Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and...

24
RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A ´ gnes Luka ´cs 1 *, Ferenc Keme ´ny 1,2 , Jarrad A. G. Lum 3 , Michael T. Ullman 4 * 1 Department of Cognitive Science, Budapest University of Technology and Economics, Budapest, Hungary, 2 Institute of Psychology, University of Graz, Graz, Austria, 3 Cognitive Neuroscience Unit, School of Psychology, Deakin University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia, 4 Brain and Language Lab, Department of Neuroscience, Georgetown University, Washington, DC, United States of America * [email protected] (A ´ L); [email protected] (MU) Abstract We examined learning and retention in nonverbal and verbal declarative memory in Hungar- ian children with (n = 21) and without (n = 21) SLI. Recognition memory was tested both 10 minutes and one day after encoding. On nonverbal items, only the children with SLI improved overnight, with no resulting group differences in performance. In the verbal domain, the chil- dren with SLI consistently showed worse performance than the typically-developing children, but the two groups showed similar overnight changes. The findings suggest the possibility of spared or even enhanced declarative memory consolidation in SLI. Introduction An increasing awareness of the importance of memory systems in language has inspired researchers to examine these systems in Specific Language Impairment (SLI). Much of the memory research in SLI has focused on working memory, suggesting impairments in this domain, in particular of verbal working memory [14]. More recently however, research has begun to examine long-term memory systems, in particular procedural and declarative memory. In this spirit, the Procedural Deficit Hypothesis (PDH) posits that the pattern of language and other deficits in SLI can be largely explained by abnormalities of brain struc- tures underlying procedural memory, in particular frontal and basal ganglia structures [5,6]. Crucially, the PDH also proposes that declarative memory generally remains relatively spared or even enhanced in the disorder, and that it plays important compensatory roles for grammatical and other impairments [6,7]. Although an increasing number of studies of SLI have focused on procedural memory [8], declarative memory has received much less attention. Declarative memory The declarative memory system is rooted in the hippocampus and other medial temporal lobe structures [912]. The system underlies the learning, consolidation (the stabilization of memories PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 1 / 23 a1111111111 a1111111111 a1111111111 a1111111111 a1111111111 OPEN ACCESS Citation: Luka ´cs A ´ , Keme ´ny F, Lum JAG, Ullman MT (2017) Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment. PLoS ONE 12(1): e0169474. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 Editor: Etsuro Ito, Waseda University, JAPAN Received: September 8, 2016 Accepted: December 17, 2016 Published: January 3, 2017 Copyright: © 2017 Luka ´cs et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Data Availability Statement: All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files. Funding: This work was supported by the Hungarian National Science Foundation (OTKA) under OTKA K 83619 ’Nonlinguistic abilities in Specific Language Impairment’ to A ´ gnes Luka ´cs, and by the National Institutes of Health under RO1 HD049347 and the Mabel H. Flory Trust to Michael Ullman. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Transcript of Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and...

Page 1: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Learning and Overnight Retention in

Declarative Memory in Specific Language

Impairment

Agnes Lukacs1*, Ferenc Kemeny1,2, Jarrad A. G. Lum3, Michael T. Ullman4*

1 Department of Cognitive Science, Budapest University of Technology and Economics, Budapest, Hungary,

2 Institute of Psychology, University of Graz, Graz, Austria, 3 Cognitive Neuroscience Unit, School of

Psychology, Deakin University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia, 4 Brain and Language Lab, Department of

Neuroscience, Georgetown University, Washington, DC, United States of America

* [email protected] (AL); [email protected] (MU)

Abstract

We examined learning and retention in nonverbal and verbal declarative memory in Hungar-

ian children with (n = 21) and without (n = 21) SLI. Recognition memory was tested both 10

minutes and one day after encoding. On nonverbal items, only the children with SLI improved

overnight, with no resulting group differences in performance. In the verbal domain, the chil-

dren with SLI consistently showed worse performance than the typically-developing children,

but the two groups showed similar overnight changes. The findings suggest the possibility of

spared or even enhanced declarative memory consolidation in SLI.

Introduction

An increasing awareness of the importance of memory systems in language has inspired

researchers to examine these systems in Specific Language Impairment (SLI). Much of the

memory research in SLI has focused on working memory, suggesting impairments in this

domain, in particular of verbal working memory [1–4]. More recently however, research

has begun to examine long-term memory systems, in particular procedural and declarative

memory. In this spirit, the Procedural Deficit Hypothesis (PDH) posits that the pattern of

language and other deficits in SLI can be largely explained by abnormalities of brain struc-

tures underlying procedural memory, in particular frontal and basal ganglia structures

[5,6]. Crucially, the PDH also proposes that declarative memory generally remains relatively

spared or even enhanced in the disorder, and that it plays important compensatory roles

for grammatical and other impairments [6,7]. Although an increasing number of studies of

SLI have focused on procedural memory [8], declarative memory has received much less

attention.

Declarative memory

The declarative memory system is rooted in the hippocampus and other medial temporal lobe

structures [9–12]. The system underlies the learning, consolidation (the stabilization of memories

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 1 / 23

a1111111111

a1111111111

a1111111111

a1111111111

a1111111111

OPENACCESS

Citation: Lukacs A, Kemeny F, Lum JAG, Ullman

MT (2017) Learning and Overnight Retention in

Declarative Memory in Specific Language

Impairment. PLoS ONE 12(1): e0169474.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474

Editor: Etsuro Ito, Waseda University, JAPAN

Received: September 8, 2016

Accepted: December 17, 2016

Published: January 3, 2017

Copyright: © 2017 Lukacs et al. This is an open

access article distributed under the terms of the

Creative Commons Attribution License, which

permits unrestricted use, distribution, and

reproduction in any medium, provided the original

author and source are credited.

Data Availability Statement: All relevant data are

within the paper and its Supporting Information

files.

Funding: This work was supported by the

Hungarian National Science Foundation (OTKA)

under OTKA K 83619 ’Nonlinguistic abilities in

Specific Language Impairment’ to Agnes Lukacs,

and by the National Institutes of Health under RO1

HD049347 and the Mabel H. Flory Trust to Michael

Ullman. The funders had no role in study design,

data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or

preparation of the manuscript.

Page 2: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

after their initial acquisition), storage, and use of multiple types of knowledge, including of facts

(semantic knowledge), events (episodic knowledge), and words (lexical knowledge). Learning in

declarative memory can be very fast, and can occur after as little as a single presentation of the

stimulus. The system underlies not only explicit knowledge, that is, available to conscious aware-

ness, but also implicit knowledge [10,13,14]. The functional neuroanatomy of the system has

been quite well studied. Briefly, the hippocampus and other medial temporal lobe structures are

critical for learning and consolidating new knowledge that depends on this system, though ulti-

mately the long-term storage of this knowledge depends largely on neocortical regions, particu-

larly in the temporal lobes [9–12,15]. Note that the declarative memory systems refers here to the

entire neurocognitive system involved in the learning, consolidation, representation, and use of

the relevant knowledge, not just to those portions underlying learning and consolidating new

knowledge, which is how some researchers refer to the system.

Declarative memory in SLI

Procedural memory has been increasingly well studied in SLI, with numerous studies now sug-

gesting deficits in this domain, including in consolidation [2,16–20]. For a recent meta-analy-

sis of procedural memory in SLI, which reveals clear impairments in this domain, see Lum,

Conti-Ramsden, Morgan and Ullman [8]. However, we still know very little about the status of

declarative memory in the disorder.

In the nonverbal domain, studies testing declarative memory have generally revealed largely

intact performance in SLI. No differences have been reported between SLI and typically devel-

oping (TD) groups in a range of tasks probing a variety of types of nonverbal stimuli, including

abstract visual and spatial information, faces, and complex nonverbal sounds [2,21–25]. When

such tasks have used easily verbalizable items (e.g., pictures of everyday events), SLI deficits

have been found in some [2,23] but not other [21,24] studies. Such impairments may be due to

the association of these items with language [26]. Type of task also seems to have an influence.

For example, Kuppuraj et al. [25] found intact nonverbal declarative memory performance in

SLI with incidental encoding and later recognition (similar to the approach employed here),

while performance lagged behind controls with intentional encoding and later recall.

In the verbal domain, studies have generally–but not always [21,27,28]–found impairments

in tasks probing declarative memory [2,21–24,27–31]. However, most of these studies used list

learning tasks, which rely heavily on working memory (due to the repetition of items during the

learning phase). Since verbal working memory is often impaired in SLI [1–4,6], any deficits at

these tasks could be due to problems with working memory rather of declarative memory itself

[2,6]. Indeed, verbal declarative memory deficits observed in a range of tasks in Lum et al. [2]

were reduced or eliminated after covarying out working memory (and were completely absent

after language abilities were controlled for). More recently, Lum, Ullman, and Conti-Ramsden

[32] found that in a list learning task only those children with SLI who had working memory

deficits showed impairments at declarative memory; those without working memory problems

showed normal performance at the task. Together, the data suggest that declarative memory

deficits in SLI may be due largely, if not entirely, to accompanying working memory deficits, as

well as to their underlying language impairments.

