Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

138
Reliable individual functional networks and their relationship to behavior Emily S. Finn, PhD Section on Functional Imaging Methods Laboratory of Brain & Cognition, NIMH emily.fi[email protected] Taking Connectivity to a Skeptical Future Educational Workshop | OHBM Annual Meeting June 25, 2017 @esfinn

Transcript of Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Page 1: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Reliable individual functional networks and their relationship to behavior

Emily S. Finn, PhDSection on Functional Imaging MethodsLaboratory of Brain & Cognition, [email protected]

Taking Connectivity to a Skeptical Future Educational Workshop | OHBM Annual MeetingJune 25, 2017

@esfinn

Page 2: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Outline

Introduction:Why study individual differences?

1 How to get good brain data

2 How to get good behavioral data

3

Page 3: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Outline

Introduction:Why study individual differences?

1 How to get good brain data

2

How to relate brain to behavior

4

How to get good behavioral data

3

Page 4: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Outline

Introduction:Why study individual differences?

1 How to get good brain data

2

How to relate brain to behavior

4

Learn more5How to get good behavioral data

3

Page 5: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Outline

Introduction:Why study individual differences?

1 How to get good brain data

2

How to relate brain to behavior

4

Learn more5How to get good behavioral data

3

Page 6: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Why study individual differences?

Controls

Patients

Page 7: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Why study individual differences?

Controls

Patients

• Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors

Page 8: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Why study individual differences?

Controls

Patients

• Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors

• Eventual clinical applications:

Page 9: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Why study individual differences?

Categorical approach

Controls

Patients

• Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors

• Eventual clinical applications:

Page 10: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Why study individual differences?

Categorical approach

Controls

Patients

• Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors

• Eventual clinical applications:

Page 11: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Why study individual differences?

Categorical approach

Controls

Patients

• Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors

• Eventual clinical applications:

Page 12: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Why study individual differences?

Categorical approach

Controls

Patients

• Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors

• Eventual clinical applications:

p < 0.05*

Page 13: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Why study individual differences?

Categorical approach Dimensional approach

Controls

Patients

• Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors

• Eventual clinical applications:

p < 0.05*

Page 14: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Why study individual differences?

Categorical approach Dimensional approach

Controls

Patients

• Basic neuroscience: mapping from individual brains to individual behaviors

• Eventual clinical applications:

p < 0.05*

Page 15: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Why study individual differences?Motor

Mt

EmotionEm

LanguageLg

Working memoryWM

RestingR1

+

RestingR2

+

Page 16: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Why study individual differences?Motor

Mt

EmotionEm

LanguageLg

Working memoryWM

RestingR1

RestingR2

Page 17: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Why study individual differences?Motor

Mt

EmotionEm

LanguageLg

Working memoryWM

RestingR1

RestingR2

Page 18: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Why study individual differences?

Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)

Page 19: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Why study individual differences?

0.93 0.84 0.63

0.72 0.79 0.67

0.64 0.60 0.54

R1

WM

Mt

R2 Lg Em

Data

base

Target

ID rate

0.5 1.0

Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)

Page 20: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Why study individual differences?

You always look most like yourself, regardless of what you’re doing

0.93 0.84 0.63

0.72 0.79 0.67

0.64 0.60 0.54

R1

WM

Mt

R2 Lg Em

Data

base

Target

ID rate

0.5 1.0

Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)

Page 21: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Why study individual differences?

You always look most like yourself, regardless of what you’re doing

0.93 0.84 0.63

0.72 0.79 0.67

0.64 0.60 0.54

R1

WM

Mt

R2 Lg Em

Data

base

Target

ID rate

0.5 1.0

Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)

Page 22: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Why study individual differences?

You always look most like yourself, regardless of what you’re doing

0.93 0.84 0.63

0.72 0.79 0.67

0.64 0.60 0.54

R1

WM

Mt

R2 Lg Em

Data

base

Target

ID rate

0.5 1.0

Individual differences in FC predict individual differences in behavior

Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)

Page 23: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Outline

Introduction:Why study individual differences?

1 How to get good brain data

2

How to relate brain to behavior

4

Learn more5How to get good behavioral data

3

Page 24: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Outline

Introduction:Why study individual differences?

