Grammar in Discrimination,Imitation

14
Wiley and Society for Research in Child Development are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Child Development. http://www.jstor.org Normal and Scrambled Grammar in Discrimination, Imitation, and Comprehension Author(s): John Neil Bohannon III Source: Child Development, Vol. 47, No. 3 (Sep., 1976), pp. 669-681 Published by: on behalf of the Wiley Society for Research in Child Development Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1128181 Accessed: 10-06-2015 02:47 UTC Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/ info/about/policies/terms.jsp JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:46 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

description

psychology

Transcript of Grammar in Discrimination,Imitation

Wiley and Society for Research in Child Development are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access toChild Development.

http://www.jstor.org

Normal and Scrambled Grammar in Discrimination, Imitation, and Comprehension Author(s): John Neil Bohannon III Source: Child Development, Vol. 47, No. 3 (Sep., 1976), pp. 669-681Published by: on behalf of the Wiley Society for Research in Child DevelopmentStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1128181Accessed: 10-06-2015 02:47 UTC

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/ info/about/policies/terms.jsp

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:46 UTCAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Normal and Scrambled Grammar in Discrimination, Imitation, and Comprehension

John Neil Bohannon III Emory University

BOHANNON, JOHN NEIL, III. Normal and Scrambled Grammar in Discrimination, Imitation, and Comprehension. CHILD DEVELOPMENT, 1976, 47, 669-681. Previous studies suggested the possibil- ity that 5-7-year-old children may not be able to discriminate between normal and scrambled syntax. This study examined the relationship between syntax discrimination and other language skills with 50 children each in kindergarten, first, and second grade. In addition to the discrimination task, the children were asked to imitate and show comprehension (select a criterion picture from an array after hearing a stimulus sentence) of normal and scrambled grammar sentences. The results showed that the percentage of dis- criminators (those who made less than 5 errors on the discrimination task) changed significantly over age from the minority (22%) in kindergarten to the majority (78%) in second grade. While minimal differences existed between discriminators and nondiscriminators in imitating or comprehending scrambled sen- tences, children who discriminated between random and grammatically correct sentences were superior for both imitation and comprehension of normal sentences.

As unlikely as it may seem, 5- and 6-year-old children may be unable to tell the difference be- tween normal and scrambled syntax. Previous studies have shown that this age child (a) has failed to express a listening preference for normal over scrambled stories (Bohannon & Friedlander 1973; Rileigh 1973), (b) has produced only grammatical prepositional phrases (to the store) even when the use of ungrammatical phrases (the store to) was modeled (Liebert, Odom, Hill, & Huff 1969; Odom, Liebert, & Hill 1968), (c) has imitated both normal and scrambled sentences to an equally poor degree (Frasure & Entwisle 1973; Weener 1971; McNeill, Note 1; Weener & Wright, Note 2), and (d) has failed to recognize grammatical sen- tences when presented in a list containing both normal and scrambled items (Vasta & Liebert 1973).

The reason why the above mass of data has been ignored may be that the results were consid- ered methodological artifacts. That is, 5- and 6-year-old children may have failed to distinguish the grammatical from the ungrammatical in cer- tain contexts; but it was no more than a parlor trick, essentially unrelated to the mainstream of language development.

Fortunately, recent work (Bohannon 1975) has laid the artifact hypothesis to rest. This study assessed both syntax discrimination and sentence imitation, a measure gaining increasing accep- tance for testing emerging language skills (Slobin & Welsh, Note 3). The results from first-, second-, and fifth-grade children (18 per grade) indicated that (a) some children did not discriminate normal from scrambled syntax in a successive discrimina- tion task, (b) the ability to discriminate was a func- tion of age (40% of first-grade, 75% of second- grade, and 95% of fifth-grade children could learn the discrimination task with five errors or less), and (c) the ability to discriminate was related to normal sentence imitations (r = .63, p< .01).

The results further suggested that at least two variables were associated with the ability to im- itate sentences: age and the ability to discriminate. Weener (1971) has suggested that increased age may provide increased memory capacity (such capacity was reflected in an increased ability to imitate scrambled sentences). Bohannon's data did not provide the opportunity to examine the rela- tive effects of these variables because only four of his sample of 18 second graders failed to discrimi- nate. Only by assessing a large number of second

This study was done in partial fulfillment of the requirements of the Ph.D. degree at the State University of New York at Stony Brook. The author wishes to thank Dr. Gabriel Massaro, assistant superintendent of the Harborfields School District; Dr. Donald White, principal of East Side School; Joan Kolombotovich, principal of Broadway School; Maurice Beulieau, principal of Washington Drive School; and the teachers, parents, and children of these schools for their assistance and cooperation in the course of this study. Thanks are also due Drs. Francis Palmer, Herbert Kaye, Roger Schwaneveldt, and Aaron Lipton for their help in the preparation of the manuscript and Meredith Publishing Corp., for their permission to use the pictures in Language Lotto. Reprint requests should be sent to Dr. J. N. Bohannon, Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia 30322.

[Child Development, 1976, 47, 669-681. @ by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc. All rights reserved.]

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:46 UTCAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

670 Child Development

graders and identifying a substantial sample of nondiscriminators at that age level could the rela- tionship between age and the ability to discrimi- nate syntax be examined. At the same time, a sub- stantial population of kindergarten children could be examined, thus extending the results to a younger age group.