However, almost all studies of declarative memory in SLI have focused on the initial stages

of learning, in which the acquired knowledge is typically tested after a short delay of minutes

following encoding (enough time to reduce the likelihood of maintaining the information in

working memory). But subsequent retention of that knowledge is also critical, since the goal of

learning is generally to retain information beyond the range of a few minutes. This is especially

the case, of course, with language. To our knowledge, however, there is only one study that

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 2 / 23

Competing Interests: The authors have declared

that no competing interests exist.

Page 3: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

specifically examined longer-term retention, i.e., one day or more after initial learning [33]. In

this study, word learning was tested in a heterogeneous group of young adults with different

forms of language impairment (with previous diagnoses not only of “language impairment”,

but also of dyslexia, learning disability, dysnomia, or a combination of these) and was com-

pared to a TD group of the same age. The study was designed to separately examine learning

word forms, word referents, and links between form and referent. Retrieval of items was tested

immediately after training; after 12 hours (either involving sleep or not); 24 hours after train-

ing; and one week later. The main result relevant to retention was that after delays involving

sleep the language-impaired group did not differ from the TD group at remembering referents

or form-referent links, but did show impairments at retrieving word forms. The authors con-

cluded that the “consolidation of declarative memory is a relative strength for young adults

with LI [language impairment]”, though primarily when the items were not purely verbal in

nature, perhaps since learning novel word forms may also depend on procedural memory [6].

However, weaknesses of the study involving participant criteria suggest caution in generalizing

the results. Moreover, a dearth of direct group comparisons in the paper makes it difficult to

interpret the results. Finally, purely nonverbal information was not tested, since even the non-

verbal referents were learned initially in the context of the novel words that referred to them.

Together with the fact that this is the only study to date to examine retention in SLI suggests

that further studies are needed.

The present study

The aim of the present study was to examine both learning and retention in children with SLI,

as compared to TD children, in both nonverbal and verbal domains. The study was designed

to minimize the influence of functions that can affect performance on declarative memory

tasks, and are often impaired in SLI, namely working memory and free recall–both of which

depend on frontal/basal ganglia circuits, which appear to be abnormal in SLI [6,34]. We there-

fore tested declarative memory with a recognition memory task, following incidental encod-

ing. To test initial learning we probed recognition memory 10 minutes after encoding. Both

nonverbal items (pictures of real and novel objects) and verbal items (auditorily presented real

and novel words) were examined. To test for retention we probed recognition memory of the

same items 24 hours later.

Consistent with the PDH and previous studies, we predicted largely intact recognition mem-

ory for nonverbal items in SLI at initial learning (after the short delay of 10 minutes). In con-

trast, we expected the possibility of impairments at recognition memory for verbal items at this

time point. Given the dearth of previous evidence regarding retention in declarative memory in

SLI, we had no clear predictions for recognition memory following the overnight delay.

Materials and Methods

The Budapest University of Technology and Economics Behavioral and Biomedical Institu-

tional Review Board reviewed and approved the study (IRB #: IRB00004964 Project Title: Non-

linguistic abilities in Specific Language Impairment). All children were tested with the informed

written consent of their parents (by asking them individually to sign a detailed consent form),

in accordance with the principles set out in the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the

Budapest University of Technology and Economics Behavioral and Biomedical Institutional

Review Board. In approving this Research Project, the Review Board followed the requirements

of the Common Rule and the Helsinki Agreement.

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 3 / 23

Page 4: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

Participants

Children with SLI were recruited from two special schools for children with language impairment,

which were in and near Budapest, Hungary. The children had been referred to these schools by

speech and language therapists working in clinical practice. Recruitment and screening for this

study lasted between 2 and 3 months at each school. No eligible children declined participation.

All children in the SLI group met the following inclusion and exclusion criteria, which are com-

monly used in SLI research [1,35]. They scored at least 1.5 standard deviations (SDs) below the

mean for their age on at least two of the following four language screening tests: (1) a Hungarian

version of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test [36,37]; (2) the Hungarian version of the Test for

the Reception of Grammar [38,39]; (3) the Hungarian Sentence Repetition Test [40]; and (4) a

Hungarian nonword repetition test [41]. Their nonverbal IQ as measured by the Raven Progres-

sive Matrices (RPM) was in the normal range, corresponding to a score above 85 IQ points [42].

Their hearing was also assessed as normal. Typically developing children were recruited from two

regular schools, which had no special selection processes for children. All TD children scored nor-

mally, that is, above 1.5 SDs below the mean for their age, on all four language screening tests. The

TD children were matched individually (pair-wise) to children with SLI on chronological age and

sex; the two groups were also matched group-wise on nonverbal IQ [42]. None of the TD children

were known to have been diagnosed with any neurodevelopmental, psychiatric, or neurological

disorder. Similarly, none of the children with SLI had any known current or past neurodevelop-

mental, psychiatric, or neurological disorders other than SLI; e.g., children with comorbid autism

or ADHD were excluded. A total of 42 children were tested: 21 with SLI and 21 who were typically

developing (TD). Demographic and screening data for the two groups are shown in Table 1.

Procedure

Declarative memory was tested with a recognition memory task developed by the Brain and

Language Lab at Georgetown University, and modified as appropriate for the Hungarian

Table 1. Demographic and screening data for the two groups.

SLI TD Group

Comparisons

n 21 21

Sex 15M, 6F 15M, 6F

Age (years) 8.89 (1.06) 8.85 (1.03) F(1, 40) = 0.02,

p = .884

Vocabulary (PPVT; raw scores) 98.00 (19.60) 124.43 (12.50) F(1, 40) = 27.14,

p < .001

Grammar (TROG; blocks raw score) 13.52 (2.02) 18.05 (1.47) F(1, 40) = 69.21,

p < .001

Sentence Repetition (raw scores) 20.24 (8.33) 37.67 (2.80) F(1, 40) = 82.60,

p < .001

Nonword Repetition (span) 3.29 (1.27) 6.48 (0.98) F(1, 40) = 82.98,

p < .001

Nonverbal IQ (RPM; standard score) 103.90 (9.82) 107.38 (11.19) F(1, 40) = 1.14,

p = .291

Note. Means (and standard deviations) are shown for each variable. Results from one-way ANOVAs are

shown for group differences. SLI: specific language impairment; TD: typically developing; M: male; F:

female. Vocabulary scores are computed from the PPVT (Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test), grammar

scores from the TROG (Test for the Reception of Grammar), and nonverbal IQ scores from the RPM

(Raven’s Progressive Matrices); see main text.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474.t001

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 4 / 23

Page 5: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

participants in the present study. The task assesses encoding, initial learning (recognition after

a short delay of 10 minutes) and retention (recognition again after a delay of 1 day) in declara-

tive memory. Separate subtasks examine nonverbal and verbal learning, using two sets of sti-

muli: the nonverbal subtask visually presents pictures of real and novel objects, while the

verbal subtask auditorily presents words and nonwords.

Each of the two subtasks consists of three phases. First, in the Encoding phase, participants

are presented with 32 real and 32 novel items; that is, pictures of 32 real and 32 made-up objects

in the nonverbal subtask, or 32 real and 32 made-up words in the verbal subtask. Participants

are asked to make a real/novel decision on each item, that is, an object decision in the nonverbal

subtask, and a lexical decision in the verbal subtask; see below for further details. This incidental

encoding task is followed by an initial Recognition phase after a 10-minute delay, and a Reten-

tion phase after a 24-hour delay. These two phases are virtually identical. Both phases present all

64 target items that were seen or heard in the encoding phase (old items), together with 64 foils

(new items), for a total of 128 items. Half the old items and half the new items are real (objects

or words) and half are novel (novel objects or nonwords). The foils (new items) are entirely new

in both the Recognition and Retention phases; that is, the foils in the Retention phase are foils

that were not presented previously in the Recognition phase.

Stimuli were presented on a Lenovo z61m PC laptop running Windows 7, using E-Prime 1.2

[43]. A display resolution of 1024 x 768 pixels was used. The objects in the nonverbal subtask

were presented as 640 x 480 pixel pictures. Children sat approximately 40–60 centimeters from

the screen. Testing took place in a quiet room in the children’s school. For the verbal subtask, the

stimuli were presented via headphones to further decrease noise. Responses were made on the

left and right buttons located just below the touchpad on the laptop (see below for Instructions).

Items in the nonverbal subtask were presented with the following presentation and timing

parameters in all three phases (Encoding, Recognition, Retention). A 1000 millisecond (msec)

preparation period with a fixation cross at the center of the screen signaled the imminent pre-

sentation of each new item; during the first 200 msec of this preparation period a tone was also

presented. After this 1000 msec preparation period, the picture appeared in the center of the

screen for 500 msec. If the participant responded during this 500 msec presentation period,

the item disappeared. Following the disappearance of the picture (at or before 500 msec), the

fixation cross reappeared on the screen. If the participant responded prior to 5000 msec after

the appearance of the picture on the screen (i.e., during the allowable response period, includ-

ing during the initial 500 msec), the next item was when the experimenter pressed a mouse

button, at which point the 1000 msec preparation period for the next item began. If the partici-

pant did not respond within the 5000 msec response period, then at 5000 msec a 1000 msec

time-out period occurred (a 400 msec time-out tone, together with a fixation cross which

lasted the full 1000 msec; note that the time-out tone had a different frequency from the tone

in the preparation period), after which the 1000 msec presentation period for the next item

began.