1 How to get good brain data

2

How to relate brain to behavior

4

Learn more5How to get good behavioral data

3

Page 25: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop
Page 26: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Do you need HCP-quality data?

Page 27: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Do you need HCP-quality data?A. Not really

Page 28: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Do you need HCP-quality data?A. Not really

ID is fairly robust even at more standard spatial & temporal resolutions:

Page 29: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Do you need HCP-quality data?A. Not really

Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016)

ID is fairly robust even at more standard spatial & temporal resolutions:

Page 30: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Do you need HCP-quality data?A. Not really

Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016)

ID is fairly robust even at more standard spatial & temporal resolutions:

Courtesy of Jason Druzgal

Page 31: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Do you need HCP-quality data?A. Not really

• More nodes —> higher identification rate

Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016)

ID is fairly robust even at more standard spatial & temporal resolutions:

Courtesy of Jason Druzgal

Page 32: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Do you need HCP-quality data?A. Not really

• More nodes —> higher identification rate

‣ Parcellation method (random vs. functional) did not matter

Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016)

ID is fairly robust even at more standard spatial & temporal resolutions:

Courtesy of Jason Druzgal

Page 33: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Do you need HCP-quality data?A. Not really

• More nodes —> higher identification rate

‣ Parcellation method (random vs. functional) did not matter‣ Caution: Higher resolution may amplify effects of anatomical diffs/registration error

Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016)

ID is fairly robust even at more standard spatial & temporal resolutions:

Courtesy of Jason Druzgal

Page 34: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Do you need HCP-quality data?A. Not really

• More nodes —> higher identification rate

‣ Parcellation method (random vs. functional) did not matter‣ Caution: Higher resolution may amplify effects of anatomical diffs/registration error‣ Parcellations in the 200-300 node range seem like a good compromise

Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016)

ID is fairly robust even at more standard spatial & temporal resolutions:

Courtesy of Jason Druzgal

Page 35: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop
Page 36: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. What about amount of data?

Page 37: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. What about amount of data?A. Scan duration matters!

Page 38: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. What about amount of data?A. Scan duration matters!

Longer acquisitions are better:

Page 39: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. What about amount of data?A. Scan duration matters!

Longer acquisitions are better:• higher reliability within subjects

Birn et al., NeuroImage (2013)

Page 40: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. What about amount of data?A. Scan duration matters!

Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)

• higher identifiability across subjects

Longer acquisitions are better:• higher reliability within subjects

Birn et al., NeuroImage (2013)

Page 41: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. What about amount of data?A. Scan duration matters!

Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)

• higher identifiability across subjects

‣ higher sampling rate (shorter TR) cannot make up for shorter scan duration

Longer acquisitions are better:• higher reliability within subjects

Birn et al., NeuroImage (2013)

Page 42: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. What about amount of data?A. Scan duration matters!

Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)

• higher identifiability across subjects

‣ higher sampling rate (shorter TR) cannot make up for shorter scan duration

Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016)

Longer acquisitions are better:• higher reliability within subjects

Birn et al., NeuroImage (2013)

Page 43: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. What about amount of data?A. Scan duration matters!

Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)

• higher identifiability across subjects

‣ higher sampling rate (shorter TR) cannot make up for shorter scan duration

Airan et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2016)

Shah et al., Brain & Behav (2016)

Longer acquisitions are better:• higher reliability within subjects

Birn et al., NeuroImage (2013)

Page 44: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop
Page 45: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Does scan condition matter?

Page 46: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Does scan condition matter?A. Yes!

Page 47: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Does scan condition matter?A. Yes!Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences, but tasks may increase signal-to-noise

Page 48: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Does scan condition matter?A. Yes!Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences, but tasks may increase signal-to-noise

0.93 0.84 0.63

0.72 0.79 0.67

0.64 0.60 0.54

R1

WM

Mt

R2 Lg Em

Data

base

Target

ID rate

0.5 1.0

Page 49: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Does scan condition matter?A. Yes!Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences, but tasks may increase signal-to-noise

Page 50: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Does scan condition matter?A. Yes!Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences, but tasks may increase signal-to-noise

Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)

Page 51: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Does scan condition matter?A. Yes!Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences, but tasks may increase signal-to-noise

Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)

Page 52: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Does scan condition matter?A. Yes!Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences, but tasks may increase signal-to-noise

Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)

Page 53: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Does scan condition matter?A. Yes!Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences, but tasks may increase signal-to-noise

Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)

Page 54: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Does scan condition matter?A. Yes!Rest has become the default condition for FC & individual differences, but tasks may increase signal-to-noise

‣ for a set scan duration, tasks may be more reliable than rest‣ tasks may converge faster on a subject’s “true” profile

Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)

Page 55: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop
Page 56: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Is rest best?

