Emerging Adulthood 2014 Lindell 79 91

download Emerging Adulthood 2014 Lindell 79 91

of 7

Transcript of Emerging Adulthood 2014 Lindell 79 91

  • 8/19/2019 Emerging Adulthood 2014 Lindell 79 91

    1/14

     http://eax.sagepub.com/ Emerging Adulthood

     http://eax.sagepub.com/content/2/2/79The online version of this article can be found at:

     DOI: 10.1177/2167696813502778

     2014 2: 79 originally published online 5 September 2013Emerging Adulthood Anna K. Lindell, Nicole Campione-Barr and Kelly Bassett Greer

    CollegeAssociations Between Adolescent Sibling Conflict and Relationship Quality During the Transition

     

    Published by:

     http://www.sagepublications.com

    On behalf of:

     

    Society for the Study of Emerging Adulthood

     can be found at:Emerging Adulthood Additional services and information for

    http://eax.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts: 

    http://eax.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions: 

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints: 

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:

    What is This? 

    - Sep 5, 2013OnlineFirst Version of Record

    - Apr 30, 2014Version of Record>> 

    by Alina Ciucas on June 19, 2014eax.sagepub.comDownloaded from  by Alina Ciucas on June 19, 2014eax.sagepub.comDownloaded from 

    http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/content/2/2/79http://eax.sagepub.com/content/2/2/79http://www.sagepublications.com/http://www.ssea.org/http://eax.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://eax.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://eax.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://eax.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://eax.sagepub.com/content/early/2013/09/04/2167696813502778.full.pdfhttp://eax.sagepub.com/content/2/2/79.full.pdfhttp://eax.sagepub.com/content/2/2/79.full.pdfhttp://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://eax.sagepub.com/content/early/2013/09/04/2167696813502778.full.pdfhttp://eax.sagepub.com/content/2/2/79.full.pdfhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://eax.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://eax.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://www.ssea.org/http://www.sagepublications.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/content/2/2/79http://eax.sagepub.com/

  • 8/19/2019 Emerging Adulthood 2014 Lindell 79 91

    2/14

     Article

    Associations Between Adolescent SiblingConflict and Relationship Quality Duringthe Transition to College

    Anna K. Lindell1, Nicole Campione-Barr 1, and Kelly Bassett Greer 1

    Abstract

    Family researchers have acknowledged the importance of sibling relationships across the life span, but little is known about howthis relationship functions during the transition from adolescence to emerging adulthood. The present study therefore examined

    how two domains of adolescent sibling conflict (equality and fairness issues and personal domain invasions) are related to siblingrelationship quality after firstborns leave home to attend college, from the perspectives of firstborn and secondborn siblings from

    the same families. Our findings suggest that frequent or intense sibling conflicts during adolescence may be related to morepositive and less negative sibling relationships the first year after older siblings leave home, and that conflicts surrounding issues of 

    equality and fairness may facilitate more egalitarian relationships during emerging adulthood for some siblings. It therefore seemsthat the sibling relationship during the adolescent years may have important implications for its long-term functioning intoadulthood.

    Keywords

    college, family relationships, longitudinal, siblings, life events

    Siblings are an important aspect of the family that are often

    neglected by family researchers (see Brody, 1998; Dunn,

    2002; McHale, Kim, & Whiteman, 2006 for reviews), yet these

    relationships have key implications for family dynamics and 

    individual development (Brody, 2004), and provide much sup-

     port during adulthood (Cicirelli, 1995). Given the importance

    of sibling relationships during both early and later years, it is

    surprising that there has been little investigation of this

    relationship during emerging adulthood.

    A growing body of literature has examined aspects of the

     parent–child relationship during emerging adulthood (e.g.,

    Kloep & Hendry, 2010; Padilla-Walker & Nelson, 2012), but

    the lack of research on siblings is troubling given their integral

    role within the family context (Cox, 2010). Family systems the-

    ory highlights the interdependent and transactional nature of 

    family members and their influences on one another; thus,

    understanding the nature of only select dyads within the family

    results in an incomplete or even incorrect picture of familyfunctioning (Minuchin, 1985). Systems theory recognizes, in

     particular, that the transitions of one family member (i.e., to

    emerging adulthood) impact the entire family system. In line

    with this assertion, Conger and Little (2010) suggested that

    transitioning to emerging adulthood involves a major ‘‘recen-

    tering’’ of close relationships, including those between sib-

    lings, as emerging adults establish greater independence from

    their natal family and develop new roles. As a result, siblings’

    day-to-day relationship becomes far more voluntary than ever 

     before, likely changing several aspects of the relationship.

    The present study aims to uncover some of these changes by

    investigating associations between sibling conflict and other 

    aspects of relationship quality during the transition from ado-

    lescence to emerging adulthood, or more specifically to the

     present study, to college. In 2010, approximately 41%  of 18-

    through 24-year-olds in the United States were enrolled in

    either 2- or 4-year colleges (U.S. Department of Education,

    2011), indicating that college is a salient part of emerging

    adulthood for many Americans. Family processes during late

    adolescence may be especially important for college students,

    who often remain tied to their natal families in ways that those

    leaving home to lead more independent lifestyles (i.e., full-time

    employment, military) do not. Many college students are

     partially, if not mostly, financially dependent on their parents

    (Padilla-Walker, Nelson, & Carroll, 2012), and may spend the

    majority of their school breaks at their natal homes. Thus,

    although college students adopt many responsibilities of inde-

     pendent living, they are often still only semiautonomous(Arnett, 2000), with family processes remaining particularly

    salient factors for them. The present study also expands on

    1 Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO,

    USA

    Corresponding Author:

    Anna K. Lindell, MA, Department of Psychological Sciences, University of 

    Missouri, 210 McAlester Hall, Columbia, MO 65211, USA.

    Email: [email protected]

    Emerging Adulthood

    2014, Vol. 2(2) 79-91

    ª 2013 Society for the

    Study of Emerging Adulthood

    and SAGE Publications

    Reprints and permission:

    sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

    DOI: 10.1177/2167696813502778

    ea.sagepub.com

     by Alina Ciucas on June 19, 2014eax.sagepub.comDownloaded from 

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://ea.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://ea.sagepub.com/http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

  • 8/19/2019 Emerging Adulthood 2014 Lindell 79 91

    3/14

     previous research by examining the sibling relationship from

    the perspective of both transitioning adolescents and their 

    younger sibling, since this transition also likely impacts

    younger siblings who stay behind (Whiteman, McHale, &

    Crouter, 2011).

