Funded by NIH grant RO1 HD-4152 to J. Arnold NSF BCS-0318456 and NSF BCS-0318456 to Z. Griffin Why...

1
Funded by NIH grant RO1 HD-4152 to J. Arnold and NSF BCS-0318456 NSF BCS-0318456 to Z. Griffin QuickTi TIFF (Unc are need Why do speakers modulate acoustic prominence? Listener-oriented explanation: Acoustic prominence (duration, pitch movement, high pitch, intensity) communicates something to the listener • the referent is discourse-new / not-salient (Brown, 1983) • contrast (Terken & Hirschberg, 1994) • informativeness (Bolinger, 1986) • accent signals the focus structure (Selkirk, 1996) • provides additional information in the case of low predictability words • duration shorter when predictable (Jurafsky et al., 2001; Gahl & Garnsey 2004) • words less intelligible when predictable (Fowler & Housum, 1987, Bard & Aylett, 2004) Speaker-internal effort also increases acoustic prominence: • Speaker disfluency correlates with characteristics of acoustic prominence • longer duration (Bell et al., 2003) • higher pitch (Christodoulou & Arnold, Prosody08 poster) • Bard & Aylett (2004): intelligibility loss for repeated words occurs even with a new addressee • Higher pitch, longer duration, more pauses for unpredictable game moves (Watson, Arnold, & Tanenhaus, 2008) Research Question Does nonlinguistic, task-unrelated effort also increase acoustic prominence? Test case: Memory Load Story-telling experiment 1.Subject sees panel 1 and hears sentence 2.Subject sees panel 2, repeats sentence 1 and adds a line to the story Concurrent Digit load How acoustically prominent is the reference to the main character? 1.The main character (Here Mickey) is larger / more interesting start of response 2.Only names included (pronouns excluded) 3.Only two-character conditions included (“single char” condition yielded pronouns) Results: Longer Duration Under Memory Load Multilevel model (SAS proc mixed) of log duration: significant effects: not significant effects in model ( = marginal): load (0, 2, or 3 digits) p < .02) condition target word (p < .05) digit span utterance length (p < .05) number digits remembered continuation (p < .05) disfluency control variables (List, load order, block , list order) Load also decreased frequency of pronoun use Mickey first: Mickey did some tricks with Daisy at the Western theme park. OR Mickey second: Daisy did some tricks with Mickey at the Western theme park. Mickey did some tricks with Daisy at the Western theme park. . . . . Mickey was good with the rope. References Babwah, L. (2008). Distraction and Reference in Discourse. Honors Thesis, UNC Chapel Hill. Bard, E. G., & Aylett, M. P. (2004). Referential form, word duration, and modeling the listener in spoken dialogue. In J. C. Trueswell & M. K. Tanenhaus (Eds.), Approaches to studying world-situated language use: Bridging the language-as-product and language-as-action traditions (pp. 173-191). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Bell, A., Jurafsky, D., et al.. (2003). Effects of disfluencies, predictability, and utterance position on word form variation in English conversation. JASA, 113(2), 1001-1024. Bolinger, D. (1986). Intonation and its parts melody in spoken English. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Brown, G. (1983). Prosodic structure and the given/new distinction. In A. Cutler & D. R. Ladd (Eds.), Prosody, models, and measurements. New York: Springer-Verlag. Christodoulou, A., & Arnold, J. E. (2008). Effects of production difficulty on prosody. Poster; Prosody08. Fowler, C. A., & Housum, J. (1987). Talkers’ signaling of ‘‘new’’ and ‘‘old’’ words in speech and listeners’ perception and use of the distinction. JML, 26, 489–504. Gahl, S. & Garnsey, S. M. (2004). Knowledge of grammar, knowledge of usage: Syntactic probabilities affect pronunciation variation. L anguage, 80, 748-775 Jurafsky, D., Bell, A., Gregory, M., & Raymond, W. (2001). Probabilistic relations between words... In J. Bybee & P. Hopper (Eds.), Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure (pp. 229-254). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Selkirk, E. O. (1996). Sentence prosody: Intonation, stress and phrasing. In J. A. Goldsmith (Ed.), The handbook of phonological theory (pp. 550–569). Cambridge, Mass, USA: Blackwell. Terken, J., & Hirschberg, J. (1994). Deaccentuation of words representing ‘given’ information…. Language and Speech, 37, 125-145. Watson, D., Arnold, J.E., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2008). Tic tac TOE: Effects of predictability and importance on acoustic prominence in language production. Cognition, 106, 1548-1557. Longer duration with load 315 320 325 330 335 340 no load 2 digit load 3 digit load Longer duration with trouble remembering load 290 300 310 320 330 340 350 360 370 380 no load 2 digit load 3 digit load remember 0 remember 1 remember 2 remember 3 Conclusions •Nonlinguistic task demands increase acoustic prominence • Memory task: effort affects duration • Distraction task: effort affects pitch • Production-internal load decreases both acoustic and lexical attenuation •The semantic/ pragmatic functions of acoustic prominence may partly reflect associated speaker-internal cognitive demands Longer duration for longer utterances 290 300 310 320 330 340 350 short (2-4 words) medium (5-7 words) long (8+ words) No load 2 5 3 9 6 Converging Evidence: Babwah (2008) Instructions to given or new entities Secondary, nonlinguistic task (responding to beeps) can distract speaker Distraction also decreased pronoun use Click on the cow. (beep! beep!) Now move the carrot to the circle. D istraction leads to h igh er pitch 163 164 165 166 167 168 169 170 171 172 173 distraction none D istraction given new Cognitive Effort Influences Acoustic Prominence: The Impact of Memory Resources Jennifer E. Arnold and Zenzi M. Griffin UNC Chapel Hill Georgia Tech

