2011 Rugby World Cup, tackle completion % alone was not

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    31/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 28

    enough to give an indication of success. However, combining the percentage with the

    number of attempted tackles gives a greater indication of a team's defensive

    capabilities. This study suggests that maintaining a high tackle completion % whilst

    attempting a greater number of tackles than the opposition is related to match

    success. However, due to the insignificant difference between winners and losers

    here, it is suggested that further research into this area will give a greater

    understanding of the importance of this performance indicator.

    There was also found to no clear difference in the number of initial breaks made

    per game by winning (mean = 2.50) and losing (mean = 2.63) teams. This agrees

    with Wheeler, Askew and Sayers (2010), who reported that the number of line breaks

    by a team was not associated with success in Rugby Union. It does however,

    disagree with Ortega, Villarejo and Palao (2009) and Diedrick and Van Rooyen

    (2011) who both report winning teams to successfully break the defensive line more

    frequently. It could be suggested that due to the mixed results found regarding this

    performance indicator, that it perhaps is not vital to match success. It is suggested

    that the frequency of initial breaks has no influence on the outcome of knockout

    matches at the 2011 Rugby World Cup. Further research into this area is certainly

    required to gain a clearer understanding as to the influence of initial breaks on match

    outcome.

    These findings have suggested that, despite previous research suggesting

    otherwise, lineout success %, tackle completion % and initial breaks do not

    discriminate between winning and losing teams at the 2011 Rugby World Cup

    knockout stages. However, a high tackle completion % combined with a greater

    number of attempted tackles than the opposition may contribute to discriminating

    between winners and losers.

    5.4. Limitations and Future Recommendations

    5.4a. Sample Size

    One particular limitation regarding the methodology of the study was the sample

    size. As only the knockout matches were analysed, any firm conclusions can only be

    made specifically to the 2011 Rugby World Cup. The issue with a small sample size

    is that there is often not enough data to represent a population, meaning that results

    that are presented as being significant are sometimes not the case. Effect size was

    calculated in order to give an indication as to the meaningfulness of results, which

    helps to overcome the issue of a small sample. Although this calculation suggested

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    32/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 29

    that some variables (turnovers conceded, % turnovers conceded between own 22m

    and halfway, total kicks out of hand, total passes, pass completion, ruck frequency,

    % penalties conceded between own 22m and halfway) would possibly be significant

    had the sample size been larger, this calculation is not without its flaws. Cohen's d

    calculation uses the means of the two groups to calculate effect size. This means

    that a solitary unusual value may considerably affect the results (Thomas, Nelson

    and Silverman 2011). This could give a false indication of the meaningfulness of the

    results. Therefore, it is recommended that conclusions from this study should only be

    applied to the 2011 World Cup and not to knockout rugby as a whole.

    5.4b. Reliability

    Overall, the reliability of the study was sound. However, what was unexpected was

    that the inter-observer analysis showed more reliable results than the intra-observer

    analysis (Table 4.1 and 4.2). A possible explanation for this was that the intra-

    analysis was undertaken on only three matches in comparison to eight matches for

    the inter-observer test, meaning that the slightest discrepancies were likely to be

    magnified due to the very small sample size. It is recommended that reliability

    analysis should be undertaken on the same size sample for both inter-observer and

    intra-observer tests. Generally, the reliability of the study was strong, giving an

    indication that the results presented possess good validity.

    5.4c. Future Recommendations

    As this was the first study to look at identifying what performance indicators

    discriminate winners and losers in knockout rugby, specifically the 2011 World Cup, it

    can act as a starting point for future research into this specific area. The review of

    relevant literature, asserted by the findings from this study, have suggested that

    performance indicators that relate to match success are varied across competition

    formats. Therefore, future research should look to compare similar competition

    formats to attempt to build profiles of what match indicators are key in each

    respective format. It is suggested that to enhance the understanding of what

    performance indicators are key in knockout rugby, follow-up studies should be

    completed looking at recent knockout competitions, i.e. Heineken Cup, domestic

    league play-off matches. This will then create a larger pool of information, meaning

    better-founded conclusions can be made as to what is the best tactical approach in

    knockout rugby. It is also recommended that a review of knockout rugby takes place

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    33/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 30

    annually in order to maintain awareness of the development of the game in this

    format.

    5.5. Conclusion

    This study presents game statistics that help to identify what performance

    indicators discriminate winning and losing teams during the knockout stages at the

    2011 Rugby World Cup. These statistics can help to guide possible training

    interventions for sides competing in future knockout tournaments. Key conclusions

    are presented along with practical recommendations for each point.

    Firstly, the results of the study showed that the Experimental Law Variations had

    influenced the game in terms of the number of rucks that took place per game,

    signalling that teams were more prepared to carry the ball at the 2011 World Cup in

    comparison to previous years. However, despite this increase, it appeared that

    carrying and rucking the ball was not the best approach to win matches. It was found

    that kicking the ball, as a form of attack was more suitable, advocating a more

    territory-based approach as opposed to a possession one. Therefore it is suggested

    that kicking-based drills are included as an important part of training sessions prior to

    knockout competitions.

