Individual Differences in Working Memory Training Research Poster

1
-2 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 0 20 40 60 80 PERFORMANCE (N-BACK LEVEL)) AGE (IN YEARS) PERFORMANCE GAINS BY AGE Baseline Performance Average Performance in last three sessions Gain in Performance Baseline (2 nd Order Poly) Last three sessions (2 nd Order Poly) Gain in performance (2 nd Order Poly) Individual Differences in Working Memory Training Shafee Mohammed 1 ; Benjamin Katz 2 ; Chelsea M. Parlett 1 ; Martin Buschkuehl 3 ; and Susanne M. Jaeggi 1 University of California, Irvine 1 , University of Michigan 2 , MIND Research Institute 3 INTRODUCTION RESEARCH QUESTIONS How do individual difference factors, such as age, baseline ability, and n-back training type (spatial vs. verbal), influence the outcome of training? How does one’s pattern of performance on the training task influence transfer gains on untrained tasks? Data: 418 participants from varied age range, education level, and geographic locations. At least 15 sessions of working memory training. Anguera et al. (2012). Behavioral Brain Research. Buschkuehl et al. (2014). Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience. Jaeggi et al. (2011). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. Jaeggi et al. (2011). Paper presented at the Eighteenth Annual Cognitive Neuroscience Society Meeting. Jaeggi et al. (2014). Memory and Cognition. Jaeggi et al. (2008). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. Jaeggi et al. (2010). Intelligence. Jonides et al. (2010). Presented at the Office of Naval Research - Contractor's Meeting, Arlington, VA. Katz et al. (under review). Seidler et al. (2010). Technical Report No. M-CASTL 2010-01, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor. Stepankova et al. (2014). Developmental Psychology. Tsai et al. (in preparation) Zhang et al. (2014). 26th Annual Convention of the Association for Psychological Science, San Francisco, CA. METHOD RESULTS Age, country in which the study was conducted, and baseline abilities predict one’s ability to improve at WM training. Gender, N-back type, and supervision have no significant effect. Prediction accuracy is significantly different from chance with nearly 76% (0.02) (200 iterations) on an average. DISCUSSION CONCLUSIONS Baseline characteristics, and age are important factors. Age, and baseline performance, are good predictors of training performance (R 2 = 0.30). Further analysis will include non-linear mixed effect models and cluster analyses. Working memory is critically important for success in school and at work (Gathercole et al., 2003; Higgins et al., 2007). One’s performance on an adaptive and challenging longitudinal working memory intervention may serve as a useful assay to investigate cognitive plasticity. Error Matrix Target Label Predicted Label Count 0 0 34 0 1 10 1 1 33 1 0 12 Accuracy: 76% (0.02) Authors’ note: MB is employed at MIND Research Institute, whose interest is related to this work. SMJ has an indirect financial interest in the MIND Research Institute. REFERENCES Gathercole, S. E., Brown, L., & Pickering, S. J. (2003). Working memory assessments at school entry as longitudinal predictors of National Curriculum attainment levels. Educational and Child Psychology, 20(3), 109-122. Higgins, D. M., Peterson, J. B., Pihl, R. O., & Lee, A. G. (2007). Prefrontal cognitive ability, intelligence, Big Five personality, and the prediction of advanced academic and workplace performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(2), 298. Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Shah, P., & Jonides, J. (2014). The role of individual differences in cognitive training and transfer. Memory & Cognition, 42(3), 464-480. PREDICTORS OF TRAINING Not every person improves equally on a WM training task. Identifying the factors that potentially contribute to explaining such individual differences is the key to improving training. Data mining and machine learning can be viable approaches to investigating plasticity. Figure 2. Average performance on the first three sessions(blue), performance on last three sessions (gray), and performance gain (purple) as a function of Age. Figure 1. An example showing visual and auditory 2-back tasks. The stimuli are presented chronologically from left to right as a visual stimulus or an auditory stimulus respectively. Participants should match the current stimulus with the n th stimulus (2 nd in this case.) Table 1. The accuracy of the algorithm to detect whether or not a subject is in the top fifty percentile. Table 4. Values in the descriptive table are raw scores and standardized units for regression. Numbers in bold are statistically significant (in this case, p<0.01). Subjects aged 10-20, from US, Male, Supervised (as opposed to Unsupervised), Trained on spatial n-back, are left out as controls. Descriptive statistics Regression Results Sample Characteristics Mean SD Beta SE Age (Range 7-78 years) 22.51 16.11 Age Squared 765.68 1313.59 Age Centered 0 16.11 0.25 0.01 Age Centered Squared 258.89 627.57 -0.48 0.00 Gender (Female Proportion) 0.51 0.02 0.08 Baseline N-Back Level 2.64 0.95 0.11 0.05 Final N-Back level 3.74 1.64 Gain 1.11 1.044 Training Characteristics Supervised participants 0.43 0.08 0.01 Location (US Participants) 0.81 -0.23 0.01 N-back Type -0.03 0.01 Spatial 0.35 Verbal 0.11 Object Total Single N-back 0.60 Total Dual N-back 0.40 Total Observations 418 R-Squared 0.300 Gathering the data and cleaning Descriptive statistical analysis Data mining methods – Logistic classifier and Decision tree analysis Non-linear mixed models (planned) Nearest neighbor analysis (planned) Class Probability 1 0.715 0 0.678 0 0.870 0 0.567 1 0.502 0 0.503 Table 2. The prediction probability of each individual’s accuracy to detect whether or not each subject is in the top fifty percentile. The individual’s in the bold have a low predictive probabilities. Figure 3. The prediction accuracies over 200 iterations. Attained prediction range is 0.69 - 0.81. -2 0 2 4 6 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 N-back level Age in years Training gains based on training locations Non-US Participants US Participants Figure 4. The difference between the US (in red) and non-US (blue) participant training gains. The individuals with negative post training gains are exclusively from the US. N-Back Level Age Figure 7. (right) shows a decision tree model used to predict and classify individuals. Each individual starts at the purple dot and moves along the branches of the tree based on a decision choice for each condition. Only a single branch is shown in full detail for illustration purposes. Figure 5. (left) The baseline performance (top graph) and the gains (bottom graph) of the top 50% of the participants (in green) and bottom 50% of the participants (in red). True Positive Rate False Positive Rate Schema Number of examples 315 Number of features 7 Number of unpacked features 7 Number of classes 2 Model Properties Number of Trees 10 Max tree depth 6 Training accuracy 0.92 Validation Accuracy 0.85 Training log-loss 0.27 Validation log-loss 0.39 Figure 6.(left) ROC curve obtained by aggregating all predictions from 200 repetitions. Table 3.(above) Shows the model characteristics and the validation estimates. DATA SOURCES

