Food and Emotion

download Food and Emotion

of 8

Transcript of Food and Emotion

  • 7/29/2019 Food and Emotion

    1/8

    Food and emotion

    Laura Canetti a, Eytan Bachar b, Elliot M. Berry a,*

    a Department of Human Nutrition and Metabolism, Hebrew University / Hadassah Medical School, Jerusalem 91120, Israelb Department of Psychiatry, Hadassah University Hospital, Jerusalem 91120, Israel

    Received 10 December 2001; accepted 4 March 2002

    Abstract

    The relationship between eating and emotion has always interested researchers of human behavior. This relationship

    varies according to the particular characteristics of the individual and according to the specific emotional state. We

    consider findings on the reciprocal interactions between, on the one hand, emotions and food intake, and, on the other,

    the psychological and emotional consequences of losing weight and dieting. Theories on the relationship between

    emotions and eating behaviors have their origin in the literature on obesity. The psychosomatic theory of obesity

    proposes that eating may reduce anxiety, and that the obese overeat in order to reduce discomfort. The internal/external

    theory of obesity hypothesizes that overweight people do not recognize physiological cues of hunger or satiety because

    of faulty learning. It thus predicts that normal weight people will alter (either increase or decrease) their eating whenstressed, while obese people will eat regardless of their physiological state. The restraint hypothesis postulates that

    people who chronically restrict their food intake overeat in the presence of disinhibitors such as the perception of

    having overeaten, alcohol or stress. These theories are examined in the light of present research and their implications

    on eating disorders are presented.

    # 2002 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.

    Keywords: Eating behavior; Eating disorders; Emotion; Obesity

    In their eating behavior, human beings are very

    much affected by their emotions: food choices,

    quantity and frequency of meals are all dependenton many variables not necessarily related to their

    physiological needs. The increasing prevalence of

    eating disorders and obesity in Western societies

    has raised many questions about the role that

    emotions play in the etiology of these problems.

    That these changes have occurred in a relatively

    short time frame suggests that environmental and

    psychological, rather than metabolic or genetic,

    causes are responsible.Although eating behavior has been studied in

    animals from a biological viewpoint, we will focus

    on human studies as the purpose of this article is to

    present eating behavior from a psychological

    viewpoint. It is widely accepted that the eating

    behavior in humans, changes according to changes

    in their emotional arousal (anxiety, anger, joy,

    depression, sadness and other emotions). How-

    ever, it is not possible to make a general statement

    about these relationships since the relation be-* Corresponding author. Fax: '/972-2-643-1105

    E-mail address: [email protected] (E.M. Berry).

    Behavioural Processes 60 (2002) 157/164

    www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc

    0376-6357/02/$ - see front matter # 2002 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.

    PII: S 0 3 7 6 - 6 3 5 7 ( 0 2 ) 0 0 0 8 2 - 7

    mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]
  • 7/29/2019 Food and Emotion

    2/8

    tween eating and emotion differs according to the

    particular characteristics of the individual and

    according to the specific emotional state. This

    paper will look first at the influence of emotionson eating behavior, then the influence of emotion

    on dieting and finally will discuss different theories

    dealing with the relationship between eating beha-

    vior and emotions and their implications for eating

    disorders.

    1. The influence of emotions on eating behavior

    Emotions differ in their antecedent conditions,

    physiological correlates, frequency of occurrenceand duration (Scherer et al., 1986). The associa-

    tions between a particular emotion and eating

    behavior should be stronger if this emotion occurs

    more frequently in eating contexts than other

    emotions (Macht and Simons, 2000). Early re-

    search paid little attention to the differential

    effects of different emotions. More recently a

    number of studies have compared various emo-

    tions, but research on their differential role

    characteristics is still sparse. Mehrabian (1980)

    inv

    estigated the relationship between differentemotions and amount of food intake. He found

    that higher food consumption was reported during

    boredom, depression and fatigue and lower food

    intake was reported during fear, tension and pain.

    Lyman (1982) showed a greater tendency to

    consume healthy foods during positive emotions

    and a greater tendency to consume junk food

    during negative emotions. Patel and Schlundt

    (2001) found that meals eaten in positive and

    negative moods were significantly larger than

    meals eaten in a neutral mood and that positive

    mood has a stronger impact than negative moodson food intake. Macht (1999) studied the differ-

    ential impact of anger, fear, sadness and joy.

