Eysenck Personality to Communicate

download Eysenck Personality to Communicate

of 48

Transcript of Eysenck Personality to Communicate

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    1/48

    The Effects of Personality Traits on General Disclosiveness

    Aaron D. Johnson

    Thesis submitted to the Eberly College of Arts and Sciences at West Virginia University

    in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of

    Masters of Arts

    inCommunication Studies

    James C. McCroskey, Ed.D., Chair

    Matthew M. Martin, Ph.D.

    Robert A. Barraclough, Ed.D.

    Department of Communication Studies

    Morgantown, West Virginia

    2001

    Keywords: Disclosiveness, Communibiology, and Temperament.

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    2/48

    ABSTRACT

    The Effects of Personality on General Disclosiveness

    Aaron D. Johnson

    The purpose of this study was to examine the potential relationship between anindividuals temperament and her/his general disclosiveness. This study utilized

    Eysencks model of temperament and the Five Factor model of personality to predict an

    individuals level of general disclosiveness and the sub-components of disclosiveness.The results indicated that both models were relatively equal predictors of general

    disclosiveness. Both models were moderately strong predictors of the valence and

    honesty dimensions of an individuals disclosiveness. A moderately strong negativecorrelation between neuroticism and the valence dimension of disclosiveness was

    established. Finally, psychoticism and three factors (openness, agreeableness, and

    conscientiousness) of the Five Factor model were not found to predict the exact same

    levels of an individuals disclosiveness.

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    3/48

    iii

    ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

    I would like to thank the following individuals. Dr. James C. McCroskey for

    patiently working with me. Dr. Matthew M. Martin and Dr. Robert A. Barraclough for

    keeping me on my toes and serving time on my committee. I appreciate the help that all

    of you have given me.

    I would also like to thank my parents, sister, and brother-in-law for continually

    encouraging and supporting me.

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    4/48

    iv

    TABLE OF CONTENTS

    ABSTRACT ii

    ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

    CHAPTER 1 1INTRODUCTION

    General Disclosiveness

    Eysencks Model of TemperamentFive Factor Model of Personality

    Hypotheses and Research Questions

    CHAPTER 2 14

    METHODS

    Participants

    ProcedureInstruments

    CHAPTER 3 17RESULTS

    Hypotheses

    Research Questions

    CHAPTER 4 22

    DISCUSSION

    REFERENCES 28

    TABLE 1 32

    TABLE 2 33

    TABLE 3 34

    TABLE 4 35

    TABLE 5 36

    TABLE 6 37

    TABLE 7 38

    TABLE 8 39

    APPENDIX 40

    iii

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    5/48

    1

    Chapter 1

    The Effects of Personality Traits on Self-Disclosiveness

    According to Wigley (1995) the knowledge of traits might help one to

    understand the behaviors of individuals interacting with the persons who own the traits

    (p. 350). A more generalized knowledge of human communication may potentially be

    revealed through the study of biological origins (Horvath, 1998). Phillips and Matheny

    (1997) stated, individual differences in responding to specific situations, usually

    assumed to be environmental effects, are influenced by genetic factors (p. 135).

    According to Eysenck (1986) the evidence from many different investigators in the

    genetics of personality is quite clear-cut; genetic factors are more important than

    environmental factors (p. 16). Working from these ideas and more, Beatty and

    McCroskey (1997) introduced the Communibiological Model. So far, most research

    conducted under this model appears to have focused on examining the traits of

    communication apprehension (Beatty, McCroskey, & Heisel, 1998; Neuliep, Chadouir,

    McCroskey, & Heisel, 2000) and verbal aggressiveness (Valencic, Beatty, Rudd, Dobos,

    & Heisel, 1998). However, the construct of general disclosiveness appears to be

    untouched by researchers using the Communibiological Model.

    Evidence for a temperamental trait perspective of human communication can be

    found in twin studies (Buss & Plomin, 1984; Horvath, 1998; Phillips & Matheny, 1997).