It was also the case that Bohannon used only sentence imitation as a measure of language de- velopment. Although some investigators argue that it is a valid assessment of production (McNeill 1970; Menyuk 1969; Slobin & Welsh, Note 3) and comprehension (Carroll 1972; McNeill 1970), im- itation remains a measure of productive speech. If comprehension and production are different lin- guistic processes (Miller & Ervin 1964), their rela- tionship to the ability to discriminate should be examined independently.

A technique similar to the one used by Fras- er, Bellugi, and Brown (1963) would provide an independent measure of comprehension. In this procedure the child listens to a sentence and selects one picture from an array as the best rep- resentative of the meaning of the sentence. Since the child need not speak to respond correctly, a measure of comprehension is achieved relatively independent of productive speech processes. Fur- thermore, if the stimulus sentences were both normal and scrambled, the comprehension of each type could be compared.

The present study examined the relationships between the ability of children in kindergarten, first, and second grade to discriminate between random and normal syntax and the ability to im- itate and comprehend both random and normal sentence constructions. From previous work it was assumed that the ability to discriminate and age are positively related and that a large sample of children were needed to provide substantial groups of discriminators and nondiscriminators at each age level. Given those samples and indepen- dent measures of imitation and comprehension, it is hypothesized that (a) the proportion of children who could discriminate syntax should increase from ages 5 to 7, (b) there should be no differences between discriminators and nondiscriminators in imitating or in comprehending scrambled sen- tences, and (c) discriminators should be superior to nondiscriminators in the imitation and com- prehension of grammatical sentences.

Method

Design.--The design of this study was a 3 (kindergarten, first, and second grade) x 2 (dis-

criminator and nondiscriminator children) x 2 (random and normal sentences) for multiple mea- sures (sentence imitation and comprehension). Grades, ability to discriminate, and stimulus lists were between-subject factors, and syntax was a within-subject variable.

Subjects.--Children were from kindergarten, first, and second grades (50 children per grade) in two suburban Long Island school systems, for a total of 150 children (77 females, 73 males). Mean ages of the children were 5-9 (SD = 3.4 months), 6-9 (SD = 3.5 months), and 7-9 (SD = 3.7 months), kindergarten through second grade, re- spectively.

Materials.-The stimuli for the discrimina- tion task were identical with those used by Bohan- non (1975). They consistecd of 24 different sen- tences, five to 15 words in length, and requiring 3-9 sec of presentation time. Half the sentences were in normal word order and half in scrambled word order. The sentences were recorded on tape cassettes (male narrator) by a Fisher RC70 tape deck.

The imitation stimuli were identical with those used by Weener and Wright (Note 2). Two lists of six items were generated, half the items with correct grammar and the other half with scrambled word order. Each item consisted of two five-word sentences. The normal grammar five- word sentences had an adjective-plural noun- transitive verb-adjective-plural noun structure (e.g., Good girls wear pretty coats, Strong lions climb tall trees). Sentences in the scrambled syn- tax condition had the word order randomized within sentences (e.g., Girls pretty good wear coats, Climb lions trees tall strong). Sentences were assigned to the lists so that no two versions of the same question occurred in the same list (see table 1). Thus no child heard both versions of the same sentence. The sentences were recorded by a male narrator on tape cassettes with a Fisher RC70 tape deck. All stimuli were narrated with a lively intonation pattern and had durations of between 7 and 8 sec.

The comprehension stimuli were: (1) 20 sen- tences, five to 15 words in length; (2) each sen- tence with a normal and scrambled version; and (3) 20 arrays of four pictures. Each array contained only one picture which corresponded to the mean- ing of the stimulus sentence (see fig. 1). Pictures for the arrays were simple line drawings taken from Gotkin's Language Lotto.1 Twenty sen- tences, half normal and half scrambled, were ran- domly assigned to one of two lists with the stipula-

? 1966 by the Meredith Publishing Corp., New York. Used by permission.

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:46 UTCAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

John Neil Bohannon II 671

TABLE 1

SENTENCE IMITATION STIMULI

List 1

In ............. Good girls wear pretty coats. Strong lions climb tall trees. 2r ............. Horses heavy big pull wagons. Soldiers bad brave men fight. 3n ............. Cool waters wash dry sand. Hungry bears eat sweet apples. 4r ............. Children happy games play funny. Tools fresh meat sharp cut. 5n ............. Little deer jump high fences. Nice people help sick boys. 6r ............. Fish small ducks young like. Hair long wind warm blows.

List 2

In .............. Happy children play funny games. Sharp tools cut fresh meat. 2r ............. Girls pretty good wear coats. Climb lions trees tall strong. 3n ............. Big horses pull heavy wagons. Brave soldiers fight bad men. 4r ........... Waters dry cool sand wash. Eat apples sweet bears hungry. 5n ............. Young ducks like small fish. Warm wind blows long hair. 6r ............. Fences jump high deer little. Nice help boys people sick.

tion that no two versions of the same sentence occur in the same list.

Procedure. -The order of presentation of the three tasks (discrimination, imitation, and com-

prehension) was randomized across children. All subjects were tested in a single test session lasting about 25 min by the same male examiner. Only one child left the testing situation prior to comple- tion of all tasks. He was dropped from the study.