    Subjects reported experiencing higher levels of

    hunger during anger and joy than during fear

    and sadness. They also reported that during anger

    there was an increase of impulsive eating (fast,

    irregular and careless eating directed at any food

    type available), and that during joy there was an

    increase of hedonic eating (the tendency to eat

    because of the pleasant taste of the food or

    because the consumed food is thought to be

    healthy). Thus, Machts study showed stronger

    influences of anger and joy on eating than of

    sadness and fear. This author suggests that angerand joy have a greater influence because these

    emotions are in general, more frequently experi-

    enced than sadness and fear.

    2. The influence of weight loss and dieting on

    emotion

    The classic work by Keys et al. (1950) showed

    that weight loss, even in normal weight men, may

    lead to physiologic and psychological moodchanges, some of which were quite similar to those

    found in anorectic subjects. The famous experi-

    ment, which was carried out on conscientious

    objectors, showed that problems, which are con-

    sidered to be characteristic of females with eating

    disorders, might also occur in males after con-

    siderable weight loss (mean 26%). The men

    complained of apathy, depression, irritability and

    moodiness; they also became preoccupied with

    food in thoughts and conversation. They collected

    recipes, became angry at food wastage and wouldtoy with their meals, sometimes taking up to 2 h to

    complete them. Thus, weight loss per se (whether

    in the obese or those of normal weight) may be the

    common trigger, which in certain predisposed

    individuals precipitates an abnormal response to

    food and body weight.

    Significant weight loss may also be accompanied

    by persistent physical and behavioral symptoms.

    These include mood changes and depression, cold

    intolerance, hair loss, and carotenemia and idea-

    tion similar to that found in patients with anorexia

    nervosa, accompanied by issues of control, regi-mentation, compulsive exercising, and preoccupa-

    tion with food and body image, even though the

    subjects may still be obese. Ironically an obese

    subject who goes from (say) 130/100 kg may have

    behavioral and physiological changes similar to

    those in an anorexic one at 30 kg weight (Berry,

    1999).

    Male patients who developed eating disorders

    after gastroplasty or bilio-pancreatic by-pass sur-

    gery, afford another example of the relationship

    L. Canetti et al. / Behavioural Processes 60 (2002) 157/164158

  • 7/29/2019 Food and Emotion

    3/8

    between dieting and mood changes. Bonne et al.

    (1996) described two morbidly obese young men

    who, following gastric surgery and weight losses of

    over 80 kg, became anorexic with intakes of lessthan 500 kcal/d and required psychiatric hospita-

    lization. At the other extreme are weight-restored

    anorexic patients who still have problems of size

    over-estimation with different sensory mod-

    alities*/visual, tactile and oral (Berry et al.,

    1995). Over 30 years ago Glucksman and Hirsch

    (1969) showed, using a distorting lens, that dieting

    may lead to a change in body image. Both weight

    loss and maintenance were associated with over-

    estimation of neutral and personal shapes.

    The National Weight Control Registry(NWCR) in the USA has sought to characterize

    successful dieters who have maintained an average

    weight loss of 13 kg for at least 5 years. While

    these subjects in some studies showed no different

    eating disorder pathology from normal obese

    subjects (Klem et al., 1997), other work from the

    same group noted among 784 subjects that 14%

    had worse thoughts about food and 20% about

    weight, than before their dieting (Klem et al.,

    1998). However, it remains a moot point whether

    it is possible to extrapolate from these ratherunique successful subjects (c5% of dieters) to the

    majority of unsuccessful yo-yo dieters.

    3. Theories relating emotions and eating behaviors

    The assumption that affect and eating are

    related has its origins in the literature on obesity.

    Thus, earlier theories explained overeating in

    obese individuals, while more recent theories aim

    at explaining eating behavior in a normal weight

    population. The following section looks at bothtypes of theory.

    3.1. Psychosomatic theories of obesity

    3.1.1. The Kaplan and Kaplan psychosomatic

    theory of obesity

    Kaplan and Kaplan (1957) proposed that obese

    people overeat when anxious and eating reduces

    this anxiety. The mechanism by which eating

    reduces anxiety is not completely understood but

    may involve differential effects of protein and

    carbohydrate intakes affecting the synthesis of

    brain neurotransmitters, in particular serotonin.