    According to Buss and Plomin (1984) emotionality, activity level, and sociability are

    stable enduring traits of an inherited genetic component. Buss and Plomin divided

    emotionality into expressions, feelings, and arousal; but focused on how an individuals

    state of emotional arousal differs from her/his baseline arousal. Emotionality appears to

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    6/48

    2

    be related to Eysencks concept of neuroticism (Buss & Plomin, 1984). Sociability is a

    component of extroversion (Eysenck, 1986; Buss & Plomin, 1984; Weaver, 1998).

    According to Buss and Plomin sociability is the tendency to prefer the presence of

    others to being alone (p. 63). Intrinsic rewards of social interaction (sociability) include:

    presence, attention, shared activities, responsivity, and stimulation.

    Horvath (1995) examined the biological origins of communicator style using a

    twin design. She found that genetics accounts for about 74% of the variance in

    sociability and 78% of the variance for the open subscale of communicator style. From

    these results, one could speculate that an individuals level of general disclosiveness is

    genetically derived. In order for an individual to communicate in a disclosive manner,

    he/she must be somewhat open.

    In order for an individual to be open and disclosive, the individual must first

    initiate communication. According to McCroskey and Richmond (1998) the WTC

    [willingness to communicate] trait is an individuals predisposition to initiate

    communication with others (p. 120). Some individuals believe that human

    communication centers on the willingness to communicate trait (McCroskey &

    Richmond, 1998). Wigley (1995) examined the relationship between an individuals

    willingness to communicate and her/his general disclosiveness and found a moderate

    positive relationship. An established moderate relationship indicates that willingness to

    communicate and general disclosiveness are two separate constructs. A measure of ones

    general disclosiveness would very likely fail to account for the amount of phatic

    communication occurring during an interaction. General disclosiveness appears as

    though it may be more of a second order factor to the willingness to communicate

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    7/48

    3

    construct. The purpose of this study was to examine the potential relationship between

    ones temperament and ones general disclosiveness.

    General Disclosiveness

    In order for dyadic parties to build on an initial interaction, they either must reveal

    or expose components of each of their selves to one another through self-disclosure.

    According to Wheeless and Grotz (1976) a self-disclosure is any message about the self

    that a person communicates to another (p. 338). Wheeless and Grotz (1976) stated that

    the perception of the self-disclosive messages by the individuals influence the degree of

    self-disclosure taking place. The amount of self-disclosure in which any individual

    engages may be dependent upon the individuals predisposition to disclose or the

    environment in which the interaction takes place. According to Wheeless (1978) self-

    disclosure can be conceptualized at two levels: as a general disclosiveness or openness to

    other people (Wheeless, 1976) or as a communication phenomenon occurring between

    specific individuals (Wheeless, 1978; Wheeless & Grotz, 1976). The former refers to an

    individuals trait whereas the latter refers to a culturally bound communication episode.

    Situational self-disclosure has been previously researched with a variety of

    variables such as interpersonal solidarity (Wheeless, 1976; 1978), interpersonal trust

    (Wheeless & Grotz, 1977; Wheeless, 1978), perceived understanding (Martin, Anderson,

    & Mottet, 1999), interpersonal motives (Martin & Anderson, 1995), and communication

    apprehension (Wheeless, Nesser, & McCroskey, 1986). However, trait disclosiveness

    has not been researched as frequently. Wheeless et al. (1986) examined the relationship

    between general disclosiveness and communication apprehension and found a negative

    relationship between these variables. Wigley (1995) explored the relationship between

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    8/48

    4

    an individuals general disclosiveness and her/his likelihood of being selected as a

    member of a jury panel. Wigley (1995) found that individuals reporting high amounts of

    disclosiveness are more likely to be selected as members of a jury. Also, empanelled

    jurors seemed to be predisposed to report more positive disclosiveness than unselected

    potential jurors (Wigley, 1995).