CIDQ2

FIG. 1.-Example of picture array for comprehension test. Normal version of the stimulus sentence: "Nice boys beat loud drums and other boys shake bells." The random syntax version was, "Loud beat drums boys nice bells and boys other shake." Copyright Meredith Publishing Corporation, all rights reserved.

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:46 UTCAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

672 Child Development Discrimination procedure.--Children were

tested individually and instructed in a conversa- tional manner:

We're going to play a listening and a guessing game and I'll tell you all about it. You know that people talk differ- ently. Not only do people sound different, but some people may use different words or sentences to say the same thing. I have two friends, Norman and Ralph, who talk differently. I went out and wrote down some of the things these guys have said and I put them on tape. [Experimenter points to the tape recorder.] I'm going to play these things for you, one by one, and you have to guess who said it. To make the game easier, I have an example of something that Norman has said and some- thing that Ralph has said. Listen closely. Here is Nor- man. [Experimenter plays the recorded example sen- tence, "Mother told you to wash your hands."] Did you hear that? That was Norman. Now here's how Ralph would say the same thing. Here is how Ralph would say that. [Examiner plays the recorded sentence, "Told mother you hands to your wash."] Did you hear that? That was Ralph. Now let's play the game.

Children were presented the stimulus sen- tences one by one via a Sharp cassette tape deck and were required to respond "Norman" or "Ralph" to each stimulus. All subjects received appropriate feedback ("right" or "wrong") after each response. The children were tested until they gave eight consecutive correct responses or until they completed 24 trials. They were clas- sified as "discriminator" if they achieved eight consecutive responses within 24 trials or made less than five errors.

Imitation procedure.-Subjects were seated in front of an acoustical speaker and a microphone. Instructions were delivered in a conversational manner:

We're going to play a listening and a talking game. The way to play is to listen here [experimenter points to speaker] and to talk into here [experimenter points to the microphone]. When you hear something, I want you to say the same thing. That is the game. I want you to listen [experimenter again points to the speaker] and then to say the same thing [experimenter again points to the microphone]. Don't worry if you can't remember all of it. Just do the best you can. Do you think you know how to play? Good. Let's practice. What if you heard "dog." What would you do?

Depending upon the child's response, the experimenter appropriately instructed the child to speak louder or talk into the microphone. Pretrain- ing continued with the delivery of two other prac- tice stimuli, "the big dog" and "big dogs chase little cats." When the experimenter was satisfied with the child's knowledge of the procedure, test- ing began. Half the children received list 1 and half list 2.

During testing, the imitation stimuli were de- livered by a Sharp cassette tape deck, and the subjects' responses were recorded on a Fisher RC70 cassette tape deck for later transcription and scoring. Stimulus delivery depended upon the child's responses in that subsequent items were not presented until the child finished responding to the previous stimulus. Responses were trans- cribed and scored using the Weener (1971) method. Subjects' scores consisted of the number of words imitated regardless of word order.

Comprehension procedure.--Children were seated in front of the experimenter and were in- structed in a conversational manner:

We're going to play a picture guessing game. I'm going to say a sentence and I want you to point to the picture that best goes with that sentence. Now look at these pictures [the first practice array consisted of pictures of a coat, a puppet show, a pot holder, and a string of beads]. What picture would you pick if I said, "Where is the coat." [After the child responded, the experimenter re- vealed the second practice array consisting of pictures of a doll, some bells, some blocks and some cookies.] Now what picture would you pick if I said, "Everyone likes cookies." Now, do you think you know how to play the game? Good. Let's begin.

No child needed extra instruction or failed to perform the practice items. Therefore, upon com- pletion of practice, each child began the com- prehension test of 20 items. Half the children re- ceived stimulus sentence list 1 and half received list 2. Prior to sentence stimulus delivery, chil- dren were allowed to view the appropriate picture array. After stimulus narration, subjects were al- lowed as much time as they needed to respond. If a child stated that he/she did not know the answer, he was encouraged to guess. Regardless of the re- sponse, the experimenter responded positively and warmly. A correct response was defined as the selection of the picture corresponding to the meaning of the normal syntax version of the stimu- lus sentence, regardless of delivered stimulus word order. Subject scores consisted of the num- ber of correct picture selections.

Results

Discrimination. -On the basis of five errors or less to criterion performance, the kindergarten children yielded 11 discriminators (22%) versus 39 nondiscriminators; first grade provided 29 dis- criminators (58%) versus 21 nondiscriminators; and second grade provided 39 discriminators (78%) versus 1ii nondiscriminators. Age and the ability to discriminate syntax varied positively and in the predicted direction, X2(2) = 31.12, p < .001.

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:46 UTCAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

John Neil Bohannon III 673

The relationship between the ability to dis- criminate syntax and age on the imitation and comprehension of normal and scrambled senten- ces is examined below.