The presentation and timing parameters for the words/nonwords in the verbal subtask

were identical to those for the real/novel objects in the nonverbal task except that the presenta-

tion duration of the stimulus (word/nonword) was variable (rather than the consistent 500

msec duration in the nonverbal subtask), lasting the duration of the sound file of the item. As

in the nonverbal subtask, the response period was 5000 msec from the onset of the stimulus.

The following instruction procedures were followed for both subtasks. All instructions were

given in Hungarian. Each of the three phases (Encoding, Recognition, Retention) began with

instructions, which were presented on the screen and also read out and explained by the exper-

imenter. Before all three phases, participants were instructed to place their left and right index

fingers on the left and right buttons located just below the touchpad on the laptop, and to

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 5 / 23

Page 6: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

make a response by pressing one of these buttons. In the Encoding phase, participants were

instructed to decide as quickly and accurately as possible whether the object or word was Real

or Made-up, and to press one of two buttons accordingly. In both the Recognition and Reten-

tion phases, participants were instructed to indicate by button press whether the item they

were presented with was or was not previously presented in the Encoding phase (Yes/No deci-

sion on whether they had seen or heard the item earlier). Instructions were followed by 3 prac-

tice items in the encoding phase, and 6 practice items in both the Recognition phase (3 old,

that is, the three practice items from encoding; and 3 new, that is, that were not presented as

practice or experimental items during Encoding) and the Retention phase (3 old, the same

practice items as in Encoding and Recognition; and 3 new, which had not been presented dur-

ing either Encoding or Recognition).

For each subtask two versions (A and B) were created. In one version the left button was

associated with Real (Encoding) or Yes, presented earlier (Recognition and Retention), and

the left button with Made-up/No, while in the other group, the mapping was the reverse. The

two versions were alternatively assigned to consecutive participants within each participant

group (SLI and TD). During the task, a reminder appeared at the bottom of the screen indicat-

ing the mapping of the buttons.

Participants (within each group and version) were randomly assigned to one of two subtask

orders. In one order, participants were first given the Encoding phase of the verbal subtask, fol-

lowed by the Encoding phase of the nonverbal subtask, then the Recognition phase of the verbal

subtask, then the Recognition phase of the nonverbal subtask, and 24 hours later the Retention

phases of first the verbal then the nonverbal subtask. In the other order subjects were given first

the nonverbal and then the verbal subtask for each phase. Participants were given the Encoding

and Recognition phases at varying times, between about 9am and 4pm, and were tested on

Retention about 24 hours later. Encoding phases took 5–7 minutes to complete, and Recogni-

tion and Retention phases took between 9–11 minutes to complete. Participants took a short

self-paced break between the two Encoding phases. The delay between each Encoding phase

and its corresponding Recognition phase was about 10 minutes.

Stimuli

The real and made-up object stimuli in the nonverbal task were identical to the stimuli devel-

oped in the original version of the task developed in the Brain and Language Lab. These items

were validated as real and novel objects by the Hungarian experimenters. The items were black

and white line drawings of real and made-up objects. Images of real objects had been taken

from various sources, and then modified as necessary. Sources included a number of different

clipart galleries (including free websites and purchased collections) and the Snodgrass and

Vanderwart [44] set of pictures. Images for made-up objects had been taken from Eals and Sil-

verman [45], Cornelissen et al. [46] and Williams and Tarr [47]; they were then modified as

necessary, including to reduce nameability. Low nameability was confirmed in previous pilot

studies run in the Brain and Language Lab. All images were resized, touched up, rotated, and/

or converted to black-and-white to create the final set of stimuli produced by the Brain and

Language Lab. The items were presented in a pseudo-randomized order, with no more than 3

consecutive real or made-up objects.

Stimuli in the verbal subtask comprised auditorily presented real words (concrete nouns)

and made-up words (Hungarian adaptations of the English nonwords created by the Brain

and Language Lab). The nonwords were matched to the real words in phonological length

(range: 1–5 syllables) and syllable structure (e.g., a CVCC word was matched as closely as pos-

sible to a CVCC nonword) for all three sets of real/made-up words: the target items, the foils

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 6 / 23

Page 7: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

presented during Recognition, and the foils presented during Retention. Additionally, these

three sets were matched on the same factors. All items conformed to the rules of Hungarian

phonotactics. All words and nonwords were digitally recorded by a male native Hungarian

speaker in 16 bit, 44100 Hz, 705 kbps mono–not stereo–wave format (though these were later

presented to participants to both ears via the headphones). All sound files were edited to reduce

their length to the length of the word or nonword. The length of the sound files ranged between

465 and 1311 msec. See Fig 1 for examples of stimuli in both the nonverbal and verbal subtasks.

Results

Between and within group differences in recognition memory were examined with 2 (Group:

SLI vs. TD) X 2 (Real/Novel: Real vs. Novel) X 2 (Delay: 10 minutes/Recognition vs. 24 hours/

Retention) mixed ANOVAs run separately on the Nonverbal and Verbal subtasks. In these

analyses Group was the between-subject factor, and Real/Novel and Delay were within-subject

factors. In order to minimize effects of response bias, accuracy was entered in the analyses as

normalized d' (d-prime) scores (d’ = z(hit rate)—z(false alarm rate)). For all analyses, we report

partial eta-squared (ηp2) as a measure of effect size. Reaction times were also analyzed, using

median RTs (based on raw RTs for correct responses only); however, these analyses are not

presented here, since there were no main effects of Group or interactions with Group (all

ps> 0.1).

Recognition and retention of nonverbal information

Performance on the Nonverbal subtask is shown in Table 2. Both the SLI and TD groups

showed above-chance performance at both Real and Novel items, for both Recognition and

Retention (Table 2).

The 2 (Group) X 2 (Real/Novel) X 2 (Delay) ANOVA yielded main effects of Delay (F(1, 40) =

11.774, p = .001, ηp2 = .227), with better performance at Retention than Recognition over both

groups, and of Real/Novel (F(1, 40) = 95.450, p< .0001, ηp2 = .705), with Real objects recognized

better than Novel ones. There was no main effect of Group (F(1, 40) = 1.63, p = .209, ηp2 = .039).

However, the two significant main effects were qualified by a significant interaction between

Group and Delay (F(1, 40) = 1.478, p = .034, ηp2 = .107) and an interaction between Group and

Real/Novel that approached significance (F(1, 40) = 1.575, p = .057, ηp2 = .088); see Figs 2 and 3.

There were no other interactions (Real/Novel X Delay; F(1, 40) = .433, p = .514, ηp2 =. 011; Group

X Real/Novel X Delay: F(1, 40), = .651, p = .424, ηp2 = .016).

We first followed up on the Group X Delay interaction, collapsing over both object types

(Real and Novel). There was no effect of Delay in the TD group; that is, there was no signifi-

cant difference in accuracy for the TD children between Recognition and Retention (F(1, 20) =

2.004, p = .172, ηp2 = .091). However, for the SLI group, accuracy was significantly higher at

Retention than Recognition (F(1, 20) = 20.340, p< .001, ηp2 = .504). Additionally, the SLI

group was worse than the TD group at Recognition (F(1, 40) = 4.211, p = .047), but not at

Retention (F(1, 40) = .133, p = .718). See Fig 2. S1 Fig shows d’ performance at Recognition

and Retention at the nonverbal subtask for each child with SLI (S1 (A)) and each TD child (S1

(B)).

Next, we followed up on the Group X Real/Novel interaction that approached significance,

collapsing over both Delay periods (10 minutes/Recognition and 24 hours/Retention). The effect

of Real/Novel was significant in both the TD and SLI groups, with better performance (i.e., col-

lapsed over both Delay periods) on Real than Novel objects in both groups, though this effect was

larger for the TD children (TD: F(1, 20) = 79.707, p< .0001, ηp2 = .799; SLI: F(1, 20) = 49.626,

p< .0001, ηp2 = .713). Additionally, the effect of Group (again, collapsed over both Delay periods)

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 7 / 23

Page 8: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

Fig 1. Example stimuli from the nonverbal subtask (real and made-up objects) and the verbal subtask (real and made-up words).

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474.g001

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 8 / 23

Page 9: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

Table 2. Recognition and retention accuracy for nonverbal information.

SLI TD

Recognition (10 minute delay)

Real

d’ 1.41 (1.12) *** 2.17 (1.05) ***

Hit rate 0.77 (0.20) 0.78 (0.19)

False alarm rate 0.33 (0.32) 0.14 (0.15)

Novel

d’ 0.65 (0.75) ** 0.92 (0.78) ***

Hit Rate 0.38 (0.23) 0.38 (0.16)

False Alarm Rate 0.21 (0.25) 0.16 (0.17)

Retention (24 hour delay)

Real

d’ 1.90 (1.21) *** 2.18 (0.95) ***

Hit rate 0.74 (0.23) 0.75 (0.22)

False alarm rate 0.20 (0.24) 0.11 (0.10)

Novel

d’ 1.12 (0.76) *** 1.12 (0.94) ***

Hit Rate 0.45 (0.23) 0.38 (0.21)

False Alarm Rate 0.15 (0.21) 0.10 (0.11)

Note. Accuracy in the Nonverbal subtask, showing means (and standard deviations) of d’, as well as of hit

rates and false alarm rates. SLI: specific language impairment; TD: typically developing. Asterisks indicate

performance greater than chance (mean d’s significantly greater than zero, one-sample t-tests, df = 20):

***: p < .001

**: p < .01

*: p < .05.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474.t002

Fig 2. Nonverbal subtask performance by Group (SLI vs. TD) and Delay (10 minutes/Recognition vs. 24 hours/Retention),

showing mean d’ scores and standard errors.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474.g002

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 9 / 23

Page 10: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

showed a trend for Real objects, with somewhat better performance by the TD than the SLI

group (F(1, 40) = 2.947, p = .094, ηp2 = .069), while it there was no group difference for Novel

objects, F(1, 40) = .256, p = .616, ηp2 = .006. See Fig 3.