    Sibling Relationship Quality Various sibling relationship qualities are related to both posi-

    tive and negative outcomes among adolescents (e.g., Padilla-

    Walker, Harper, & Jensen, 2010), and given the importance

    of siblings later in life (e.g., Cicirelli, 1995), it may be partic-

    ularly fruitful to examine some of these qualities during the

    transition to emerging adulthood to better understand how sib-

    lings establish patterns during adolescence that set the stage for 

    their relationships as adults. Aquilino (2006) has suggested that

    siblings must achieve a sufficiently egalitarian relationship in

    order to remain close later in life and that this may begin to

    finalize during emerging adulthood. While sibling relation-

    ships are considerably less hierarchical than parent–child 

    relationships, older siblings typically do hold more power dur-

    ing childhood and adolescence (Perlman, Siddiqui, Ram, &

    Ross, 2000), but by late adolescence, the relationship becomes

    increasingly egalitarian (Buhrmester & Furman, 1990). Shortt

    and Gottman (1997) found that college-aged siblings were gen-

    erally successful at resolving power differences, but those who

    were not close and who had more conflictive relationships

    struggled to resolve these power imbalances. Due to the signif-

    icant lifestyle changes that typically occur at the beginning of 

    emerging adulthood, some of these important changes in

    sibling power differential may occur during older siblings’ first

    year away from home.

    Throughout adolescence, some sibling relationship quali-ties, including warmth and closeness, decline significantly

    (Buhrmester & Furman, 1990), but Kim, McHale, Osgood, and 

    Crouter (2006) found that sibling intimacy may actually

    increase toward the end of adolescence, particularly for 

    mixed-sex sibling dyads. In line with these findings, a cross-

    sectional Israeli study (Scharf, Shulman, & Avigad-Spitz,

    2005) showed that emerging adults (compared to adolescents)

    had less conflicted, yet warmer relationships with their sib-

    lings, supporting the possibility of a rebound in positivity after 

    the end of adolescence. Whiteman, McHale, and Crouter 

    (2011) have shown that an especially robust increase in inti-

    macy may occur during the first year firstborns leave the family

    home.

    Sibling Conflict

    Although sibling conflict is often considered a negative aspect

    of siblings’ relationship quality (along with antagonism, criti-

    cism, and domineering behaviors; Adams & Laursen, 2007),

    it is not particularly indicative on its own of overall relationship

    negativity. Indeed, McGuire, McHale, and Updegraff (1996)

    identified the presence of one common sibling relationship

    typology among those in middle childhood that included simul-

    taneously warm  and  hostile/conflictive features (endorsed by

    16.5% of their sample). Therefore, given conflict’s unique role

    in the sibling relationship, it should be afforded special atten-

    tion separate from the broader concept of ‘‘negativity.’’

    Conflict is a common feature of sibling relationships, yet

    frequent conflict during childhood and adolescence (over and 

    above parent–child relationship features) has been shown to

     be related to poor adjustment outcomes (e.g., Campione-Barr,Greer, & Kruse, 2013; Kim, McHale, Crouter, & Osgood,

    2007; Stocker, Burwell, & Briggs, 2002) and social functioning

    (Criss & Shaw, 2005; Dunn, 2002; Kim et al., 2007). Conflict

    may also be beneficial, however, by teaching siblings effective

    conflict resolution strategies (Vandell & Bailey, 1992) which

    can then be applied in other relationships. It is therefore impor-

    tant to develop a better understanding of the long-term implica-

    tions (i.e., into adulthood) of this common aspect of sibling

    relationships.

    Although sibling conflict tends to decline in frequency rel-

    atively steadily through adolescence (Buhrmester & Furman,

    1990) and is not particularly prevalent during emerging adult-

    hood (Scharf et al., 2005), Whiteman et al. (2011) found thatsibling conflict decreased at a rate more rapid than prior trajec-

    tories only after firstborns actually left home. Importantly, sis-

    ters experienced a slower decline in conflict than brothers did,

    highlighting the fact that gender is an important contributor to

    individuals’ perceptions of the sibling relationship. Similarly,

    Stocker, Lanthier, and Furman (1997) found that emerging

    adult dyads including at least one sister were more conflictive

    than those without any females. However, despite higher levels

    of conflict among these dyads, females also tend to experience

    warmer sibling relationships (Stocker, Lanthier, & Furman,

    1997), even during emerging adulthood (Scharf et al., 2005),

    further highlighting that conflictive relationships should not

    necessarily be interpreted as poor overall.

    Given the prevalence and unique nature of conflict among

    siblings, some researchers have examined more specific nuan-

    ces of this process by differentiating between various domains

    of conflict based on their content. While this has not yet been

    examined among emerging adults, Campione-Barr and Sme-

    tana (2010) have cross-sectionally investigated different

    domains of conflict among adolescent siblings informed by a

    social domain perspective (Smetana, 2013; Turiel, 2002) and 

    found that siblings engage in two common domains of conflict:

    conflicts over personal domain invasions (e.g., borrowing with-

    out permission) and socially regulated (aspects of moral and 

    conventional domains) issues of equality and fairness (e.g.,whose turn it is to do chores). They found that adolescent sib-

    lings experienced more frequent conflicts over personal

    domain invasions (compared to those involving equality and 

    fairness issues), and both frequent and intense (i.e., ‘‘heated’’)

    conflicts of this type were related to less trust and communica-

    tion within the relationship. Although there has not yet been a

    confirmation of the directionality of these effects, it seems

    likely that conflict may be associated with future relationship

    qualities.