Imitation. -Sentence imitation scores were analyzed by two separate 3 (grades) x 2 (discrimi- nation) x 2 (lists) x 2 (syntax) ANOVAs. Grade, discrimination, and lists were between-subject ef- fects and syntax was a within-subject effect. The first ANOVA, a least-squares analysis, yielded several negative sum squares because of the abrupt increase in the proportion of discriminators over age. The grade x discriminator (G x D) in- teraction was influenced most, with the result that all factors depending upon the G x D factor (G x D x syntax, G x D x list and G x D x list x syntax) were rendered meaningless. The error terms were inflated as well and made the analysis fairly conservative. It should be noted that the sum squares of the other factors in the analysis not dependent on the G x D interaction were unaf- fected. Therefore, as a check upon the significance levels of the affected factors another unweighted means ANOVA was computed on the means of the G x D x L x S interaction.

This analysis treated each of the 24 means as a single score, thus overestimating the four-way in- teraction and slightly underestimating the lower- order factors. For example, the means for the grade main effect in the least-squares analysis are kindergarten = 3.88 words per sentence, first grade = 4.62, and second grade = 5.23. The grade means in the unweighted means analysis are kindergarten = 4.04 words per sentence, first grade = 4.56, and second grade = 5.12. Even with this reduction of variance, the pattern of sig- nificant effects is the same for both the least- squares analysis and the unweighted means analy- sis. Therefore, only the F ratios of the more ap- propriate least squares will be reported.

The least-squares analysis revealed significant main effects for grades, F(2,138) = 11.89, p < .01; discrimination, F(1,138) = 16.82, p < .01; lists, F(1,138) = 5.32, p < .05; and syntax, F(1,138) = 266.59, p < .01. The results of individual t-test comparisons show:

1. Second graders imitate all sentences bet- ter (p < .01) than first-grade and kindergarten children (means = 5.23, 4.62, 3.88 words per sen- tence, respectively). First graders were also super- ior (p < .01) to kindergarten children.

2. All children imitate normal syntax sen- tences better than scrambled sentences (means = 5.80 vs. 3.45 words per sentence).

3. Discriminator children imitate better than nondiscriminator children (means = 5.02 vs. 4.08 words per sentence).

4. List 1 was easier to imitate than list 2 (means = 4.84 vs. 3.86 words per sentence).

The significant grade x syntax interaction, F(2,138) = 14.00, p < .01, subsequent t-test comparisons revealed:

1. There were no significant increases over age in the random syntax imitations (means = 3.27, 3.26, 3.83 words per sentence, for kinder- garten, first, and second grade, respectively).

2. There was a significant (p < .01) increase in normal syntax imitations between kindergarten and first grade. First and second graders were not significantly different (means = 4.49, 5.98, 6.63 words per sentence for kindergarten, first, second grade, respectively).

The significant discriminator x syntax in- teraction, F(1,138) = 24.40, p < .01, and subse- quent t-test comparisons revealed:

1. Discriminator children were superior (p < .01) to nondiscriminators on normal syntax imita- tions (means = 6.46 vs. 4.85 words per sentence, respectively).

2. But there were no significant differences between discriminators and nondiscriminators when imitating scrambled sentences (means 3.57 vs. 3.32 words per sentence). There were no other significant effects.

Comprehension. -The comprehension scores were analyzed by two 3 (grades) x 2 (discrimina- tion) x 2 (lists) x 2 (syntax) ANOVAs, with grade, discrimination, and lists the between-subjects fac- tors and syntax the within-subject factor. The ef- fect of the changing number of discriminators and nondiscriminators over age on the sum squares of the grade x discrimination interaction in the least-squares analysis was similar to that in the imitation analysis. The problem was dealt with in the same manner by checking the spurious sum squares by means of an unweighted means analy- sis. The pattern of significant effects was identical over both analyses; therefore the more appropriate least-squares F ratios will be reported.

The analysis revealed significant main effects for grades, F(2,138) = 26.27, p < .01; discrimina- tion, F(1,138) = 78.54, p < .01; lists, F(1,138) = 9.99, p < .01; and syntax, F(1,138) = 242.29, p < .01. There were no other significant effects.

Further examination of the main effects using t-test comparisons revealed:

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:46 UTCAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

674 Child Development 1. Second-grade children (mean = 8.26

items) comprehended significantly (p < .01) more items than both first-grade (mean = 7.73 items) and kindergarten children (mean = 6.81 items), and first-grade subjects were significantly (p < .01) better than kindergarten subjects.

2. Discriminator children (mean = 8.29 items) comprehended more items than nondis- criminator children (mean = 6.83 items).

3. The children's performance on list 2 (mean = 7.86) was slightly superior to their performance on list 1 (mean = 7.34 items).

4. All children comprehended more of the normal syntax items (mean = 8.47 items) than random syntax items (mean = 6.74 items).

Discussion

Discrimination and imitation.--Few 5-year- olds and most 7-year-olds discriminate between normal and scrambled grammar under the condi- tions of this study. The age at which half the chil- dren can discriminate appears to be 6 (first grade). This finding validates the author's previous work (Bohannon 1975). Indeed, the comparative data are highly consistent. The first study, conducted during the first half of the school year, found that 40% and 71% of the first and second grades, re- spectively, could discriminate. This study con- ducted with children who were on the average 4 months older than the children of the first study found 58% of first graders and 78% of second grad- ers could discriminate syntax. These combined data reflected the behavior of 204 children in three different schools. The children were predomi- nantly middle class, and the schools were almost certainly above average in their educational pro- gram. Whether SES or the caliber of school sys- tems would alter the percentages of discriminators by gtade is, of course, an empirical question which must be tested by further research.