    Research on both situation-based disclosures and trait disclosiveness, has used a

    multi-dimensional scale to assess the construct. According to Wheeless and Grotz (1976)

    the construct of self-disclosure is composed of five sub-dimensions: amount of

    disclosure, intended disclosure, valence of disclosure, control of depth of disclosure, and

    honesty-accuracy of disclosure. Some of these dimensions were derived from other

    researchers (Altman & Taylor, 1983). For example, Altman and Taylor (1983) described

    breadth in terms the number of categories revealed and also the time individuals spent

    interacting. Wheeless and Grotz (1976) used this description of breadth to develop their

    self-disclosive sub-dimension of amount of disclosure. According to Wheeless and Grotz

    the amount of disclosure is concerned with both the frequency and the duration of the

    disclosive messages or message units (p. 338). The sub-dimension of intent deals with

    an individuals conscious decision and willingness to reveal information about her/him

    self. The valence sub-dimension of self-disclosure is the level of positive or negative

    information disclosed, as it is perceived by either participant involved in the interaction.

    According to Wheeless and Grotz the control of depth sub-dimension of self-disclosure is

    a function of the self-perceived intimacy of the information topic revealed (p. 338).

    The fifth and final sub-dimension of self-disclosure, ones honesty-accuracy of

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    9/48

    5

    disclosure, is concerned with the individuals precision of her/his self-perceptions and

    her/his subsequent ability to verbalize those perceptions in a sincere manner.

    Eysencks Model of Temperament

    A large portion of the Communibiological Model is based on the work of Hans

    Eysenck (Beatty et al., 1998). Eysenck devised a model of temperament that is primarily

    based on individuals physiological differences. Although Eysenck (1967) considered

    learned habits to be important, he believed personality differences to be primarily

    governed by ones genetic heritage. Ones tools to interpret and react upon his/her

    environment are genetically limited. The interpretation of any stimuli seems to involve

    some type of physiological arousal. The level of physiological arousal that is stimulated

    is limited to ones genetically based physiological thresholds. According to Eysenck

    (1967) an individuals behaviors are limited to the interpretation of the stimuli by which

    he/she is physiological aroused. Individuals that are easily aroused, have relatively weak

    physiological thresholds, whereas those individuals who are not very easily aroused tend

    to have strong physiological thresholds. Eysenck (1967) found three dimensions of an

    individuals temperament based on the arousal level sensed through ones physiological

    thresholds. These dimensions are neuroticism, extroversion-introversion, and

    psychoticism. These dimensions have demonstrated consistency over time and

    generalizability across cultures (Eysenck, 1986). According to Eysenck (1986) these

    dimensions represent general ways in which people interact. These dimensions might be

    seen as the primary influences on communication (Weaver, 1998) and thus are very

    relevant to interpersonal communication theory (Beatty et al., 1998).

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    10/48

    6

    Neuroticism is ones level of emotional stability (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985). An

    individual who is highly neurotic tends to be more susceptible to emotional instabilities

    and nervous behaviors. According to Eysenck (1967) some people have a nervous

    system that is more responsive to stimuli. Lower physiological thresholds lead to an

    increase in sensation to various stimuli. A highly responsive nervous system is more

    sensitive to various stimuli due to these low physiological thresholds. An increased level

    of physiological responsiveness will influence ones emotional and behavioral reaction to

    a perceived stressor.

    An individual high in neuroticism might attempt to avoid stimulating social

    situations in order to maintain some level of comfort. According to Eysenck and

    Eysenck (1985) there is a negative relationship between social intimacy and neuroticism.

    One might suspect that an individual high in neuroticism would be socially apprehensive.

    Neuliep et al. (2000) found a positive relationship between neuroticism and

    communication apprehension (a fear or anxiety associated with a real or anticipated

    communication episode with another). The primary physiological link to anxiety

    (neuroticism) appears to be the behavioral inhibition system (Strelau, 1983). According

    to Beatty and McCroskey (2001) the behavioral inhibition system plays a role in

    avoidance behavior. An overactive behavioral inhibition system tends to cause an

    individual to be anxiety prone (Beatty et al., 1998; Valencic et al., 1998). According to

    Eysenck and Eysenck (1985) since neurotic people possess a labile and overactive

    autonomic nervous system, they are susceptible to fear and anxiety (p. 315).