    Learning factors are also probably involved, e.g.an earlier association of pleasurable, non-anxious

    situations with feeding. However, these authors

    felt that the anxiety-reducing effects of eating

    cannot be solely explained on the basis of learned

    habits. They hypothesized that there is some

    degree of physiological incompatibility between

    the act of eating and intense fear or anxiety and

    that while eating, these emotions are temporarily

    diminished. Obese individuals are unable to dis-

    tinguish between hunger and anxiety because they

    learnt to eat in response to anxiety as well as inresponse to hunger. Thus, eating in order to reduce

    anxiety may lead to compulsive overeating and

    obesity.

    3.1.2. Bruchs theory

    Bruch (1973) connected overeating to faulty

    hunger awareness. This theory proposes that the

    experience of hunger is not innate but learning is

    necessary for its organization into recognizable

    patterns. In the case of obese people something

    had gone wrong in the experiential and interper-sonal process surrounding the satisfaction of

    nutritional and other bodily needs. Incorrect and

    confusing early experiences had interfered with

    their ability to recognize hunger and satiation.

    These early experiences had also interfered with

    the ability to differentiate hunger (the urge to eat),

    from other signals of discomfort that have nothing

    to do with food deprivation like emotional tension

    states aroused by a great variety of conflicts and

    problems. Such individuals do not recognize when

    they are hungry or satiated, nor do they differ-

    entiate need for food from other uncomfortablesensations and feelings. They require signals com-

    ing from outside to know when to eat and how

    much; since their own inner awareness has not

    been programmed correctly (Bruch, 1973). Thus,

    according to this theory, a person will overeat in

    response to emotional tension and uncomforta-

    ble sensations and feelings. Both Kaplan and

    Kaplan and Bruchs theories reach the same

    prediction: that obese individuals will overeat in

    response to uncomfortable emotional states.

    L. Canetti et al. / Behavioural Processes 60 (2002) 157/164 159

  • 7/29/2019 Food and Emotion

    4/8

    When summarizing her lifetime work in the

    field, Bruch (1985) who is considered a leading

    figure in the psychoanalytic thinking about anor-

    exia and bulimia, pointed to self psychology as thetheory which conceptualizes best her approach.

    Self psychology views food (its consumption in

    bulimia and its avoidance in anorexia) as the main

    stabilizing factor of dysphoric emotions and self

    esteem (Bachar et al., 1999).

    3.2. Schachters internal/external theory of

    obesity

    This theory (Schachter et al., 1968; Schachter,

    1971) makes somewhat different predictions fromthe theories described above. Here the physiologi-

    cal concomitants of fear and anxiety would lead

    normal weight people to suppress their consump-

    tion, but would not affect obese peoples con-

    sumption due to their insensitivity to internal cues.

    Like Bruch (1973), he hypothesized that the

    recognition of a set of physiological cues, including

    gastric contractions, as hunger was a learned

    phenomenon and that normal weight people had

    learned to label appropriately gastric contractions

    as hunger, whereas ov

    erweight people had not.Because gastric contractions decrease during

    stress, normal weight individuals will decrease

    their eating when stressed but such a decrease

    would have no effect on the eating of the obese. As

    a consequence of poor understanding of internal

    physiological cues, obese people will rely much

    more on external cues both to initiate and stop

    eating.

    While psychosomatic theories predict that obese

    people will increase their eating when they are

    stressed in order to reduce anxiety, Schachters

    theory predicts that normal weight people mayeither decrease or increase their eating when

    stressed, while obese people will not decrease it.

    A first study performed by Schachter et al. (1968)

    confirmed this prediction. They found that for

    normal weight subjects, stress decreased eating

    among those who were hungry and had no effect

    on those who were not hungry, while overweight

    subjects ate the same amount of food irrespective

    of their physiological state. However, later re-

    search did not replicate these previous findings

    (Lowe and Fisher, 1983; Pine, 1985; Reznick and

    Balch, 1977; Ruderman, 1983; Slochower et al.,

    1981) and only one study (McKenna, 1972)

    confirmed Schachters prediction but for palatablefood only.

    The question of whether emotions do influence

    eating behavior has been thoroughly studied in the

    obese population. These studies findings are

    closer to psychosomatic theories than to Schach-

    ters theory but they also shed light on the

    complexity of the eating behavior in obese people.

    A review of the field (Ganley, 1989) concluded that

    in massively obese subjects seeking treatment,

    emotional eating appears to be very common.