Transcript of Funded by NIH grant RO1 HD-4152 to J. Arnold NSF BCS-0318456 and NSF BCS-0318456 to Z. Griffin Why...

Page 1: Funded by NIH grant RO1 HD-4152 to J. Arnold NSF BCS-0318456 and NSF BCS-0318456 to Z. Griffin Why do speakers modulate acoustic prominence? Listener-oriented.

Funded by NIH grant RO1 HD-4152 to J. Arnold and NSF BCS-0318456 NSF BCS-0318456 to Z. Griffin

QuickTime™ and aTIFF (Uncompressed) decompressorare needed to see this picture.

Why do speakers modulate acoustic prominence?

Listener-oriented explanation:• Acoustic prominence (duration, pitch movement, high pitch, intensity) communicates something to the listener

• the referent is discourse-new / not-salient (Brown, 1983)• contrast (Terken & Hirschberg, 1994)• informativeness (Bolinger, 1986)• accent signals the focus structure (Selkirk, 1996)• provides additional information in the case of low predictability words

• duration shorter when predictable (Jurafsky et al., 2001; Gahl & Garnsey 2004)• words less intelligible when predictable (Fowler & Housum, 1987, Bard & Aylett, 2004)

Speaker-internal effort also increases acoustic prominence:

• Speaker disfluency correlates with characteristics of acoustic prominence

• longer duration (Bell et al., 2003)• higher pitch (Christodoulou & Arnold, Prosody08 poster)

• Bard & Aylett (2004): intelligibility loss for repeated words occurs even with a new addressee• Higher pitch, longer duration, more pauses for unpredictable game moves (Watson, Arnold, & Tanenhaus, 2008)

Research Question

Does nonlinguistic, task-unrelated effort also increase acoustic prominence?

Test case: Memory Load

Story-telling experiment

1.Subject sees panel 1 andhears sentence

2.Subject sees panel 2, repeats sentence 1and adds a line to thestory

Concurrent Digit load

How acoustically prominent is the reference to the main character?