    The dimension of extroversion is concerned with ones drive to seek sensation

    (Eysenck, 1986). An introverted person is one who is considered to be shy or quiet,

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    11/48

    7

    whereas an extroverted person is one who is considered to be very out-going. According

    to Eysenck (1967) an individuals level of extroversion is based on that individuals brain

    attempting to balance her/his inhibition and excitation. Excitation is the brain waking

    itself up to its environment or becoming very sensitive and alert. Inhibition is the brain

    calming itself down or relaxing its senses to the stimulation of its environment.

    According to Eysenck (1967) a highly extroverted individual has a strong inhibition

    towards sensing information from his/her environment, whereas a highly introverted

    individual has a weak inhibition towards sensing information from his/her environment.

    A highly extroverted persons inhibition is due to a low level of sensitivity to stimuli

    stemming from his/her high physiological thresholds. Therefore, an extroverted person is

    outgoing (seeks sensation) in order to maintain a balanced level of excitation in his/her

    brain. According to Eysenck and Eysenck (1985) extroverts may seek out personal

    contacts in order to prevent the level of arousal from becoming too low (p. 313). A

    highly introverted person is extremely sensitive to stimuli because of his/her low

    physiological thresholds. Therefore, in order for an introvert to maintain a balanced level

    of excitation in his/her brain, he/she will very likely shy away from stimuli.

    According to Eysenck (1986) an extroverted individual is very likely to be

    sociable, active, assertive, and lively. An introverted individual would display behaviors

    that oppose these descriptions. According to Eysenck and Eysenck (1985) introverts

    will show a greater tendency than extroverts to reduce interpersonal intimacy (p. 314).

    Neuliep et al. (2000) found a negative relationship between extroversion and

    communication apprehension. Extroverts tend to demonstrate more immediacy

    nonverbally than introverts (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985). The primary physiological link

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    12/48

    8

    to the extroversion-introversion dimension appears to be the level of cortical arousal.

    Eysenck and Eysenck stated that extroverts have chronically lower levels of cortical

    arousal than introverts. Cortical arousal levels appear to be manipulated by the

    behavioral approach system. According to Beatty and McCroskey (2001) the behavioral

    approach system serves to energize goal-directed behaviorto acquiring rewards orto

    reduce or avert punishment (p. 98).

    A concept previously mentioned is that of sensation seeking. The concept of

    sensation seeking was designed to assess individual differences in response to sensory

    deprivation (Zuckerman, 1985). According to Zuckerman (1985) sensation seeking has

    something to do with the biological trait of arousability in response to stimuli of moderate

    to high intensities (p. 103). An individuals level of sensation seeking may be related to

    the individuals level of extroversion. Zuckerman reported a significant positive

    correlation between extroversion and sensation seeking. Perhaps these two constructs

    share similar biological bases. An individuals level of cortical arousal is related to

    her/his level of sensation seeking.

    The third dimension is psychoticism. Psychoticism is the ability for individuals to

    think, feel, and reason in the realities of their everyday world. According to Eysenck

    (1967) a person who is high in psychoticism is more likely to exhibit reckless behavior,

    disregard common sense, and/or inappropriately express him/her self emotionally. A

    person who is high in psychoticism is not necessarily a psychotic. Instead, this person is

    just more susceptible to certain behaviors deemed to be psychotic given certain stimuli.

    An individual who is high in psychoticism may lack empathy, be very egocentric, and

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    13/48

    9

    may act very impulsively, whereas an individual low in psychoticism is more likely to

    behave in a very moral manner (Eysenck, 1986).

    Eysenck and Eysenck (1985) stated that those who engage in antisocial behavior,

    especially criminals, tend to score high in psychoticism. According to Beatty and

    McCroskey (2001) the antisocial orientation of psychoticism should spark interest in

    many communication scholars. Valencic et al. (1998) found a positive relationship

    between verbal aggressiveness and psychoticism. The primary physiological link to

    psychoticism appears to be the fight or flight system (Valencic et al., 1998). According

    to Beatty and McCroskey (2001) the fight or flight system is the neurobiological basis of

    active avoidance and aggression.

