Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

96
Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci

Transcript of Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Page 1: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Experimenting with Experimental Brain

Science

Dr. Michael Raucci

Page 2: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 3: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Education is not the filling of a pail, but the lighting of a fire. William Butler Yeats

Page 4: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

The Art of Teaching

The Science of Teaching

Page 5: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Artist

Philosopher

Scientist

Technician

Teacher

Entertainer

Page 6: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Teacher Duties

Page 7: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Goals Today

1) Not to teach you anything

• The idea today is to allow guided exploration result in self realization and illumination leading to discussion.

Page 8: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

2) We are going to act like really lousy scientists

For fun I want to, when ever possible, extrapolate what we discover, to what this might mean if it the process we were examining worked similarly for other parts of the brain.

Page 10: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

"I'll believe it when I see it with my own two eyes”

We need to realize that that's not enough.

Page 11: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Waterfall

by M.C. Escher

What is Reality and What is Perception?

Is there such a thing as reality?

Page 12: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

The M.C. Escher painting exploits the rules of depth and proximity and our understanding of the physical world to create an illusion.

Page 13: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• Is there perception without consciousness?• Is there anything without perception• If a tree falls in the woods and there is no one

and nothing to record its happening does it make a sound?

• Has it even fallen?• Are there stimuli that we can’t perceive?

Page 14: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Seeing things through the eyes of N Evolutionary Biologist

Page 16: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 17: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 18: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Brightness depends on

context

Page 19: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• Luminance in a physical measure the luminosity of A and B is identical, Brightness is something your brain constructs. B is brighter.

• Lateral inhibition is the ability of a neuron to suppress the output of its neighbor so that only the most stimulated form our perception increasing sharpness and contrast

Page 20: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 22: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• As with many so-called illusions, this effect really demonstrates the success rather than the failure of the visual system. The visual system is not very good at being a physical light meter, but that is not its purpose. The important task is to break the image information down into meaningful components, and thereby perceive the nature of the objects in view.

Page 23: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• Similarly, the eye will compensate for colour contrast depending on the colour cast of the surrounding area.

Page 24: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Hermann Grid Illusion

Page 25: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• Hermann grid illusion • The illusion is characterized by "ghostlike"

grey blobs perceived at the intersections of a white (or light-colored) grid on a black background. The grey blobs disappear when looking directly at an intersection.

Page 26: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Scintillating Grid Illusion

Page 27: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• In this example of lateral inhibition, again the intensity at a point in the visual system is the result of not one receptor but multiple.

• Staring at an intersection defeats the illusion at that intersection

Page 28: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

OUCHI IllUSION - apparent motion

Page 29: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• brain of excessive stimulation of a specific type - brightness, tilt, color, movement, etc. The theory is that stimuli have individual dedicated neural paths in the early stages of visual processing, and that repetitive stimulation of only one or a few channels causes a physiological imbalance that alters perception.

Page 30: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 31: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Pop Out - often avoided by animal populations

Page 32: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• There is no light source• Light could be coming from the top or the

bottom• Your brain makes a choice. It’s a reasonable

choice based upon evolutionary assumptions• What is it? Why?

Page 33: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 34: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 35: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Floor tiles at the Basilica of St. John Lateran in Rome

Page 36: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

It's a Lufthansa 747-400 and a United Airlines 757-200 that were on simultaneous approaches to runways 28L and 28R at San Francisco (SFO). The separation requirement for flying parallel and simultaneous approaches is 225 meters (738 feet). These two aircraft are at a safe distance for the approaches they are each flying. Due to the Lufthansa 747 being three times larger than the 757 and being slightly behind, gives us this illusion.

Page 37: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 38: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 40: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Lines and Horizons

Page 41: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 42: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 44: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• This illusion demonstrates the effect of some simple image processing occurring at the retina combined with some complex processing in the cortical cells of the striate cortex. The incoming image is first filtered by the centre-surround operator of the retina. The apparent tilt of the mortar lines is caused by orientation-sensitive simple cells in the striate cortex. The cells interact with one another to interpret the diagonal bands produced by the retina as a single continuous line, tilted in the direction of the diagonal bands.