    Most studies reported a strong relationship be-tween eating and negative emotions or stressful life

    events. The emotional eating occurs episodically

    and not on a regular basis; it is done secretively, is

    associated with different emotions in different

    individuals and is characterized by the use of

    high-calorie or high carbohydrate food (Ganley,

    1989). Emotional eating has been found to be most

    frequent when people are alone, when the meal is a

    supper or a snack, and when the meal is eaten at

    home compared to away from home (Baumeister

    et al., 1994). Emotional eating is prev

    alent acrossthe various social classes and the sexes. Studies

    consistently report that emotional eating is most

    often precipitated by negative emotions such as

    anger, depression, boredom, anxiety and loneliness

    and often bears an episodic relationship to stress-

    ful periods of life (Ganley, 1989).

    The impact of positive mood on food intake has

    not been as well studied as that of negative moods.

    Studies that have looked at such relationships

    yield conflicting results: Schmitz (1996) and Davis

    et al. (1985) did not find any correlation between

    food intake and positive moods. HoweverSchlundt et al. (1988) found that positive mood

    was related to overeating in social situations. A

    recent study (Patel and Schlundt, 2001) showed

    that food intake is larger for both positive and

    negative moods compared to a neutral mood.

    These authors propose that mood effects, whether

    positive or negative, both involve a disinhibition of

    eating control. They also suggest that positive

    mood may increase food intake via an associative

    learning mechanism where happiness has been

    L. Canetti et al. / Behavioural Processes 60 (2002) 157/164160

  • 7/29/2019 Food and Emotion

    5/8

    associated with eating more food. These findings

    can be also in line with Bruchs hypothesis that

    obese individuals do not differentiate need for

    food from emotional tension states*/in this casethe tension being a pleasant one.

    All studies reviewed were performed on obese

    populations and most of them found significant

    differences between the obese subjects and the

    normal weight control groups or significant

    weight-dependent correlations in the area of emo-

    tional eating. The conclusion that obese people

    engage in significantly more emotional eating

    compared to non-obese seems quite robust, and

    provides support to the psychosomatic theories.

    However, this conclusion has been criticized byAllison and Heshka (1993), who claim that per-

    haps obese persons report more emotional eating

    than the non-obese because they are complying

    with a social role.

    3.3. The restraint hypothesis

    Investigators in the field observed that obese

    people are almost always trying to restrain their

    food intake. Thus, the question concerning eating

    patterns of obese people should be rephrased tothe role of dieting in obese as well as normal

    weight individuals. The restraint hypothesis was

    originally developed by Herman and Mack (1975)

    and further elaborated by Herman and Polivy

    (1980). According to these researchers the balance

    between the desire for food and the effort to resist

    that desire affects eating behaviors, and restraint is

    the cognitive effort to resist that desire. Restrained

    eaters constantly worry about what they eat and

    chronically restrict their food intake in order to

    avoid becoming fat. At the other end are the

    unrestrained eaters who eat freely and do notworry about their food intake or its consequences.

    These authors also postulated a disinhibition

    hypothesis: according to which, self control of

    restrained eaters may be temporarily released by

    disrupting events or disinhibitors which include

    specific cognitions (the perception of having

    overeaten), alcohol or strong emotional states

    (such as anxiety and depression).

    A review of the literature (Ruderman, 1986)

    concluded that this hypothesis has been empiri-

    cally confirmed. Most attention has focused on the

    assumption that the perception of having over-

    eaten disinhibits restrained eaters. This has been

    manipulated by having subjects eat a pre-loadbefore a taste test. Overall, studies show that the

    perception of having eaten a high calorie pre-load

    leads to overeating in chronically restrained eaters,

    who tend to think in a rigid, all-or-nothing

    fashion. The influence of alcohol has also been

    studied but results are not clear. The effects of

    emotional states on the consumption of restrained

    and unrestrained eaters have been examined. Her-

    man and Polivy (1984) have hypothesized that

    strong emotions make demands on restrained

    eaters energies, thereby temporarily decreasingtheir motivation to diet and allowing them to

    overeat. Although the restraint hypothesis predicts

    that any strong emotion would disinhibit the

    restrained eaters, research has focused principally

    on the effects of anxiety and depression on eating

    (Ruderman, 1986). In their first study Herman and

    Polivy (1975) found, as expected, that unrestrained

    eaters ate significantly less in the high than in the

    low anxiety condition. However, restrained eaters

    ate slightly, but not significantly, more in the high

    than in the low anxiety condition. Poliv

    y andHerman (1976) found among clinically depressed

    patients, that unrestrained eaters reported a sig-

    nificant weight loss and restrained eaters a sig-

    nificant weight gain after the onset of depression.