    Five Factor Model of Personality

    Not all researchers feel as though Eysencks Model of Temperament is the best

    model to utilize when exploring the interaction of personality traits and communication

    traits. Some people feel the current knowledge of the biological mechanisms of the brain

    is still quite primitive (McCrae & John, 1992). According to McCrae and John (1992)

    the five-factor model is more appealing than other models because overall, it is more

    comprehensive. A problem with Eysencks model is that it collapses agreeableness and

    conscientiousness in its conceptualization of psychoticism. Without the examination of

    all five factors the most relevant traits may be overlooked (McCrae & John, 1992).

    McCrae and Costa (1989) believed that this model is useful for the exploration of

    interpersonal behavior. A broad definition of interpersonal behavior might include social

    interaction or communication. McCrae and John (1992) suggest the use of this model to

    clarify or examine issues in fields related to psychology. According to McCrae and John

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    14/48

    10

    (1992) anywhere personality assessment has been employed may benefit from a

    consideration of the five-factor model (p. 206). Some communication scholars have

    examined the personalitys influence on communication.

    The Five-Factor Model of Personality contains five traits: extroversion,

    neuroticism, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness (McAdams,

    1992; McCrae, 1996; McCrae & Costa, 1989; McCrae & John, 1992; Miller, 1990). The

    traits of extroversion and neuroticism were adapted from other personality scholars like

    Eysenck (McCrae & John, 1992). Openness to experience, agreeableness, and

    conscientiousness were added to fulfill the aim of a comprehensive measure of

    personality (McCrae, 1996; McCrae & John, 1992). An individual high in agreeableness

    can be characterized as altruistic, nurturing, caring, and supportive; whereas someone low

    on this trait would be indifferent toward others, self-centered, and spiteful (McCrae &

    John, 1992). Conscientiousness is a trait that organizes and directs behavior as well as

    one that keeps impulsive behavior in check (McCrae & John, 1992). An individual high

    in openness does not restrain her/him self from exploration of feelings, sensations, and

    values; instead he/she tends to be curious, original, and generally holds a broad range of

    interests (McCrae & John, 1992).

    However comprehensive the five-factor model may be, it is not without its critics.

    Eysencks (1986) model of temperament is grounded in biological mechanisms.

    Biological mechanisms are the basic ingredients of all human existence. Certainly, they

    are the basis of all communication. Human communication begins and ends with basic

    biological mechanisms. These biological mechanisms allow humans to sense their

    environment in the form of sound, sight, touch, taste, and smell. The inability to sense

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    15/48

    11

    the environment in at least one of these five forms makes human communication

    impossible. According to McAdams (1992) The Big Five are in no way akin to the

    basic elements of personality[the] basic ingredientsof personality (p. 339).

    Instead, the Big Five make nice surface level characterizations of behaviors (McAdams,

    1992). Eysencks (1986) explanations of behavior are deeper and provide some degree of

    causality. According to McAdams (1992) For Eysenckwhat provides ultimate

    legitimacy[is his] biological underpinning (p. 341).

    Development of Hypotheses and Research Questions

    Eysenck and Eysenck (1985) stated that extroverts have chronically lower levels

    of cortical arousal. Extroverts can be described as individuals who are sociable (Weaver,

    1998). An individual who is more sociable or extroverted is one who is more likely to

    initiate an interaction with another. According to McCroskey and Richmond (1998) an

    individual who displays a high willingness to communicate is more likely to initiate

    communication. Wigley (1995) found a positive relationship between an individuals

    willingness to communicate and her/his disclosiveness. Wheeless et al (1986) found a

    negative relationship between disclosiveness and communication apprehension. Hence,

    the following hypothesis was advanced:

    H1: Extroversion will be positively correlated with general disclosiveness.