To understand, at a more fine-grained level, the increase in performance between Recognition

and Retention found for the SLI group but not the TD group, we tested whether this pattern held

separately for Real and Novel items. Note that although the three-way interaction between

Group, Real/Novel and Delay was not significant, such higher-level interactions are difficult to

obtain, leaving open the possibility that the observed patterns for the SLI and TD groups could

differ between Real and Novel items. However, consistent with the lack of a three-way interaction,

the SLI group showed an increase in performance between Recognition and Retention for both

types of items (Real: F(1, 20) = 7.745, p = .011, ηp2 = .279; Novel: F(1, 20) = 11.65, p = .003, ηp

2 =

.368), whereas the TD group did not show an increase for either (Real: F(1, 20) = 0.20, p = .889,

ηp2 = .001; Novel: F(1, 20) = 1.412, p = .249, ηp

2 = .066). Interestingly, however, the two groups

did not differ for either Real or Novel items at either Delay period, with the notable exception of

Real items at Recognition, where the TD group showed superior performance (Recognition: Real:

F(1, 40) = 5.150, p = .029, ηp2 = .114; Novel: F(1, 40) = 1.349, p = .252, ηp

2 = .033. Retention: Real:

F(1, 40) = .724, p = .400, ηp2 = .018; Novel: F(1, 40) = .000, p = .998, ηp

2 = .000).

Recognition and retention of verbal information

Performance on the Verbal subtask is shown in Table 3. Both the SLI and TD groups showed

above-chance performance at both Real and Novel items, for both Recognition and Retention,

with the exception of the SLI group at Novel items at Recognition (Table 3).

The 2 (Group) X 2 (Real/Novel) X 2 (Delay) ANOVA yielded main effects of Group (F(1, 40) =

21.86, p = .0001, ηp2 = .353), with better overall performance by the TD children than children

with SLI, and Real/Novel (F(1, 40) = 24.173, p< .0001, ηp2 = .377), with Real words recognized

Fig 3. Nonverbal subtask performance by Group (SLI vs. TD) and Real/Novel, showing mean d’ scores and standard errors.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474.g003

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 10 / 23

Page 11: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

better than Novel ones. There was no main effect of Delay (F(1, 40) = .148, p = .703, ηp2 = .004).

However, the main effect of Real/Novel was qualified by a significant Real/Novel X Delay interac-

tion (F(1, 40) = 20.359, p< .0001, ηp2 = .337, see Fig 4 below). No other interactions were signifi-

cant (Group X Real/Novel: F(1, 40) = 1.700, p = .200, ηp2 = .041; Group X Delay: F(1, 40) = .952,

p = .335, ηp2 = .023; Group X Real/Novel X Delay: F(1, 40) = .221, p = .641, ηp

2 = .005).

To investigate the Real/Novel X Delay interaction, we examined the effect of Delay sepa-

rately for Novel and Real words, collapsing across the two participant groups. For both types

of words there were significant effects of Delay, but in opposite directions for the two types of

words. Whereas performance on Novel words increased between Recognition and Retention

(F(1, 41) = 5.487, p = .024, ηp2 = .118), performance on Real words decreased during this inter-

val (F(1, 41) = 12.099, p = .001, ηp2 = .228). Additionally, the effect of Real/Novel was signifi-

cant at Recognition (F(1, 41) = 38.935, p< .0001, ηp2 = .487), but only approached significance

at Retention (F(1, 41) = 3.480, p = .069, ηp2 = .078)–though in both cases performance on Real

words was higher than on Novel words. See Fig 4.

Comparison of performance on the nonverbal and verbal subtasks

To compare overall performance on nonverbal and verbal items directly, we ran a 2 (Group: SLI

vs. TD) X 2 (Modality: Nonverbal vs. Verbal) X 2 (Delay: 10 minutes/Recognition vs. 24 hours/

Retention) mixed ANOVA on normalized d’ scores (for simplicity, averaged over Real and Novel

items). This analysis yielded main effects of Modality (F(1,40) = 43.721, p< 0.001, ηp2 = 0.522),

with better performance on the nonverbal than verbal subtask, of Delay (F(1,40) = 7.688, p = 0.008,

Table 3. Recognition and retention accuracy for verbal information.

SLI TD

Recognition (10 minute delay)

Real

d’ 0.83 (0.83) *** 1.51 (0.49) ***

Hit Rate 0.71 (0.17) 0.74 (0.09)

False Alarm Rate 0.42 (0.31) 0.22 (0.13)

Novel

d’ 0.05 (0.61) 0.91 (0.62) ***

Hit Rate 0.28 (0.17) 0.42 (0.20)

False Alarm Rate 0.27 (0.23) 0.16 (0.14)

Retention (24 hour delay)

Real

d’ 0.65 (0.65) *** 1.14 (0.69) ***

Hit Rate 0.55 (0.27) 0.64 (0.21)

False Alarm Rate 0.35 (0.27) 0.27 (0.21)

Novel

d’ 0.32 (0.43) ** 1.09 (0.67) ***

Hit Rate 0.33 (0.17) 0.52 (0.23)

False Alarm Rate 0.05 (0.03) 0.06 (0.03)

Note. Accuracy in the verbal subtask, showing means (and standard deviations) of d’, as well as of hit rates

and false alarm rates. SLI: specific language impairment; TD: typically developing. Asterisks indicate

performance greater than chance (mean d’s significantly greater than zero, one-sample t-tests, df = 20)

***: p < .001

**: p < .01

*: p < .05.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474.t003

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 11 / 23

Page 12: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

ηp2 = 0.161), with better performance at Retention than Recognition, and of Group (F(1,40) =

7.038, p = 0.011, ηp2 = 0.150), with the TD group showing better performance overall than the chil-

dren with SLI. However, these main effects were qualified by an interaction between Group and

Modality that approached significance (F(1,40) = 3.822, p = 0.058, ηp2 = 0.087), and significant

interactions both between Group and Delay (F(1,40) = 5.426, p = 0.025, ηp2 = 0.119) and Modality

and Delay (F(1,40) = 15.189, p< 0.001, ηp2 = 0.275). The three-way interaction was not significant

(F(1,40) = 2.708, p = 0.108, ηp2 = 0.063).

Following up on these interactions, we found, first of all, that Modality had a significant effect

in both groups, with better performance on the nonverbal than verbal subtask, though this effect

was larger in the SLI group (SLI: F(1,20) = 34.636, p< 0.001, ηp2 = 0.634; TD: F(1,20) = 11.531,

p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.366). The Group by Delay interaction was explained by better overall perfor-

mance (over both the Nonverbal and Verbal conditions) at Retention than Recognition by the SLI

group but not the TD group (SLI: F(1,20) = 11.348, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.362; TD: F(1,20) = 0.115,

p = 0.738, ηp2 = 0.006). Finally, follow up analyses for the Modality by Delay interaction revealed a

significant effect, over both groups, in the nonverbal subtask, with better performance at Reten-

tion than Recognition, but not for in the verbal subtask (Objects: F(1,20) = 16.947, p< 0.001,

ηp2 = 292; Words: F(1,20) = 0.654, p = 0.423, ηp

2 = 0.016).

Incidental encoding

This paper investigates potential SLI/TD differences in learning and retention in declarative

memory. Hence our analyses focus on examining Recognition and Retention differences

between the two groups. However, each subtask also includes an incidental encoding phase,

in which participants had to judge whether the stimulus they saw or heard was real or novel.

Performance at the encoding phases of both the Nonverbal and Verbal subtasks is shown in

Fig 4. Verbal subtask performance by Delay (10 minutes/Recognition vs. 24 hours/Retention) and Real/Novel, showing mean d’

scores and standard errors.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474.g004

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 12 / 23

Page 13: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

Table 4. Analyses were run with normalized d’ as the dependent measure. Both the SLI and TD

groups showed above-chance performance at both subtasks (Table 4).

To examine potential group differences in incidental encoding we performed separate one-

way ANOVAs on the Nonverbal and Verbal subtasks. These revealed group differences in

both subtasks, with the TD group showing significantly better performance than the SLI group

in categorization accuracy (i.e., Real vs. Novel) for both Nonverbal and Verbal stimuli (Non-

verbal: F(1, 40) = 4.789, p = .035, ηp2 = .107; Verbal: F(1, 40) = 28.273, p< .001, ηp

2 = .414); see

Table 4.