    Ruderman (1986) reaches the conclusion that

    negative affective states generally increase the

    consumption of restrained eaters, but their impact

    on unrestrained eaters is unclear: negative affect

    diminished consumption in the Herman and

    Polivy study (1975), marginally reduced it in the

    Baucom and Aiken study (1981) and did not

    significantly affect it in the Ruderman study(1985). A more recent study (Schotte et al., 1990)

    on negative affects induced by viewing frightening

    films, replicated the finding that they trigger

    overeating in restrained subjects. Again, in unrest-

    rained eaters, such negative affect did not signifi-

    cantly affect food intake. More recently, research

    in the field also examined affects other than

    anxiety and depression and reached similar results:

    Cools et al. (1992) showed that exposure to a

    segment from an amusing comedy film disinhib-

    L. Canetti et al. / Behavioural Processes 60 (2002) 157/164 161

  • 7/29/2019 Food and Emotion

    6/8

    ited eating in restrained eaters. Sheppard-Sawyer

    et al. (2000) tested whether film-induced sadness*/

    a state characterized by high negative valence, but

    low arousal*/enhanced food intake in restrainedeaters. They found that exposure to sad film

    segments significantly reduced food intake in

    unrestrained eaters, but only increased it non-

    significantly in restrained eaters. The authors

    hypothesized that restrained participants may not

    exhibit disinhibited eating when exposed to mood

    changes that did not threaten their self-esteem.

    4. Eating disorders

    Although psychosomatic theories and the re-

    straint hypothesis were related to observations of

    eating behavior in obese people, these theories may

    also explain behavior of subjects with eating

    disorders. The restraint hypothesis has been theo-

    retically applied (Polivy and Herman, 1985), and

    empirically tested (Cooper and Bowskill, 1986;

    Davis et al., 1988) in these disorders. According to

    the restraint hypothesis, chronic dieters restrain

    their food intake until a disinhibitor causes a

    temporary break in the diet regimen with ov

    er-eating. The disinhibitors might be the forced

    consumption of high caloric food, the intake of

    alcohol or experiencing tension states such as

    anxiety and depression. Bulimic patients and

    bulimic anorexics will binge eat, thus it may be

    that the cause of such bingeing is restraint in the

    intake of food prior to the development of the

    disorder. This assumption concerning the devel-

    opment of binge eating is confirmed by the

    observation that in most cases, bulimia develops

    several months after the onset of dieting (Boskind-

    Lodahl, 1976; Boskind-Lodahl and Sirlin, 1977;Garfinkel et al., 1980; Pyle et al., 1981), and by the

    fact that bulimics often have binge episodes after

    negative emotional states. For example, bulimics

    report more negative mood in the hour prior to a

    binge episode, compared with their moods prior to

    consuming a snack or meal (Davis et al., 1988). In

    another study, bulimic patients were significantly

    more depressed, anxious, lonely and bored in the 3

    h before a binge episode, compared with baseline

    ratings of the 3 h after the episode (Cooper and

    Bowskill, 1986). The observation that patients

    with bulimia nervosa almost always report that

    tension precipitates bulimic episodes, is also in line

    with the psychosomatic theories of obesity pre-dicting that overeating reduces tension states.

    5. Conclusions

    Emotions do influence eating behavior in hu-

    man beings. Negative emotions have been thor-

    oughly studied and it is well established that they

    increase food consumption. Positive emotions also

    increase food intake but this is less conclusive. It

    seems that frequent emotions such as joy andanger have a greater impact on food intake

    compared to less frequent ones. The above con-

    clusions are valid for normal weight as well as

    overweight people. However, the influence of

    emotions on eating behavior is stronger in obese

    people than in the non-obese, and it is stronger in

    people on diets than in non-dieters. The conclusion

    that obese people engage in significantly more

    emotional eating than the non-obese has been

    confirmed empirically and is in line with the

    psychosomatic theories of obesity. Dieters arealso more prone to emotional eating as proposed

    by the restraint theory. Binge eating in bulimic

    subjects might be understood as the undesired

    outcome of restrained eating.

    References

    Allison, D.B., Heshka, S., 1993. Emotion and eating in obesity?

    A critical analysis. International Journal of Eating Dis-

    orders 13, 289/295.