Page 45: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 46: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 47: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• The imaginary white triangle is one instance in which brain fills in some logical ingredients to complete the picture

• Clouds – spotting recognizable shapes in clouds • Jesus grilled –cheese sandwiches• Fashion- the goal of some fashions is by

concealing the exact shapes of the wearers body they give ample room for the brain to imagine the idealized representation it expects

Page 48: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• The brain has a need to see familiar simple objects and has a tendency to create a "whole" image from individual elements…Gestalt

• Another explanation of the is based in evolutionary psychology and the fact that in order to survive it was important to see form and edges. The use of perceptual organization to create meaning out of stimuli is the principle behind other well-known illusions including impossible objects.

• Our brain makes sense of shapes and symbols putting them together like a jigsaw puzzle, formulating that which isn't there to that which is believable.

Page 49: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Filling In

Page 50: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 51: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 52: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• One common fallacy is to assume there is an image inside your eyeball, the optical image, exciting photoreceptors on your retina and then that image is transmitted faithfully along a cable called the optic nerve and displayed on a screen called the visual cortex. Now this is obviously a logical fallacy because if you have a screen and an image displayed on a screen in the brain, then you need another little chap in there watching that image, and there is no little chap in your head. And if you think about it, that wouldn't solve the problem either because then you'd need another little guy in his head looking at the image in his brain …….

Page 53: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• So the first thing you have to do to understand perception is to get rid of the idea of images in the brain and think instead of transforms or symbolic representations of objects and events in the external world. Just as little squiggles of ink, print or writing, or dots and dashes in the Morse code can symbolize or represent something even they don't physically resemble what they are representing, similarly the action of nerve cells in your brain, the patterns of firing, represent objects and events in the external world even though they don't in any way resemble what's out there in the world.

Page 54: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Ignoring Effect• Not only can your bran bring things into existence it can

also block out the things it wants to ignore.• Brain is hard-wired to focus attention on threatening

sounds and sights. To assist the brain filters out repetitive, unchanging stimuli like a whirring air conditioner or the rocking of a boat at sea.

• At the lowest level, constantly stimulated neurons temporarily stop firing. (For this reason, your eyes jitter back and forth even when you hold your gaze steady. If they didn’t, the same neurons would always be stimulated by the sight in front of you. They’d get tired out, stop firing and everything would fade into blackness

Page 55: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• Adpatation to different levels of brightness• DOORWAY experiment – pushing hands

against sides of door – brain becomes accustomed to needing extra effort to keep arms up .

• Walk away with arms ni same position.• Sensation of arms drifting upwards• Ignoring smells - hard not to – p 87 manual

Page 56: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Pattern Completion and Ambiguity

Page 57: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 58: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 61: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

To make sense of the world it is necessary to organize incoming sensations into information which is meaningful.Gestalt psychologists believe one way this is done is by perceiving individual sensory stimuli as a meaningful whole.[2] Gestalt organization can be used to explain many illusions including the Duck-Rabbit illusion where the image as a whole switches back and forth from being a duck then being a rabbit and why in the figure-ground illusion the figure and ground are reversible.

Page 62: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 63: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Priming and Pattern Recognition

• Rorsach• Hard priming• Soft priming

• Primacy contributers– Time proximity– Implicit– Humans– danger

Page 64: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 65: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 66: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• When looking at the inkblot most of us are aware of the imaginative power we’re investing however there are cases where your brain performs the same task without you realizing the creative leap its taking.