These group differences in incidental encoding could potentially affect the group differ-

ences reported above for Recognition and Retention. Note that because the encoding was inci-

dental, there is no clear direct relation between success at this phase (distinguishing Real and

Novel items) and actually learning the material, which is tested later in Recognition and Reten-

tion. Nevertheless, we ran ANCOVAs parallel to the ANOVAs presented above for both the

Nonverbal and Verbal subtasks, with encoding (normalized d’) included as a covariate.

Importantly, the same pattern of critical results was obtained as for the ANOVAs presented

above. The ANCOVA for the Nonverbal task crucially yielded an interaction between Group and

Delay (F(1, 39) = 7.068, p = .011, ηp2 = .153; all other effects: ps> .3). Likewise, the ANCOVA for

the Verbal subtask yielded a similar pattern of results as the ANOVA presented above for this sub-

task, namely only a borderline significant interaction between Real/Novel and Delay (F(1, 39) =

3.833, p = .057, ηp2 = .089), with no other significant effects (ps> .1).

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to examine both learning and retention in declarative memory

in SLI, of both nonverbal and verbal information. In order to obtain a clear picture of the sta-

tus of declarative memory, the study attempted to minimize the influence of functions that can

affect measures of learning and retention in this system and are often impaired in SLI, namely

free recall and short-term memory. We aimed to achieve this goal by probing declarative

memory with a recognition memory task, following incidental encoding. To test for learning

we examined recognition memory 10 minutes after encoding, separately for nonverbal items

(real and novel objects) and verbal items (real and novel words). To test for retention of this

information in the same individuals, we then examined recognition memory of the same items

24 hours later, allowing us to investigate potential differences in overnight retention between

Table 4. Encoding accuracy in the Nonverbal and Verbal subtasks.

SLI TD

Nonverbal

d’ 1.99 (1.13) *** 2.65 (0.80) ***

Hit Rate 0.87 (0.14) 0.91 (0.08)

False Alarm Rate 0.27 (0.24) 0.16 (0.13)

Verbal

d’ 2.43 (0.74) *** 3.49 (0.54) ***

Hit Rate 0.87 (0.06) 0.93 (0.05)

False Alarm Rate 0.14 (0.13) 0.03 (0.04)

Note. Accuracy in the Encoding phase, showing means (and standard deviations) of d’, as well as of hit rates

and false alarm rates. SLI: specific language impairment; TD: typically developing. Asterisks indicate

performance greater than chance (mean d’s significantly greater than zero, one-sample t-tests, df = 20):

***: p < .001.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474.t004

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 13 / 23

Page 14: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

the SLI and TD groups. To our knowledge, this is the first study to test incidental learning in

declarative memory in SLI, and the second (after McGregor et al., 2013) to test for overnight

retention in this system in the disorder.

Analyses revealed the following pattern. In the nonverbal domain we found a Group (SLI

vs. TD) by Delay (10 minutes vs. 24 hours) interaction. Analyses revealed that the children

with SLI improved significantly in their recognition memory between testing for learning (at

the short delay, that is, after 10 minutes) and testing for retention one day later, whereas the

TD children showed no change in performance during this period. This pattern held for both

real and novel objects. Interestingly, the two groups did not differ significantly in their recog-

nition memory for either real or novel objects at either delay period, except for real objects

after the short delay, when the children with SLI performed worse than the TD children.

In the verbal domain, a main effect of group showed that the TD children performed better

overall at recognition memory than the children with SLI, that is, over both real and novel

words, over both delay periods. Additionally, analyses revealed an improvement of recognition

memory for novel words between the short delay and one day later, over both groups. In con-

trast, recognition memory for real words decreased during this time period over the two

groups. Unlike in the nonverbal domain, no interaction between Group and Delay was found.

An analysis directly comparing performance between the nonverbal and verbal subtasks

revealed a Group by Delay interaction over both subtasks, due to better overall performance at

the long than short delays only in the SLI group, with no three-way Group by Modality by

Delay interaction. Additionally, this analysis revealed overall better performance (over both

delay periods) at the nonverbal than verbal items in both groups, with this difference being

larger in the SLI group.

The results suggest the following patterns regarding declarative memory in SLI, at least

when tested with a recognition memory paradigm with incidental encoding, with recognition

tested minutes after encoding and then again one day later. In the nonverbal domain, children

with SLI appear to have recognition memory deficits only for real objects, and only at a short

delay of minutes. Importantly, they do not show recognition memory deficits for real objects

one day after learning the items, and do not show impairments for novel objects at either delay

period. Moreover, only children with SLI improve at remembering items between initial learn-

ing and testing one day later, and in fact do so for both real and novel items. In contrast, TD

children show no changes in performance during this period. In the verbal domain, children

with SLI appear to have recognition memory deficits for both real and novel words, at both

short and long delays. However, the changes in performance between the short and long delays

do not differ between the groups for verbal items.

A key question is how the observed patterns may best be interpreted. First of all, it does not

seem likely that these findings can be accounted for by differences between the TD and SLI

groups at the incidental encoding task. Success at distinguishing real and novel items in this

task does not have any clear relation with actually encoding the material. Moreover, ANCO-

VAs with performance from the encoding task covaried out yielded similar patterns to those

from the ANOVAs without this factor included.

Second, it might be argued that the improvements between the two delay periods observed

in the nonverbal task for the SLI but not the TD group could be due to ceiling effects for the

latter. On this view, the lack of an increase between the two delay periods for the TD group

might simply be explained by the fact that their performance was already very good at the non-

verbal task after the short delay, and hence they had less room for improvement. Indeed, the

highest performance at the short delay was observed for the TD group, for real objects, with an

accuracy score (over hits and correct rejections) of 82%. However, the TD group’s accuracy for

novel objects was only 61%, yet they showed no improvements for either novel or real objects.

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 14 / 23

Page 15: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

In contrast, the SLI group showed improvements at both real and novel objects, even though

their accuracy at real objects was higher at the short delay (72%) than it was for the TD group

for novel ones (i.e., 61%). Moreover, even for real objects, accuracy for the TD group at the

short delay (i.e., 82%) was not particularly close to ceiling (i.e., 100%). Indeed, the variance of

d-prime scores for real objects at the short delay did not differ between the TD and SLI groups,

also arguing against ceiling effects for the TD group for this condition (see Table 2; Levene’s

test for equality of variance: F(1, 40) = .319; p = .575). Together, the data suggest that ceiling

effects are unlikely to explain the pattern of improvements at the nonverbal task between the

two delays for the SLI but not the TD group.

Third, it might be suggested that the improvements observed between the short and long

delays for the SLI but not the TD group could be explained by worse initial learning by the

children with SLI. In particular, since the children with SLI showed worse performance than

the TD children at real objects at the short delay, it could be argued that they would be posi-

tioned to show more additional learning with an additional exposure (i.e., of the target items

during the recognition memory task at the short delay)–assuming a classic non-linear (e.g.,

log-shaped) learning curve [48,49], since the performance of the children with SLI at the short

delay is “further left” on the curve. On this view, such additional learning could result in

greater improvements in the SLI than TD group between the short and long delay. Alterna-

tively, it might be argued that the children with SLI in particular understood the instructions

better the second time, at the long delay. In either case, however, the children with SLI were

not significantly worse than the TD children on novel objects at the short delay; yet the same

pattern was observed on these items as on the real objects, namely, an improvement between

the two delays for the children with SLI but not the TD children. Additionally, the perfor-

mance at the short delay was lower for both participant groups on novel objects than even the

SLI group on real objects (see Table 2), yet only the SLI group showed improved performance

between the delays, moreover on both real and novel objects. Overall, this suggests that lower

performance at the short delay is unlikely to account for the increases between the delays

observed for the SLI group but not the TD group.

We suggest instead that the group differences observed in the changes between testing for

initial learning and for retention 24 hours later for both real and novel items in the nonverbal

task could be due to group differences in consolidation. Consolidation, as we have seen above,

refers to the stabilization of memories after their initial acquisition. This process, which depends

on the medial temporal lobes as well as neocortical regions [11,50,51], and whose molecular

mechanisms are quite well studied [52–54], has been examined not only extensively in non-

human animals, but also in humans. In humans, consolidation has been observed for both ver-

bal and nonverbal information, over various time periods, ranging from hours to days to weeks

[55–58]. Studies have revealed the importance of sleep in consolidation, showing that sleep can

help preserve information, often with better retrieval of the learned information after a period

involving sleep than after the same period without sleep [56,59–62]. Some of these studies show

that sleep can lead to enhanced retrieval not only as compared to conditions without sleep, but

even as compared to initial learning [61,62].

Based on the results from the present study, we suggest that the children with SLI may show

consolidation strengths in declarative memory, as compared to TD children, at least for non-

verbal items over the course of 24 hours with sleep. These strengths seem to hold for different

types of nonverbal items, given that increases between the two delays were found for children

with SLI but not TD children for both real and novel nonverbal items. Indeed, these strengths

seem to lead to normal recognition performance in children with SLI after 24 hours even for

items that showed impaired performance at initial learning (i.e., real objects, at the short

delay).