    In a later study, Campione-Barr, Greer, and Kruse (2013)

    examined longitudinal associations between these same

    80   Emerging Adulthood 2(2)

     by Alina Ciucas on June 19, 2014eax.sagepub.comDownloaded from 

    http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/

  • 8/19/2019 Emerging Adulthood 2014 Lindell 79 91

    4/14

    domains of conflict and emotional adjustment among adoles-

    cent siblings. Using actor–partner interdependence modeling

    (APIM; Kenny, Kashy, & Cook, 2006) to examine dyadic,

     bidirectional associations, Campione-Barr et al. found that not

    all domains of sibling conflict similarly influence adjustment,

    and that, notably, many of these effects differed by ordinal

     position and gender composition, exemplifying the nuanced elements of the sibling relationship that impact their percep-

    tions of conflict. Generally, however, conflicts over personal

    domain invasions were related to increased anxiety and lower 

    self-esteem 1 year later, while equality and fairness conflicts

    were related to increased depressive symptoms. Campione-

    Barr et al. reasoned that equality and fairness conflicts facilitate

    social comparison, which according to social comparison the-

    ory (Festinger, 1954) tends to result in poorer emotional adjust-

    ment when the comparison is unfavorable, particularly for girls

    and older siblings when feeling unfairly treated in the family

    context (e.g., Shanahan, McHale, Crouter, & Osgood, 2008).

    The Present Study 

    The current study attempted to advance Campione-Barr and 

    colleagues’ (2010, 2013) findings by investigating associations

     between two domains of sibling conflict during firstborn ado-

    lescents’ final year of high school and three aspects of sibling

    relationship quality (positivity, negativity, and relative power)

    the following year after moving out of their natal home to

    attend college. Data were also collected from emerging adults’

    next youngest siblings to examine transactional processes in

    the relationship, since family systems theory recognizes that

    transitions experienced by one family member challenge the

    entire family system (Minuchin, 1985). Consequently, we uti-

    lized APIM (Kenny et al., 2006) to investigate nuanced dyadic

    explanations of these processes. Specifically, APIM allows for 

    examination of both ‘‘actor’’ (see Figure 1, lines  a, b) and 

    ‘‘partner’’ effects (lines  c, d ). In a longitudinal context such

    as ours, actor effects include associations between the Time 1

    and Time 2 variables from the perspective of one dyad mem-

     ber, whereas partner effects are associations between one mem-

     ber’s report of the Time 1 variable and their sibling’s report of 

    the Time 2 variable.

    Hypotheses.   We first hypothesized that more frequent and 

    intense conflicts over personal domain invasions would be

    associated with lower positivity and higher negativity 1 year later, given Campione-Barr and Smetana’s (2010) finding of 

    cross-sectional negative associations between this type of con-

    flict and trust and communication. We expected both ‘‘actor’’

    and ‘‘partner’’ effects in this context, given the interdependent

    nature of family relationships.

    We next expected that frequent and intense conflicts over 

    equality and fairness issues would be associated with greater 

     power differential 1 year later (both ‘‘actor’’ and ‘‘partner’’

    effects) in light of previous work suggesting that this type of 

    conflict facilitates social comparison (Campione-Barr et al.,

    2013). In this context, if one sibling consistently ‘‘wins’’ this

    type of conflict, then that sibling’s dominance in the relation-

    ship should increase.

    It is also likely that birth order, gender, and dyad gender 

    composition moderate these associations. We expected that

    associations would be most prevalent from the perspective of 

    younger siblings, since they tend to value sibling interactions

    more than older siblings do (Buhrmester & Furman, 1990).

    Finally, we expected females to be more sensitive to these pro-cesses than males; consequently, stronger associations should 

     be present in sister–sister dyads, followed by mixed-sex dyads.

    It has been well established that males and females judge their 

    satisfaction in and closeness of their relationships differently

    (e.g., Wood & Inman, 1993), and that adolescent females report

    more intimate sibling relationships than males (Kim, McHale,

    Osgood, & Crouter, 2006).

    Method

    Participants

    Forty-eight sibling dyads were initially recruited, along with atleast one parent/adoptive parent, as part of a larger 4-year long-

    itudinal study. For the larger study, each family in a small

    Midwestern city school district with a child in 8th, 10th, or 

    12th grade (3,028 families) was mailed a recruitment letter, and 

    145 families (one parent, firstborn, and secondborn child

  • 8/19/2019 Emerging Adulthood 2014 Lindell 79 91

    5/14

  • 8/19/2019 Emerging Adulthood 2014 Lindell 79 91

    6/14

    We examined (separately for conflict frequency and inten-

    sity, and from both siblings’ perspectives) the influence of both

    domains of conflict (equality and fairness and personal domain

    invasions) on sibling relationship qualities 1 year later (sepa-

    rately for positivity, negativity, and relative power). While the

    two conflict domains were included in each model, conflict fre-

    quency and intensity were examined in separate models

     because previous research has shown that they impact develop-

    ment (Laursen, Coy, & Collins, 1998) and relationships (Mon-

    temayor, 1983) differently. Furthermore, Campione-Barr and 

    colleagues (2010, 2013) found nuanced differences between

    the frequency and intensity of conflict when examining their associations with adjustment and relationship quality, further 

    indicating a need to examine these aspects separately.

    In our models, we also examined moderating effects of sib-

    ling ordinal position and the genders of both siblings. West,

    Popp, and Kenny (2008) have highlighted the importance of 

    considering the genders of both actors and partners in APIM,

    and although our sample size (45 dyads; 90 individuals) is

    smaller than ideal, APIM has been successfully conducted with

    samples smaller than ours, even when examining similar 

    higher-order moderations (e.g., Puccinelli & Tickle-Degnen,

    2004). In order to examine these moderations, we initially ran

    first-order models including only main effects of both domains

    of conflict (frequency or intensity), then incrementally added two-way, three-way, and four-way interactions between con-

    flict, both sibling genders, and ordinal position to the models

    to interpret significant higher-order interactions that may be

     present. Thus, we examined a total of 24 models.

    Correlations Between Conflict and Relationship Qualities

    Siblings’ reports of each aspect of relationship quality were

    moderately to highly correlated from Time 1 to Time 2, and 

    negativity and relative power were correlated for older (but not

    younger) siblings (see Table 1). Both aspects and domains of 

    conflict were significantly correlated as well, and in a few (but

    not all) circumstances, conflict variables were moderately asso-

    ciated with negativity. For younger siblings, the frequency and 

    intensity of equality and fairness conflicts were associated with

    negativity at Time 2, while for older siblings, the frequency and 

    intensity of personal invasion conflicts were associated with

    negativity at Time 2.