Page 57: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Is rest best?A. Probably not

Page 58: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Is rest best?A. Probably not

Tasks may stabilize individuals’ functional architecture, increase SNR:

Page 59: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Is rest best?A. Probably not

‣ some task pairs give better ID than the two rest sessions

Tasks may stabilize individuals’ functional architecture, increase SNR:

Page 60: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Is rest best?A. Probably not

‣ some task pairs give better ID than the two rest sessions

Target

Data

base

Day 1 Day 2

ID ra

te

Day

1D

ay 2

Tasks may stabilize individuals’ functional architecture, increase SNR:

Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)

Page 61: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Is rest best?A. Probably not

‣ some task pairs give better ID than the two rest sessions

Target

Data

base

Day 1 Day 2

ID ra

te

Day

1D

ay 2

‣ consider a combination of rest and task

Tasks may stabilize individuals’ functional architecture, increase SNR:

Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)

Page 62: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Is rest best?A. Probably not

‣ some task pairs give better ID than the two rest sessions

Target

Data

base

Day 1 Day 2

ID ra

te

Day

1D

ay 2

‣ consider a combination of rest and task

Tasks may stabilize individuals’ functional architecture, increase SNR:

Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)

Finn et al., Nat Neurosci (2015)

Page 63: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not

Page 64: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not

Consider naturalistic tasks:

Page 65: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not

Consider naturalistic tasks:

Inscapes: Vanderwal et al., NeuroImage 2015headspacestudios.org

Page 66: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not

Consider naturalistic tasks:

Page 67: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not

Consider naturalistic tasks:

‣ ID rate is just as good as (if not better than) rest

Page 68: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not

Consider naturalistic tasks:

Session 1

Rest Inscapes Ocean’s 11

Session 2

‣ ID rate is just as good as (if not better than) rest

Page 69: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not

Consider naturalistic tasks:

Session 1

Rest Inscapes Ocean’s 11

Session 2

‣ ID rate is just as good as (if not better than) rest

Vanderwal et al., NeuroImage (2017)

Page 70: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not

Consider naturalistic tasks:

Session 1

Rest Inscapes Ocean’s 11

Session 2

‣ ID rate is just as good as (if not better than) rest

Vanderwal et al., NeuroImage (2017)

Page 71: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not

Page 72: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not

Tasks also have purely practical advantages:

Page 73: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

‣ increase subject compliance (i.e., decrease head motion), especially in certain populations

Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not

Tasks also have purely practical advantages:

Page 74: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

‣ increase subject compliance (i.e., decrease head motion), especially in certain populations

Huijbers et al., NeuroImage (2017)

Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not

Tasks also have purely practical advantages:

Page 75: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

‣ increase subject compliance (i.e., decrease head motion), especially in certain populations

Child

ren

Adul

ts

Vanderwal et al., NeuroImage (2015)Huijbers et al., NeuroImage (2017)

Q. Is rest best? A. Probably not

Tasks also have purely practical advantages:

Page 76: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Outline

Introduction:Why study individual differences?

1 How to get good brain data

2

How to relate brain to behavior

4

Learn more5How to get good behavioral data

3

Page 77: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Outline

Introduction:Why study individual differences?

1 How to get good brain data

2

How to relate brain to behavior

4

Learn more5How to get good behavioral data

3

Page 78: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

How to choose behavior

Page 79: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

How to choose behaviorIs it stable?

Page 80: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

How to choose behaviorIs it stable?

• Trait vs. state• State variables may be better

suited to within-subject analysis

Page 81: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

How to choose behaviorIs it stable?

• Trait vs. state• State variables may be better

suited to within-subject analysis

Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)

Page 82: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

How to choose behaviorIs it stable?

• Trait vs. state• State variables may be better

suited to within-subject analysis

Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)

Page 83: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

How to choose behaviorIs it stable?

• Trait vs. state• State variables may be better

suited to within-subject analysis

Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)

Page 84: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

How to choose behaviorIs it stable?

Does it show a good distribution in your population?

• Trait vs. state• State variables may be better

suited to within-subject analysis

Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)

Page 85: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

How to choose behaviorIs it stable?

Does it show a good distribution in your population?

• Trait vs. state• State variables may be better

suited to within-subject analysis

Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)

Page 86: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

How to choose behaviorIs it stable?

Does it show a good distribution in your population?

• Trait vs. state• State variables may be better

suited to within-subject analysis

Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)

Page 87: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

How to choose behaviorIs it stable?

Does it show a good distribution in your population?

• Trait vs. state• State variables may be better

suited to within-subject analysis

Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)

Page 88: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

How to choose behaviorIs it stable?

Does it show a good distribution in your population?

• Trait vs. state• State variables may be better

suited to within-subject analysis

Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)

Page 89: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

How to choose behaviorIs it stable?

Does it show a good distribution in your population?

Is it something you expect to be reflected in brain function?

• Trait vs. state• State variables may be better

suited to within-subject analysis

Betzel et al., Sci Rep (2017)

Page 90: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Behavior: Mitigating confounds

Page 91: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Behavior: Mitigating confoundsMany behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion!

Page 92: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Behavior: Mitigating confoundsMany behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion!

Siegel et al., Cerebral Cortex (2016)

Negatively: Positively:

Page 93: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Behavior: Mitigating confoundsMany behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion!

Siegel et al., Cerebral Cortex (2016)

Negatively: Positively:

Geerligs et al., Hum Brain Mapp (2017)

Age, vascular health:

Page 94: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Behavior: Mitigating confoundsMany behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion!

Page 95: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Behavior: Mitigating confoundsMany behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion!

‣ Check for correlation in your sample

Page 96: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Behavior: Mitigating confoundsMany behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion!

‣ Check for correlation in your sample‣ Consider excluding particularly high-motion subjects

Page 97: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Behavior: Mitigating confoundsMany behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion!

‣ Check for correlation in your sample‣ Consider excluding particularly high-motion subjects‣ Choose appropriate preprocessing techniques

Page 98: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Behavior: Mitigating confoundsMany behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion!

‣ Check for correlation in your sample‣ Consider excluding particularly high-motion subjects‣ Choose appropriate preprocessing techniques

Ciric et al., NeuroImage (2017)

Page 99: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Behavior: Mitigating confoundsMany behaviors/phenotypes are correlated with head motion!

‣ Check for correlation in your sample‣ Consider excluding particularly high-motion subjects‣ Choose appropriate preprocessing techniques‣ Use motion as an explicit covariate

Ciric et al., NeuroImage (2017)

Page 100: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Outline

Introduction:Why study individual differences?

1 How to get good brain data

2

How to relate brain to behavior

4

Learn more5How to get good behavioral data

3

Page 101: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Outline

Introduction:Why study individual differences?

1 How to get good brain data

2

How to relate brain to behavior

4

Learn more5How to get good behavioral data

3

Page 102: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Brain ↔ behavior: Cross-validate!

Page 103: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Brain ↔ behavior: Cross-validate!Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample

Page 104: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Brain ↔ behavior: Cross-validate!Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample

‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset)

Connectome-based Predictive ModelingShen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)

Page 105: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Brain ↔ behavior: Cross-validate!Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample

‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset)

Connectome-based Predictive ModelingShen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)

2

7.5

4

6.3

Page 106: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Brain ↔ behavior: Cross-validate!Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample

‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset)

Connectome-based Predictive ModelingShen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)

2

7.5

4

6.3

Page 107: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Brain ↔ behavior: Cross-validate!Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample

‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset)

Connectome-based Predictive ModelingShen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)

Pred

icte

d be

hav

Observed behav

2

7.5

4

6.3

Page 108: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Brain ↔ behavior: Cross-validate!Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample

‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset)

Connectome-based Predictive ModelingShen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)

Pred

icte

d be

hav

Observed behav

2

7.5

4

6.3

Page 109: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Brain ↔ behavior: Cross-validate!Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample

‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset)

Connectome-based Predictive ModelingShen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)

Pred

icte

d be

hav

Observed behav

2

7.5

4

6.3

Page 110: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Brain ↔ behavior: Cross-validate!Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample

‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset)

Connectome-based Predictive ModelingShen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)

Pred

icte

d be

hav

Observed behav

2

7.5

4

6.3

Page 111: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Brain ↔ behavior: Cross-validate!Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample

‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset)

Connectome-based Predictive ModelingShen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)

Pred

icte

d be

hav

Observed behav

2

7.5

4

6.3

Page 112: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Brain ↔ behavior: Cross-validate!Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample

‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset)

Connectome-based Predictive ModelingShen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)

Pred

icte

d be

hav

Observed behav

2

7.5

4

6.3

Page 113: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Brain ↔ behavior: Cross-validate!Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample

‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset)

Connectome-based Predictive ModelingShen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)

Pred

icte

d be

hav

Observed behav

2

7.5

4

6.3

Page 114: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Brain ↔ behavior: Cross-validate!Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample

‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset)

Connectome-based Predictive ModelingShen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)

Pred

icte

d be

hav

Observed behav

2

7.5

4

6.3

Page 115: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Brain ↔ behavior: Cross-validate!Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample

‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset)

Connectome-based Predictive ModelingShen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)

Pred

icte

d be

hav

Observed behav

2

7.5

4

6.3

Page 116: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Brain ↔ behavior: Cross-validate!Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample

‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset)

‣ even better: cross-dataset

Sustained attention model

Connectome-based Predictive ModelingShen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)

Pred

icte

d be

hav

Observed behav

2

7.5

4

6.3

Rosenberg et al., Nature Neuroscience (2016)Rosenberg et al., Trends in Cog Sci (2017)

Page 117: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Brain ↔ behavior: Cross-validate!Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample

‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset)

‣ even better: cross-dataset

Sustained attention model

Connectome-based Predictive ModelingShen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)

Pred

icte

d be

hav

Observed behav

2

7.5

4

6.3

Rosenberg et al., Nature Neuroscience (2016)Rosenberg et al., Trends in Cog Sci (2017)

Observed behav

Pred

icted

beh

av

n = 25 adults

Page 118: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Brain ↔ behavior: Cross-validate!Find a brain-behavior relationship in one sample, see if it holds in another sample

‣ leave-one-subject-out (within dataset)

‣ even better: cross-dataset

Sustained attention model

Connectome-based Predictive ModelingShen et al., Nature Protocols (2017)

Pred

icte

d be

hav

Observed behav

2

7.5

4

6.3

Rosenberg et al., Nature Neuroscience (2016)Rosenberg et al., Trends in Cog Sci (2017)

Observed behav

Pred

icted

beh

av

n = 25 adults

Observed ADHD score

n = 113 children

Pred

icted

ADH

D sc

ore

Page 119: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop
Page 120: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. What is the best brain state?

Page 121: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. What is the best brain state?A. Maybe it depends on your behavior

Page 122: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. What is the best brain state?A. Maybe it depends on your behavior

Certain task conditions generate better predictions of behavior:

Page 123: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. What is the best brain state?A. Maybe it depends on your behavior

n = 716, 10-fold cross-validationConnectome-based Predictive Modeling (CPM; Shen et al., Nat Protocols 2017)

Mod

el in

put d

ata

Target behavior

Certain task conditions generate better predictions of behavior:

Page 124: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Q. What is the best brain state?A. Maybe it depends on your behavior

n = 716, 10-fold cross-validationConnectome-based Predictive Modeling (CPM; Shen et al., Nat Protocols 2017)

Mod

el in

put d

ata

Target behavior

Certain task conditions generate better predictions of behavior:

Consider tailoring scan condition to behavior of interest

➡“Stress test”?

Page 125: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Take-home points

Introduction:Why study individual differences?

1 How to get good brain data

2 How to get good behavioral data

3

How to relate brain to behavior

4

Learn more5

Page 126: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Take-home points

Introduction:Why study individual differences?

1 How to get good brain data

2 How to get good behavioral data

3

How to relate brain to behavior

4

Learn more5

• Data quality ≠ critical

Page 127: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Take-home points

Introduction:Why study individual differences?

1 How to get good brain data

2 How to get good behavioral data

3

How to relate brain to behavior

4

Learn more5

• Data quality ≠ critical• Longer scans are better!

Page 128: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Take-home points

Introduction:Why study individual differences?