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 15 / 23

Page 16: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

The lower recognition memory performance of the SLI than TD group at real (but not

novel) objects at the short delay might be explained by the fact that these items are associated

with verbal labels, which could impair their processing. This would not be surprising, given

the language difficulties found in children with SLI, including with phonology. It is also consis-

tent with the particular impairment observed in this study for the SLI group at the verbal task,

including at encoding. More generally, the findings strengthen the view that declarative mem-

ory problems in SLI in the verbal domain, in particular with word learning, may be due pri-

marily to language problems rather than to declarative memory per se [2,6].

Although the SLI group was worse than the TD group at verbal items at both the short and

long delays, the change in performance between the delays did not differ between the groups.

This suggests that although consolidation was not enhanced in the SLI group for verbal items,

neither was it impaired; rather, the children with SLI showed evidence for normal consolida-

tion in the verbal domain. However, various questions remain about SLI and TD consolidation

of verbal items. First, future studies may elucidate why, over both groups, there was a decrease

between the two delays for real words, but an increase for novel words. Second, potential

group differences between the two groups in consolidation may also be revealed by further

research. Although in the current study there was no interaction between group and delay for

the verbal material, exploratory analyses on each group, carried out separately for real and

novel words as was done for the nonverbal material, suggested an intriguing pattern. For real

words, although both groups showed signs of a decrease in performance between the short

and long delay, this reached significance only for the TD group (SLI: F(1, 20) = 3.035, p = .097,

ηp2 = .132; TD: F(1, 20) = 9.787, p = .005, ηp

2 = .329). Moreover, for novel words, although

both groups showed increases between the two delays, this effect reached borderline signifi-

cance for the SLI group (F(1, 20) = 4.314, p = .051, ηp2 = .177) but not for the TD group (F(1,

20) = 1.591, p = .222, ηp2 = .074). These exploratory analyses suggest that TD but not SLI chil-

dren might show a decrement in performance for real words between the two delays, while

only the children with SLI show an improvement at novel words, hinting at the possibility of

SLI consolidation strengths in the verbal domain as well. The Group by Delay interaction

yielded by the analyses comparing performance on the nonverbal and verbal subtasks, due to

better overall performance, only in the SLI group, at the long than short delays over both sub-

tasks, with no three-way Group by Modality by Delay interaction, further supports the possi-

bility of SLI consolidation strengths in the verbal domain. Future studies focusing on this

issue, with large sample sizes and other tasks, may be useful.

The factors and mechanisms underlying the apparent SLI strengths at consolidation in

declarative memory remain to be elucidated. One obvious possibility, though still speculative,

is that they may be related in some way to sleep, since previous evidence suggests that sleep, in

particular Slow Wave Sleep, is especially important for consolidation in this system [60,63–

65]. Indeed, as discussed above, sleep has been found to lead to improvements at remembering

items as compared to initial learning, as was observed in the present study. Perhaps children

with SLI spend more time in Slow Wave Sleep, or have more efficient sleep-related consolida-

tion processes for declarative memory, as compared to TD children. However, further research

is required before sleep-related factors can be identified as a source of the observed patterns.

Alternatively or in addition, declarative memory consolidation advantages in SLI might be

related to the “seesaw” effect, that is, to the enhancement of declarative memory due to impair-

ments of procedural memory [5,12]. On this view, as suggested by the Procedural Deficit

Hypothesis (PDH) and the broader Declarative/Procedural model framework upon which the

PDH is built (see below), the procedural memory impairments in SLI that appear to lead to

their grammatical (and other) deficits may be associated with improvements of declarative

memory, due to the seesaw effect. Such an interaction between memory systems might be

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 16 / 23

Page 17: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

expected particularly in developmental disorders, given the continuing interactions between

systems during development [66,67]. Although the mechanisms of the seesaw effect remain to

be elucidated [5,12], and could indeed be related to sleep, the present findings suggest that at

least one manifestation of these effects might be related to consolidation, rather than (just) ini-

tial learning. Note that no seesaw effect was reported in children with SLI in Kuppuraj et al.

[25]; however, this does not counter the possibility of seesaw effects in consolidation in SLI,

since in that study retention was examined only one hour after encoding, at which point strong

consolidation effects might not be expected.

Implications and future directions

Although this is the first study to suggest possible SLI strengths in declarative memory, and

further research and confirmation is clearly needed, the study has various potential implica-

tions, and opens up new avenues of research.

First of all, the results support and suggest refinements of the PDH of SLI, specifically

regarding the status of declarative memory. In addition to positing abnormalities of brain

structures underlying procedural memory, the PDH hypothesizes that in individuals with SLI

declarative memory should be largely spared, particularly for nonverbal information, and may

even show advantages compared to TD individuals, due to the seesaw effect [5–7]. The find-

ings from the present study are consistent with these predictions, and refine them by revealing

not only the normal attainment of nonverbal knowledge in SLI, but, for the first time, apparent

retention strengths in declarative memory, which may be related to consolidation.

Additionally, the results suggest future areas of research for the Declarative/Procedural (DP)

model, on which the PDH is based. In particular, the possibility of consolidation strengths in

declarative memory in SLI, together with evidence suggesting consolidation impairments in pro-

cedural memory in SLI [17], suggest that dissociations between lexical/declarative memory and

grammatical/procedural processes may extend to consolidation. Future studies should thus further

investigate consolidation in the two memory systems and how these might affect language [68].

Apparently normal (or possibly enhanced) consolidation in the verbal domain in SLI might

help explain the relative sparing of lexical knowledge in children with the disorder, compared to

aspects of grammar [1,6], since learning of lexical but not grammatical knowledge seems to rely

critically on declarative memory [5,12]. It could also at least partly explain the observation that

lexical abilities appear to gradually improve as children with SLI get older [69,70], in particular

because declarative memory improves during childhood [12]. Note that a dependence of lexical

memory on declarative memory does not preclude an additional reliance of lexical memory on,

or interactions with, various functions impaired in SLI, such as phonology, syntax, working

memory, or recall, which would be expected to lead to some level of lexical deficits, perhaps

continuing throughout the lifespan [6]. Additionally, note that normal or enhanced consolida-

tion in declarative memory is consistent with and may help explain why this memory system

seems to play a compensatory role for grammar in children with SLI (5–7).

The findings reveal, for the first time, the possibility that children with SLI show cognitive

strengths, as compared to typically developing children. Strengths in various domains and

functions have been observed for a variety of disorders, including in declarative memory in

both dyslexia and autism [7,71–73]. However, to our knowledge cognitive strengths have

never been reported for SLI, in any domain. The findings presented here suggest that children

with SLI also show such strengths, perhaps in consolidation in declarative memory. Further

studies examining this issue seem warranted.

Possible strengths in declarative memory in SLI are also consistent with the compensation

underdiagnosis hypothesis [7]. On this view, SLI may be underdiagnosed partly as a result of

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 17 / 23

Page 18: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

compensation by declarative memory, in particular for grammatical/procedural deficits. If

indeed aspects of declarative memory are enhanced in SLI, including possibly in the verbal

domain (see above), this would facilitate compensation, potentially increasing underdiagnosis

of the disorder.

The findings suggest the need for further investigation of declarative memory consolidation

in SLI. This could elucidate how broadly the apparent consolidation strengths in this system

may hold in SLI–for example, across age groups in both children and adults, types of items (e.g.,

other types of nonverbal knowledge, such as faces or scenes), tasks (including those that are less

episodic in nature, as well those that involve free or cued recall), and delay periods, and how

these may interact with sleep and the time of day during which encoding occurs [33]. (Note that

comparisons between the present study and McGregor et al. (2013) are difficult, given the diffi-

culty in interpreting results from that study, and the differences between the studies, including

incidental vs. intentional training, testing with recognition vs. recall, and the nature of the par-

ticipants; see Introduction). Of particular interest, it remains to be seen whether children with

SLI might show better memory than TD children for nonverbal (and perhaps verbal) informa-

tion after longer periods, during which additional consolidation could take place. The apparent

procedural memory consolidation deficits in SLI [17] should also be further examined, includ-

ing the underlying mechanisms. Overall, such investigations of consolidation in SLI may eluci-

date not only the nature of SLI, but also of consolidation more generally.

The particular patterns of performance of hits and false alarms in the present study also

warrant further investigation. This pattern suggests that the SLI consolidation strengths might

be due to a larger reduction of false alarms between Recognition and Retention in the SLI than

the TD group, rather than an increase in the number of hits, at least in the case of real objects

(see Table 2), and perhaps also for novel words (see Table 3). Although research is sparse on

hits versus false alarms in retention and consolidation, there is some evidence in the literature

that in recognition memory tasks, sleep reduces false recognition (false alarms), while it does

not affect correct recognition (hits) [74], consistent with the pattern observed here. Alterna-

tively, it might be argued that participants with SLI may have better understood the require-

ments of the recognition memory task the second time (i.e., at Retention), perhaps leading to a

more consistent rejection of foils, i.e. to a lower number of false alarms. On this account, how-

ever, it is unclear why an SLI reduction in false alarms from Recognition to Retention would

be found for real but not novel objects (see Table 2), and perhaps novel but not real words (see

Table 3). Future studies, with larger numbers of participants, might clarify these issues. Addi-

tionally, it remains to be seen why the impaired SLI performance on some conditions (e.g.,

real objects and real words at the short delay), as compared to TD children, seems to be pri-

marily due to differences in false alarms rather than in hits (see Tables 2 and 3).