1. The main character (Here Mickey) is larger / more interesting start of response2. Only names included (pronouns excluded)3. Only two-character conditions included (“single char” condition yielded pronouns)

Results: Longer Duration Under Memory Load

Multilevel model (SAS proc mixed) of log duration:significant effects: not significant effects in model ( = marginal):load (0, 2, or 3 digits) p < .02) conditiontarget word (p < .05) digit spanutterance length (p < .05) number digits rememberedcontinuation (p < .05) disfluency

control variables (List, load order, block, list order)

• Load also decreased frequency of pronoun use

Mickey first: Mickey did some tricks with Daisy at the Western theme park.

ORMickey second: Daisy did some tricks

with Mickey at the Western theme park.

QuickTime™ and aTIFF (Uncompressed) decompressor

are needed to see this picture.

Mickey did some tricks with Daisy at the Western theme park. . . . . Mickey was good with the rope.

References • Babwah, L. (2008). Distraction and Reference in Discourse. Honors Thesis, UNC Chapel Hill.• Bard, E. G., & Aylett, M. P. (2004). Referential form, word duration, and modeling the listener in spoken dialogue. In J. C. Trueswell & M. K. Tanenhaus (Eds.), Approaches to studying world-

situated language use: Bridging the language-as-product and language-as-action traditions (pp. 173-191). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.• Bell, A., Jurafsky, D., et al.. (2003). Effects of disfluencies, predictability, and utterance position on word form variation in English conversation. JASA, 113(2), 1001-1024.• Bolinger, D. (1986). Intonation and its parts melody in spoken English. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.• Brown, G. (1983). Prosodic structure and the given/new distinction. In A. Cutler & D. R. Ladd (Eds.), Prosody, models, and measurements. New York: Springer-Verlag.• Christodoulou, A., & Arnold, J. E. (2008). Effects of production difficulty on prosody. Poster; Prosody08.• Fowler, C. A., & Housum, J. (1987). Talkers’ signaling of ‘‘new’’ and ‘‘old’’ words in speech and listeners’ perception and use of the distinction. JML, 26, 489–504. • Gahl, S. & Garnsey, S. M. (2004). Knowledge of grammar, knowledge of usage: Syntactic probabilities affect pronunciation variation. Language, 80, 748-775 • Jurafsky, D., Bell, A., Gregory, M., & Raymond, W. (2001). Probabilistic relations between words... In J. Bybee & P. Hopper (Eds.), Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure (pp. 229-

254). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. • Selkirk, E. O. (1996). Sentence prosody: Intonation, stress and phrasing. In J. A. Goldsmith (Ed.), The handbook of phonological theory (pp. 550–569). Cambridge, Mass, USA: Blackwell.• Terken, J., & Hirschberg, J. (1994). Deaccentuation of words representing ‘given’ information…. Language and Speech, 37, 125-145. • Watson, D., Arnold, J.E., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2008). Tic tac TOE: Effects of predictability and importance on acoustic prominence in language production. Cognition, 106, 1548-1557.

Longer duration with load

315

320

325

330

335

340

no load 2 digit load 3 digit load

msec

Longer duration with trouble remembering load

290

300

310

320

330

340

350

360

370

380

no load 2 digit load 3 digit load

msec

remember 0

remember 1

remember 2

remember 3

Conclusions •Nonlinguistic task demands increase acoustic prominence

• Memory task: effort affects duration• Distraction task: effort affects pitch

• Production-internal load decreases both acoustic and lexical attenuation•The semantic/ pragmatic functions of acoustic prominence may partly reflect associated speaker-internal cognitive demands

Longer duration for longer utterances

290

300

310

320

330

340

350

short (2-4words)

medium (5-7words)

long (8+ words)

msec

No load 2 5 3 9 6

Converging Evidence: Babwah (2008)

Instructions to given or new entitiesSecondary, nonlinguistic task (responding to beeps) can distract speaker

Distraction also decreased pronoun use

Click on the cow.(beep! beep!)Now move the carrot to the circle.

Distraction leads to higher pitch

163164165166167168169170171172173

distract ion none

Distract ion

Hzgiven

new

Cognitive Effort Influences Acoustic Prominence: The Impact of Memory Resources Jennifer E. Arnold and Zenzi M. GriffinUNC Chapel Hill Georgia Tech