Transcript of Individual Differences in Working Memory Training Research Poster

Page 1: Individual Differences in Working Memory Training Research Poster

Printing:This poster is 48” wide by 36” high. It’s designed to be printed on a large

Customizing the Content:The placeholders in this formatted for you. placeholders to add text, or click an icon to add a table, chart, SmartArt graphic, picture or multimedia file.

Tfrom text, just click the Bullets button on the Home tab.

If you need more placeholders for titles, make a copy of what you need and drag it into place. PowerPoint’s Smart Guides will help you align it with everything else.

Want to use your own pictures instead of ours? No problem! Just rightChange Picture. Maintain the

-2

-1

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

0 20 40 60 80

PE

RF

OR

MA

NC

E (

N-B

AC

K L

EV

EL

))

AGE (IN YEARS)

PERFORMANCE GAINS BY AGEBaseline Performance

Average Performance in last three sessions

Gain in Performance

Baseline (2nd Order Poly)

Last three sessions (2nd Order Poly)

Gain in performance (2nd Order Poly)

Individual Differences in Working Memory TrainingShafee Mohammed1; Benjamin Katz2; Chelsea M. Parlett1; Martin Buschkuehl3; and Susanne M. Jaeggi1

University of California, Irvine1, University of Michigan2, MIND Research Institute3

INTRODUCTION

RESEARCH QUESTIONS

• How do individual difference factors, such as age, baseline

ability, and n-back training type (spatial vs. verbal), influence

the outcome of training?

• How does one’s pattern of performance on the training task

influence transfer gains on untrained tasks?

• Data:

• 418 participants from varied age range, education level,

and geographic locations.

• At least 15 sessions of working memory training.

Anguera et al. (2012). Behavioral Brain Research.

Buschkuehl et al. (2014). Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience.

Jaeggi et al. (2011). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the

United States of America.

Jaeggi et al. (2011). Paper presented at the Eighteenth Annual Cognitive

Neuroscience Society Meeting.

Jaeggi et al. (2014). Memory and Cognition.

Jaeggi et al. (2008). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the

United States of America.

Jaeggi et al. (2010). Intelligence.

Jonides et al. (2010). Presented at the Office of Naval Research - Contractor's

Meeting, Arlington, VA.

Katz et al. (under review).

Seidler et al. (2010). Technical Report No. M-CASTL 2010-01, University of

Michigan, Ann Arbor.

Stepankova et al. (2014). Developmental Psychology.

Tsai et al. (in preparation)

Zhang et al. (2014). 26th Annual Convention of the Association for Psychological

Science, San Francisco, CA.

METHOD

RESULTS

• Age, country in which the study was conducted, and baseline abilities

predict one’s ability to improve at WM training.

• Gender, N-back type, and supervision have no significant effect.

• Prediction accuracy is significantly different from chance with nearly

76% (0.02) (200 iterations) on an average.

DISCUSSION

CONCLUSIONS

• Baseline characteristics, and age are important factors.