    In addition to this, a teams ability to concede fewer turnovers than the opposition

    is related to match success. Therefore, offence sessions based around rucking and

    ball retention in contact will help to reduce the incidence of conceding turnovers at

    the breakdown. Defence sessions based around competing at rucks, slowing the ball

    down and the jackal technique will also help to improve the chances of forcing

    turnover ball from the opposition.

    Conceding more penalties further away from the defensive try line also

    discriminated between winning and losing teams at the 2011 Rugby World Cup. A

    possible approach to help reduce the penalty frequency in the defensive half would

    be to not commit many players to the breakdown when on defence. Fewer players

    competing at the breakdown and more players set up in the defensive line will lead to

    a reduction in the chance of conceding a penalty, whilst the defensive line will not be

    outnumbered should the opposition move out wide in attack.

    In summary, this study shows that there were few significant differences between

    winning and losing teams at the 2011 Rugby World Cup. However, it was identified

    that the most appropriate form of attack was kicking the ball more frequently,

    whereas rucking, carrying and passing the ball more frequently was a less successful

    approach. A good tactical kicking game, in combination with the ability to concede

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    34/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 31

    few turnovers and concede a higher proportion of penalties in the attacking half were

    related to match success. A good defence was also found to have some importance,

    with winning teams able to complete a similar percentage of tackles to losing teams,

    whilst attempting a greater amount of tackles. It is important to note that the findings

    of this study should be used carefully. They only represent the knockout stages at the

    2011 World Cup, so to generalise them across different competition formats is ill-

    advised. Further research should look at other knockout competitions in an attempt to

    find the most effective tactical approach to knockout rugby.

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    35/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 32

    References

    ATKINSON, G. and NEVILL, A. M. (1998). Statistical methods for assessing

    measurement error (reliability) in variables relevant to sports medicine. Sports

    Medicine (Auckland, N.Z.), 26 (4), 217-217.

    AUSTIN, Damien, GABBETT, Tim and JENKINS, David (2011). The physical

    demands of Super 14 Rugby Union. Journal of science and medicine in sport, 14 (3),

    259-263.

    BARTLETT, R. (2001). Performance analysis: Can bringing together biomechanics

    and notational analysis benefit coaches? International Journal of Performance

    Analysis in Sport, 1 (1), 122-122.

    BLAND, J. Martin and ALTMAN, Douglas G. (2010). Statistical methods forassessing agreement between two methods of clinical measurement. International

    Journal of Nursing Studies, 47 (8), 931-936.

    BODDINGTON, Michele and LAMBERT, Mike (2004). Quantitative and qualitative

    evaluation of scoring opportunities by South Africa in World Cup Rugby

    2003. International Journal of Performance Analysis in Sport, 4 (2), 32-32.

    BRACEWELL, P. (2002). Implementing statistics in a diagnostic coaching structure

    for Rugby. Res. Lett. Inf. Math. Sci., 3 (1), 79-84.

    COHEN, J. (1969). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. San Diego,

    CA: Academic Press.

    COOPER, Stephen-Mark et al. (2007). A simple statistical method for assessing the

    reliability of data entered into sport performance analysis systems. International

    Journal of Performance Analysis in Sport, 7(1), 87-87.

    CSATALJAY, Gabor et al. (2009). Performance indicators that distinguish winning

    and losing teams in basketball. International journal of performance analysis in

    sport, 9 (1), 60-60.

    CUNNIFFE, Brian et al. (2009). An evaluation of the physiological demands of elite

    rugby union using global positioning system tracking software. Journal of Strength

    and Conditioning Research / National Strength & Conditioning Association, 23 (4),

    1195-1203.

    DIEDRICK, Emile and VAN ROOYEN, Michele (2011). Line Break Situations in

    International Rugby. International Journal of Performance Analysis in Sport, 11, (13)

    522-534

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    36/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 33

    EAVES, Simon and HUGHES, Mike (2003). Patterns of play of international rugbyunion teams before and after the introduction of professional status. InternationalJournal of Performance Analysis in Sport, 3 (2), 103-103.

    EAVES, Simon, HUGHES, Mike and LAMB, Kevin (2005). The consequences of theintroduction of professional playing status on game action variables in internationalnorthern hemisphere Rugby Union Football. International Journal of PerformanceAnalysis in Sport, 5 (2), 58-58.

    FRANKS, I.M. and MILLER, G. (1991). Training coaches to observe andremember. Journal of Sports Sciences, 9 (3), 285-297.

    GRHAIGNE, J. F., BOUTHIER, D. and DAVID, B. (1997). Dynamic-system analysisof opponent relationships in collective actions in soccer. Journal of Sports

    Sciences, 15 (2), 137-149.

    HUGHES, Mike (2004). Performance analysis - a 2004 perspective. InternationalJournal of Performance Analysis in Sport, 4 (1), 103-103.