• Have you ever thought you heard the phone ringing or a person calling your name while running a noisy appliance like a vacuum. This effect is spurred by the brain’s pattern matching system

Page 67: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 69: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Peripheral Vision

• Color tabs

Page 70: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 72: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Stepping Feet

• http://psy2.ucsd.edu/~sanstis/Foot.html• http://www.michaelbach.de/ot/mot_feet_lin/

Page 73: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Flash-lag

• http://www.michaelbach.de/ot/mot_flashlag1/

Page 74: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• The flash lag illusion or flash-lag effect is a visual illusion wherein a flash and a moving object that appear in the same location are perceived to be displaced from one another (MacKay, 1958; Nijhawan, 1994). Several explanations for this simple illusion have been explored in the neuroscience literature.

• Contents• [hide]• 1 Motion extrapolation • 2 Latency difference • 3 Motion integration and postdiction • 4 See also • 5 References • 6 External links • [edit] Motion extrapolation• The first proposed explanation for the flash-lag effect is that the visual system is predictive, accounting for neural delays by

extrapolating the trajectory of a moving stimulus into the future (Nijhawan, 1994; Khurana and Nijhawan, 1995). In other words, when light from a moving object hits the retina, a certain amount of time is required before the object is perceived. In that time, the object has moved to a new location in the world. The motion extrapolation hypothesis asserts that the visual system will take care of such delays by extrapolating the position of moving objects forward in time.

• [edit] Latency difference• A second proposed explanation is that the visual system processes moving objects more quickly than flashed objects. This

latency-difference hypothesis asserts that by the time the flashed object is processed, the moving object has already moved to a new position (Baldo and Klein, 1995; Whitney & Murakami, 1998; Purushothaman et al., 1998). The latency-difference proposal tacitly rests on the assumption that awareness (what the subject reports) is an on-line phenomenon, coming about as soon as a stimulus reaches its "perceptual end-point" (Zeki & Bartels, 1998).

• [edit] Motion integration and postdiction• Eagleman & Sejnowski (2000abc) proposed a third alternative: visual awareness is neither predictive nor on-line, but is instead

postdictive, such that the percept attributed to the time of the flash is a function of events that happen in the ~80 msec following the flash. This postdictive framework is consistent with findings in other fields, such as backward masking in visual psychophysics (Bachmann, 1994), or the Color Phi phenomenon (Kolers & von Grunau, 1976). In backward masking, a stimulus followed in rapid succession by a second stimulus can block or modify the perception of the first one. In the color phi phenomenon, 2 colored dots presented sequentially within a small time and distance will appear to have changed color in the middle of their apparent trajectory. Since the viewer cannot know what the color of the second dot will be until having seen the second dot, the only explanation is that the conscious percept attributed to the 'trajectory' of the dots is formed after the second dot has 'arrived' at its destination. Eagleman & Sejnowski found that the perception attributed to the time of the flash depends on events in the next ~80 msec after the flash. In this way, they drew a correspondence between the flash-lag effect and the Fröhlich effect (Fröhlich, 1923), wherein the first position of a moving object entering a window is misperceived.

Page 75: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Movement Distortions & Illusions

Page 76: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 77: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.
Page 78: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Rotating Snakes Illusion

• After each saccade, the previously viewed dots aren’t quite where your brain expects them to be, and so it assumes that they’ve shifted ever so slightly to the side. This creates the impression of motion

Page 79: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

eyetracking

http://www.poynterextra.org/eyetrack2004/

Page 80: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Messing with your Brain

Sit so that the white surface or wall is on your right. Hold the bottom of the mirror with your left hand, and put the mirror edge against your nose so that the reflecting surface of the mirror faces sideways, toward the white surface.While keeping the mirror edge against your nose, rotate the mirror so that your right eye sees just the reflection of the white wall, while your left eye looks forward at the face of a friend who is sitting a couple of feet away (see diagram). Move your hand in front of the white surface as if passing a blackboard eraser over the surface. Watch as parts of your friend's face disappear.It will help if your friend is sitting very still against a plain, light-colored background. You should also try to keep your own head as still as possible.If you have trouble seeing your friend's face disappear, one of your eyes might be stronger than the other. Try the experiment again, but this time switch the eye you use to look at the person and the eye you use to look at the wall.Individuals vary greatly in their ability to perceive this effect; a few people may never succeed in observing it. You may have to try this several times. Don't give up too soon! Give yourself time to see the effect.