Pertinent to the relationship between the ability to discriminate and sentence imitation per- formance, figure 2 emphasizes a finding of conse- quence: the ability to discriminate syntax relates only to the imitation of grammatical sentences. Discriminators were no better at imitating scram- bled sentences than nondiscriminating children.

However, the grade x syntax interaction shows that other variables operate in the ability to imitate. This finding is inconsistent with predic- tions by the author that the imitation of normal syntax would be exclusively a function of the abil- ity to discriminate. Had that been found, no in- crease in imitations of normal syntax would have

occurred with age among nondiscriminators. This is clearly not the case (see fig. 2).

One possibility does exist for the original hy- pothesis. Some children may be able to discrimi- nate syntax but fail to respond correctly on the discrimination measure for other reasons. The re- verse is unlikely as chance acts against a true non- discriminator's misclassification as a discriminator. Thus, if a percentage of those classified as nondis- criminators may have differentiated normal from scrambled syntax but responded incorrectly, the increase in normal syntax imitations of nondis- criminators over age seen in figure 2 could be ex- plained. Several high imitation scores by misclas- sified discriminators would not greatly influence the mean score for kindergarten nondiscriminators (N = 39). But a few such scores in the smaller sample of nondiscriminators in the first (N = 21) and second grade (N = 11) could inflate those means to produce results similar to figure 2.

To explore the possibility that some children may have been misclassified as nondiscriminators, and to assess the effect of more explicit instruc- tions on the ability to discriminate, additional sub- jects were exposed to a modification of the original instructions on the discrimination task. These children were given the original measure of dis- crimination until 10 were identified as nondis- criminators by the original criteria. Eight were kindergarteners and one each was in the first and second grades. After these subjects had completed the original measure and were unable to discrimi- nate, they were rerun with the same materials but with different instructions. They were told, "Norman talks correctly like your teacher, and you and I do; he makes sense [and the tape of Norman was played again]. Ralph talks funny, mixes up his words and does not talk like your teacher [and the tape of Ralph was played again]."

With the additional instructions, three of the 10 children (the second grader and two kindergar- teners) reached the original criterion of discrimin- ating, but seven (the first grader and six kindergar- teners) did hot. Those results suggest (1) that some children may have distinguished word order but responded incorrectly, and were misclassified as nondiscriminators, and (2) that the inability to dis- criminate found with the original measure seems relatively impervious to more explicit instructions.

Thus, the increased ability of nondiscrimina- tors to imitate normal syntax with age may be an artifact. Some children might have distinguished word order but responded incorrectly, with the result that they were misclassified. Imitation and comprehension tests were not given to the addi-

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:46 UTCAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

John Neil Bohannon III 675

100 Discriminator O Nondiscriminator

90

w 80

70 7) Normal

0 Syntax -) * 60

8 50

Scrambled

o 40 0 Syntax

0 P 20

10

Kg 1 2

GRADES FIG. 2.-Imitation scores for discriminator and nondiscriminator children over grades

tional sample of 10, so these additional data do not provide information about possible differences be- tween the seven and three with respect to their sentence imitation ability.

The inability of the seven to discriminate even with instruction appears to validate the orig- inal measure and to speak to the strength of the phenomena. It is reasonable to assume that some children are on the threshold of being able to dis- criminate and that, while on that threshold, they are more aware of word-order differences than those who have not approached it. But for seven to be uninfluenced by the instruction implies that the inability to discriminate is a more fundamental phenomena than may have been previously sup- posed.

Comprehension.--Before discussing the re-

sults of the comprehension test some caveats should be noted. The measurement tool was cho- sen for its independence from assessments of

productive speech. An identical comprehension procedure has been shown to relate to other as- pects of language development like spontaneous speech (Fraser et al. 1963), but language com- prehension is certainly more complex than what this measure alone reflects.

Spearritt (1962) suggests listening compre- hension involves attentional, motivational, audit- ory, memory, and semantic factors. The pick-a- picture method may only involve a subset of these, such as attentional, auditory, and semantic factors. Syntax may not even have been a necessary factor for responding correctly to all items. Correct per- formance on the sentence, "Big pianos make nice music," could have been achieved by com- prehending the word "piano" and matching that with the picture of a piano in the array. Other sentences, like "Mother says to put cups on top of saucers," require comprehension of a grammatical relationship rather than single words, particularly

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:46 UTCAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

676 Child Development

since the picture array provided pictures of both a cup on a saucer and a saucer on a cup. Syntax was presumably more critical for correct interpretation of the second sentence.

The pick-a-picture method may elicit re- sponses dependent upon different levels of com- prehension-some grammatical, some lexical. With this qualification on the measure, the results of the comprehension procedures can be exam- ined.

The measure of comprehension indicated that children who discriminate syntax comprehend both normal and scrambled sentences better than nondiscriminators (see fig. 3). Were it not for the scrambled sentence imitations one might suggest that discriminators are simply "brighter" or more "attentive," and the ability to discriminate is a function of these general factors. For that hypoth- esis to hold, however, a reasonable explanation must be found for the discriminators' nonsuperior-

ity in the imitation of scrambled sentences. Would not brighter or more attentive children reproduce more words from random word strings?