1 How to get good brain data

2 How to get good behavioral data

3

How to relate brain to behavior

4

Learn more5

• Data quality ≠ critical• Longer scans are better!• Consider using tasks

Page 129: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Take-home points

Introduction:Why study individual differences?

1 How to get good brain data

2 How to get good behavioral data

3

How to relate brain to behavior

4

Learn more5

• Data quality ≠ critical• Longer scans are better!• Consider using tasks ‣ improve compliance

Page 130: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Take-home points

Introduction:Why study individual differences?

1 How to get good brain data

2 How to get good behavioral data

3

How to relate brain to behavior

4

Learn more5

• Data quality ≠ critical• Longer scans are better!• Consider using tasks ‣ improve compliance‣ increase sensitivity

Page 131: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Take-home points

Introduction:Why study individual differences?

1 How to get good brain data

2 How to get good behavioral data

3

How to relate brain to behavior

4

Learn more5

• Data quality ≠ critical• Longer scans are better!• Consider using tasks ‣ improve compliance‣ increase sensitivity

• Choose an interesting behavior with a good distribution

Page 132: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Take-home points

Introduction:Why study individual differences?

1 How to get good brain data

2 How to get good behavioral data

3

How to relate brain to behavior

4

Learn more5

• Data quality ≠ critical• Longer scans are better!• Consider using tasks ‣ improve compliance‣ increase sensitivity

• Choose an interesting behavior with a good distribution

• Consider potential confounds and take steps to mitigate them

Page 133: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Take-home points

Introduction:Why study individual differences?

1 How to get good brain data

2 How to get good behavioral data

3

How to relate brain to behavior

4

Learn more5

• Data quality ≠ critical• Longer scans are better!• Consider using tasks ‣ improve compliance‣ increase sensitivity

• Choose an interesting behavior with a good distribution

• Consider potential confounds and take steps to mitigate them

• Cross-validate whenever possible

Page 134: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Take-home points

Introduction:Why study individual differences?

1 How to get good brain data

2 How to get good behavioral data

3

How to relate brain to behavior

4

Learn more5

• Data quality ≠ critical• Longer scans are better!• Consider using tasks ‣ improve compliance‣ increase sensitivity

• Choose an interesting behavior with a good distribution

• Consider potential confounds and take steps to mitigate them

• Cross-validate whenever possible

• Consider tailoring your scan condition to behavior of interest

Page 135: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Take-home points

Introduction:Why study individual differences?

1 How to get good brain data

2 How to get good behavioral data

3

How to relate brain to behavior

4

Learn more5

• Data quality ≠ critical• Longer scans are better!• Consider using tasks ‣ improve compliance‣ increase sensitivity

• Choose an interesting behavior with a good distribution

• Consider potential confounds and take steps to mitigate them

• Cross-validate whenever possible

• Consider tailoring your scan condition to behavior of interest

Page 136: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Open data sets with brain & behavior

➡ Use these on their own or in combination with your own data to generate or test hypotheses, see if a finding generalizes, etc

Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort

Page 137: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Further reading

Building a science of individual differences from fMRIDubois & Adolphs, Trends in Cognitive Sciences (2016)

From regions to connections and networks: new bridges between brain and behaviorMisic & Sporns, Current Opinion in Neurobiology (2016)

Can brain state be manipulated to emphasize individual differences in functional connectivity?Finn et al., NeuroImage (2017)

Prediction as a humanitarian and pragmatic contribution from human cognitive neuroscienceGabrieli, Ghosh & Gabrieli, Neuron (2015)

Selected reviews:

Page 138: Finn ohbm2017 ed_workshop

Learn more at OHBM 2017

Symposium: Collect your thoughts: Individual differences in the networks underlying intelligenceTues 8:00-9:15am

Symposium:Relating connectivity to inter- and intra-individual differences in attention and cognitionWeds 8:00-9:15am

Poster #4042: Can brain state be manipulated to emphasize individual differences in functional connectivity? Finn et al.

Poster #4040: Large-scale functional connectivity networks predict attention fluctuations Rosenberg et al.

Poster #2110: Functional connectivity-based predictors of naturalistic reading comprehension Jangraw et al.

Poster #4029: FMRI connectivity is differentially associated with performance across tasks in a multi-task study Topolski et al.

@esfinn