Normally, your two eyes see very slightly different pictures of the world around you. Your brain analyzes these two pictures and then combines them to create a single, three-dimensional image.In this Snack, the mirror lets your eyes see two very different views. One eye looks straight ahead at another person, while the other eye looks at the white wall or screen and your moving hand. Your brain tries to put together a picture that makes sense by selecting bits and pieces from both views.Your brain is very sensitive to changes and motion. Since the other person is sitting very still, your brain emphasizes the information coming from the moving hand, and parts of the person's face disappear. No one knows how or why parts of the face sometimes remain, but the eyes and the mouth seem to be the last features to disappear. The lingering mouth gives rise to the name of this exhibit. The name for this exhibit derives from the Cheshire Cat in Lewis Carroll's story Alice's Adventures in Wonderland. The cat disappears, leaving behind only its smile.

Page 81: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Hollow Face Illusion

Page 82: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Hollow Face Illusion

Page 83: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• Says Richard Gregory, "The strong visual bias of favouring seeing a hollow mask as a normal convex face (figure 1), is evidence for the power of top-down knowledge for vision (Gregory 1970). This bias of seeing faces as convex is so strong it counters competing monocular depth cues, such as shading and shadows, and also very considerable unambiguous information from the two eyes signaling stereoscopically that the object is hollow. (Lighting a concave face from below to reverse the shading cues making them closer to those of a convex face lit from above can reinforce the illusion.)

Page 84: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• The brain is a pattern matching machine. You can make it more conservative in which it will miss things or you can make it more aggressive in which it will occasionallly invent them. The brain does both, although it is more likely to imagine something into existence than block it out because this proves to be the safer survival strategy

Page 85: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Your Brain – the missing manual

• The human brain was cobbled together over vast ceans of time , it’s no surrise that its parts don’t always work in harmony. For example, a sudden scare can cause your brain to briefly shut down its higher level functioning and respond with the survival strategies that are coded at a deeper level.

• Similar battle with perception of optical illusions.• Last major change in brain 100,000 years ago.• So how well does the brain adapt to fast cars, fast

food, and chronic stress

Page 86: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• Overstimulating part of the brain’s visual processing system..like the sfterimage you get when you stare into the sun

• Part of the brain’s strategy when picking out shapes involvrs emphasizing edges and contrasts.

Page 87: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• It has long been apparent that the perceived brightness of objects does not correspond in any simple way to their luminance (i.e., to the measured intensity of light corrected for the spectral sensitivity of the human visual system) (Figure 1A). In particular, two surfaces returning the same amount of light to the eye can look differently bright if the surfaces are observed in different contexts, a phenomenon called simultaneous brightness contrast (most psychologists prefer to call this 'lightness' contrast to distinguish the appearance of surfaces that reflect light from the appearance of endogenous sources of light; for present purposes, this distinction is not critical, and the term 'brightness' is used here in its ordinary meaning of perceived intensity).

• The explanation of this remarkable effect found in many textbooks is predicated on lateral interactions among retinal ganglion cells or other lower order visual neurons, which demonstrably cause distorted rates of neuronal firing at contrast boundaries, presumably to enhance the detection of edges (Figure 1B). This interpretation implies that the relative intensities perceived in response to such stimuli are, in effect, 'readouts' of the relative firing rate of neurons at the input stages of the visual. On this basis, any target predominantly surrounded by an area of higher luminance should look darker than the same target predominantly surrounded by an area of lower luminance.

• Figure 1 « Close• Figure 1 / Standard demonstration of simultaneous brightness contrast, and the conventional explanation of this effect. A) A target (the diamond) on a less luminant background (left) is perceived as being

brighter than the same target on a more luminant background (right), even though the two targets are physical identical, and appear so if both are presented on the same background (as shown above). B) Diagram of the usual explanation of this phenomenon, based on the center-surround receptive field properties of retinal ganglion cells. the center-surround receptive field organization of input level neurons will, as illustrated here, cause less lateral inibition, and therefore more signal passed centrally from high contrast boundaries than from lower ones.