    Eysenck and Eysenck (1985) stated that individuals high in neuroticism tended to

    experience interpersonal intimacy negatively. Intimacy can be described as a close

    association between individuals. In order to become close or intimate, individuals often

    need to share or disclose information to one another. Hence, the following hypothesis

    was advanced:

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    16/48

    12

    H2: Neuroticism will be negatively correlated with general disclosiveness.

    According to Eysenck (1986) individuals high is psychoticism tend to be

    aggressive, cold, antisocial, impulsive, and unempathetic. Individuals who measure high

    in psychoticism are very likely to communicate in an uncontrolled manner. Valencic et

    al. (1998) found a positive relationship between verbal aggressiveness and psychoticism.

    While one could speculate that an individual who measures high on psychoticism is not

    going to disclose friendly warm fuzzies; it is unclear whether or not this individual

    would disclose some other type of message. Also, Eysenck and Eysenck (1985) stated

    that adequate measures of psychoticism have become available only in recent years. Due

    to the uncertainty, the following nondirectional hypothesis was advanced:

    H3: Psychoticism will be correlated with general disclosiveness.

    According to Beatty and McCroskey (2000a) individual differences in the

    thresholds of the neurobiological systems responsible for the cluster of behaviors and

    feelings commonly referred to as introversion (I) or neuroticism (N) are inherited (p.

    25). Each individual inherits her/his unique combination of personalized genetic

    components. Therefore, three possible combinations of Eysencks super traits may

    produce unique levels of general disclosiveness. Beatty et al. (1998) argued that an

    individual who is high in communication apprehension tends to engage in the avoidance

    tendencies that represent a neurotic introvert. According to Beatty and McCroskey

    (2000b) multiple neurobiological systems influence complex human behaviors. Based on

    this ideology, the following research question was advanced:

    RQ1: Will general disclosiveness be correlated with a combination of the

    components of Eysencks model and/or the Five Factor model?

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    17/48

    13

    Wheeless (1978) validated a measure of general disclosiveness that measures five

    subcomponents of disclosiveness. The five subcomponents are: intent to disclose,

    amount of disclosure, valence of disclosure, honesty-accuracy of disclosure, and control

    of depth of the disclosure. Eysenck (1986) described an individual low in psychoticism

    to be unimpulsive and in control. Perhaps, there is a relationship between psychoticism

    and the general disclosiveness component of control. Based on this reasoning, the

    following research question was advanced:

    RQ2: To what extent can Eysencks model and/or the Five Factor model predict

    the subcomponents of disclosiveness (intent, amount, valence, control of depth, and

    honesty-accuracy)?

    Previously, it had been mentioned that adequate measures of psychoticism have

    become available only in recent years (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985). The Five Factor

    Model of temperament (McCrae & Costa, 1985) includes three dimensions (openness,

    agreeableness, conscientiousness) believed to be secondary factors of psychoticism

    (Eysenck, 1986). In order to better identify the elements of psychoticism potentially

    related to general disclosiveness, the following research questions were advanced:

    RQ3: To what extent are correlations of psychoticism with disclosiveness similar

    to correlations of openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness with disclosiveness?

    RQ4: To what extent do openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness predict

    psychoticism?

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    18/48

    14

    Chapter 2

    Method

    Participants

    The participants for this study were 399 undergraduate students enrolled in

    introductory communication courses at a Middle Atlantic University. The average age of

    this sample was 19.9 with a range of 18-41. There were 202 males and 188 females with

    9 non-reports. Additionally, there were 195 freshman, 78 sophomores, 69 juniors, and 48

    seniors with 9 non-reports in this sample.

    Procedure

    During a normal class period, students were asked to complete a questionnaire

    consisting of Eysenck and Eysencks (1985) extroversion, neuroticism, and psychoticism

    questionnaires, Wheelesss (1978) General Disclosiveness Scale, a measure based on the

    Five Factor Model of Temperament (McCrae & Costa, 1985), and some demographic

    questions (sex, age, year in school). The students were asked to fill out the

    questionnaires as they pertained to themselves. The students were requested to omit their

    names or any other identity revealing information from the questionnaire in order to

    guarantee anonymity. Participation was voluntary and could be one way in which the

    students could receive extra credit.