    Bachar, E., Latzer, Y., Kreitler, S., Berry, E.M., 1999.

    Empirical comparison of two psychological therapies: self

    psychology and cognitive orientation in the treatment of

    anorexia and bulimia. The Journal of Psychotherapy

    Practice and Research 8, 115/128.

    Baucom, D.H., Aiken, P.A., 1981. Effect of depressed mood on

    eating among nonobese dieting and nondieting persons.

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 41, 577/585.

    Baumeister, R.F., Heatherton, T.F., Tice, D.M., 1994. Losing

    Control: How and How People Fail at Self-Regulation.

    Academic Press, Inc, San Diego, CA.

    Berry, E.M., Fried, S., Edelstein, E., 1995. Abnormal oral

    sensory perception in patients with a history of anorexia

    L. Canetti et al. / Behavioural Processes 60 (2002) 157/164162

  • 7/29/2019 Food and Emotion

    7/8

    nervosa and the relationship between physiological and

    psychological improvement in this disease. Psychotherapy

    and Psychosomatics 63, 32/37.

    Berry, E.M., 1999. The reduced obese syndrome and eatingdisorders. In: Guy-Grand, B., Ailhaud, G. (Eds.), Progress

    in Obesity Research, vol. 8. John Libbey & Co, London, pp.

    777/780.

    Bonne, O.B., Bashi, R., Berry, E.M., 1996. Anorexia nervosa

    following gastroplasty in the male: two cases. International

    Journal of Eating Disorders 19, 105/108.

    Boskind-Lodahl, M., 1976. Cinderellas stepsisters: a feminist

    perspective on anorexia nervosa and bulimia. Signs: Journal

    of Women in Culture and Society 2, 324/356.

    Boskind-Lodahl, M., Sirlin, J., 1977. The gorging-purging

    syndrome. Psychology Today 3, 50/52.

    Bruch, H., 1973. Eating Disorders: Obesity, Anorexia Nervosa

    and the Person Within. Basic Books, New York.

    Bruch, H., 1985. Four decades of eating disorders. In: Garner,D.M., Garfinkel, P.E. (Eds.), Handbook of Psychotherapy

    for Anorexia Nervosa and Bulimia. Guilford Press, New

    York.

    Cools, J., Schotte, D.E., McNally, R.J., 1992. Emotional

    arousal and overeating in restrained eaters. Journal of

    Abnormal Psychology 101, 348/351.

    Cooper, P.J., Bowskill, R., 1986. Dysphoric mood and over-

    eating. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 25, 155/156.

    Davis, R., Freeman, R.J., Garner, D.M., 1988. A naturalistic

    investigation of eating behavior in bulimia nervosa. Journal

    of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 56, 273/279.

    Davis, R., Freeman, R.J., Solyom, L., 1985. Mood and food: an

    analysis of bulimic episodes. Journal of Psychiatric Re-search 19, 331/335.

    Ganley, R.M., 1989. Emotion and eating in obesity: a review of

    the literature. International Journal of Eating Disorders 8,

    343/361.

    Garfinkel, P.E., Moldofsky, H., Garner, D.M., 1980. The

    heterogeneity of anorexia nervosa: bulimia as a distinct

    subgroup. Archives of General Psychiatry 37, 1036/1040.

    Glucksman, M.L., Hirsch, J., 1969. The response of obese

    patients to weight reduction. Psychosomatic Medicine 31,

    1/7.

    Herman, C.P., Mack, D., 1975. Restrained and unrestrained

    eating. Journal of Personality 43, 647/660.

    Herman, C.P., Polivy, J., 1975. Anxiety, restraint, and eating

    behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 84, 666/672.Herman, C.P., Polivy, J., 1980. Restrained eating. In: Stunkard,

    A.B. (Ed.), Obesity. Saunders, Philadelphia.

    Herman, C.P., Polivy, J., 1984. A boundary model for the

    regulation of eating. In: Stunkard, A.B., Stellar, E. (Eds.),

    Eating and its disorders. Raven Press, New York, pp. 141/

    156.

    Kaplan, H.I., Kaplan, H.S., 1957. The psychosomatic concept

    of obesity. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 125,

    181/201.

    Keys, A., Brozek, J., Henschel, A., Mickelsen, O., Taylor, H.L.,

    1950. The Biology of Human Starvation. University of

    Minnesota Press, Minneapolis.