• Despite the apparent concordance of perception and retinal physiology in this instance, a number of observations indicate that identical targets embedded in scenes that have exactly the same local contrast relationships with their surrounds can nonetheless look differently bright (Figure 2). Indeed it is even possible to construct stimuli in which a target in a predominantly higher luminance surround looks brighter than an identical target in a predominantly lower luminance surround (Figure 2B). How, then, can these seeming contradictions in the relationship of luminance and brightness be explained?

• Figure 2 « Close• Figure 2 / Evidence that distorted neuronal responses to local contrast (Figure 1B) cannot explain simultaneous brightness contrast. A) In the Wertheimer-Benary stimulus, two equiluminant targets (the gray

triangles) elicit different sensations of brightness despite having the same local contrast relationships (for most observers the upper triangle looks slightly brighter/lighter than the lower one). B) White's illusion is particularly interesting because it generates a perception of relative brightness that is similar to the sensations elicited in Figure 1A, despite the fact that the local contrast of the patches (set inset left) is more or less opposite the standard brightness contrast stimulus shown in Figure 1A. Thus, the targets that appear brighter (the patches on the left) are mainly surrounded by areas of higher luminance, whereas the targets that appear darker are surrounded mainly by areas of lower luminance. C) Differences in lightness/brightness of equiluminant targets in the absence of any differences at all in local luminance contrast. Top panel - Light and dark surrounds with equiluminant test diamonds on the adjacent faces of a cube. Middle panel - The same cube rotated 180°. Bottom panel - Graph showing the average adjustment made by observers to equalize the brightness of the two test targets in the upper and middle panels.

• In terms of a wholly empirical strategy of vision, the explanation of the difference in perceived brightness of the two equiluminant targets in Figure 1A and in this Demonstration is summarized in Figure 3 (Williams et al, 1998a and b; see also Lotto and Purves, 1999). Since the amount of light returned to eye from any portion of a scene depends on both the illumination of the relevant surfaces and their reflectances (among other factors), the equiluminant returns from the targets are inherently ambiguous. Such stimuli will often have been generated by similarly reflective surfaces on differently reflective surrounds under the same illuminant; the same luminance profiles, however, will often have signified differently reflective target surfaces under different amounts of illumination.

• Figure 3 « Close• Figure 3 / A probabilistic explanation of simultaneous brightness contrast effects. A) A standard simultaneous brightness contrast stimulus. B and C) Cartoons illustrating the two major categorical sources of the

stimulus in (A). The different lightness/brightness of the two identical targets in (A) is seen because the response to the stimulus incorporates all its possible sources in proportion to their past frequency of occurrence, which differs in natural scenes.

• Since dealing successfully with this or any stimulus depends on responding appropriately to the sources of the retinal stimulus rather than the stimulus as such, the visual system can only solve this problem on the basis of past experience. If this idea is correct, then to the extent that the stimulus is consistent with similarly reflective target surfaces under the same illuminant, the targets will tend to appear similarly bright. However, in so far as the stimulus is consistent with the past experience of the visual system with differently reflective objects in different levels of illumination, the targets will tend to appear differently bright. Because the standard simultaneous brightness contrast stimulus is consistent with either of these possible sources, the pattern of neural activity elicited - that is, the percept experienced when looking at the stimulus in Figure 1A or Figure 3A (or the related demonstrations) - is a manifestation of both possibilities (and indeed all of the many other possibilities not illustrated) in proportion to their relative frequency of occurrence in past experience with stimuli of this general sort.