The answer may be in the comprehension test itself. An analysis of discriminators' and nondis- criminators' performance on the easy and difficult items of the test was conducted to determine whether discriminator children performed better on both. If they are brighter or more attentive, should they not perform better on both easy and difficult test items?

Those items upon which children made 40 or more errors were classified as difficult. Those with 31 or less errors were classified as easy.

For difficult items (see fig. 4) the comprehen- sion results are very similar to the results of the imitation test (fig. 1). Discriminators are superior to nondiscriminators only on the normal syntax items (means = 84% vs. 61.5% items correct,

0)

0 U

ct

H

4J

0)

100

90

80

70

60

50

40'

30'

20

10

Normal Syntax

Scrambled Syntax

Discriminator Nondiscriminator

K9 1 2

GRADES FIG. 3.--Comprehension scores for discriminators and nondiscriminators over grades for all 20 items

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:46 UTCAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

John Neil Bohannon III 677

U 0)

u 0 U o U)

1 H

U) U NJ

uc

100

90

80

70

60

50"

40"

30"

20

1

Normal Syntax

Scrambled Syntax

Discriminator Nondiscriminator

Kg 1 2

GRADES

FIG. 4.-Mean comprehension scores for discriminator and nondiscriminator children over grades for eight difficult items.

t(148) = 6.99, p < .001). No significant differences existed for the comprehension of difficult scram- bled items (means = 48.5% vs. 45% items correct for discriminators and nondiscriminators respec- tively, t(148) = .88, N.S.). It was only the easy scrambled items (fig. 5) on which discriminators were superior and which provided the difference between groups when all items were analyzed to- gether. Presumably, discriminators should have been superior on both easy and difficult items if brightness or attentiveness was the cause.

Figure 5 reveals a ceiling effect. Discrimina- tor children responded correctly almost 90%-97% of the time in both normal and scrambled easy items. Only the nondiscriminators' performance on the scrambled items was under 80%.

Performance on difficult (fig. 4) and easy (fig.

5) items was similar except for discriminators' re- sponding to random syntax. Discriminators com- prehended more easy normal syntax items (means = 97% vs. 85% items correct, t(148) = 6.68, p < .001) and more easy scrambled syntax items (89.5% vs. 72% items correct, t(148) = 6.68, p < .001) than nondiscriminators. The last finding is in contrast to the discriminators' performance on difficult scrambled items, where they were no dif- ferent from nondiscriminators.

An examination of the imitation (fig. 2) and comprehension of difficult items (fig. 4) reveals three levels of performance. Poorest was the im- itation and comprehension of scrambled sentences by both discriminators and nondiscriminators. In- termediate was the performance of nondiscrimina- tors on normal syntax sentences. The highest level was attained by discriminator children in com-

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:46 UTCAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

678 Child Development

4-

4)

0

U

H

100

90

80

70'

60

50

40

30

20

10

Normal Syntax

Scrambled Syntax

Discx imina tox Nondiscriminator

Kg 1 2

GRADES FIG. 5.--Comprehension scores for discriminators and nondiscriminators over grades for the easy items

prehending and imitating grammatical sentences. Three different processing levels or strategies may be involved.

The first and poorest strategy is exhibited by nondiscriminator children on all scrambled sen- tences and by discriminators on difficult scram- bled sentences. The children seemed to use iso- lated words as the sole basis for response. For the comprehension task, some individual words were recalled and applied to the picture arrays. Occa- sionally this was correct, which accounts for re- sponses above the level of chance. Support for this is found in the imitation test where children would often invent sentences, only one or two words of which came from the imitation stimulus.

The second process was used by nondis- criminators when processing grammatical sen- tences. This may be similar to the first strategy with the addition of extra cues supplied by the structure of the sentence. These cues may serve

only as aids to lexical memory, since nondis- criminators seem to gain little from sentence structure and cannot detect the absence of gram- matical cues on the discrimination test.

Finally, discriminator children processing grammatical constructions use their knowledge of sentence structure to comprehend. sentences far better than simple memory alone can provide. It is the author's opinion that they alone are able efficiently to combine linguistic skills with mem- ory for better performance.

The discriminators' performance on the easy scrambled sentences may have been a variation of the third strategy. These children, when con- fronted with scrambled sentences, are aware of the sentences' ungrammatical nature. They ac- tively attempt to recover the structure of the mixed up lexical elements. When the sentence is long or its normal version complex, the meaning of the original sentence is not attained. Thus, their

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:46 UTCAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

John Neil Bohannon III 679

imitation and comprehension performance on the long scrambled sentences was no better than non- discriminators'. But when the sentences were shorter and grammatically simple as in the easy comprehension items, this strategy regained most of the meaning of the sentences. Since nondis- criminator children seem to be unaware of the un- grammaticality of the scrambled sentences, they may not have tried to restructure the scrambled items, and thus their performance was relatively poor.

The comprehension strategy hypothesis con- forms to the data and is preferable to a general factor explanation. If discriminator children were brighter or more attentive their comprehension performance on the difficult scrambled items should have been superior to that of nondis- criminator children. Indeed, the similarity be- tween the pattern of imitation (fig. 2) and com- prehension of difficult items (fig. 4) is striking. It must be concluded that discriminators are distin- guishable from nondiscriminators primarily in their dealings with structured language.

