Lecture 11

19
LECTURE: 11 Memory

description

Psychology

Transcript of Lecture 11

Page 1: Lecture 11

LECTURE: 11

Memory

Page 2: Lecture 11

THE SENSORY REGISTERS Many Psychologists view memory as a series of

steps in which we encode, store and retrieve information, much like a computer does.

This is called the information processing model of memory.

The first step in the model is inputting data through our senses into temporary holding bins called sensory registers.

These registers give us a brief moment to decide whether something deserves our attention.

Page 3: Lecture 11

VISUAL AND AUDITORY REGISTERS Information entering a sensory register

disappears very quickly if isn’t processed further.

Information in the visual register lasts for only about a quarter of a second before it is replaced by new information.

If sounds faded from our auditory register as rapidly as this, spoken language would be more difficult. Information in the auditory register can linger for several seconds.

Page 4: Lecture 11

INITIAL PROCESSING: WHY DOES SOME INFORMATION CAPTURE OUR ATTENTION WHILE OTHER INFORMATION GOES UNNOTICED The next step in the memory process is

attention selectively looking at, listening to, smelling , tasting or feeling what we deem to be important.

The nervous system seems to automatically filter out peripheral information, allowing us to zero in on what is essential at a particular time.

Unattended information receives at least some processing, however, so we can quickly shift attention to it if it suddenly strikes us as significant.

Page 5: Lecture 11

SHORT-TERM MEMORY Short-term memory holds whatever

information we are actively attending to at any given time.

Its two primary tasks are to1. store new information briefly and

2. to “work” on information that we currently have in mind.

This second function is why STM is also called working memory.

Page 6: Lecture 11

CONTD.How does Chunking Improve the Capacity of STM?

Short term memory can hold only as much information as can be repeated or rehearsed in 1-1/2 to 2 seconds.

This is usually somewhere between 5 and 10 items.

By clustering bits of related information into larger groups(called chunking).

However, the amount of information held in STM can be expanded even though the 5-to-10 item limit still holds true. Each item is simply larger.

Page 7: Lecture 11

CONTD.Storage of Information in STM

Information can be stored in STM according to the way it sounds or the way it looks.

Verbal information is encoded by sound, even if we see the words written rather than hear them spoken.

Interestingly, the capacity for visual encoding in STM seems to be greater than for encoding by sound.

Page 8: Lecture 11

CONTD.Retention and Retrieval in STM

Information in STM disappears in 15-20 seconds unless it is rehearsed or practiced.

Decay theory holds that material that is not actively renewed simply fades away with the passage of time.

Interference Theory adds to this the idea of new information following old and pushing it out of the way. Both theories have merit.

Page 9: Lecture 11

ROTE AND ELABORATIVE REHEARSAL Rote Rehearsal: Repeating something over and

over again is known as rote rehearsal. It is useful for remembering something for a brief time, but it is not the most efficient way to remember something permanently.

Elaborative Rehearsal: The best way to remember something is to practice elaborative rehearsal in which you think about the meaning of new information and link it to information you already know. This kind of processing is deeper than simple rote repetition and more likely to transfer data from short term to long term memory (LTM).

Page 10: Lecture 11

LONG TERM MEMORY(LTM) In discussing long- term memory, psychologists

sometimes distinguish among semantic memory, episodic and procedural memory.

Semantic memory: Stores general facts and information, much as an encyclopedia or dictionary does, with numerous cross-references to related ideas.

Episodic Memory: Stores memories of events that have happened, much as a diary might.

Procedural Memory: Stores information relating to skills, habits, and other complex perceptual- motor tasks.

Page 11: Lecture 11

CONTD.Encoding information in the LTM

There are several ways we encode information in long term memory.

Sometimes memories are encoded in terms of nonverbal images, such as shapes, sounds, smells, tastes and touch sensations.

More commonly, however memories are encoded by their meaning. You extract the significant highlights from what you read, hear or experience, and lodge those in LTM, linking them to other material already stored there . In this way your LTM memories become categorized and interrelated, much like information in an elaborate filing system.

Page 12: Lecture 11

CONTD.Implicit and Explicit Memories

Implicit Memory: Human memory is filled with information not deliberately placed there. This is called implicit memory or memory for things that were either not deliberately encoded into long-term storage or were retrieved without conscious intention.

Explicit Memory: memory for information that is intentionally committed to and retrieved from LTM, such as the facts you learn when studying for a test.

Page 13: Lecture 11

CONTD.Storage and Retrieval in LTM

Not everything stored in LTM is recalled with precision. One reason is interference from bits of information that are similar to the one we want to retrieve. This can cause confusion and sometimes lead to the tip-of-the –tongue phenomenon.

The effects of interference are not limited to TOT episodes. In fact interference often explains why people can not retrieve information form LTM. Interference can come from two directions:

Retroactive Interference: The process by which new information interferes with old information already in memory.

Proactive Interference: The process by which old material already in memory interferes with new information.

In addition, people may unknowingly reconstruct their memories over time. For instance, they may recast negative memories to make them less painful, or they may embellish the past to fit a desired self-image.

Page 14: Lecture 11

EVENTS THAT ARE MOST LIKELY TO BE REMEMBERED Autobiographical memory: refers to

recollection of events from one’s life. Not all of these events are recalled with

equal clarity, of course and some are not recalled at all.

People generally can not remember events that occurred before age 2 (a phenomenon called infantile amnesia), and our memories are typically strongest for events that had a major impact on our lives or that aroused strong emotion.

Page 15: Lecture 11

WHAT FACTORS CAN INFLUENCE HOW WELL YOU REMEMBER A SPECIFIC INCIDENT. The values and customs of a given culture

have a profound effect on what people remember and how easily they recall it.

So do the emotions we attach to a memory, with some emotion- laden events being remembered for life .

Also effecting how well we remember are the strategies we use to store and retrieve information.

Page 16: Lecture 11

FLASHBULB MEMORIES Vivid flashbulb memories sometimes form for

events that are linked to some powerful emotion, such as shock, terror, grief or joy. Such memories can remain highly detailed and unchanged throughout the years because of the strong emotion that prevailed when they were formed.

Page 17: Lecture 11

IMPROVING MEMORY The key to improving long-term memory lies

in organizing and encoding information more effectively.

Techniques called mnemonics can sometimes help you do this. Examples of mnemonics are rhymes and jingles for remembering particular dates or other facts. Other mnemonics rely on mental imagery to provide retrieval cues.

Memory can also be improved by developing a strong motivation to remember and by practicing memory skills in a deliberate way.

Page 18: Lecture 11

WHERE IN THE BRAIN ARE MEMORIES LOCATED? Some memories may be localized in certain

parts of the brain. The cerebellum, for example stores some of the learning that occurs in classical conditioning.

In many cases, however, memories are stored more diffusely. This is shown by the fact that damage to one part of the brain may diminish but not erase many of our memories.

Page 19: Lecture 11

BIOLOGICAL FACTORS THAT CAN CAUSE MEMORY LOSS Accidents, surgery, poor diet, and disease can

lead to brain damage and memory loss. For instance, head injury often cause retrograde amnesia, an inability to remember what happened right before the accident.

The condition called Alzheimer’s disease is an example of memory loss due to disease , one that involves atrophy and abnormal growth of brain structures. Interestingly, the memory loss associated with chronic alcoholism may be caused by a vitamin deficiency due to poor diet.