The findings of the present study suggest that retention in declarative memory should also

be further examined in other disorders. It should be investigated particularly in disorders that

may be related to SLI, as evidenced by comorbidities with SLI and similar patterns of deficits

and spared functions, including of declarative memory [7,75]. These may include dyslexia,

autism, Tourette syndrome, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and ADHD [7]. Indeed, one study

found enhanced declarative memory in dyslexia, although the superior performance was

observed across both short and long (one day) delays, with no improvement between them

[72]. Future studies on consolidation in such disorders seem warranted.

The results of the present study also have methodological implications. In particular, the

study suggests that examining the status of declarative memory may be usefully carried out

with tasks that minimize the involvement of other functions that interact with, but are not nec-

essary for, the functioning of this system, such as free recall and working memory. This

approach seems particularly important in the many developmental and other disorders where

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 18 / 23

Page 19: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

these functions are problematic [7]. More generally, examining the status of a neurocognitive

function may be best carried out by tasks that minimize other, non-critical, functions, espe-

cially if these may be impaired.

Finally, the study of course has limitations, which could be addressed by future research.

For example, future studies could attempt to match the TD and SLI groups on performance of

real objects at the short delay, in order to test whether group differences in consolidation

would still be found, even for real objects. Additionally, whereas in this study the same target

items were presented at both the short and long delays, future studies could include different

target items in the different test sessions, thereby avoiding potential problems of group learn-

ing differences, as discussed above.

Conclusion

In conclusion, the present study reveals normal and perhaps even enhanced consolidation in

declarative memory in SLI. To our knowledge this is the first demonstration of apparent cog-

nitive strengths in children with SLI. The findings, should they be supported by further studies,

have a range of basic research and clinical implications for SLI as well as for related disorders,

and open up new avenues of research.

Supporting Information

S1 Fig. Individual participant performance on the nonverbal task for the SLI (A) and TD

(B) groups. The figures show d’ performance for each individual, collapsed over both Real and

Novel items, at both Recognition (10 minutes after encoding) and Retention (24 hours after

encoding). SLI: children with specific language impairment; TD: typically-developing children;

d’: d-prime scores.

(DOCX)

S1 Dataset. Dataset behind analyses in ‘Learning and overnight retention in declarative

memory in specific language impairment’.

(XLS)

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to the children for their participation and to the schools for accommo-

dating this research. We very much appreciate the help of the speech and language therapists

in screening, and thank Kata Fazekas, Enikő Ladanyi and Borbala Győri for collecting the data.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: AL MU.

Formal analysis: AL FK JL MU.

Funding acquisition: AL MU.

Investigation: AL FK.

Methodology: AL FK JL MU.

Project administration: AL.

Supervision: AL MU.

Visualization: AL FK MU.

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 19 / 23

Page 20: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

Writing – original draft: AL FK MU.

Writing – review & editing: AL FK MU.

References1. Leonard LB. Children with Specific Language Impairment. MIT Press; 2014.

2. Lum JAG, Conti-Ramsden G, Page D, Ullman MT. Working, declarative and procedural memory in spe-

cific language impairment. Cortex. 2012; 48: 1138–1154. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.06.001 PMID:

21774923

3. Marton K, Schwartz RG. Working Memory Capacity and Language Processes in Children With Specific

Language Impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2003; 46: 1138. PMID: 14575348

4. Montgomery JW, Magimairaj BM, Finney MC. Working Memory and Specific Language Impairment: An

Update on the Relation and Perspectives on Assessment and Treatment. Am J Speech Lang Pathol.

2010; 19: 78. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2009/09-0028) PMID: 19948760

5. Ullman MT. Contributions of memory circuits to language: the declarative/procedural model. Cognition.

2004; 92: 231–270. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.008 PMID: 15037131

6. Ullman MT, Pierpont EI. Specific Language Impairment is not Specific to Language: the Procedural Def-

icit Hypothesis. Cortex. 2005; 41: 399–433. PMID: 15871604

7. Ullman MT, Pullman MY. A compensatory role for declarative memory in neurodevelopmental disor-

ders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015; 51: 205–222. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.008 PMID:

25597655

8. Lum JAG, Conti-Ramsden G, Morgan AT, Ullman MT. Procedural learning deficits in specific language

impairment (SLI): A meta-analysis of serial reaction time task performance. Cortex. 2014; 51: 1–10. doi:

10.1016/j.cortex.2013.10.011 PMID: 24315731

9. Eichenbaum H. The Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory: An Introduction. Oxford University Press;

2011.

10. Henke K. A model for memory systems based on processing modes rather than consciousness. Nat

Rev Neurosci. 2010; 11: 523–532. doi: 10.1038/nrn2850 PMID: 20531422

11. Squire LR, Wixted JT. The Cognitive Neuroscience of Human Memory Since H.M. Annu Rev Neurosci.

2011; 34: 259–288. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113720 PMID: 21456960

12. Ullman MT. The declarative/procedural model: A neurobiological model of language learning, knowl-

edge and use. The Neurobiology of Language. Elsevier; 2016.

13. Chun MM. Contextual cueing of visual attention. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000; 4: 170–178. PMID: 10782102

14. Schendan HE, Searl MM, Melrose RJ, Stern CE. An fMRI Study of the Role of the Medial Temporal

Lobe in Implicit and Explicit Sequence Learning. Neuron. 2003; 37: 1013–1025. PMID: 12670429

15. Shimamura AP. Hierarchical relational binding in the medial temporal lobe: The strong get stronger. Hip-

pocampus. 2010; 20: 1206–1216. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20856 PMID: 20824723

16. Gabriel A, Stefaniak N, Maillart C, Schmitz X, Meulemans T. Procedural Visual Learning in Children

With Specific Language Impairment. Am J Speech-Lang Pathol Am Speech-Lang-Hear Assoc. 2012;

21.

17. Hedenius M, Persson J, Tremblay A, Adi-Japha E, Verıssimo J, Dye CD, et al. Grammar predicts proce-

dural learning and consolidation deficits in children with Specific Language Impairment. Res Dev Disa-

bil. 2011; 32: 2362–2375. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.07.026 PMID: 21840165

18. Kemeny F, Lukacs A. Impaired procedural learning in language impairment: results from probabilistic

categorization. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2009; 32: 249–58. doi: 10.1080/13803390902971131 PMID:

19548167

19. Lukacs A, Kemeny F. Domain-General Sequence Learning Deficit in Specific Language Impairment.

Neuropsychology. 2014; 28.

20. Tomblin JB, Mainela-Arnold E, Zhang X. Procedural Learning in Adolescents With and Without Specific

Language Impairment. Lang Learn Dev. 2007; 3: 269–293.

21. Baird G, Dworzynski K, Slonims V, Simonoff E. Memory impairment in children with language

impairment. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2010; 52: 535–540. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2009.03494.x PMID:

19807770

22. Bishop DVM, Hsu H. The declarative system in children with specific language impairment: a compari-

son of meaningful and meaningless auditory-visual paired associate learning. BMC Psychol. 2015; 3: 3.

doi: 10.1186/s40359-015-0062-7 PMID: 25780564

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 20 / 23

Page 21: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

23. Dewey D, Wall K. Praxis and memory deficits in language-impaired children. Dev Neuropsychol. 1997;

13: 507–512.

24. Riccio CA, Cash DL, Cohen MJ. Learning and memory performance of children with Specific Language

Impairment (SLI). Appl Neuropsychol. 2007; 14: 255–261. doi: 10.1080/09084280701719203 PMID:

18067421

25. Kuppuraj S, Rao P, Bishop DV. Declarative capacity does not trade-off with procedural capacity in chil-

dren with specific language impairment. Autism Dev Lang Impair. 2016; 1.

26. Lum JAG, Conti-Ramsden G. Long-term memory: A review and meta-analysis of studies of declarative

and procedural memory in specific language impairment. Top Lang Disord. 2013; 33: 282–297. PMID:

24748707

27. Records NL, Tomblin JB, Buckwalter PR. Auditory verbal learning and memory in young adults with

specific language impairment. Clin Neuropsychol. 1995; 9: 187–193.

28. Shear PK, Tallal P, Delis DC. Verbal learning and memory in language impaired children. Neuropsycho-

logia. 1992; 30: 451–458. PMID: 1620325

29. Duinmeijer I, de Jong J, Scheper A. Narrative abilities, memory and attention in children with a specific

language impairment. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2012; 47: 542–555. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-6984.2012.

00164.x PMID: 22938065

30. Lum JAG, Gelgic C, Conti-Ramsden G. Procedural and declarative memory in children with and without

specific language impairment. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2010; 45: 96–107. doi: 10.3109/

13682820902752285 PMID: 19900077

31. Nichols S, Jones W, Roman MJ, Wulfeck B, Delis DC, Reilly J, et al. Mechanisms of verbal memory

impairment in four neurodevelopmental disorders. Brain Lang. 2004; 88: 180–189. doi: 10.1016/S0093-

934X(03)00097-X PMID: 14965540

32. Lum JAG, Ullman MT, Conti-Ramsden G. Verbal declarative memory impairments in specific language

impairment are related to working memory deficits. Brain Lang. 2015; 142: 76–85. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.