Theoretical discussion.-Why do 6-year-old children (nondiscriminators), considered language sophisticates by some (Lennenberg 1967; McNeill 1970), fail to notice anything peculiar about sen- tences like, "Told mother you hands to your wash?" There are at least two hypotheses that would correctly identify such sentences during the Norman-Ralph test: (1) Label ungrammatical sen- tences with "Ralph." (2) Label incomprehensible sentences with "Ralph."

Posttest questions about the nature of the dis- criminative stimuli revealed that discriminator children selected either hypothesis (1) or (2) to direct their responding. But it must be noted that random responding does not exclude the possibil- ity that nondiscriminators tried these hypotheses during testing. It is conceivable that immature re- ceptive grammars would be flexible enough to

permit considerable input distortion (a point to be dealt with later). It is also possible that partial comprehension is subjectively more similar to fragmentary comprehension (nondiscriminator comprehension scores, fig. 3) than full compre- hension versus partial comprehension (discrimina- tor scores, fig. 3). If either of the above is true, it would have rendered the normal and scrambled sentences sufficiently similar for the nondis- criminators to respond randomly. Yet what is re- ally puzzling is the relationship between this "lin- guistic awareness" of word-order differences and the ability to imitate and comprehend structured grammar.

Relevant suggestions do exist in the litera-

ture. There are some (DeVilliers & DeVilliers 1972; Gleitman & Gleitman 1970; Gleitman, Gleitman, & Shipley 1972; Shipley, Smith, & Gleitman 1969) who state that for 2- and 3-year-old children just such a relationship between recogni- tion of grammaticality and other language skills does exist. They agree with generative grammar- ians (e.g., Chomsky 1965) in assuming that the child's recognition of grammaticality in his own and others' speech is essential for language acqui- sition. It is not enough for the child to follow the rules of grammar; the child must also be able to reflect on those rules (Gleitman et al. 1972).

A corollary to the above would predict that a child who recognizes grammaticality is at a higher level of language development than one who does not recognize this distinction. On this Gleitman et al. (1972) and the present study agree. But these authors insist children as young as 2 and 3 years can distinguish grammaticality in sentences they hear. If the Gleitman et al. (1972) position is strictly interpreted, the nondiscriminators in this study should have acquired only the rudiments of language. This is clearly not the case, as nondis- criminator children did perform significantly bet- ter on normal syntax sentences than scrambled sentences.

Perhaps children can acquire grammar in productive speech without being aware of the rules of grammar they use. Bloom (1974) has sug- gested that children may practice with grammati- cal structures in productive speech before com- plete mastery of those structures. This implies a dual (receptive and productive) rule system of lan- guage. It accounts for the nondiscriminators' abil- ity to use structure in productive speech yet re- main unaware of the grammatical/ungrammatical distinction in receptive speech. Thus, a child's ability to discriminate syntax may not be a deter- mining factor in language acquisition but may be a result of attaining a certain level of grammatical skill.

A similar suggestion has been made for con- cept acquisition. Vygotsky (1965) suggests a prein- tellective verbal stage in cognitive development when the child acquires and uses concepts before being aware of the rules determining them. He argues that "a concept can become conscious only [after] it has become part of a system ... [and] ... becoming conscious of our operations . .. leads to their mastery" (p. 92). In this way, Vygotsky sees awareness as both a symptom and determinant. After the child has used concepts he becomes aware of the rules he used in their formation. He will subsequently use that awareness in the forma- tion of new concepts and in understanding old

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:46 UTCAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

680 Child Development ones better. Vygotsky argues further that the school experience facilitates focusing on the rules of concept formation and accelerates awareness of those rules.

If the rules of syntax are equated to concepts, Vygotsky's position about concept acquisition ap- pears relevant. The age of discrimination (or awareness to Vygotsky) is 6 years, when the sub- jects of the present study entered their first year of serious language instruction.

The research on the acquisition of linguistic skills has to date largely emphasized the produc- tive speech of the- preschool child (Brown & Fraser 1964; Lennenberg 1967). Only a few inves- tigators (Fodor, Bever, & Garrett 1974) have sug- gested that the comprehension process may be dif- ferent from processes used in productive speech. Fewer still (Bloom 1974) have suggested that productive skills may precede receptive skills. The data reported here lend substance to the argument for separate processes.

A single-process theory must account for these facts: (1) children who produce grammatical sentences may not be able to discriminate be- tween the presence or absence of grammar in sen- tences they hear, (2) children who can discrimi- nate derive more information from grammatical constructions in both comprehension and imita- tion, and (3) the age at which most children be- come discriminators exceeds considerably that at which most researchers claim the rules of gram- mar are acquired. A dual- or multiple-process theory would appear to conform better to those facts.

A wealth of data from Brown (1973) and oth- ers shows that children readily utilize many grammatical sentence constructions in productive speech. Data from the present study suggest that in the receptive mode of language the rules of syntax do not completely control sentence com- prehension. Two grammatical processes, recep- tion and production, seem to be required. A theory which restricts the child's understanding to grammatical structures already mastered for pro- ductive speech must be questioned. If this were the case, the holophrastic child could not com- prehend structures exceeding one word in length. There must be some heuristic process which al- lows the child to obtain meaning from complex adult utterances about whose structure the child knows but little.