    Instruments

    The main concern of this study was bridging a connection of ones general

    disclosiveness to ones temperament. General disclosiveness was measured by using

    Wheeless (1978) General Disclosiveness Scale (see appendix). The General

    Disclosiveness Scale is a 5-dimension, 31-item Likert-type scale. Wheeless previously

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    19/48

    15

    reported the following reliabilities on each dimension as follows: intent = .85, amount =

    .88, valence = .91, depth = .84, and honesty = .87. The current investigation yielded the

    following reliabilities for the dimensions of disclosiveness: intent = .70, amount = .77,

    valence = .83, depth = .80, and honesty = .80. Reliabilities for the General

    Disclosiveness Scale have ranged from .65 to .90 (Wheeless, 1978). The current

    investigation revealed a reliability of .82 for the general disclosiveness scale.

    In order to measure temperament, participants were asked to complete Eysenck

    and Eysencks (1985) extroversion, neuroticism, and psychoticism questionnaires (see

    appendix). Eysenck and Eysencks (1985) extroversion, neuroticism, and psychoticism

    questionnaires include a total of 32 Likert-type items. In a series of studies, Neuliep et al.

    (2000) reported the following reliabilities: extroversion ranged from .73 to .80,

    neuroticism ranged from .75 to .86, and psychoticism ranged from .61 to .68. The current

    investigation produced the following reliabilities for the Eysenck and Eysenck (1985)

    questionnaires: extroversion = .77, neuroticism = .81, and psychoticism = .67.

    In order to explore research questions three and four, participants were asked to

    complete a measure based on McCrae and Costas (1985) Five Factor Model of

    temperament (see appendix). The Five Factor Model based measure is a series of bi-

    polar scales. Neuliep et al. (2000) reported the following reliabilities: extroversion = .80,

    neuroticism = .87, openness = .71, agreeableness = .80, and conscientiousness = .80. In

    the current investigation, the measure based on the five factor model yielded the

    following reliabilities: extroversion = .83, neuroticism, = .85, openness = .72,

    agreeableness = .82, and conscientiousness = .79.

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    20/48

    16

    Simple and multiple correlation procedures were used to explore the hypotheses

    and research questions of the current study. A p-value of .05 was set as a criterion to

    determine significance.

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    21/48

    17

    Chapter 3

    Results

    Hypotheses

    The first hypothesis argued that there would be a significant positive correlation

    between extroversion and general disclosiveness. Correlations between general

    disclosiveness scores and scores on Eysencks Model of Temperament obtained from this

    study are reported in Table 1. Correlations between general disclosiveness scores and

    scores on the measurement based on the Five Factor Model obtained from this study are

    reported in Table 2. Results of the simple correlation analyses supported this hypothesis.

    Significant positive correlations between general disclosiveness and extroversion were

    obtained from both Eysencks Model of Temperament (r = .29, p < .001) and the Five

    Factor Model (r = .37, p < .001).

    The second hypothesis argued that there would be a significant negative

    correlation between neuroticism and general disclosiveness. Results of the simple

    correlation analysis supported this hypothesis. Significant negative correlations between

    general disclosiveness and neuroticism were obtained from both Eysencks Model of

    Temperament (r = -.29, p < .001) and neuroticism from the Five Factor Model (r = -.26, p

    < .001).

    The third hypothesis argued that there would be a significant correlation between

    psychoticism and general disclosiveness, but did not predict the direction of this

    correlation. Results of the simple correlation analysis revealed a significant negative

    correlation between psychoticism and general disclosiveness (r = -.23, p < .001).

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    22/48

    18

    Research Questions

    The first research question addressed the possibility of general disclosiveness

    being correlated to a combination of extroversion, neuroticism and psychoticism. A

    significant multiple correlation coefficient of R = .42 was obtained [F(3, 399) = 28.42, p

    < .001]. The results of the multiple correlation between general disclosiveness and the

    combination of extroversion, neuroticism, and psychoticism are presented in Table 5.