2015.01.008 PMID: 25660053

33. McGregor KK, Licandro U, Arenas R, Eden N, Stiles D, Bean A, et al. Why Words Are Hard for Adults

With Developmental Language Impairments. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2013; 56: 1845. doi: 10.1044/

1092-4388(2013/12-0233) PMID: 24023376

34. Ullman MT, Pullman MY, Lovelett JT, Pierpont EI, Turkeltaub PE. The neuroanatomy of specific lan-

guage impairment. Annu Rev Psychol. To appear;

35. Tager-Flusberg H, Cooper J. Present and Future Possibilities for Defining a Phenotype for Specific Lan-

guage Impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1999; 42: 1275. PMID: 10515521

36. Csanyi FY. Peabody szokincsteszt [Peabody Vocabulary Test]. 1974th ed. Budapest: Barczi Gusztav

Gyogypedagogiai Főiskola; 1974.

37. Dunn L. The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. 1959th ed. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Ser-

vice; 1959.

38. Bishop DVM. Test For Reception of Grammar. 1983rd ed. Manchester, U.K.: Medical Research Coun-

cil; 1983.

39. Lukacs A, Rozsa S, Győri M. A TROG pszichometriai jellemzőinek magyar vizsgalata, a normak kialakı-

tasa. TROG—Test for Reception of Grammar. 2012th ed. Budapest: OS Hungary Tesztfejlesztő Kft.;

2012. pp. 47–86.

40. Kas B, Lukacs A. Magyar Mondatutanmondasi Teszt. in preparation. in preparation.

41. Racsmany M, Lukacs A, Nemeth D, Pleh C. A verbalis munkamemoria magyar nyelvű vizsgaloeljara-

sai. Magy Pszichol Szle. 2005; 60: 479–506.

42. Raven J, Court J, Raven J. Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Raven’s Colored Matrices. [Internet].

1987th ed. London: H. K. Lewis; 1987. Available: 1987

43. Psychology Software Tools, Inc. [Internet]. 2012. Available: http://www.pstnet.com.

44. Snodgrass JG, Vanderwart M. A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image

agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. J Exp Psychol [Hum Learn]. 1980; 6: 174–215.

45. Eals M, Silverman I. The Hunter-Gatherer theory of spatial sex differences: Proximate factors mediating

the female advantage in recall of object arrays. Ethol Sociobiol. 1994; 15: 95–105.

46. Cornelissen K, Laine M, Renvall K, Saarinen T, Martin N, Salmelin R. Learning new names for new

objects: Cortical effects as measured by magnetoencephalography☆. Brain Lang. 2004; 89: 617–622.

doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2003.12.007 PMID: 15120553

47. Williams P, Tarr MJ. Structural processing and implicit memory for possible and impossible figures. J

Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1997; 23: 1344–1361. PMID: 9372604

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 21 / 23

Page 22: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

48. Hull CL. Principles of behavior: an introduction to behavior theory. Oxford: Appleton-Century; 1943.

49. Rescorla RA, Wagner AW. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of rein-

forcement and nonreinforcement. Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory. New York:

Appleton-Century-Crofts; 1972. pp. 64–99.

50. Nader K, Schafe GE, LeDoux JE. The labile nature of consolidation theory. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2000; 1:

216–219. doi: 10.1038/35044580 PMID: 11257912

51. Wixted JT. The Psychology and Neuroscience of Forgetting. Annu Rev Psychol. 2004; 55: 235–269.

doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141555 PMID: 14744216

52. Alberini CM, LeDoux JE. Memory reconsolidation. Curr Biol. 2013; 23: R746–R750. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.

2013.06.046 PMID: 24028957

53. Dudai Y. The neurobiology of consolidations, or, how stable is the engram? Annu Rev Psychol. 2004;

55: 51–86. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142050 PMID: 14744210

54. Frankland PW, Bontempi B. The organization of recent and remote memories. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;

6: 119–130. doi: 10.1038/nrn1607 PMID: 15685217

55. Brown H, Weighall A, Henderson LM, Gaskell GM. Enhanced recognition and recall of new words in 7-

and 12-year-olds following a period of offline consolidation. J Exp Child Psychol. 2012; 112: 56–72. doi:

10.1016/j.jecp.2011.11.010 PMID: 22244988

56. Ellenbogen JM, Hulbert JC, Jiang Y, Stickgold R. The Sleeping Brain’s Influence on Verbal Memory:

Boosting Resistance to Interference. Rogers N, editor. PLoS ONE. 2009; 4: e4117. doi: 10.1371/

journal.pone.0004117 PMID: 19127287

57. Henderson LM, Weighall AR, Brown H, Gaskell M. Consolidation of vocabulary is associated with sleep

in children: Sleep-associated consolidation of vocabulary. Dev Sci. 2012; 15: 674–687. doi: 10.1111/j.

1467-7687.2012.01172.x PMID: 22925515

58. Sheth BR, Varghese R, Truong T. Sleep Shelters Verbal Memory from Different Kinds of Interference.

SLEEP. 2012;

59. Barrett TR, Ekstrand BR. Effect of sleep on memory. 3. Controlling for time-of-day effects. J Exp Psy-

chol. 1972; 96: 321–327. PMID: 4345763

60. Diekelmann S, Born J. The memory function of sleep. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;

61. Payne JD, Tucker MA, Ellenbogen JM, Wamsley EJ, Walker MP, Schacter DL, et al. Memory for

semantically related and unrelated declarative information: the benefit of sleep, the cost of wake. PloS

One. 2012; 7: e33079. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0033079 PMID: 22457736

62. Tucker MA, Fishbein W. Enhancement of declarative memory performance following a daytime nap is

contingent on strength of initial task acquisition. Sleep. 2008; 31: 197–203. PMID: 18274266

63. Holz J, Piosczyk H, Feige B, Spiegelhalder K, Baglioni C, Riemann D, et al. EEG sigma and slow-wave

activity during NREM sleep correlate with overnight declarative and procedural memory consolidation:

EEG sigma and SWA and memory consolidation. J Sleep Res. 2012; 21: 612–619. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-

2869.2012.01017.x PMID: 22591117

64. Marshall L, Born J. The contribution of sleep to hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation. Trends

Cogn Sci. 2007; 11: 442–450. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.001 PMID: 17905642

65. Mednick SC, Cai DJ, Shuman T, Anagnostaras S, Wixted JT. An opportunistic theory of cellular and

systems consolidation. Trends Neurosci. 2011; 34: 504–514. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.06.003 PMID:

21742389

66. Karmiloff-Smith A. Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends

Cogn Sci. 1998; 2: 389–398. PMID: 21227254

67. Thomas M, Karmiloff-Smith A. Are developmental disorders like cases of adult brain damage? Implica-

tions from connectionist modelling. Behav Brain Sci. 2002; 25: 727-750-787.

68. Morgan-Short K, Finger I, Grey S, Ullman MT. Second Language Processing Shows Increased Native-

Like Neural Responses after Months of No Exposure. Stamatakis EA, editor. PLoS ONE. 2012; 7:

e32974. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0032974 PMID: 22470434

69. Rice ML. Language growth and genetics of specific language impairment*. Int J Speech Lang Pathol.

2013; 15: 223–233. doi: 10.3109/17549507.2013.783113 PMID: 23614332

70. Rice ML, Hoffman L. Predicting Vocabulary Growth in Children With and Without Specific Language

Impairment: A Longitudinal Study From 2;6 to 21 Years of Age. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2015; 58:

345. doi: 10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-14-0150 PMID: 25611623

71. Boucher J, Mayes A, Bigham S. Memory in autistic spectrum disorder. Psychol Bull. 2012; 138: 458–

496. doi: 10.1037/a0026869 PMID: 22409507

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 22 / 23

Page 23: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

72. Hedenius M, Ullman MT, Alm P, Jennische M, Persson J. Enhanced Recognition Memory after Inciden-

tal Encoding in Children with Developmental Dyslexia. Chao L, editor. PLoS ONE. 2013; 8: e63998. doi:

10.1371/journal.pone.0063998 PMID: 23717524

73. Walenski M, Mostofsky SH, Gidley-Larson JC, Ullman MT. Brief Report: Enhanced Picture Naming in

Autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008; 38: 1395–1399. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0513-y PMID: 18163206

74. Fenn KM, Gallo DA, Margoliash D, Roediger HL, Nusbaum HC. Reduced false memory after sleep.

Learn Mem Cold Spring Harb N. 2009; 16: 509–513.

75. Krishnan S, Watkins KE, Bishop DVM. Neurobiological Basis of Language Learning Difficulties. Trends

Cogn Sci. 2016; 20: 701–714. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.06.012 PMID: 27422443

Learning and Retention in SLI

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 January 3, 2017 23 / 23

Page 24: Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in ...€¦ · RESEARCH ARTICLE Learning and Overnight Retention in Declarative Memory in Specific Language Impairment A´ gnes

S1 Fig. Individual participant performance on the nonverbal task for the SLI (A) and TD (B) groups. The figures show d’ performance for each individual, collapsed over both Real and Novel items, at both Recognition (10 minutes after encoding) and Retention (24 hours after encoding). SLI: children with specific language impairment; TD: typically-developing children; d': d-prime scores.

A. SLI B. TD