• Age, and baseline performance, are good predictors of training

performance (R2 = 0.30).

• Further analysis will include non-linear mixed effect models and

cluster analyses.

• Working memory is critically important for success in school

and at work (Gathercole et al., 2003; Higgins et al., 2007).

• One’s performance on an adaptive and challenging

longitudinal working memory intervention may serve as a

useful assay to investigate cognitive plasticity.

Error Matrix

Target

Label

Predicted

LabelCount

0 0 34

0 1 10

1 1 33

1 0 12

Accuracy: 76% (0.02)

Authors’ note: MB is employed at MIND Research Institute, whose interest is related tothis work. SMJ has an indirect financial interest in the MIND Research Institute.

REFERENCES

Gathercole, S. E., Brown, L., & Pickering, S. J. (2003). Working memory assessments at school entry

as longitudinal predictors of National Curriculum attainment levels. Educational and Child

Psychology, 20(3), 109-122.

Higgins, D. M., Peterson, J. B., Pihl, R. O., & Lee, A. G. (2007). Prefrontal cognitive ability,

intelligence, Big Five personality, and the prediction of advanced academic and workplace

performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(2), 298.

Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Shah, P., & Jonides, J. (2014). The role of individual differences in

cognitive training and transfer. Memory & Cognition, 42(3), 464-480.

PREDICTORS OF TRAINING• Not every person improves equally on a WM training task.

• Identifying the factors that potentially contribute to explaining

such individual differences is the key to improving training.

• Data mining and machine learning can be viable approaches to

investigating plasticity.

Figure 2. Average performance on the first three sessions(blue), performance on last three sessions (gray), and

performance gain (purple) as a function of Age.

Figure 1. An example showing visual and auditory 2-back tasks. The stimuli are

presented chronologically from left to right as a visual stimulus or an auditory

stimulus respectively. Participants should match the current stimulus with the nth

stimulus (2nd in this case.)

Table 1. The accuracy of the algorithm to detect whether or not a

subject is in the top fifty percentile.

Table 4. Values in the descriptive table are raw scores and standardized units for regression. Numbers in bold are statistically

significant (in this case, p<0.01). Subjects aged 10-20, from US, Male, Supervised (as opposed to Unsupervised), Trained on

spatial n-back, are left out as controls.

Descriptive statistics Regression ResultsSample Characteristics Mean SD Beta SE

Age (Range 7-78 years) 22.51 16.11

Age Squared 765.68 1313.59

Age Centered 0 16.11 0.25 0.01

Age Centered Squared 258.89 627.57 -0.48 0.00

Gender (Female Proportion) 0.51 0.02 0.08

Baseline N-Back Level 2.64 0.95 0.11 0.05

Final N-Back level 3.74 1.64

Gain 1.11 1.044

Training Characteristics

Supervised participants 0.43 0.08 0.01

Location (US Participants) 0.81 -0.23 0.01

N-back Type -0.03 0.01

Spatial 0.35

Verbal 0.11

Object

Total Single N-back 0.60

Total Dual N-back 0.40

Total Observations 418 R-Squared 0.300

Gathering the data and cleaning

Descriptive statistical analysis

Data mining methods – Logistic classifier and Decision tree analysis

Non-linear mixed models (planned)

Nearest neighbor analysis (planned)

Class Probability

1 0.715

0 0.678

0 0.870

0 0.567

1 0.502

0 0.503Table 2. The prediction probability of each

individual’s accuracy to detect whether or not

each subject is in the top fifty percentile. The

individual’s in the bold have a low predictive

probabilities.

Figure 3. The prediction accuracies over 200 iterations. Attained prediction range is 0.69 - 0.81.

-2

0

2

4

6

0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80

N-b

ack

leve

l

Age in years

Training gains based on training locations

Non-US Participants

US Participants

Figure 4. The difference between the US (in red) and non-US (blue) participant training gains. The individuals

with negative post training gains are exclusively from the US.

N-B

ack L

eve

l

Age

Figure 7. (right) shows a

decision tree model used to

predict and classify individuals.

Each individual starts at the

purple dot and moves along the

branches of the tree based on a

decision choice for each

condition. Only a single branch

is shown in full detail for

illustration purposes.

Figure 5. (left) The baseline

performance (top graph) and the

gains (bottom graph) of the top

50% of the participants (in green)

and bottom 50% of the participants

(in red).

Tru

e P

ositiv

e R

ate

False Positive Rate

Schema

Number of examples 315

Number of features 7

Number of unpacked features 7

Number of classes 2

Model Properties

Number of Trees 10

Max tree depth 6

Training accuracy 0.92

Validation Accuracy 0.85

Training log-loss 0.27

Validation log-loss 0.39

Figure 6.(left) ROC curve obtained by aggregating all

predictions from 200 repetitions. Table 3.(above) Shows

the model characteristics and the validation estimates.

DATA SOURCES