    Longitudinal Associations Between Conflict Frequency 

    and Relationship Qualities

    In this section of analyses, associations between conflict fre-

    quency and three aspects of relationship quality 1 year later 

    were tested. Three separate APIM models were run: one each

    for positivity, negativity, and relative power, controlling for 

    gender, ordinal position, age (to tease apart the effects of age

    and ordinal position), and the Time 1 report of the aspect of 

    relationship quality being tested in order to examine effects

    above and beyond any associations between Time 1 conflict

    and Time 1 relationship quality (see Tables 2–4). For simpli-

    city, and due to the large number of analyses, significant inter-

    actions are discussed only if simple slopes analyses produced 

    significant findings, and significant findings with regard to

    control and demographic variables will not be discussed,though they may be referenced in Tables 2–4. Further, we only

    interpret the highest ordered interactions in each model, and 

    only those not subsumed under higher-order interactions pres-

    ent in higher-order models.

    Positivity.   In the third-order model examining associations

     between conflict frequency and positivity, there was a margin-

    ally significant interaction for Partner Gender  Birth Order 

    Actor ratings of the Frequency of Equality and Fairness Con-

    flicts. Simple slopes analyses revealed that the more frequent

    emerging adults with younger sisters reported conflicts over 

    Table 1.  Correlations Among Emerging Adult and Sibling Reports of Conflict (Time 1) and Relationship Qualities (Times 1 and 2).

    OlderSibling,M (SD) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

    YoungerSibling,M (SD)

    1. Age (T1) 17.56 (.50) — .22 .03 .24 .03 .31*   .33* .06 .06   .28* .02   .10 14.54 (1.17)2. Gender .11 — .06 .24þ .01 .14   .15 .04 .26þ .06 .18 .21

    3. EF frequency 1.87 (.64)   .34* .00 — .61** .94** .62** .07 .04 .30*   .06 .00 .34* 1.83 (.76)4. IP frequency 1.72 ( .64)   .21   .01 .56** — .65** .76**   .05 .04 .26þ .17   .06 .21 1.74 (.74)5. EF intensity 1.49 ( .54)   .35*   .21 .74** .48** — .70** .02 .00 .24   .12   .10 .35* 1.45 (.55)6. IP intensity 1.70 (.70)   .26þ .01 .67** .80** .69** —     .31* .05 .22   .19   .12 .20 1.75 (.87)7. Power (T1) 1.97 (.83)   .12   .03 .14 .18 .19 .11 — .17 .09 .37* .21 .22 2.94 (.97)8. Positivity (T1) 2.99 (.90) .17 .17   .07 .00   .22   .07 .22 —     .28þ .09 .53**   .07 3.05 ( .91)9. Negativity (T1) 2.28 (.77)   .21   .05 .04 .46** .11 .34* .33*   .16 — .24þ .10 .54** 2.60 ( .91)

    10. Power (T2) 1.80 (.72)   .05   .17   .21   .10   .12   .21 .39**   .06 .30* — .01 .51** 2.71 (.90)11. Positivity (T2) 2.98 (.75)   .06   .07 .03   .02   .07   .04 .12 .75**   .18   .09 —     .08 2.82 ( .88)12. Negativity (T2) 2.09 (.82)   .30*   .11 .10 .39** .23 .30** .38**   .01 .79** .55** .00 — 2.45 (.77)

    Note. T1 ¼ Time 1 report; T2 ¼ Time 2 report; EF ¼ equality and fairness issues; IP ¼ personal domain invasions; Frequency ¼ conflict frequency; Intensity ¼conflict intensity.Gender coded as 1 ¼ male, 0 ¼ female.Older sibling reports are on the bottom diagonal and younger sibling reports are on the top diagonal.þp < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01.

    Lindell et al.   83

     by Alina Ciucas on June 19, 2014eax.sagepub.comDownloaded from 

    http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/

  • 8/19/2019 Emerging Adulthood 2014 Lindell 79 91

    7/14

    http://eax.sagepub.com/

  • 8/19/2019 Emerging Adulthood 2014 Lindell 79 91

    8/14

    http://eax.sagepub.com/

  • 8/19/2019 Emerging Adulthood 2014 Lindell 79 91

    9/14

    http://eax.sagepub.com/

  • 8/19/2019 Emerging Adulthood 2014 Lindell 79 91

    10/14

    equality and fairness issues, the more positively they rated their 

    relationship a year later (t ¼ 3.23,  p  < .01). This suggests that

    frequent conflicts over equality and fairness issues may

    actually be beneficial to siblings, at least in dyads with younger 

    sisters.

    Negativity.   In the third-order model examining associations between conflict frequency and negativity, there was a signif-

    icant interaction for Actor Gender  Birth Order  Partner rat-

    ings of the Frequency of Equality and Fairness Conflicts.

    Simple slopes revealed that the more frequent younger siblings

    of female emerging adults reported conflicts over equality and 

    fairness issues, the less negatively their older sisters rated the

    relationship a year later (t ¼ 2.59,  p ¼ .02).

    There was also a significant interaction for Partner Gender 

     Birth Order     Actor ratings of the Frequency of Personal

    Domain Invasion Conflicts. Simple slopes revealed that the

    more frequent emerging adults with younger sisters reported 

    conflicts over personal domain invasions, the less negativity

    they reported a year later (t ¼ 3.29,  p  < .01).

    Like the findings for positivity, these results suggest that

    frequent conflicts may actually be beneficial to sibling relation-

    ships in that less negativity is perceived the following year,

     particularly when the dyad includes at least one sister. Specif-

    ically, dyads including an older sister may be perceived as less

    negative if there are frequent conflicts over equality and 

    fairness issues, and dyads including a younger sister may seem

    less negative with more frequent conflicts over personal

    domain invasions the previous year.

    Relative Power.   In the second-order model examining associa-

    tions between conflict frequency and relative power, there wasa significant interaction for Partner Gender  Actor ratings of 

    the Frequency of Personal Domain Invasion Conflicts. Simple

    slopes revealed that the more frequent participants with broth-

    ers reported conflicts over personal domain invasions, the more

     power they felt their brothers had the following year (t ¼ 2.41,

     p  ¼   .02). This suggests that for dyads including at least one

     brother, relationships characterized by frequent conflicts over 

     personal domain invasions may be at particular risk for becom-

    ing male powered.