    The results of the multiple correlation between general disclosiveness and the

    combination of extroversion, neuroticism, openness, agreeableness, and

    conscientiousness (from the Five Factor model) are presented in Table 6. A significant

    multiple correlation coefficient of R = .45 was obtained [F(5, 399) = 19.84, p < .001].

    The second research question explored what subcomponents of disclosiveness

    would be correlated with the dimensions of Eysencks Model of Temperament and the

    dimensions of the Five Factor Model. Correlations between the five dimensions of

    disclosiveness scores and scores on Eysencks Model of Temperament obtained from this

    study are reported in Table 3. Correlations between the five dimensions of disclosiveness

    scores and scores on the measurement based on the Five Factor Model obtained from this

    study are reported in Table 4. The correlations found to be .30 or better are listed below.

    Simple correlation analyses indicated that the disclosive dimension of valence was

    significantly negatively correlated to neuroticism (r = -.41, p < .001) of Eysencks model

    and significantly negatively correlated to neuroticism (r = -.41, p < .001) of the Five

    Factor model. The disclosive dimension of honesty was significantly negatively

    correlated to neuroticism (r = -.33, p < .001) of Eysencks model, significantly negatively

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    23/48

    19

    correlated to neuroticism (r = -.30, p < .001) and significantly positively correlated to

    conscientiousness (r = .37, p < .001) of the Five Factor model.

    In order to explore what of the five dimensions of disclosiveness would be

    correlated to a combination of the dimensions of Eysencks Model of Temperament and

    the dimensions of the Five Factor Model, a multiple correlation analysis was utilized.

    Multiple correlation coefficients between the five dimensions of disclosiveness and

    combinations of the dimensions of Eysencks Model of Temperament are reported in

    Table 5. Multiple correlation coefficients between the five dimensions of disclosiveness

    and combinations of the dimensions of the Five Factor Model are reported in Table 6.

    The two strongest predictors are listed below. A significant multiple correlation

    of R = .46 was obtained [F(3, 399) = 35.08, p < .001] between valence and a combination

    of the dimensions of Eysencks Model of Temperament. A significant multiple

    correlation of R = .42 was obtained [F(3, 399) = 27.78, p < .001] between honesty and a

    combination of the dimensions of Eysencks Model of Temperament. A significant

    multiple correlation of R = .48 was obtained [F(5, 399) = 23.28, p < .001] between

    valence and a combination of the dimensions of the Five Factor Model. A significant

    multiple correlation of R = .47 was obtained [F(5, 399) = 22.14, p < .001] between

    honesty and a combination of the dimensions of the Five Factor Model.

    The extroversion, neuroticism, and psychoticism measures have been linked to the

    behavioral activation system, the behavioral inhibition system, and the fight or flight

    system respectively (Beatty & McCroskey, 2001). However, each of these measures are

    not perfect estimates of these supertraits. The magnitude of the correlation coefficient

    between disclosiveness and a combination of the dimensions of Eysencks model (r =

  • 8/2/2019 Eysenck Personality to Communicate

    24/48

    20

    .42) is modest and underscores the potential predictive power of the communibiological

    model until it is corrected for attenuation ((R) = .53). The same observation occurs with

    the correlation coefficient between disclosiveness and a combination of the dimensions of

    the Five Factor model that is (R = .45) before correction for attenuation and ((R) = .55)

    after. Tables 5 and 6 also report all of the attenuated and disattenuated correlation

    coefficients for the moderated (e.g. extroversion in conjunction with neuroticism) and

    unmoderated variables.

    The third research question inquired as to whether the correlation of psychoticism

    with disclosiveness was similar to the multiple correlation of openness, agreeableness,

    and conscientiousness with disclosiveness. The results previously listed indicate that

    psychoticism is significantly negatively correlated to general disclosiveness (r = -.23, p