    HUGHES, Mike and BARTLETT, Roger (2002). The use of performance indicators inperformance analysis. Journal of Sports Sciences, 20 (10), 739-754.

    HUGHES, M., COOPER, S-M. and NEVILL, A. (2002). Analysis procedures for non-parametric data from performance analysis. International Journal of PerformanceAnalysis in Sport, 2 (1), 6-6.

    IRB (2003). RWC 2003: Statistical Review and Match Analysis. [online]. Lastaccessed 25th March 2012 at:http://www.irbplayerwelfare.com/pdfs/gameanalysis/RWC_Analysis_2003.pdf

    IRB (2005). Changes in the Playing of International Rugby Over a 20 Year Period.[online]. Last accessed 25th March 2012 at:http://www.irb.com/mm/document/newsmedia/0/comparison1983to2003_541.pdf

    IRB (2007). Statistical Review and Match Analysis: IRB game analysis. [online]. Lastaccessed 25th March 2012 at:http://www.irb.com/mm/document/home/0/071031ctrwc07statscomm_3811.pdf

    IRB (2008). The IRB Guide to Experimental Law Variations. [online]. Last accessed19th March 2012 at:http://www.irb.com/mm/document/newsmedia/0/irbelvguideenfinal_5097.pdf

    JAMES, Nic, MELLALIEU, Stephen D. and JONES, Nicholas M. P. (2005). Thedevelopment of position-specific performance indicators in professional rugbyunion. Journal of Sports Sciences, 23 (1), 63-72.

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    37/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 34

    JAMES, Nic, TAYLOR, Joe and STANLEY, Simon (2007). Reliability procedures for

    categorical data in performance analysis. International Journal of Performance

    Analysis in Sport, 7 (1), 1-1.

    JENKINS, Rhian, MORGAN, Lynsey and O'DONOGHUE, Peter (2007). A case study

    into the effectiveness of computerised match analysis and motivational videos within

    the coaching of a league netball team. International Journal of Performance Analysis

    in sport, 7 (2), 59-59.

    JONES, Nicholas, MELLALIEU, Stephen and JAMES, Nic (2004). Team

    performance indicators as a function of winning and losing in rugby

    union. International Journal of Performance Analysis in Sport, 4 (1), 61-61.

    LAGO-BALLESTEROS, J. and LAGO-PENAS, C. (2010). Performance in teamsports: Identifying the keys to success in soccer. Journal of Human Kinetics, 25 (3),

    85-91

    LAGO-PENAS, C., LAGO-BALLESTEROS, J. and REY, E. (2011). Differences in

    performance indicators between winning and losing teams in the UEFA Champions

    League. Journal of Human Kinetics, 27 (3), 137-148.

    LAMES, Martin and MCGARRY, Tim (2007). On the search for reliable performance

    indicators in game sports. International Journal of Performance Analysis in

    Sport, 7 (1), 62-62.

    LIEBERMANN, Dario et al. (2002). Advances in the application of information

    technology to sport performance. Journal of Sports Sciences, 20 (10), 755-769.

    MCGARRY, Tim (2009). Applied and theoretical perspectives of performance

    analysis in sport: Scientific issues and challenges. International Journal of

    Performance Analysis in Sport, 9 (1), 128-128.

    O'DONOGHUE, Peter (2006). The use of feedback videos in sport. International

    journal of Performance Analysis in Sport, 6 (2), 1-1.

    O'DONOGHUE, Peter (2010). Research methods for sports performance

    analysis. London, Routledge.

    ORTEGA, E., VILLAREJO, D. and PALAO, J.M. (2009). Differences in game

    statistics between winning and losing rugby teams in the Six Nations

    tournament. Journal of Sports Science and Medicine, 8 (4), 523-527.

    PRIM, Sebastian, VAN ROOYEN, Michele and LAMBERT, Michael (2006). A

    comparison of performance indicators between the four South African teams and the

  • 7/31/2019 Luke Bishop Research Project

    38/40

    Key Performance Indicators that discriminate winning and losing in the knockout stages of the 2011

    Rugby World Cup. Luke Bishop

    Page 35

    winners of the 2005 super 12 rugby competition. What separates top from

    bottom? International Journal of Performance Analysis in Sport, 6 (2), 126-126.

    REILLY, Thomas and GILBOURNE, David (2003). Science and Football: A review of

    applied research in the football codes. Journal of Sports Sciences, 21 (9), 693-705.

    SASAKI, Koh et al. (2007). Scoring profiles and defence performance analysis in

    Rugby Union. International Journal of Performance Analysis in Sport, 7 (3), 46-46.

    STANHOPE, J. and HUGHES, M. (1997). An analysis of scoring in the 1991 rugby

    union World Cup for men. In Hughes, M. (Ed.) (1997). Notational Analysis of Sport I

    & II. University of Wales Institute Cardiff. Cardiff, Wales, pp. 167 176.