    Longitudinal Associations Between Conflict Intensity and Relationship Qualities

    In this section of analyses, associations between conflict inten-

    sity and the three aspects of relationship quality 1 year later 

    were tested using three separate APIM models in a method 

    similar to those examining conflict frequency (see Tables 2– 

    4 for test statistics).

    Positivity. In the models examining associations between conflict

    intensity and positivity, there were no significant results of 

    interest, so no further interpretations were made.

    Negativity.   In the third-order model examining associations

     between conflict intensity and negativity, there was a signifi-

    cant interaction for Actor Gender  Birth Order  Partner rat-

    ings of the Intensity of Equality and Fairness Conflicts. Simple

    slopes revealed that the more intense younger siblings of 

    female emerging adults reported conflicts over equality and 

    fairness issues, the lower negativity their older sisters reported a year later (t ¼ 2.40,  p ¼ .02).

    Additionally, there was a significant interaction for Partner 

    Gender Birth Order Actor ratings of the Intensity of Equal-

    ity and Fairness Conflicts. Simple slopes revealed that the less

    intense emerging adults with younger sisters reported conflicts

    over equality and fairness issues, the lower negativity they

    reported a year later (t ¼ 3.04,  p ¼ .01).

    Finally, there was a significant interaction for Partner Gen-

    der  Birth Order  Actor ratings of the Intensity of Personal

    Domain Invasion Conflicts. Simple slopes revealed that the

    more intense emerging adults with younger sisters rated 

    conflicts over personal domain invasions, the less negativity

    they reported a year later (t ¼ 2.17,  p ¼ .04).These findings suggest several nuanced patterns, particu-

    larly for dyads including a sister. While more intense conflicts

    over equality and fairness issues as reported by younger sib-

    lings with older sisters are related to lower reports of negativity

    from older sisters, for older siblings with  younger  sisters, it is

    rather  less  intense conflicts of this type that are related to less

    negativity. In contrast, for these older siblings with younger 

    sisters, more  intense conflicts over personal domain invasions

    are actually related to less negativity the next year. Therefore,

    it seems that later negativity is differentially associated with the

    ordinal position of sisters, as well as conflict domain when con-

    sidering conflict intensity.

    Relative Power.   In the second-order model examining associa-

    tions between conflict intensity and relative power, there was

    a significant interaction for Birth Order  Actor ratings of the

    Intensity of Personal Domain Invasion Conflicts. Simple slopes

    revealed that the more intense older siblings rated conflicts

    over personal domain invasions, the more power they felt they

    had the following year (t ¼ 2.88,  p ¼ .01).

    In the third-order model, there was a marginally significant

    interaction for Actor Gender   Partner Gender   Partner rat-

    ings of the Intensity of Equality and Fairness Conflicts. Simple

    slopes indicated that the more intense sisters with brothers

    rated conflicts over equality and fairness issues, the less power their brothers perceived the following year (t ¼ 2.47, p ¼ .02).

    Finally, in the fourth-order model, there was a significant

    interaction for Actor Sex Partner Sex Birth Order  Actor 

    ratings of the Intensity of Equality and Fairness Conflicts. Sim-

     ple slopes revealed that the more intense younger sisters of 

    female emerging adults reported conflicts over equality and 

    fairness issues, the more power they felt their older sisters had 

    a year later (t ¼ 2.27, p ¼ .03), and that the more intense female

    emerging adults with younger sisters reported conflicts over 

    equality and fairness issues, the more power they felt their 

    younger sisters had a year later (t ¼ 2.04, p ¼ .05). Finally, the

    Lindell et al.   87

     by Alina Ciucas on June 19, 2014eax.sagepub.comDownloaded from 

    http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/

  • 8/19/2019 Emerging Adulthood 2014 Lindell 79 91

    11/14

    more intense younger brothers of female emerging adults rated 

    these types of conflicts, the more power they felt they had 1

    year later (t ¼ 2.22,  p ¼ .04).

    Taken together, these results indicate that, similar to fre-

    quent conflicts, intense conflicts over equality and fairness

    issues may be an effective method of increasing one’s power 

    within the sibling relationship, especially in dyads includingat least one sister. Additionally, intense conflicts over personal

    domain invasions may facilitate more power within the

    relationship for older siblings.

    Discussion

    The present study examined older and younger siblings’

     perceptions of their conflicts and other relationship qualities

    during an important developmental event: firstborns’ transi-

    tions to college. We were interested in associations between

    two aspects (frequency and intensity) and domains (equality

    and fairness, personal domain invasions) of conflict and threefacets of relationship quality (positivity, negativity, and rela-

    tive power) the following year. Although sibling conflict is in

    itself an aspect of sibling relationship quality, our data indi-

    cated that conflict was not necessarily indicative of overall

    relationship negativity, suggesting an important distinction

     between these two concepts consistent with McGuire et al.

    (1996). In general support of our hypotheses, our results

    indicated sibling conflict is related to later relationship quality

    during this transition (over and above the quality of the rela-

    tionship at Time 1), though these associations differed based 

    on the aspect and content/domain of the conflicts, the aspect

    of relationship quality investigated, and structural features of 

    the relationship.Our findings advance the study of sibling relationships in

    several important ways. Currently, relatively little is known

    about emerging adult sibling relationships, and even less is

    known about this relationship during the transition to emerging

    adulthood (e.g., Whiteman et al., 2011). The present study is

    the first to our knowledge that has directly examined how sib-

    ling conflict and relationship quality are associated across this

    transition, and many of our findings indicate several processes

    that appear to be unique to this developmental period, particu-

    larly among sibling dyads that include at least one sister.

    Importantly, our findings suggest that sibling conflict during

    late adolescence may have some beneficial implications for the

    long-term functioning of the relationship.

    Key Findings

    We expected that more frequent and intense conflicts would be

    associated with lower positivity, higher negativity, and less

    egalitarian relationships 1 year later, given previous research

    (Campione-Barr & Smetana, 2010) that has found cross-

    sectional associations between these constructs during the ado-

    lescent years. Our hypotheses were only somewhat supported,

    however, indicating that there might be something unique

    about the transition to college that upsets these prior patterns.

    While Campione-Barr and Smetana (2010) found conflicts

    over personal domain invasions to be associated with lower 

    trust and communication among adolescent siblings, our results

    suggest that during the transition to college, both frequent and 

    intense conflicts over these issues were actually associated with

    less  negative relationships once older siblings moved away to

    college. Similarly, we found that more frequent conflicts over equality and fairness issues were typically associated with less

    negative and more positive relationships over time. (Note that

    while the interaction for positivity was only marginal, the sim-

     ple slope for emerging adults with younger sisters was highly

    significant.) Given these findings, it may be that siblings who

     previously struggled over such issues while living together now

    view their relationship in a more positive light simply by

    getting a ‘‘break’’ from living with one another.

    Importantly, however, among dyads including a younger 

    sister, intense conflicts over equality and fairness issues were

    related to more negative relationships the following year, indi-

    cating that we should still be cautious about the role sibling

    conflict plays during this developmental transition. It is possi- ble that as younger siblings strive to gain more autonomy and 

     perceived maturity during adolescence, they may push back 

    more intensely against their older siblings to gain a more equal

    role in the relationship. Our findings suggest that older siblings

    may not yet be comfortable with their younger siblings (sisters

     particularly) approaching their level of independence, maturity,

    and power within the family. This provides further evidence of 

    the importance of considering the content of sibling conflict, as

    well as conflict frequency and intensity separately, as they do

    appear to differentially impact relationships during this devel-

    opmental transition.

    Most of the associations we found between conflict and 

    relationship qualities were present in sibling dyads that included 

    at least one sister. Previous research has highlighted that females

    tend to report more intimacy and warmth in the sibling relation-

    ship during both adolescence (Kim et al., 2006) and emerging

    adulthood (Scharf et al., 2005), and our findings add another 

    important piece to this conversation. Specifically, conflict within

    these dyads was typically related to better relationships once sib-

    lings no longer lived together year round. It may be that relation-

    ships including sisters are uniquely immune to some detrimental

    effects of conflict in that these dyads’ relationships do not appear 

    to suffer despite the presence of conflict and that conflict may

    actually benefit their relationship over and above the prior qual-

    ity of their relationship. This is particularly encouraging, giventhe important positive implications sisters have on their siblings’

    well-being later in adulthood (Cicirelli, 1995).

    Our results generally support the notion that during the tran-

    sition to college, some topics of sibling conflict may actually

     play a protective role within the relationship. Previous research

    has suggested that sibling conflict increases social competence

    (Howe & Recchia, 2008; Vandell & Bailey, 1992), so perhaps

    with more conflict experience (especially by the end of adoles-

    cence), siblings are capable of more enriching and satisfactory

    relationships despite their squabbles. Although the siblings in

    the present study reported relatively low frequencies and 

    88   Emerging Adulthood 2(2)

     by Alina Ciucas on June 19, 2014eax.sagepub.comDownloaded from 

    http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/

  • 8/19/2019 Emerging Adulthood 2014 Lindell 79 91

    12/14

    intensities of conflict, the presence of at least some conflict

    does appear to facilitate more positive and less negative rela-

    tionships across this transition.

    Importantly, conflicts over equality and fairness issues in

     particular may help promote the development of more egalitar-

    ian sibling relationships, as indicated by our finding that

    younger brothers who report more intense conflicts over theseissues actually perceive more power within their relationship

    once their older sisters leave home. This finding is encoura-

    ging, given that achieving a relatively egalitarian sibling

    relationship is an important task of emerging adulthood (Aqui-

    lino, 2006). However, not all conflicts appear to assist such a

    task, as evidenced by our finding that more intense conflicts

    over personal domain invasions were associated with older 

    siblings’ perceptions of more power the following year, main-

    taining the power imbalances typical of earlier years (Perlman

    et al., 2000). In such families where this type of conflict is

    common, it may be helpful for parents to encourage sibling dei-

    dentification (Feinberg, McHale, Crouter, & Cumsille, 2003;

    Schachter, Shore, Feldman-Rotman, Marquis, & Campbell,1976), the process through which siblings actively pursue

    different domains of their lives to minimize competition and,

    thereby, conflict. By forging their own paths, younger siblings

    might be less interested in invading their older siblings’

     personal domain, perhaps facilitating an appropriately egalitar-

    ian relationship during emerging adulthood.

    Limitations and Contributions

    Although the present study advances the study of emerging adult

    sibling relationships in important ways, future research should 

    address some key limitations of thisstudy. First, the SiblingIssues

    Checklist (Campione-Barr & Smetana, 2010) was developed 

     based on pilot data from sixth to ninth graders, a sample younger 

    than those examined here, so future research on older adolescents

    and emerging adults should investigate other potential sources of 

    conflict. Additionally, it does not ask siblings to report who

    initiates conflicts. While older siblings tend to be the aggressors

    earlier in childhood (Vandell & Bailey, 1992), this pattern is less

    clear later on when younger siblings are more capable of standing

    their ground. Although we speculate that younger siblings would 

     be more likely to invade the personal domain of their older 

    siblings than vice versa (i.e., ‘‘tagging along’’ with their older 

    sibling), our data cannot confirm these patterns.

    Most of our participants were White and middle- to upper-middle class, and sibling relationships among other groups may

    follow a different course than our study suggests. Further, all

    firstborn participants left home to attend college, but not every-

    one attends college, and of those who do, many live at home

    while doing so. Indeed, Whiteman et al. (2011) illustrated that

    family relationships only improve during the transition to

    college if the firstborn child actually moves out of the family

    home. Future work should examine whether the same processes

    highlighted in the present study also apply when the oldest does

    not move away for college or pursues alternative paths, and 

    among minority and low socioeconomic status families.

    Finally, although we found support for several important

    moderations with regard to sibling ordinal position and gender 

    that were consistent with previous research highlighting the

    unique perceptions of females (Kim et al., 2006) and younger 

    siblings (Buhrmester & Furman, 1990), our sample was some-

    what small. Although similar analyses have been successfully

    conducted in previous literature with samples even smaller thanours (e.g., Puccinelli & Tickle-Degnen, 2004), future research

    examining larger samples of dyads is needed to replicate and 

    confirm the presence of these moderations.

    Despite these limitations, the present study offers several

    important contributions to the study of sibling relationships.

    Because of the transactional nature of sibling relationships,

    we utilized APIM (Kenny et al., 2006) to capture both actor and 

     partner effects in associations between sibling conflict and rela-

    tionship qualities during this developmental transition. Our 

    data indicated several significant partner effects that would 

    have otherwise gone unnoticed, indicating that the perception

    of one sibling is often related to the perceptions of the other 

    across time. We also identified important gender differences,highlighting that this contributes to many aspects of sibling

    relationships.

    While a growing body of research continues to examine

    sibling relationships during childhood and adolescence, the

    emerging adult years also appear to be particularly important.

    For the first time, the relationship is relatively voluntary and 

    not exclusively enforced by parents, so siblings have the oppor-

    tunity to develop their relationship on a new level in prepara-

    tion for their relationship as adults. By understanding how

    sibling relationships evolve during this developmental period,

    we might gain a better understanding of how earlier years setup

    siblings for enriching familial relationships during adulthood.

    Declaration of Conflicting Interests

    The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to

    the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

    Funding

    The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the

    research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This research is

    supported by the University of Missouri-Columbia Research Council

    (URC-10-094) and the University of Missouri System Research Board 

    (RB-08-025).

    ReferencesAdams, R. E., & Laursen, B. (2007). The correlates of conflict:

    Disagreement is not necessarily detrimental.   Journal of Family

     Psychology,  21, 445–458. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.21.3.445

    Aquilino, W. S. (2006). Family relationships and support systems in

    emerging adulthood. In J. J. Arnett & J. L. Tanner (Eds.), Emerging 

    adults in America: Coming of age in the 21st century   (pp.

    193–217). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    doi:10.1037/11381-008

    Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of development

    from the late teens through the twenties.   American Psychologist ,

    55, 469–480. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.55.5.469

    Lindell et al.   89

     by Alina Ciucas on June 19, 2014eax.sagepub.comDownloaded from 

    http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/

  • 8/19/2019 Emerging Adulthood 2014 Lindell 79 91

    13/14

    Brody, G. H. (1998). Sibling relationship quality: Its causes and con-

    sequences.  Annual Review of Psychology,  49, 1–24. doi:10.1146/

    annurev.psych.49.1.1

    Brody, G. H. (2004). Siblings’ direct and indirect contributions to

    child development.   Current Directions in Psychological Science,

    13, 124–126. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00289.x

    Buhrmester, D., & Furman, W. (1990). Perceptions of sibling relation-ships during middle childhood and adolescence.   Child Develop-

    ment ,  61, 1387–1398. doi:10.2307/113075

    Campione-Barr, N., Greer, K. B., & Kruse, A. (2013). Differential

    associations between domains of sibling conflict and adolescent

    emotional adjustment.   Child Development ,   84, 938–954. doi:10.

    1111/cdev.12022

    Campione-Barr, N., & Smetana, J. G. (2010). ‘‘Who said you can wear 

    my sweater?’’ Adolescent siblings’ conflicts and associations with

    relationship quality.   Child Development ,   81, 464–471. doi:10.

    1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01407.x

    Cicirelli, V. G. (1995). Sibling relationships across the life span. New

    York, NY: Plenum.

    Conger, K. J., & Little, W. M. (2010). Sibling relationships during thetransition to adulthood. Child Development Perspectives, 4, 87–94.

    doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00123.x

    Cox, M. J. (2010). Family systems and family relationships.   Child 

     Development Perspectives,   4, 95–96. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.

    2010.00124.x

    Criss, M. M., & Shaw, D. S. (2005). Sibling relationships as contexts

    for delinquency training in low-income families.  Journal of Family

     Psychology,  19, 592–600. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.19.4.592

    Dunn, J. (2002). Sibling relationships. In P. K. Smith & C. H. Hart

    (Eds.),  Blackwell handbook of childhood social development  (pp.

    223–237). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.

    Feinberg, M. E., McHale, S. M., Crouter, A. C., & Cumsille, P. (2003).

    Sibling differentiation: Sibling and parent relationship trajectories

    in adolescence.  Child Development ,  74, 1261–1274. doi:10.1111/

    1467-8624.00606

    Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes.  Human

     Relations,  7 , 117–140. doi:10.1177/001872675400700202

    Furman, W. (1996). The measurement of friendship perceptions:

    Conceptual and methodological issues. In W. M. Bukowski, A.

    F. Newcomb & W. W. Hartup (Eds.),   The company they keep:

     Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp. 41–65). New York,

     NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1985). Children’s perceptions of the

     personal relationships in their social networks. Developmental Psy-

    chology, 21, 1016–1024. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.21.6.1016Howe, N., & Recchia, H. E. (2008). Siblings and sibling rivalry. In M.

    M. Haith & J. B. Benson (Eds.),  Encyclopedia of infant and early

    childhood development   (Vol.   3, pp. 154–164). San Diego, CA:

    Academic Press.

    Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Cook, W. L. (2006).  Dyadic data anal-

     ysis. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Kim, J.-Y., McHale, S. M., Crouter, A. C., & Osgood, D. (2007).

    Longitudinal linkages between sibling relationships and adjust-

    ment from middle childhood through adolescence. Developmental 

     Psychology,  43, 960–973. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.960

    Kim, J.-Y., McHale, S. M., Osgood, D. W., & Crouter, A. C. (2006).

    Longitudinal course and family correlates of sibling relationships

    from childhood through adolescence.   Child Development ,   77 ,

    1746–1761.

    Kloep, M., & Hendry, L. B. (2010). Letting go or holding on? Parents’

     perceptions of their relationships with their children during emer-

    ging adulthood. British Journal of Developmental Psychology

    , 28

    ,817–834. doi:10.1348/026151009X480581

    Laursen, B., Coy, K. C., & Collins, W. A. (1998). Reconsidering

    changes in parent-child conflict across adolescence: A meta-anal-

    ysis. Child Development ,  69, 817–832. doi:10.2307/1132206

    McGuire, S., McHale, S. M., & Updegraff, K. (1996). Children’s

     perceptions of the sibling relationship in middle childhood:

    Connections within and between family relationships.   Personal 

     Relationships,  3, 229–239.

    McHale, S. M., Kim, J.-Y., & Whiteman, S. D. (2006). Sibling rela-

    tionships in childhood and adolescence. In P. Noller & J. A. Feeney

    (Eds.),   Close relationships: Functions, forms and processes   (pp.

    127–149). Hove, England: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis.

    Minuchin, P. (1985). Families and individual development: Provoca-tions from the field of family therapy.   Child Development ,   56 ,

    289–302. doi:10.2307/1129720

    Montemayor, R. (1983). Parents and adolescents in conflict: All

    families some of the time and some families most of the time.

    The Journal of Early Adolescence,   3, 83–103. doi:10.1177/

    027243168331007

    Padilla-Walker, L. M., Harper, J. M., & Jensen, A. C. (2010). Self-

    regulation as a mediator between sibling relationship quality and 

    early adolescents’ positive and negative outcomes.   Journal of   

     Family Psychology, 24, 419–428. doi:10.1037/a0020387

    Padilla-Walker, L. M., & Nelson, L. J. (2012). Black hawk down?:

    Establishing helicopter parenting as a distinct construct from

    other forms of parental control during emerging adulthood. Jour-

    nal of Adolescence,   35, 1177–1190. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.

    2012.03.007

    Padilla-Walker, L. M., Nelson, L. J., & Carroll, J. S. (2012). Affording

    emerging adulthood: Parental financial assistance of their college-

    aged children.  Journal of Adult Development ,  19, 50–58. doi:10.

    1007/s10804-011-9134-y

    Perlman, M., Siddiqui, A., Ram, A., & Ross, H. S. (2000). An analysis

    of sources of power in children’s conflict interactions. In R. S. L.

    Mills & S. Duck (Eds.), The developmental psychology of personal 

    relationships  (pp. 155–174). New York, NY: John Wiley.

    Puccinelli, N. M., & Tickle-Degnen, L. (2004). Knowing too much

    about others: Moderators of the relationship between eavesdrop- ping and rapport in social interaction.  Journal of Nonverbal Beha-

    vior ,  28, 223–243. doi:10.1037/t01087-000

    Schachter, F. F., Shore, E., Feldman-Rotman, S., Marquis, R. E., &

    Campbell, S. (1976). Sibling deidentification.   Developmental 

     Psychology,  12, 418–427. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.12.5.418

    Schafer, J. L. (1997).   Analysis of incomplete multivariate data.

    London, England: Chapman & Hall.

    Scharf, M., Shulman, S., & Avigad-Spitz, L. (2005). Sibling relation-

    ships in emerging adulthood and adolescence.  Journal of Adoles-

    cent Research,  20, 64–90. doi:10.1177/0743558404271133

    90   Emerging Adulthood 2(2)

     by Alina Ciucas on June 19, 2014eax.sagepub.comDownloaded from 

    http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/http://eax.sagepub.com/

  • 8/19/2019 Emerging Adulthood 2014 Lindell 79 91

    14/14

    Shanahan, L., McHale, S. M., Crouter, A. C., & Osgood, D. W. (2008).

    Linkages between parents’ differential treatment, youth depressive

    symptoms, and sibling relationships.   Journal of Marriage and 

     Family,  70, 480–494. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2008.00495.x

    Shortt, J. W., & Gottman, J. M. (1997). Closeness in young adult

    sibling relationships: Affective and physiological processes.

    Social Development ,

      6 , 142–164. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.1997.tb00099.x

    Smetana, J. G. (2013). Moral development: The social domain theory

    view. In P. Zelazo (Ed.),   Oxford handbook of developmental 

     psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 832–866). New York, NY: Oxford Univer-

    sity Press.

    Stocker, C. M., Burwell, R. A., & Briggs, M. L. (2002). Sibling con-

    flict in middle childhood predicts children’s adjustment in early

    adolescence.   Journal of Family Psychology,   16 , 50–57. doi:10.

    1037//0893-3200.16.1.50

    Stocker, C. M., Lanthier, R. P., & Furman, W. (1997). Sibling

    relationships in early adulthood.  Journal of Family Psychology,

    11, 210–221. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.11.2.210

    Turiel, E. (2002). The culture of morality: Social development, context,and conflict . Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statis-

    tics. (2011). Table 213: Enrollment rates of 18- to 24-year-olds in

    degree-granting institutions, by level of institution and sex and 

    race/ethnicity of student: 1967 through 2010. In U. S. Department

    of Education, National Center for Education Statistics (Ed.), Digest 

    of education statistics. Retrieved from   http://nces.ed.gov/pro-

    grams/digest/d11/tables/dt11_213.asp

    Vandell, D. L., & Bailey, M. D. (1992). Conflicts between siblings. In

    C. U. Shantz & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), Conflict in child and adoles-

    cent development   (pp. 242–269). New York, NY: Cambridge

    University Press.

    West, T. V., Popp, D., & Kenny, D. A. (2008). A guide for the esti-

    mation of gender and sexual orientation effects in dyadic data:

    An actor-partner interdependence model approach.   Personality

    and Social Psychology Bulletin,   34, 321–336. doi:10.1177/

    0146167207311199

    Whiteman, S. D., McHale, S. M., & Crouter, A. C. (2011). Family

    relationships from adolescence to early adulthood: Changes in thefamily system following firstborns’ leaving home.   Journal of   

     Research on Adolescence,   21, 461–474. doi:10.1111/j.1532-

    7795.2010.00683.x

    Wood, J. T., & Inman, C. C. (1993). In a different mode: Masculine

    styles of communicating closeness.  Journal of Applied Communi-

    cation Research,  21, 279–295. doi:10.1080/00909889309365372

    Author Biographies

    Anna K. Lindell  is a doctoral student in the developmental

     psychology area of the Department of Psychological Sciences

    at the University of Missouri. Her primary research interests

    are family relationships during adolescence and emergingadulthood.

    Nicole Campione-Barr  is an assistant professor in the devel-

    opmental psychology area of the Department of Psychological

    Sciences at the University of Missouri. Her primary research

    interests are adolescents’ relationships with parents and 

    siblings.

    Kelly Bassett Greer  is a recent doctoral graduate in develop-

    mental psychology from the Department of Psychological

    Sciences at the University of Missouri. Her primary research

    interests include adolescent sibling and family relationships.

    Lindell et al.   91

    http://nces.ed.gov/programs/digest/d11/tables/dt11_213.asphttp://nces.ed.gov/programs/digest/d11/tables/dt11_213.asphttp://eax.sagepub.com/http://nces.ed.gov/programs/digest/d11/tables/dt11_213.asphttp://nces.ed.gov/programs/digest/d11/tables/dt11_213.asp