• In support of this explanation, crafting the stimulus in this Demonstration to be more consistent with differently reflective surfaces in different illuminants increases the 'illusion' of simultaneous brightness contrast (see Demonstration, for example), whereas making the stimulus less consistent with this possibility, and more consistent with the source being similar reflective objects under similar illuminants causes the targets to appear more similar, even if all the luminance relationships in the scene are preserved. Other more complex examples that support this interpretation of how lightness/brightness percepts are generated are found in this Demonstration.

• References• Lotto RB, Purves D (1999) The effects of color on brightness. Nat Neurosc 2: 1010-1014.• Purves D, Lotto B (2002) Why We See What We Do: An Empirical Theory of Vision. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.• Purves D, Lotto R B, Williams SM, Nundy S, and Yang, Z (2001) Why we see things the way we do: Evidence for a wholly empirical strategy of vision. Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc., 356:285-297.• Purves D, Williams MS, Nundy S, Lotto RB (2004) Perceiving the intensity of light. Psychological Rev. Vol 111: 142-158.• Williams SM, McCoy AN, Purves D (1998a) The influence of depicted illumination on perceived brightness. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:13296-13300.• Williams SM, McCoy AN, Purves D (1998b) An empirical explanation of brightness. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:13301-13306.• Yang Z, Purves D (2004) The statistical structure of natural light patterns determines perceived light intensity. Proc Natl Acad Sci 101: 8745-8750.

Page 88: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• The brain is a pattern matching machine. You can make it more conservative in which it will miss things or you can make it more aggressive in which it will occasionallly invent them. The brain does both, although it more likely to imagine something into existence than block it out because this proves to be the safer survival strategy

Page 89: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• The imaginary white triangle is one instance in which brain fills in some logiical ingredients to complete the picture

• Clouds – spotting recognizable shapes in clouds • Jesus grilled –cheese sandwiches• Fashion- the goal of some fashions is by

concealing the exact shapes of the wearers body they give ample room for the brain to imagine the idealized representation it expects

Page 90: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• When looking at the inkblot most of us are aware of the imaginative power we’re investing however there are cases where your brain performs the same task without you realizing the creative leap its taking.

• Have you ever thuoght you heard the phone ringing or a person calling your nam while running a noisy appliance like a vacuum. This effect is spurred by the brain’s pattern matching system

Page 91: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Ignoring Effect• Not only can your bran bring things into existence it can

also block out the things it wants to ignore.• Brain is hard-wired to focus attention on threatening

sounds and sights. To assist the brain filters out repetitive, unchanging stimuli like a whirring air conditioner or the rocking of a boat at sea.

• At the lowest level, constantly stimulated neurons temporarily stop firing. (For this reason, your eyes jitter back and forth even when you hold your gaze steady. If they didn’t, the same neurons would always be stimulated by the sight in front of you. They’d get tired out, stop firing and everything would fade into blackness

Page 92: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• Adpatation to different levels of brightness• DOORWAY experiment – pushing hands

against sides of door – brain becomes accustomed to needing extra effort to keep arms up .

• Walk away with arms ni same position.• Sensation of arms drifting upwards• Ignoring smells - hard not to – p 87 manual

Page 93: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• Theres a group of people gathered indoors in discussio there appears to be a window on the left above one woman.

• These “obvious facts” aren’t quite as obvious to people ith differernt expereinces and different assumptions engravedd ontheir brains.

• When shown to East Africans, nearly all of them said the woman on the left was balancing a box on her head and the corner of the room was a tree.

• As a Westerner who has spent much of life indoors you interpret things differerntly

• Same holds true for the faces-vase illusion

Page 94: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• Overstimulating part of the brain’s visual processing system..like the afterimage you get when you stare into the sun

• Part of the brain’s strategy when picking out shapes involves emphasizing edges and contrasts.

Page 95: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

Discussion Points

• What is reality• How can 2 people experience different

realities• Can I influence another person’s perception of

reality. How?• What are some bias that are hard-wired into

the brain?

Page 96: Experimenting with Experimental Brain Science Dr. Michael Raucci.

• Flashdrive• Gathering for gardner copies• Rubber hand• Print out discussion points• Mirror Box• Mirrors