Figure 3 shows that both discriminator and nondiscriminator children comprehended about 65% of the scrambled sentences. Since these sen- tences had no intelligible structure, comprehen-

sion must have depended at least in part on some process other than that of productive syntax-for example, the child's knowledge of semantic rela- tionships and general world knowledge. Fodor et al. (1974) have argued that there is more to sen- tence comprehension in adults than can be ex- plained by the constructs of generative (produc- tive) grammar. If true for mature speaker/ listeners, it is probably more true for children. The argument for separate processes for produc- tion and listening seems more deserving of serious consideration than has been the case until now.

In summary, this study investigated the cor- relates of the 5-7-year-old child's ability to recog- nize grammaticality in the receptive mode of lan- guage. Discriminator children, those who readily distinguished between the grammatical and un- grammatical, were consistently superior to non- discriminators in the imitation and comprehension of normal syntax. Yet there were minimal differ- ences between these groups when asked to imitate and comprehend random word strings. The results suggest an important change in language devel- opment that occurs around the age of 6 years. This change seems to enable discriminators to imitate and comprehend roughly twice as much struc- tured grammar (relative to baseline performance on scrambled grammar) as nondiscriminator chil- dren.

Reference Notes

1. McNeill, D. Development of the semantic system. Unpublished manuscript, Center for Cognitive Stud- ies, Harvard University, 1965.

2. Weener, P., & Wright, P. The effects of linguistic structure on the recall of verbal messages by chil- dren. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Philadelphia, April 1973.

3. Slobin, D., & Welsh, C. Elicited imitation as a re- search tool in developmental psycholinguistics. Un- published manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, 1967.

References

Bloom, L. Talking, understanding and thinking. In R. Schiefelbusch & L. Lloyd (Eds.), Language perspectives: acquisition, retardation, and interven- tion. Baltimore: University Park, 1974.

Bohannon, J. N. The relationship between syntax dis- crimination and sentence imitation in children. Child Development, 1975, 46, 444-451.

Bohannon, J. N., & Friedlander, B. Z. The effect of intonation on syntax recognition in elementary school children. Child Development, 1973, 44, 675-677.

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:46 UTCAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

John Neil Bohannon III 681 Brown, R. Afirst language: the early stages. Cambridge,

Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1973.

Brown, R., & Fraser, C. The acquisition of syntax. In U.

Bellugi & R. Brown (Eds.), Acquisition of language. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1964, 29(1, Serial No. 92), 43-79.

Carroll, J. B. Defining comprehension: some specula- tions. In J. B. Carroll & R. O. Freedle (Eds.), Lan- guage comprehension and the acquisition of knowl- edge. New York: Wiley, 1972.

Chomsky, N. Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cam- bridge, Mass.: M.I.T. Press, 1965.

DeVilliers, D., & DeVilliers, J. Early judgements of semantic and syntactic acceptability by children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 1972, 1, 209- 310.

Fodor, J.; Bever, T.; & Garrett, M. The psychology of language. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1974.

Fraser, C.; Bellugi, U.; & Brown, R. The control of grammar in imitation, comprehension and produc- tion. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Be- haviour, 1963, 8, 280-294.

Frasure, N. E., & Entwisle, D. R. Semantic and syntac- tic development in children. Developmental Psy- chology, 1973, 9, 236-245.

Gleitman, L., & Gleitman, H. Phrase and paraphrase. New York: Norton, 1970.

Gleitman, L.; Gleitman, H.; & Shipley, E. The emer- gence of the child as grammarian. Cognition, 1972, 1, 137-164.

Gotkin, L. Language lotto. New York: Meredith, 1966.

Lennenberg, E. The biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley, 1967.

Liebert, R.; Odom, R.; Hill, J.; & Huff, R. The effects of age and role familiarity on the production of mod-

eled language construction. Developmental Psychol- ogy, 1969, 1, 108-112.

McNeill, D. The acquisition of language: the study of developmental psycholinguistics. New York: Harper & Row, 1970.

Menyuk, P. Sentences children use. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T. Press, 1969.

Miller, W., & Ervin, S. The development of grammar in child language. In U. Bellugi & R. Brown (Eds.), The acquisition of language. Monographs of the So- ciety for Research in Child Development, 1964, 29(1, Serial No. 92), 9-34.

Odom, R. D.; Liebert, R.; & Hill, J. The effects of mod- eling cues, reward and attentional set on the pro- duction of grammatical and ungrammatical syntactic constructions. Journal of Experimental Child Psy- chology, 1968, 6, 131-140.

Rileigh, K. Children's selective listening to stories: familiarity effects involving vocabulary, syntax and intonation. Psychological Reports, 1973, 33, 255- 266.

Shipley, E.; Smith, C.; & Gleitman, L. A study in the acquisition of language: free responses to com- mands. Language, 1969, 45, 322-342.

Spearritt, D. Listening comprehension--a factorial analysis. Melbourne: Australian Council for Educa- tional Research, 1962.

Vasta, R., & Liebert, R. Auditory discrimination of novel prepositional constructions as a function of age and syntactic background. Developmental Psychology, 1973, 9, 79-82.

Vygotsky, L. S. Language and thought. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T. Press, 1965.

Weener, P. Language structure and free recall of verbal messages by children. Developmental Psychology, 1971, 5, 237-243.

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:46 UTCAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions