Frequency Weightings Based on Subjectively Dominant...

6
Frequency weightings based on subjectively dominant spectral ranges Torija Martinez, AJ, Flindell, IH, Self, RH and Torija Martinez, Antonio J Title Frequency weightings based on subjectively dominant spectral ranges Authors Torija Martinez, AJ, Flindell, IH, Self, RH and Torija Martinez, Antonio J Type Conference or Workshop Item URL This version is available at: http://usir.salford.ac.uk/id/eprint/53241/ Published Date 2015 USIR is a digital collection of the research output of the University of Salford. Where copyright permits, full text material held in the repository is made freely available online and can be read, downloaded and copied for non- commercial private study or research purposes. Please check the manuscript for any further copyright restrictions. For more information, including our policy and submission procedure, please contact the Repository Team at: [email protected] .

Transcript of Frequency Weightings Based on Subjectively Dominant...

  • F r e q u e n cy w eig h tin gs b a s e d on s u bjec tively do min a n t s p ec t r al

    r a n g e sTorija M a r tin ez, AJ, Flind ell, IH, S elf, RH a n d Torija M a r tin ez,

    Antonio J

    Tit l e F r e q u e ncy w eig h tings b a s e d on s u bjec tively do min a n t s p ec t r al r a n g e s

    Aut h or s Torija M a r tin ez, AJ, Flind ell, IH, S elf, RH a n d Torija M a r tin ez, Antonio J

    Typ e Confe r e nc e o r Works ho p It e m

    U RL This ve r sion is available a t : h t t p://usir.s alfor d. ac.uk/id/e p rin t/53 2 4 1/

    P u bl i s h e d D a t e 2 0 1 5

    U SIR is a digi t al collec tion of t h e r e s e a r c h ou t p u t of t h e U nive r si ty of S alford. Whe r e copyrigh t p e r mi t s, full t ex t m a t e ri al h eld in t h e r e posi to ry is m a d e fre ely availabl e online a n d c a n b e r e a d , dow nloa d e d a n d copied for no n-co m m e rcial p riva t e s t u dy o r r e s e a r c h p u r pos e s . Ple a s e c h e ck t h e m a n u sc rip t for a ny fu r t h e r copyrig h t r e s t ric tions.

    For m o r e info r m a tion, including ou r policy a n d s u b mission p roc e d u r e , ple a s econ t ac t t h e Re posi to ry Tea m a t : u si r@s alford. ac.uk .

    mailto:[email protected]

  • Frequency weightings based on subjectively

    dominant spectral ranges

    Antonio J. Torija, Ian H. Flindell, Rod Self

    ISVR, University of Southampton, SO17 1BJ Southampton, UK.

    Summary

    The optimisation of frequency weightings for the assessment of environmental and transportation

    noise has been an enduring research theme, with no real consensus achieved, other than increasing

    acknowledgement that the widespread adoption of the A-frequency weighting is an essentially

    arbitrary choice. Based on the results of 4 illustrative case studies, we propose a simple

    hypothesis that the 'best' frequency weighting in any specific context depends on which part of the

    frequency spectrum is subjectively dominant. This hypothesis helps to explain why different

    frequency weighting schemes appear to work 'best' in different situations and allows the selection

    of frequency weightings for use in assessment procedures to be carried out on a more rational and

    possibly less partisan basis. Further work is of course necessary to develop and extend our

    hypothesis to a wider range of different circumstances.

    PACS no. 43.50.Rq, 43.50.Qp, 43.50.Lj

    1. Introduction1

    The significant influence of frequency spectrum-

    related factors on perceived annoyance has been

    highlighted by several studies carried out on

    transportation noise [1,2].

    Even with the extensive attention in the literature,

    and widespread adoption in standards and

    regulations, still there is a lack of consensus about

    the frequency filter to be applied for evaluating

    transportation noise. Based on the physical

    dominance of the different frequency ranges: low

    (20-250Hz), mid (315-2000 Hz) and high (2500-

    20000 Hz) frequencies; diverse and, often

    opposing results regarding the most influential

    spectral region on perceived annoyance have been

    found by different research studies [2-6]. Because

    of this disparity of results, authors have pointed

    out one or another of the existing frequency

    weightings as the most appropriate for assessing

    transportation noise annoyance. All these findings

    seem to indicate that still there is an open debate,

    and that the adoption of the A-frequency weighting

    by most national standards is an essentially

    arbitrary choice.

    On the other hand, since the main purpose of

    transportation noise assessment is to estimate the

    magnitude of community annoyance, the issue

    arising should not be the identification of the

    physically dominant frequency spectrum range, but

    the identification of the one perceived as

    subjectively dominant.

    The hypothesis underlying this research is that the

    'optimal' frequency weighting in any specific

    context depends on which part of the frequency

    spectrum is subjectively dominant, and it is based

    on the findings presented by Torija and Flindell

    [7], where it was stated that whichever is the

    physically dominant part of the frequency

    spectrum is not necessarily a good guide to what

    part of the spectrum will be perceived as

    subjectively dominant.

    In order to illustrate this hypothesis the results of 4

    laboratory studies are presented and discussed.

    Thus, the performance of the application of A- and

    D-weighting filters is analyzed for assessing

    annoyance evoked by (i) urban road traffic noise

    under outdoor and indoor conditions and (ii) by

    urban traffic noise with low and high low-

    frequency content after the erection of different

    noise barriers.

    2. Road traffic noise under outdoor and indoor conditions

    2.1. Laboratory study under outdoor

    conditions

    In [7] a recording of continuous urban road traffic

    noise with physically prominent frequency

    components at low-frequency and middle/high-

    Copyright© (2015) by EAA-NAG-ABAV, ISSN 2226-5147All rights reserved

    1645

  • frequency 3rd-octave bands (Fig. 1), was used as

    the basis for all sounds reproduced in the listening

    tests. Three frequency filters were applied for

    boosting or cutting the low- frequency (LF), mid-frequency (MF), or high-frequency (HF) ranges. Twelve filtered sounds were produced by applying each filter with -9dB, -3 dB, +3dB and +9 dB relative gain setting.

    Figure 1. Frequency spectra of master recording road

    traffic noise stimuli used in outdoor and indoor

    laboratory studies.

    A five point semantic scale, according to standard ISO/TS 15666 [8]: “Not at all”, “Slightly”, “Moderately”, “Very”, and “Extremely”, was used for assessing perceived annoyance.

    2.2. Laboratory study under indoor conditions

    In [9] a continuous urban road traffic noise was

    filtered for simulating typical frequency dependent

    attenuation of double glazing sealed units made up

    from 3mm glass, 3 mm air gap, and 3 mm glass,

    according to the valued reported in [10].

    Moreover, artificial reverberation at 0.5 second

    reverberation time was added to increase the

    subjective realism of the intended indoor

    simulation. The outdoor and indoor frequency

    spectra are shown at Fig. 1. Low pass and high

    pass shelf filters were applied for boosting or

    cutting the LF, or mid/high-frequency (MHF)

    ranges by +9 dB, +3 dB, -3 dB, and -9 dB to the simulated indoor filtered sound, thereby producing 8 different sounds for the listening tests. In this laboratory study, the perceived annoyance was assessed using the relative magnitude estimation (RME) method. According to this method, the participants rated subjective annoyance of each stimulus numerically against a defined reference sound which was given an arbitrary rating of 100 (so that each reported annoyance value was referred to 100).

    2.3. A- and D-weighting for assessing road

    traffic noise annoyance

    In Fig. 2, it is observed that the application of the

    D-filter (right) for frequency weighting the sound

    level achieves higher correlations with reported

    annoyance than the A-filter (left) for the specific

    sounds used in [7]. In [7], for the filtered road

    traffic sounds tested, and under outdoor

    conditions, the MF and especially HF contents

    were found to be subjectively dominant. However,

    as indicated by the results shown in Fig. 2, the

    relatively high LF content in urban road traffic

    noise should not be neglected. Under outdoor

    conditions, the D-weighting better accounts for the

    difference in contribution to road traffic evoked

    subjective annoyance of LF, MF and HF ranges.

    Meanwhile, the A-weighing underestimates the

    contribution of LF and HF ranges, as can be seen

    in Fig. 2 – left (triangle and circle symbols).

    Fig. 3 shows the linear relationship between A-

    and D-weighted sound levels and reported

    annoyance for the stimuli used in the listening test

    presented in [9]. In [9], even under conditions

    where LF content was physically dominant (indoor

    conditions), it was found that changes in LF

    content made smaller contributions to reported

    annoyance than might be inferred from such

    physical dominance. Indeed, under the tested

    indoor conditions, equivalent changes in LF and

    MHF content led to similar changes in reported

    annoyance. Under these conditions, and as it is

    seen in Fig. 3, the A-weighting filter accounts for

    the difference in contribution to subjective

    annoyance between LF and MHF ranges, while D-

    weighting filter overestimates the contribution of

    LF.

    -10

    0

    10

    20

    30

    40

    50

    60

    70

    31

    ,5

    50

    8

    0

    12

    5

    20

    0

    31

    5

    50

    0

    80

    0

    12

    50

    2

    00

    0

    31

    50

    5

    00

    0

    80

    00

    1

    25

    00

    So

    un

    d L

    ev

    el

    (dB

    )

    1/3rd-octave Bands (Hz)

    Road Traffic Outdoor

    Road Traffic Indoor

    EuroNoise 201531 May - 3 June, Maastricht

    A.J. Torija et al.: Frequency...

    1646

  • Figure 2. Linear relationship between the A-weighted

    (left) and D-weighted (right) sound levels and

    perceived annoyance. Triangle, square and circle

    symbols correspond to LF, MF and HF filter gain,

    respectively.

    Figure 3. Linear relationship between the A-weighted

    (left) and D-weighted (right) sound levels and

    perceived annoyance. Triangle and circle symbols

    correspond to LF and MHF filter gain, respectively.

    3. Urban road traffic noise with noise barriers

    3.1. Stimuli and procedure

    For this laboratory study, two master recordings of

    continuous urban road traffic noise were selected,

    one with relatively high LF content (similar to the

    MF content), hereafter called RT1, and another

    one with relatively low LF content (as compared

    to the MF content), hereafter called RT2. These

    two master recordings were used as the basis for

    synthesising all the experimental sounds for the

    listening tests.

    To each of these two road traffic sounds, a series

    of frequency filters were applied for simulating

    the insertion loss (IL) generated by the presence of

    a selection of 10 noise barriers. The IL provided

    for each of the noise barriers was obtained from

    [11]. It should be noted that in [11], only 5 noise

    barriers were presented, so that the other 5 noise

    barriers simulated here were derived by keeping

    the same spectral pattern but reducing the

    simulated IL by 6 dB. In Fig. 4, the frequency

    spectra of each of the original master recording

    and of each of the filtered sound are shown.

    The 30 participants in this listening experiment

    assessed the annoyance evoked by all the filtered

    sounds using the RME method. Thus, for each

    master road traffic sound, RT1 and RT2, the

    participants rated subjective annoyance of each of

    the 10 stimuli (road traffic sounds filtered to

    simulate the presence of the 10 noise barriers)

    numerically against the reference sounds which

    were give an arbitrary rating of 100.

    3.2. A- and D-weighted sound levels vs.

    perceived annoyance

    As can be seen in Table I, for the road traffic noise

    with low LF content (RT2), the application of A

    and D filters for the frequency weighting of the

    sound level achieves similar results in assessing

    the perceived annoyance of the 10 filtered noise

    barrier sounds.

    PA = 0,1104·LAeq - 4,6554

    R² = 0,5019

    1,5

    2

    2,5

    3

    3,5

    4

    4,5

    55 60 65 70 75 80

    Pe

    rce

    ive

    d A

    nn

    oy

    an

    ce (

    Fiv

    e-P

    oin

    ts

    Sca

    le U

    nit

    s (1

    to

    5))

    A-weighted Sound Level (dBA)

    PA = 5,66·LAeq - 187,44 R² = 0,97

    PA = 4,30·LDeq - 166,23 R² = 0,84

    60

    80

    100

    120

    140

    160

    40 50 60 70 80

    Re

    lati

    ve

    Ma

    gn

    itu

    de

    Est

    ima

    tio

    n o

    f P

    erc

    eiv

    ed

    A

    nn

    oy

    an

    ce

    Sound Level

    PA = 0,11·LAeq - 4,66

    R² = 0,50

    1,5

    2

    2,5

    3

    3,5

    4

    4,5

    55 60 65 70 75 80

    Pe

    rce

    ive

    d A

    nn

    oy

    an

    ce (

    Fiv

    e-P

    oin

    ts

    Sca

    le U

    nit

    s (1

    to

    5))

    A-weighted Sound Level (dBA)

    PA = 0,17·LDeq - 9,43 R² = 0,78

    60 65 70 75 80 85

    D-weighted Sound Level (dBD)

    EuroNoise 201531 May - 3 June, Maastricht

    A.J. Torija et al.: Frequency...

    1647

  • Figure 4. Frequency spectra of the original and

    frequency filtered sounds to simulate the presence of

    the different noise barriers, for master urban road

    traffic noises 1 (RT1 – high LF content) and 2 (RT2 –

    low LF content).

    However, when the road traffic noise has high LF

    content (RT1), higher correlations with reported

    annoyance are obtained by using the D-weighting

    filter than by using the A-weighting filter. These

    results are consistent with the results presented in

    Section 2.3, i.e. for typical road traffic noise under

    outdoor conditions, the frequency regions

    subjectively dominant are MF, and especially HF,

    but if there is a strong component in the LF

    region, this should not be neglected.

    With high LF content, simulation of the presence

    of the different noise barriers made the LF region

    subjectively more important, and as observed in

    Table I, the A-weighting filter then

    underestimated the effect of the LF contribution,

    whilst the D-weighting filter was able to give a

    better representation of the LF contribution.

    Table I. Results of the Linear Regression analysis

    (N=10) for estimating perceived annoyance from A-

    weighted and D-weighted sound levels. p≤ 0.05.

    RT1

    R2 Adjusted

    R2

    Standard Error of the Estimate

    A-weighting 0.83 0.81 3.66

    D-weighting 0.90 0.89 2.80

    RT2

    R2 Adjusted

    R2

    Standard Error of the Estimate

    A-weighting 0.97 0.97 2.32

    D-weighting 0.97 0.97 2.45

    4. Conclusions

    In this paper, 4 case studies are presented to

    illustrate the hypothesis that the 'optimal'

    frequency weighting in any specific context

    depends on which part of the frequency spectrum

    is subjectively dominant. According to the results

    presented here, the selection of the most

    appropriate frequency weighting filter should not

    be made solely on the basis of which frequency

    region is physically dominant, but should also take

    into subjective dominance into account.

    10

    20

    30

    40

    50

    60

    70

    31,5 63 125 250 500 1000 2000 4000 8000 16000

    So

    un

    d L

    ev

    el

    (dB

    )

    Octave Bands (Hz)

    RT1

    Original NB_Timber NB_Metal NB_Transparent NB_Concrete NB_Vegetation NB_Timber_2 NB_Metal_2 NB_Transparent_2 NB_Concrete_2 NB_Vegetation_2

    31,5 63 125 250 500 1000 2000 4000 8000 16000

    Octave Bands (Hz)

    RT2

    EuroNoise 201531 May - 3 June, Maastricht

    A.J. Torija et al.: Frequency...

    1648

  • Differences in the relative contributions to

    subjective annoyance made by different spectral

    regions, i.e. low-, mid- and high-frequencies, are

    important for the selection of optimum frequency

    weightings.

    This finding may help to explain why different

    frequency weighting curves appear to work 'best'

    in different situations, but could also inform the

    selection of frequency weightings for use in

    assessment procedures to be carried out on a more

    rational and possibly less biased basis.

    Of course, further work would be necessary to

    develop and extend this research hypothesis to a

    wider range of different circumstances, i.e. across

    a wider range transportation and other noise

    sources and contexts.

    Acknowledgement

    This work is funded by the University of Malaga

    and the European Commission under the

    Agreement Grant no. 246550 of the seventh

    Framework Programme for R & D of the EU,

    granted within the People Programme, «Co-

    funding of Regional, National and International

    Programmes» (COFUND), and the Ministerio de

    Economía y Competitividad (COFUND2013-

    40259).

    References

    [1] G. Leventhall: A review of published research on low frequency noise and its effect. Report for Department for Environment, Food and Rural Affairs (DEFRA), May 2003. Available at http://archive.defra.gov.uk/environment/quality/noise/research/lowfrequency/documents/lowfreqnoise.pdf. (date last viewed 11/06/14).

    [2] H. G. Leventhall: Low frequency noise and annoyance. Noise and Health 6 (2004) 59-72.

    [3] M. E. Nilsson: A-weighted sound pressure level as an indicator of perceived loudness and annoyance of road-traffic sound. Journal of Sound and Vibration 302 (2007) 197-207.

    [4] G. R. Watts: A comparison of noise measures for assessing vehicle noisiness. Journal of Sound and Vibration 180 (1995) 493-512.

    [5] N. J. Versfeld, J. Vos: Annoyance caused by sounds of wheeled and tracked vehicle. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 101 (1997) 2677-2685.

    [6] J. Kim, C. Lim, J. Hong, S. Lee: Noise-induced annoyance from transportation noise: Short-term responses to a single noise source in a laboratory. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 127 (2009) 804-814.

    [7] A. J. Torija, I. H. Flindell: Differences in subjective loudness and annoyance depending on the road traffic noise spectrum. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 135 (2014) 1-4.

    [8] ISO, Acoustics-Assessment of noise annoyance by means of social and socio-acoustic surveys.

    ISO/TS15666:2003(E). 2003, ISO: Geneva, Switzerland.

    [9] A. J. Torija, I. H. Flindell: The subjective effect of low frequency content in road traffic noise. Journal of the Acoustical Journal of America 137 (2015) 189-198.

    [10] J. D. Quirt: Sound transmission through windows II. Double and triple glazing. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 74 (1983) 534-542.

    [11] J. Y. Hong, H. S. Jang, J. Y. Jeon: Evaluation of noise barriers for soundscape perception through laboratory experiments. Proceedings of the Acoustics 2012, Nantes, France, 2012, pp. 2159-2162.

    EuroNoise 201531 May - 3 June, Maastricht

    A.J. Torija et al.: Frequency...

    1649