    Klem, M.L., Wing, R.R., McGuire, M.T., Seagle, H.M., Hill,

    J.O., 1997. A descriptive study of individuals successful at

    long-term maintenance of substantial weight loss. American

    Journal of Clinical Nutrition 66, 239 /246.Klem, M.L., Wing, R.R., McGuire, M.T., Seagle, H.M., Hill,

    J.O., 1998. Psychological symptoms in individuals successful

    at long-term maintenance of weight loss. Health Psychology

    17, 336/345.

    Lowe, M.R., Fisher, E.B., 1983. Emotional reactivity, emo-

    tional eating, and obesity: a naturalistic review. Journal of

    Behavioral Medicine 6, 135/149.

    Lyman, B., 1982. The nutritional values and food group

    characteristics of food preferred during various emotions.

    Journal of Psychology 112, 121/127.

    Macht, M., 1999. Characteristics of eating in anger, fear,

    sadness and joy. Appetite 33, 129/139.

    Macht, M., Simons, G., 2000. Emotions and eating in everyday

    life. Appetite 35, 65/71.

    McKenna, R.J., 1972. Some effects of anxiety level and food

    cues on the eating behavior of obese and normal subjects: a

    comparison of the Schachterian and psychosomatic concep-

    tions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 22,

    311/319.

    Mehrabian, A., 1980. Basic Dimensions for a General Psycho-

    logical Theory. Oelschlager, Gunn & Hain, Cambridge.

    Patel, K.A., Schlundt, D.G., 2001. Impact of moods and social

    context on eating behavior. Appetite 36, 111/118.

    Pine, C.J., 1985. Anxiety and eating behavior in obese and

    nonobese American Indians and White Americans. Journal

    of Personality and Social Psychology 49, 774/780.

    Polivy, J., Herman, C.P., 1976. Clinical depression and weight

    change: a complex relation. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-

    ogy 85, 338/340.

    Polivy, J., Herman, C.P., 1985. Dieting and binging: a causal

    analysis. American Psychologist 40, 1193/2201.

    Pyle, R.L., Mitchell, J.E., Eckert, E.D., 1981. Bulimia: a report

    of 34 cases. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 42, 60/64.

    Reznick, H., Balch, P., 1977. The effects of anxiety and

    response cost manipulations on the eating behavior of obese

    and normal weight subjects. Addictive Behaviors 2, 219/

    225.

    Ruderman, A.J., 1983. Obesity, anxiety and food consumption.

    Addictive Behaviors 8, 235/242.

    Ruderman, A.J., 1985. Dysphoric mood and overeating: a testof restraint theorys disinhibition hypothesis. Journal of

    Abnormal Psychology 94, 78/85.

    Ruderman, A.J., 1986. Dietary restraint: a theoretical and

    empirical review. Psychological Bulletin 99, 247/262.

    Schachter, S., 1971. Some extraordinary facts about obese

    humans and rats. American Psychologist 26, 129/144.

    Schachter, S., Goldman, R., Gordon, A., 1968. Effects of fear,

    food deprivation and obesity on eating. Journal of Person-

    ality and Social Psychology 10, 90/97.

    Scherer, K.R., Wallbott, H.G., Summerfield, A.B., 1986.

    Experiencing Emotion: A Crosscultural Study. Cambridge

    University Press, Cambridge.

    L. Canetti et al. / Behavioural Processes 60 (2002) 157/164 163

  • 7/29/2019 Food and Emotion

    8/8

    Schlundt, D.G., Sbrocco, T., Bell, C., 1988. Identification of

    high risk situations in a behavioral weight loss program:

    application of the relapse model. International Journal of

    Obesity 13, 223/234.Schmitz, B.A., 1996. The relationship between affect and binge

    eating. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The

    Sciences and Engineering 56, 7055.

    Schotte, D.E., Cools, J., McNally, R.J., 1990. Film-induced

    negative affect triggers overeating in restrained eaters.

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology 99, 317/320.

    Sheppard-Sawyer, C.L., McNally, R.J., Fischer, J.H., 2000.Film-induced sadness as a trigger for disinhibited eating.

    International Journal of Eating Disorders 28, 215/220.

    Slochower, J., Kaplan, S., Mann, L., 1981. The effects of life

    stress and weight on mood and eating. Appetite 2, 115 /125.

    L. Canetti et al. / Behavioural Processes 60 (2002) 157/164164